This is a modern-English version of Zoonomia; Or, the Laws of Organic Life, Vol. I, originally written by Darwin, Erasmus. It has been thoroughly updated, including changes to sentence structure, words, spelling, and grammar—to ensure clarity for contemporary readers, while preserving the original spirit and nuance. If you click on a paragraph, you will see the original text that we modified, and you can toggle between the two versions.

Scroll to the bottom of this page and you will find a free ePUB download link for this book.


ZOONOMIA;

OR,

OR,

THE LAWS

OF

OF

ORGANIC LIFE.

VOL. I.

By ERASMUS DARWIN, M.D. F.R.S.

AUTHOR OF THE BOTANIC GARDEN.

AUTHOR OF THE BOTANICAL GARDEN.



Principiò cœlum, ac terras, camposque liquentes,

Principio cielo, y tierras, y campos fluidos,

Lucentemque globum lunæ, titaniaque astra,

Shining moon and shimmering stars,

Spiritus intùs alit, totamque infusa per artus

Spirit within nourishes and flows through all my limbs.

Mens agitat molem, et magno se corpore miscet.—VIRG. Æn. vi.

Mens moves the mass, and it mingles itself with a great body.—VIRG. Æn. vi.

Earth, on whose lap a thousand nations tread,

Earth, where a thousand nations walk,

And Ocean, brooding his prolific bed,

And Ocean, contemplating his abundant depths,

Night's changeful orb, blue pole, and silvery zones,

Night's shifting moon, blue sky, and silvery areas,

Where other worlds encircle other suns,

Where different worlds orbit other stars,

One Mind inhabits, one diffusive Soul

One Mind exists, one spreading Soul

Wields the large limbs, and mingles with the whole.

Wields the big limbs, and mixes with the whole.



THE SECOND EDITION, CORRECTED.



LONDON:
PRINTED FOR. J. JOHNSON, IN ST. PAUL'S CHURCH-YARD.
1796.

LONDON:
PRINTED FOR J. JOHNSON, IN ST. PAUL'S CHURCHYARD.
1796.

Entered at Stationers' Hall.



DEDICATION.

To the candid and ingenious Members of the College of Physicians, of the Royal Philosophical Society, of the Two Universities, and to all those, who study the Operations of the Mind as a Science, or who practice Medicine as a Profession, the subsequent Work is, with great respect, inscribed by the Author,

To the honest and creative Members of the College of Physicians, the Royal Philosophical Society, the Two Universities, and to everyone who explores the workings of the mind as a science or practices medicine as a profession, the following work is respectfully dedicated by the author,

DERBY, May 1, 1794.

DERBY, May 1, 1794.

CONTENTS.

Preface.
SECT. I. Of Motion.
II. Explanations and Definitions.
III. The Motions of the Retina demonstrated by Experiments.
IV. Laws of Animal Causation.
V. Of the four Faculties or Motions of the Sensorium.
VI. Of the four Classes of Fibrous Motions.
VII. Of Irritative Motions.
VIII. Of Sensitive Motions.
IX. Of Voluntary Motions.
X. Of Associate Motions.
XI. Additional Observations on the Sensorial Powers.
XII. Of Stimulus, Sensorial Exertion, and Fibrous Contraction.
XIII. Of Vegetable Animation.
XIV. Of the Production of Ideas.
XV. Of the Classes of Ideas.
XVI. Of Instinct.
XVII. The Catenation of Animal Motions.
XVIII. Of Sleep.
XIX. Of Reverie.
XX. Of Vertigo.
XXI. Of Drunkenness.
XXII. Of Propensity to Motion. Repetition. Imitation.
XXIII. Of the Circulatory System.
XXIV. Of the Secretion of Saliva, and of Tears. And of the Lacrymal Sack.
XXV. Of the Stomach and Intestines.
XXVI. Of the Capillary Glands, and of the Membranes.
XXVII. Of Hemorrhages.
XXVIII. The Paralysis of the Lacteals.
XXIX. The Retrograde Motions of the Absorbent Vessels.
XXX. The Paralysis of the Liver.
XXXI. Of Temperaments.
XXXII. Diseases of Irritation.
XXXIII. —— of Sensation.
XXXIV. —— of Volition.
XXXV. —— of Relation.
XXXVI. The Periods of Diseases.
XXXVII. Of Digestion, Secretion, Nutrition.
XXXVIII. Of the Oxygenation of the Blood in the Lungs and Placenta.
XXXIX. Of Generation.
XL. Of Ocular Spectra.


TO

TO

ERASMUS DARWIN,

ON HIS WORK INTITLED

ON HIS WORK TITLED

ZOONOMIA,

By DEWHURST BILSBORROW.



HAIL TO THE BARD! who sung, from Chaos hurl'd

HAIL TO THE BARD! who sang, thrown from Chaos

How suns and planets form'd the whirling world;

How suns and planets formed the spinning world;

How sphere on sphere Earth's hidden strata bend,

How layers on layers of Earth’s hidden layers curve,

And caves of rock her central fires defend;

And rocky caves protect her central fires;

5

Where gems new-born their twinkling eyes unfold,

Where newly born gems reveal their sparkling eyes,

And young ores shoot in arborescent gold.

And young metals rise like golden trees.

How the fair Flower, by Zephyr woo'd, unfurls

How the beautiful flower, courted by the gentle breeze, opens up

Its panting leaves, and waves its azure curls;

Its gasping leaves, and sways its blue curls;

Or spreads in gay undress its lucid form

Or spreads in bright attire its clear shape

10

To meet the sun, and shuts it to the storm;

To face the sun, and blocks it from the storm;

While in green veins impassion'd eddies move,

While in green veins passionate currents flow,

And Beauty kindles into life and love.

And Beauty sparks life and love.

How the first embryon-fibre, sphere, or cube,

How the first embryonic fiber, sphere, or cube,

Lives in new forms,—a line,—a ring,—a tube;

Lives in new forms—a line, a ring, a tube;

15

Closed in the womb with limbs unfinish'd laves,

Closed in the womb with incomplete limbs,

Sips with rude mouth the salutary waves;

Sips with a rude mouth the healing waves;

Seeks round its cell the sanguine streams, that pass,

Seeks around its cell the lively streams, that pass,

And drinks with crimson gills the vital gas;

And drinks the vital gas with red gills;

Weaves with soft threads the blue meandering vein,

Weaves with soft threads the blue winding vein,

20

The heart's red concave, and the silver brain;

The heart's red curve and the shiny brain;

Leads the long nerve, expands the impatient sense,

Leads the long nerve, expands the impatient sense,

And clothes in silken skin the nascent Ens.

And clothes made of silky skin the emerging Being.

Erewhile, emerging from its liquid bed,

Erewhile, emerging from its liquid bed,

It lifts in gelid air its nodding head;

It rises in cold air, its head bobbing;

25

The lights first dawn with trembling eyelid hails,

The lights first emerge with quivering eyelids,

With lungs untaught arrests the balmy gales;

With untrained lungs, it stops the gentle breezes;

Tries its new tongue in tones unknown, and hears

Tries out its new voice in unfamiliar tones, and hears

The strange vibrations with unpractised ears;

The strange vibrations to untrained ears;

Seeks with spread hands the bosom's velvet orbs.

Seeks with outstretched hands the soft curves of the chest.

30

With closing lips the milky fount absorbs;

With closed lips, the creamy source takes it all in;

And, as compress'd the dulcet streams distil,

And, as the sweet streams flow,

Drinks warmth and fragrance from the living rill;—

Drinks warmth and fragrance from the flowing stream;—

Eyes with mute rapture every waving line,

Eyes with silent delight at every waving line,

Prints with adoring kiss the Paphian shrine,

Prints with a loving kiss the Paphian shrine,

35

And learns erelong, the perfect form confess'd,

And soon realizes, the perfect form revealed,

Ideal Beauty from its mother's breast.

Ideal Beauty from its mother's breast.

Now in strong lines, with bolder tints design'd,

Now in bold lines, with brighter colors designed,

You sketch ideas, and portray the mind;

You outline concepts and express thoughts;

Teach how fine atoms of impinging light

Teach how tiny particles of light撞击.

40

To ceaseless change the visual sense excite;

To constantly change excites the visual sense;

While the bright lens collects the rays, that swerve,

While the bright lens gathers the rays that curve,

And bends their focus on the moving nerve.

And directs their attention to the moving nerve.

How thoughts to thoughts are link'd with viewless chains,

How thoughts are connected by invisible chains,

Tribes leading tribes, and trains pursuing trains;

Tribes leading other tribes, and trains chasing after trains;

45

With shadowy trident how Volition guides,

With a shadowy trident, how Volition leads,

Surge after surge, his intellectual tides;

Surge after surge, his waves of thought;

Or, Queen of Sleep, Imagination roves

Or, Queen of Sleep, Imagination wanders

With frantic Sorrows, or delirious Loves.

With frantic sorrows or wild loves.

Go on, O FRIEND! explore with eagle-eye;

Go ahead, O FFRIEND! explore with sharp vision;

50

Where wrapp'd in night retiring Causes lie:

Where hidden in the night, the reasons remain:

Trace their slight bands, their secret haunts betray,

Trace their faint paths, their hidden spots reveal,

And give new wonders to the beam of day;

And bring new wonders to the daylight;

Till, link by link with step aspiring trod,

Till, step by step, we walked forward,

You climb from NATURE to the throne of GOD.

You rise from NNATURE to the throne of GOD.

55

—So saw the Patriarch with admiring eyes

—So saw the Patriarch with admiring eyes

From earth to heaven a golden ladder rise;

From earth to heaven, a golden ladder rises;

Involv'd in clouds the mystic scale ascends,

Involved in clouds, the mysterious scale rises,

And brutes and angels crowd the distant ends.

And animals and angels fill the far ends.

TRIN. COL. CAMBRIDGE, Jan. 1, 1794.

T.R.I.N. C.O.L. CAMBRIDGE, Jan. 1, 1794.



REFERENCES TO THE WORK.

Botanic Garden. Part I.

Botanic Garden. Part 1.

Line 1. Canto I. l. 105.

Line 1. Canto I. l. 105.

—— 3. —— IV. l. 402.

—— 3. —— IV. l. 402.

—— 4. —— I. l. 140.

—— 4. —— I. l. 140.

—— 5. —— III. l. 401.

—— 5. —— III. l. 401.

—— 8. —— IV. l. 452.

—— 8. —— IV. l. 452.

—— 9. —— I. l. 14.

—— 9. —— I. l. 14.

Zoonomia.

Zoonomia.

—— 12. Sect. XIII.

12. Sect. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— 13. —— XXXIX. 4. 1.

—— 13. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— 18. —— XVI. 2. and XXXVIII.

—— 18. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— 26. —— XVI. 4.

—— 26. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— 30. —— XVI. 4.

—— 30. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— 36. —— XVI. 6.

—— 36. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— 38. —— III. and VII.

—— 38. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— 43. —— X.

—— 43. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— 44. —— XVIII. 17.

—— 44. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— 45. —— XVII. 3. 7.

—— 45. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— 47. —— XVIII. 8.

—— 47. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— 50. —— XXXIX. 4. 8.

50. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— 51. —— XXXIX the Motto.

—— 51. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__ the Motto.

—— 54. —— XXXIX. 8.

—— 54. —— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.



PREFACE.



The purport of the following pages is an endeavour to reduce the facts belonging to ANIMAL LIFE into classes, orders, genera, and species; and, by comparing them with each other, to unravel the theory of diseases. It happened, perhaps unfortunately for the inquirers into the knowledge of diseases, that other sciences had received improvement previous to their own; whence, instead of comparing the properties belonging to animated nature with each other, they, idly ingenious, busied themselves in attempting to explain the laws of life by those of mechanism and chemistry; they considered the body as an hydraulic machine, and the fluids as passing through a series of chemical changes, forgetting that animation was its essential characteristic.

The purpose of the following pages is to categorize the facts related to ANIMAL LIFE into classes, orders, genera, and species; and by comparing them with one another, to uncover the theory of diseases. It happened, perhaps unfortunately for those investigating diseases, that other sciences advanced before their own; thus, instead of comparing the properties of living beings with each other, they, in a misguided effort, focused on trying to explain the laws of life through the lens of mechanics and chemistry. They viewed the body as a hydraulic machine, and the fluids as undergoing a series of chemical transformations, forgetting that life itself was the essential characteristic.

The great CREATOR of all things has infinitely diversified the works of his hands, but has at the same time stamped a certain similitude on the features of nature, that demonstrates to us, that the whole is one family of one parent. On this similitude is founded all rational analogy; which, so long as it is concerned in comparing the essential properties of bodies, leads us to many and important discoveries; but when with licentious activity it links together objects, otherwise discordant, by some fanciful similitude; it may indeed collect ornaments for wit and poetry, but philosophy and truth recoil from its combinations.

The great CCREATOR of everything has created an endless variety of works, yet at the same time has given a certain resemblance to the features of nature, showing us that the whole is one family of one parent. This resemblance is the basis for all logical comparisons; as long as it focuses on comparing the essential properties of things, it leads us to many important discoveries. However, when it recklessly connects otherwise unrelated objects through some fanciful similarity, it might gather decorative elements for wit and poetry, but philosophy and truth shy away from its combinations.

The want of a theory, deduced from such strict analogy, to conduct the practice of medicine is lamented by its professors; for, as a great number of unconnected facts are difficult to be acquired, and to be reasoned from, the art of medicine is in many instances less efficacious under the direction of its wisest practitioners; and by that busy crowd, who either boldly wade in darkness, or are led into endless error by the glare of false theory, it is daily practised to the destruction of thousands; add to this the unceasing injury which accrues to the public by the perpetual advertisements of pretended nostrums; the minds of the indolent become superstitiously fearful of diseases, which they do not labour under; and thus become the daily prey of some crafty empyric.

The lack of a solid theory, based on a clear analogy, to guide the practice of medicine is a concern for its experts. Many unconnected facts are hard to learn and reason from, which means the art of medicine can sometimes be less effective even in the hands of its best practitioners. Meanwhile, a large number of people either dive headfirst into confusion or are misled by false theories, leading to daily practices that harm thousands. This is compounded by the constant damage caused to the public by countless ads for fake remedies; the lazy become irrationally scared of illnesses they don’t even have, making them easy targets for some unscrupulous quack.

A theory founded upon nature, that should bind together the scattered facts of medical knowledge, and converge into one point of view the laws of organic life, would thus on many accounts contribute to the interest of society. It would capacitate men of moderate abilities to practise the art of healing with real advantage to the public; it would enable every one of literary acquirements to distinguish the genuine disciples of medicine from those of boastful effrontery, or of wily address; and would teach mankind in some important situations the knowledge of themselves.

A theory based on nature, which could connect the scattered facts of medical knowledge and unify the principles of organic life, would greatly benefit society in many ways. It would allow people with average skills to practice the art of healing effectively for the public good; it would help anyone with an education identify the true followers of medicine versus those who are just boastful or cunning; and it would educate people in important situations about knowledge of themselves.

There are some modern practitioners, who declaim against medical theory in general, not considering that to think is to theorize; and that no one can direct a method of cure to a person labouring under disease without thinking, that is, without theorizing; and happy therefore is the patient, whose physician possesses the best theory.

There are some modern practitioners who criticize medical theory in general, not realizing that to think is to theorize; and no one can successfully treat a patient suffering from an illness without thinking, or in other words, without theorizing; and fortunately for the patient, the physician who has the best theory is the one to have.

The words idea, perception, sensation, recollection, suggestion, and association, are each of them used in this treatise in a more limited sense than in the writers of metaphysic. The author was in doubt, whether he should rather have substituted new words instead of them; but was at length of opinion, that new definitions of words already in use would be less burthensome to the memory of the reader.

The words idea, perception, sensation, recollection, suggestion, and association are each used in this work in a more specific way than in metaphysical writings. The author considered whether he should use new words instead; however, he ultimately decided that providing new definitions for existing words would be easier for the reader to remember.

A great part of this work has lain by the writer above twenty years, as some of his friends can testify: he had hoped by frequent revision to have made it more worthy the acceptance of the public; this however his other perpetual occupations have in part prevented, and may continue to prevent, as long as he may be capable of revising it; he therefore begs of the candid reader to accept of it in its present state, and to excuse any inaccuracies of expression, or of conclusion, into which the intricacy of his subject, the general imperfection of language, or the frailty he has in common with other men, may have betrayed him; and from which he has not the vanity to believe this treatise to be exempt.

A large part of this work has been set aside by the author for over twenty years, as some of his friends can confirm: he had hoped that by revising it frequently, it would become more deserving of the public’s acceptance; however, his ongoing commitments have partially prevented this and may continue to do so for as long as he is able to revise it. He therefore asks the kind reader to accept it in its current form and to overlook any inaccuracies in expression or conclusion that the complexity of the subject, the general limitations of language, or his own human flaws may have caused; he does not have the arrogance to believe this treatise is exempt from such issues.



ZOONOMIA.



SECT. I.

OF MOTION.

Motion.

The whole of nature may be supposed to consist of two essences or substances; one of which may be termed spirit, and the other matter. The former of these possesses the power to commence or produce motion, and the latter to receive and communicate it. So that motion, considered as a cause, immediately precedes every effect; and, considered as an effect, it immediately succeeds every cause.

Nature can be thought of as made up of two essences or substances: one we can call spirit and the other matter. The first has the ability to start or create motion, while the second can receive and transmit it. So, when we think of motion as a cause, it comes right before every effect; and when we see it as an effect, it comes right after every cause.

The MOTIONS OF MATTER may be divided into two kinds, primary and secondary. The secondary motions are those, which are given to or received from other matter in motion. Their laws have been successfully investigated by philosophers in their treatises on mechanic powers. These motions are distinguished by this circumstance, that the velocity multiplied into the quantity of matter of the body acted upon is equal to the velocity multiplied into the quantity of matter of the acting body.

The Motions of matter can be divided into two types: primary and secondary. Secondary motions are those that a body in motion gives to or receives from another body in motion. Philosophers have successfully explored their laws in their writings on mechanics. These motions are characterized by the fact that the velocity multiplied by the mass of the body being acted upon is equal to the velocity multiplied by the mass of the acting body.

The primary motions of matter may be divided into three classes, those belonging to gravitation, to chemistry, and to life; and each class has its peculiar laws. Though these three classes include the motions of solid, liquid, and aerial bodies; there is nevertheless a fourth division of motions; I mean those of the supposed ethereal fluids of magnetism, electricity, heat, and light; whose properties are not so well investigated as to be classed with sufficient accuracy.

The main movements of matter can be categorized into three groups: those related to gravity, chemistry, and life; each with its own distinct laws. While these three groups cover the movements of solids, liquids, and gases, there is still a fourth category of movements—the hypothetical ethereal fluids of magnetism, electricity, heat, and light; their properties haven't been studied enough to be classified with adequate precision.

1st. The gravitating motions include the annual and diurnal rotation of the earth and planets, the flux and reflux of the ocean, the descent of heavy bodies, and other phænomena of gravitation. The unparalleled sagacity of the great NEWTON has deduced the laws of this class of motions from the simple principle of the general attraction of matter. These motions are distinguished by their tendency to or from the centers of the sun or planets.

1st. The gravitational motions include the yearly and daily rotation of the earth and planets, the rise and fall of the ocean, the falling of heavy objects, and other phenomena of gravity. The unmatched insight of the great NEWTON has derived the laws governing this type of motion from the straightforward principle of the universal attraction of matter. These motions are characterized by their tendency to move toward or away from the centers of the sun or planets.

2d. The chemical class of motions includes all the various appearances of chemistry. Many of the facts, which belong to these branches of science, are nicely ascertained, and elegantly classed; but their laws have not yet been developed from such simple principles as those above-mentioned; though it is probable, that they depend on the specific attractions belonging to the particles of bodies, or to the difference of the quantity of attraction belonging to the sides and angles of those particles. The chemical motions are distinguished by their being generally attended with an evident decomposition or new combination of the active materials.

2d. The chemical class of motions includes all the different aspects of chemistry. Many of the facts related to these branches of science are well-established and neatly categorized; however, their laws haven't been derived from such straightforward principles as those mentioned earlier. It's likely that they depend on the specific attractions of the particles in bodies or on the varying amount of attraction related to the sides and angles of those particles. Chemical motions are characterized by the noticeable breakdown or new combination of the active materials.

3d. The third class includes all the motions of the animal and vegetable world; as well those of the vessels, which circulate their juices, and of the muscles, which perform their locomotion, as those of the organs of sense, which constitute their ideas.

3d. The third class includes all the movements of the animal and plant world; both those of the vessels that circulate their fluids, and the muscles that enable movement, as well as those of the sensory organs that create their perceptions.

This last class of motion is the subject of the following pages; which, though conscious of their many imperfections, I hope may give some pleasure to the patient reader, and contribute something to the knowledge and to the cure of diseases.

This final type of motion is the focus of the following pages; even though I’m aware of their many flaws, I hope they bring some enjoyment to the patient reader and add to our understanding and treatment of diseases.



SECT. II.

EXPLANATIONS AND DEFINITIONS.

Explanations and Definitions.

I. Outline of the animal economy.II. 1. Of the sensorium. 2. Of the brain and nervous medulla. 3. A nerve. 4. A muscular fibre. 5. The immediate organs of sense. 6. The external organs of sense. 7. An idea or sensual motion. 8. Perception. 9. Sensation. 10. Recollection and suggestion. 11. Habit, causation, association, catenation. 12. Reflex ideas. 13. Stimulus defined.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Overview of the animal economy.—__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. About the senses. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. The brain and spinal cord. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. A nerve. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. A muscle fiber. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. The primary sensory organs. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. The external sensory organs. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. An idea or physical movement. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Perception. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Sensation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. Memory and suggestion. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. Habit, cause and effect, association, linking. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. Reflex ideas. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. Definition of stimulus.



As some explanations and definitions will be necessary in the prosecution of the work, the reader is troubled with them in this place, and is intreated to keep them in his mind as he proceeds, and to take them for granted, till an apt opportunity occurs to evince their truth; to which I shall premise a very short outline of the animal economy.

Since some explanations and definitions are necessary to continue with the work, the reader is given them here and is asked to keep them in mind while reading on and to accept them as facts until there’s a good time to demonstrate their accuracy. To begin, I will provide a brief overview of the animal economy.



I.—1. The nervous system has its origin from the brain, and is distributed to every part of the body. Those nerves, which serve the senses, principally arise from that part of the brain, which is lodged in the head; and those, which serve the purposes of muscular motion, principally arise from that part of the brain, which is lodged in the neck and back, and which is erroneously called the spinal marrow. The ultimate fibrils of these nerves terminate in the immediate organs of sense and muscular fibres, and if a ligature be put on any part of their passage from the head or spine, all motion and perception cease in the parts beneath the ligature.

I.—1. The nervous system starts from the brain and spreads out to every part of the body. The nerves responsible for the senses mainly come from the part of the brain located in the head, while the nerves responsible for muscle movement mainly come from the part of the brain located in the neck and back, which is mistakenly referred to as the spinal cord. The tiny fibers of these nerves end in the sensory organs and muscle fibers, and if a band or tight material is placed on any part of their pathway from the head or spine, all movement and sensation below that point stop.

2. The longitudinal muscular fibres compose the locomotive muscles, whose contractions move the bones of the limbs and trunk, to which their extremities are attached. The annular or spiral muscular fibres compose the vascular muscles, which constitute the intestinal canal, the arteries, veins, glands, and absorbent vessels.

2. The long muscle fibers make up the movement muscles, which contract to move the bones in the limbs and torso to which they are connected. The ring or spiral muscle fibers form the vascular muscles, which make up the intestinal canal, arteries, veins, glands, and lymphatic vessels.

3. The immediate organs of sense, as the retina of the eye, probably consist of moving fibrils, with a power of contraction similar to that of the larger muscles above described.

3. The main sensory organs, like the retina in the eye, likely consist of moving fibers that can contract in a way similar to the larger muscles mentioned earlier.

4. The cellular membrane consists of cells, which resemble those of a sponge, communicating with each other, and connecting together all the other parts of the body.

4. The cell membrane is made up of cells that look like those of a sponge, communicating with each other and linking all the other parts of the body together.

5. The arterial system consists of the aortal and the pulmonary artery, which are attended through their whole course with their correspondent veins. The pulmonary artery receives the blood from the right chamber of the heart, and carries it to the minute extensive ramifications of the lungs, where it is exposed to the action of the air on a surface equal to that of the whole external skin, through the thin moist coats of those vessels, which are spread on the air-cells, which constitute the minute terminal ramifications of the wind-pipe. Here the blood changes its colour from a dark red to a bright scarlet. It is then collected by the branches of the pulmonary vein, and conveyed to the left chamber of the heart.

5. The arterial system includes the aorta and the pulmonary artery, which are accompanied throughout their entire length by their corresponding veins. The pulmonary artery receives blood from the right chamber of the heart and carries it to the vast network of the lungs, where it comes into contact with air over a surface area that's as large as the entire outer skin. This happens through the thin, moist walls of the vessels that are spread across the air sacs, which make up the tiny terminal branches of the windpipe. Here, the blood changes from dark red to bright scarlet. It is then gathered by the branches of the pulmonary vein and transported to the left chamber of the heart.

6. The aorta is another large artery, which receives the blood from the left chamber of the heart, after it has been thus aerated in the lungs, and conveys it by ascending and descending branches to every other part of the system; the extremities of this artery terminate either in glands, as the salivary glands, lacrymal glands, &c. or in capillary vessels, which are probably less involuted glands; in these some fluid, as saliva, tears, perspiration, are separated from the blood; and the remainder of the blood is absorbed or drank up by branches of veins correspondent to the branches of the artery; which are furnished with valves to prevent its return; and is thus carried back, after having again changed its colour to a dark red, to the right chamber of the heart. The circulation of the blood in the liver differs from this general system; for the veins which drink up the refluent blood from those arteries, which are spread on the bowels and mesentery, unite into a trunk in the liver, and form a kind of artery, which is branched into the whole substance of the liver, and is called the vena portarum; and from which the bile is separated by the numerous hepatic glands, which constitute that viscus.

6. The aorta is another major artery that takes blood from the left chamber of the heart after it has been oxygenated in the lungs and sends it through ascending and descending branches to every part of the body. The ends of this artery branch out either into glands, like the salivary glands and tear glands, or into capillary vessels, which are probably simpler forms of glands. In these, fluids like saliva, tears, and sweat are extracted from the blood, while the remaining blood is absorbed by veins that correspond to the artery branches. These veins have valves to prevent the blood from flowing backward and carry it back to the right chamber of the heart, where it changes to a dark red color. Blood circulation in the liver is different from this overall system; the veins that collect the returning blood from the arteries distributed across the intestines and mesentery merge into a trunk in the liver, forming a type of artery called the vena portarum, which branches throughout the liver and from which bile is extracted by numerous hepatic glands that make up this organ.

7. The glands may be divided into three systems, the convoluted glands, such as those above described, which separate bile, tears, saliva, &c. Secondly, the glands without convolution, as the capillary vessels, which unite the terminations of the arteries and veins; and separate both the mucus, which lubricates the cellular membrane, and the perspirable matter, which preserves the skin moist and flexible. And thirdly, the whole absorbent system, consisting of the lacteals, which open their mouths into the stomach and intestines, and of the lymphatics, which open their mouths on the external surface of the body, and on the internal linings of all the cells of the cellular membrane, and other cavities of the body.

7. The glands can be categorized into three systems: first, the convoluted glands, like those just mentioned, that produce bile, tears, saliva, etc. Second, the glands without convolution, such as the capillaries, which connect the endings of arteries and veins and produce mucus that lubricates the cell membrane, along with sweat that keeps the skin moist and flexible. Third, the entire system of absorption, which includes the lacteals that open into the stomach and intestines, and the lymphatic vessels that open on the outside of the body and on the internal linings of all the cells in the cell membrane and other body cavities.

These lacteal and lymphatic vessels are furnished with numerous valves to prevent the return of the fluids, which they absorb, and terminate in glands, called lymphatic glands, and may hence be considered as long necks or mouths belonging to these glands. To these they convey the chyle and mucus, with a part of the perspirable matter, and atmospheric moisture; all which, after having passed through these glands, and having suffered some change in them, are carried forward into the blood, and supply perpetual nourishment to the system, or replace its hourly waste.

These lacteal and lymphatic vessels have many valves to stop the fluids they absorb from flowing back. They end in glands called lymphatic glands, so they can be seen as long necks or mouths for these glands. They transport chyle, mucus, some perspirable matter, and moisture from the air to these glands. After passing through the glands and undergoing some changes, these substances are sent into the blood, providing constant nourishment to the body or replacing what is lost each hour.

8. The stomach and intestinal canal have a constant vermicular motion, which carries forwards their contents, after the lacteals have drank up the chyle from them; and which is excited into action by the stimulus of the aliment we swallow, but which becomes occasionally inverted or retrograde, as in vomiting, and in the iliac passion.

8. The stomach and intestines have a constant wave-like movement that pushes their contents forward, after the lacteals have absorbed the chyle from them. This movement is triggered by the food we eat but can sometimes be reversed, as seen in vomiting and in bowel obstructions.

II. 1. The word sensorium in the following pages is designed to express not only the medullary part of the brain, spinal marrow, nerves, organs of sense, and of the muscles; but also at the same time that living principle, or spirit of animation, which resides throughout the body, without being cognizable to our senses, except by its effects. The changes which occasionally take place in the sensorium, as during the exertions of volition, or the sensations of pleasure or pain, are termed sensorial motions.

II. 1. The term sensorium in the following pages is meant to refer not only to the core part of the brain, spinal cord, nerves, sensory organs, and muscles; but also to that living principle, or spirit of vitality, that exists throughout the body, though it isn't detectable by our senses, except through its effects. The changes that sometimes occur in the sensorium, like during acts of will or feelings of pleasure or pain, are referred to as sensorial motions.

2. The similarity of the texture of the brain to that of the pancreas, and some other glands of the body, has induced the inquirers into this subject to believe, that a fluid, perhaps much more subtile than the electric aura, is separated from the blood by that organ for the purposes of motion and sensation. When we recollect, that the electric fluid itself is actually accumulated and given out voluntarily by the torpedo and the gymnotus electricus, that an electric shock will frequently stimulate into motion a paralytic limb, and lastly that it needs no perceptible tubes to convey it, this opinion seems not without probability; and the singular figure of the brain and nervous system seems well adapted to distribute it over every part of the body.

2. The similarity between the texture of the brain and that of the pancreas, along with some other body glands, has led researchers in this field to believe that a fluid, possibly much more refined than the electric aura, is released from the blood by this organ for the purposes of movement and sensation. When we remember that the electric fluid itself is actually produced and released voluntarily by the torpedo fish and the gymnotus electricus, that an electric shock can often stimulate a paralyzed limb into movement, and finally that it requires no visible tubes to transmit it, this idea appears to have some validity. Additionally, the unique shape of the brain and nervous system seems well-suited to distribute it throughout the body.

For the medullary substance of the brain not only occupies the cavities of the head and spine, but passes along the innumerable ramifications of the nerves to the various muscles and organs of sense. In these it lays aside its coverings, and is intermixed with the slender fibres, which constitute those muscles and organs of sense. Thus all these distant ramifications of the sensorium are united at one of their extremities, that is, in the head and spine; and thus these central parts of the sensorium constitute a communication between all the organs of sense and muscles.

The medullary substance of the brain not only fills the cavities of the head and spine but also extends along the countless branches of the nerves to different muscles and sensory organs. In these areas, it sheds its coverings and mixes with the thin fibers that make up those muscles and sensory organs. Therefore, all these distant branches of the sensory system are connected at one point, which is in the head and spine; and these central parts of the sensory system serve as a link between all the sensory organs and muscles.

3. A nerve is a continuation of the medullary substance of the brain from the head or spine towards the other parts of the body, wrapped in its proper membrane.

3. A nerve is an extension of the brain's medullary substance from the head or spine to the other parts of the body, covered in its own membrane.

4. The muscular fibres are moving organs intermixed with that medullary substance, which is continued along the nerves, as mentioned above. They are indued with the power of contraction, and are again elongated either by antagonist muscles, by circulating fluids, or by elastic ligaments. So the muscles on one side of the forearm bend the fingers by means of their tendons, and those on the other side of the fore-arm extend them again. The arteries are distended by the circulating blood; and in the necks of quadrupeds there is a strong elastic ligament, which assists the muscles, which elevate the head, to keep it in its horizontal position, and to raise it after it has been depressed.

4. The muscular fibers are moving parts mixed with that medullary substance, which runs along the nerves, as mentioned earlier. They have the ability to contract and can be stretched again by opposing muscles, circulating fluids, or elastic ligaments. So, the muscles on one side of the forearm bend the fingers using their tendons, while those on the other side of the forearm straighten them. The arteries swell with the flowing blood; and in the necks of four-legged animals, there is a strong elastic ligament that helps the muscles that lift the head to keep it horizontal and to raise it after it has been lowered.

5. The immediate organs of sense consist in like manner of moving fibres enveloped in the medullary substance above mentioned; and are erroneously supposed to be simply an expansion of the nervous medulla, as the retina of the eye, and the rete mucosum of the skin, which are the immediate organs of vision, and of touch. Hence when we speak of the contractions of the fibrous parts of the body, we shall mean both the contractions of the muscles, and those of the immediate organs of sense. These fibrous motions are thus distinguished from the sensorial motions above mentioned.

5. The immediate organs of sense are similarly made up of moving fibers wrapped in the previously mentioned medullary substance; they are mistakenly thought to be just an extension of the nervous medulla, like the retina of the eye and the rete mucosum of the skin, which are the direct organs of vision and touch. Therefore, when we refer to the contractions of the fibrous parts of the body, we will mean both the contractions of the muscles and those of the immediate organs of sense. These fibrous motions are thus differentiated from the sensorial motions mentioned above.

6. The external organs of sense are the coverings of the immediate organs of sense, and are mechanically adapted for the reception or transmission of peculiar bodies, or of their qualities, as the cornea and humours of the eye, the tympanum of the ear, the cuticle of the fingers and tongue.

6. The external organs of sense are the outer parts of the sense organs, designed to receive or transmit specific substances or their qualities, like the cornea and fluids of the eye, the eardrum of the ear, and the skin of the fingers and tongue.

7. The word idea has various meanings in the writers of metaphysic: it is here used simply for those notions of external things, which our organs of sense bring us acquainted with originally; and is defined a contraction, or motion, or configuration, of the fibres, which constitute the immediate organ of sense; which will be explained at large in another part of the work. Synonymous with the word idea, we shall sometimes use the words sensual motion in contradistinction to muscular motion.

7. The word idea has different meanings in metaphysical writing: here, it simply refers to the perceptions of external things that our senses initially introduce us to; it is defined as a contraction, movement, or arrangement of the fibers that make up the immediate sense organ, which we will explore in more detail in another section of this work. In place of the word idea, we will occasionally use the phrase sensual motion to differentiate it from muscular motion.

8. The word perception includes both the action of the organ of sense in consequence of the impact of external objects, and our attention to that action; that is, it expresses both the motion of the organ of sense, or idea, and the pain or pleasure that succeeds or accompanies it.

8. The term perception refers to both the function of our senses in response to external stimuli and our awareness of that function; it encompasses both the sensory activity or idea and the associated feelings of pain or pleasure that follow or occur alongside it.

9. The pleasure or pain which necessarily accompanies all those perceptions or ideas which we attend to, either gradually subsides, or is succeeded by other fibrous motions. In the latter case it is termed sensation, as explained in Sect. V. 2, and VI. 2.—The reader is intreated to keep this in his mind, that through all this treatise the word sensation is used to express pleasure or pain only in its active state, by whatever means it is introduced into the system, without any reference to the stimulation of external objects.

9. The pleasure or pain which necessarily accompanies all those perceptions or ideas which we attend to, either gradually subsides, or is succeeded by other fibrous motions. In the latter case it is termed sensation, as explained in Sect. V. 2, and VI. 2.—The reader is intreated to keep this in his mind, that through all this treatise the word sensation is used to express pleasure or pain only in its active state, by whatever means it is introduced into the system, without any reference to the stimulation of external objects.

10. The vulgar use of the word memory is too unlimited for our purpose: those ideas which we voluntarily recall are here termed ideas of recollection, as when we will to repeat the alphabet backwards. And those ideas which are suggested to us by preceding ideas are here termed ideas of suggestion, as whilst we repeat the alphabet in the usual order; when by habits previously acquired B is suggested by A, and C by B, without any effort of deliberation.

10. The casual use of the word memory is too broad for what we mean: the thoughts we intentionally bring to mind are called ideas of recollection, like when we decide to recite the alphabet backwards. The thoughts that come to us based on earlier thoughts are referred to as ideas of suggestion, such as when we say the alphabet in the usual order; when, through prior habits, B is prompted by A, and C by B, without any conscious effort.

11. The word association properly signifies a society or convention of things in some respects similar to each other. We never say in common language, that the effect is associated with the cause, though they necessarily accompany or succeed each other. Thus the contractions of our muscles and organs of sense may be said to be associated together, but cannot with propriety be said to be associated with irritations, or with volition, or with sensation; because they are caused by them, as mentioned in Sect. IV. When fibrous contractions succeed other fibrous contractions, the connection is termed association; when fibrous contractions succeed sensorial motions, the connection is termed causation; when fibrous and sensorial motions reciprocally introduce each other in progressive trains or tribes, it is termed catenation of animal motions. All these connections are said to be produced by habit; that is, by frequent repetition.

11. The word association properly signifies a society or convention of things in some respects similar to each other. We never say in common language, that the effect is associated with the cause, though they necessarily accompany or succeed each other. Thus the contractions of our muscles and organs of sense may be said to be associated together, but cannot with propriety be said to be associated with irritations, or with volition, or with sensation; because they are caused by them, as mentioned in Sect. IV. When fibrous contractions succeed other fibrous contractions, the connection is termed association; when fibrous contractions succeed sensorial motions, the connection is termed causation; when fibrous and sensorial motions reciprocally introduce each other in progressive trains or tribes, it is termed catenation of animal motions. All these connections are said to be produced by habit; that is, by frequent repetition.

12. It may be proper to observe, that by the unavoidable idiom of our language the ideas of perception, of recollection, or of imagination, in the plural number signify the ideas belonging to perception, to recollection, or to imagination; whilst the idea of perception, of recollection, or of imagination, in the singular number is used for what is termed "a reflex idea of any of those operations of the sensorium."

12. It's important to note that, because of the unavoidable habits of our language, the plural forms of perception, recollection, or imagination refer to the ideas associated with each of those concepts, while the singular form of perception, recollection, or imagination is used for what we call "a reflex idea of any of those sensory processes."

13. By the word stimulus is not only meant the application of external bodies to our organs of sense and muscular fibres, which excites into action the sensorial power termed irritation; but also pleasure or pain, when they excite into action the sensorial power termed sensation; and desire or aversion, when they excite into action the power of volition; and lastly, the fibrous contractions which precede association; as is further explained in Sect. XII. 2. 1.

13. By the word stimulus is not only meant the application of external bodies to our organs of sense and muscular fibres, which excites into action the sensorial power termed irritation; but also pleasure or pain, when they excite into action the sensorial power termed sensation; and desire or aversion, when they excite into action the power of volition; and lastly, the fibrous contractions which precede association; as is further explained in Sect. XII. 2. 1.



SECT. III.

THE MOTIONS OF THE RETINA DEMONSTRATED BY EXPERIMENTS.

THE MOTIONS OF THE RETINA SHOWN THROUGH EXPERIMENTS.

I. Of animal motions and of ideas. II. The fibrous structure of the retina. III. The activity of the retina in vision. 1. Rays of light have no momentum. 2. Objects long viewed become fainter. 3. Spectra of black objects become luminous. 4. Varying spectra from gyration. 5. From long inspection of various colours. IV. Motions of the organs of sense constitute ideas. 1. Light from pressing the eye-ball, and sound from the pulsation of the carotid artery. 2. Ideas in sleep mistaken for perceptions. 3. Ideas of imagination produce pain and sickness like sensations. 4. When the organ of sense is destroyed, the ideas belonging to that sense perish. V. Analogy between muscular motions and sensual motions, or ideas. 1. They are both originally excited by irritations. 2. And associated together in the same manner. 3. Both act in nearly the same times. 4. Are alike strengthened or fatigued by exercise. 5. Are alike painful from inflammation. 6. Are alike benumbed by compression. 7. Are alike liable to paralysis. 8. To convulsion. 9. To the influence of old age.VI. Objections answered. 1. Why we cannot invent new ideas. 2. If ideas resemble external objects. 3. Of the imagined sensation in an amputated limb. 4. Abstract ideas.VII. What are ideas, if they are not animal motions?

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. The movement of animals and the formation of ideas. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. The layered structure of the retina. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. The retina's role in vision. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Light rays lack momentum. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Objects looked at for a long time appear fainter. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Black objects show luminous spectra. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Different spectra from spinning. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. After extended viewing of various colors. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. The movements of sensory organs create ideas. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Light from pressing on the eyeball, and sound from the heartbeat in the carotid artery. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Ideas during sleep can be confused with real perceptions. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. Imaginative ideas can cause pain and sickness similar to real sensations. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. When a sensory organ is damaged, the ideas related to that sense disappear. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. A comparison between muscular movements and sensory movements, or ideas. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. Both are initially triggered by irritations. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. And are connected similarly. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. Both occur almost simultaneously. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. Both become stronger or fatigued through use. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_18__. Both experience pain from inflammation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_19__. Both can be numbed by pressure. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_20__. Both are susceptible to paralysis. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_21__. To convulsions. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_22__. To the effects of aging.—__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_23__. Responses to objections. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_24__. Why we can't create new ideas. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_25__. If ideas reflect external objects. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_26__. On the imagined sensations in an amputated limb. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_27__. Conceptual ideas.—__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_28__. What are ideas, if they're not movements of living beings?

Before the great variety of animal motions can be duly arranged into natural classes and orders, it is necessary to smooth the way to this yet unconquered field of science, by removing some obstacles which thwart our passage. I. To demonstrate that the retina and other immediate organs of sense possess a power of motion, and that these motions constitute our ideas, according to the fifth and seventh of the preceding assertions, claims our first attention.

Before we can properly categorize the wide range of animal movements into natural classes and orders, we need to clear some hurdles that block our path in this still unexplored area of science. I. Our first focus should be to show that the retina and other immediate sensory organs have the ability to move, and that these movements form the basis of our ideas, as stated in the fifth and seventh assertions mentioned earlier.

Animal motions are distinguished from the communicated motions, mentioned in the first section, as they have no mechanical proportion to their cause; for the goad of a spur on the skin of a horse shall induce him to move a load of hay. They differ from the gravitating motions there mentioned as they are exerted with equal facility in all directions, and they differ from the chemical class of motions, because no apparent decompositions or new combinations are produced in the moving materials.

Animal movements are different from the communicated movements mentioned in the first section because they don't have a mechanical connection to their cause; for example, a spur touching a horse's skin will prompt him to move a load of hay. They differ from the gravitational movements discussed earlier because they can happen easily in all directions, and they differ from chemical movements because there are no obvious breakdowns or new combinations formed in the moving materials.

Hence, when we say animal motion is excited by irritation, we do not mean that the motion bears any proportion to the mechanical impulse of the stimulus; nor that it is affected by the general gravitation of the two bodies; nor by their chemical properties, but solely that certain animal fibres are excited into action by something external to the moving organ.

Hence, when we say that animal movement is triggered by irritation, we don't mean that the movement corresponds to the mechanical force of the stimulus; nor that it is influenced by the general attraction between the two bodies; nor by their chemical properties, but simply that specific animal fibers are activated by something outside the moving part.

In this sense the stimulus of the blood produces the contractions of the heart; and the substances we take into our stomach and bowels stimulate them to perform their necessary functions. The rays of light excite the retina into animal motion by their stimulus; at the same time that those rays of light themselves are physically converged to a focus by the inactive humours of the eye. The vibrations of the air stimulate the auditory nerve into animal action; while it is probable that the tympanum of the ear at the same time undergoes a mechanical vibration.

In this way, the blood flow triggers the heart's contractions, and the substances we consume activate our stomach and intestines to do their jobs. Light rays stimulate the retina, causing movement, while those same rays are physically focused by the eye's inactive fluids. Air vibrations activate the auditory nerve, and it's likely that the eardrum also experiences a mechanical vibration at the same time.

To render this circumstance more easy to be comprehended, motion may be defined to be a variation of figure; for the whole universe may be considered as one thing possessing a certain figure; the motions of any of its parts are a variation of this figure of the whole: this definition of motion will be further explained in Section XIV. 2. 2. on the production of ideas.

To render this circumstance more easy to be comprehended, motion may be defined to be a variation of figure; for the whole universe may be considered as one thing possessing a certain figure; the motions of any of its parts are a variation of this figure of the whole: this definition of motion will be further explained in Section XIV. 2. 2. on the production of ideas.

Now the motions of an organ of sense are a succession of configurations of that organ; these configurations succeed each other quicker or slower; and whatever configuration of this organ of sense, that is, whatever portion of the motion of it is, or has usually been, attended to, constitutes an idea. Hence the configuration is not to be considered as an effect of the motion of the organ, but rather as a part or temporary termination of it; and that, whether a pause succeeds it, or a new configuration immediately takes place. Thus when a succession of moving objects are presented to our view, the ideas of trumpets, horns, lords and ladies, trains and canopies, are configurations, that is, parts or links of the successive motions of the organ of vision.

Now the movements of a sense organ are a series of configurations of that organ; these configurations occur faster or slower; and whatever configuration of this sense organ, meaning whatever part of its movement is or has usually been focused on, forms an idea. Therefore, the configuration should not be seen as a result of the motion of the organ but rather as a part or temporary endpoint of it; and this holds true whether a pause follows or a new configuration happens right after. So, when a series of moving objects come into our sight, the ideas of trumpets, horns, nobles and aristocrats, trains and canopies are configurations, which means they are parts or links of the successive movements of the visual organ.

Plate I. Plate I.

These motions or configurations of the organs of sense differ from the sensorial motions to be described hereafter, as they appear to be simply contractions of the fibrous extremities of those organs, and in that respect exactly resemble the motions or contractions of the larger muscles, as appears from the following experiment. Place a circular piece of red silk about an inch in diameter on a sheet of white paper in a strong light, as in Plate I.—look for a minute on this area, or till the eye becomes somewhat fatigued, and then, gently closing your eyes, and shading them with your hand, a circular green area of the same apparent diameter becomes visible in the closed eye. This green area is the colour reverse to the red area, which had been previously inspected, as explained in the experiments on ocular spectra at the end of the work, and in Botanical Garden, P. 1. additional note, No. 1. Hence it appears, that a part of the retina, which had been fatigued by contraction in one direction, relieves itself by exerting the antagonist fibres, and producing a contraction in an opposite direction, as is common in the exertions of our muscles. Thus when we are tired with long action of our arms in one direction, as in holding a bridle on a journey, we occasionally throw them into an opposite position to relieve the fatigued muscles.

These movements or configurations of the sensory organs are different from the sensory movements that will be described later. They seem to be just contractions of the fiber endings of those organs and in that way are exactly like the movements or contractions of larger muscles, as shown by the following experiment. Place a circular piece of red silk about an inch in diameter on a sheet of white paper in bright light, as shown in Plate I. — focus on this area for a minute, or until your eyes feel a bit fatigued, and then gently close your eyes, shielding them with your hand. A circular green area of the same apparent diameter will become visible in your closed eye. This green area is the color opposite to the red area you just looked at, as explained in the experiments on visual afterimages at the end of the work, and in Botanical Garden, P. 1. additional note, No. 1. Therefore, it seems that a part of the retina, which got tired from contracting in one direction, compensates by using the opposing fibers and creating a contraction in the opposite direction, much like our muscles do. So, when our arms get tired from holding something in one position, like a bridle on a journey, we occasionally move them to a different position to relieve the fatigued muscles.

Mr. Locke has defined an idea to be "whatever is present to the mind;" but this would include the exertions of volition, and the sensations of pleasure and pain, as well as those operations of our system, which acquaint us with external objects; and is therefore too unlimited for our purpose. Mr. Lock seems to have fallen into a further error, by conceiving, that the mind could form a general or abstract idea by its own operation, which was the copy of no particular perception; as of a triangle in general, that was neither acute, obtuse, nor right angled. The ingenious Dr. Berkley and Mr. Hume have demonstrated, that such general ideas have no existence in nature, not even in the mind of their celebrated inventor. We shall therefore take for granted at present, that our recollection or imagination of external objects consists of a partial repetition of the perceptions, which were excited by those external objects, at the time we became acquainted with them; and that our reflex ideas of the operations of our minds are partial repetitions of those operations.

Mr. Locke defined an idea as "whatever is present to the mind," but this would also include the efforts of will and the feelings of pleasure and pain, along with those functions of our system that help us recognize external objects; therefore, it's too broad for our purpose. Mr. Locke also seems to have made another mistake by thinking that the mind could create a general or abstract idea purely from its own processes, which was not a copy of any specific perception—like a triangle in general that was neither acute, obtuse, nor right-angled. The clever Dr. Berkeley and Mr. Hume have shown that such general ideas do not exist in reality, not even in the mind of their famous creator. We will therefore assume for now that our memory or imagination of external objects consists of a partial repetition of the perceptions triggered by those external objects when we first encountered them; and that our reflective ideas of our mental operations are partial repeats of those operations.

II. The following article evinces that the organ of vision consists of a fibrous part as well as of the nervous medulla, like other white muscles; and hence, as it resembles the muscular parts of the body in its structure, we may conclude, that it must resemble them in possessing a power of being excited into animal motion.—The subsequent experiments on the optic nerve, and on the colours remaining in the eye, are copied from a paper on ocular spectra published in the seventy-sixth volume of the Philos. Trans. by Dr. R. Darwin of Shrewsbury; which, as I shall have frequent occasion to refer to, is reprinted in this work, Sect. XL. The retina of an ox's eye was suspended in a glass of warm water, and forcibly torn in a few places; the edges of these parts appeared jagged and hairy, and did not contract and become smooth like simple mucus, when it is distended till it breaks; which evinced that it consisted of fibres. This fibrous construction became still more distinct to the light by adding some caustic alcali to the water; as the adhering mucus was first eroded, and the hair-like fibres remained floating in the vessel. Nor does the degree of transparency of the retina invalidate this evidence of its fibrous structure, since Leeuwenhoek has shewn, that the crystalline humour itself consists of fibres. Arc. Nat. V. I. 70.

II. The following article evinces that the organ of vision consists of a fibrous part as well as of the nervous medulla, like other white muscles; and hence, as it resembles the muscular parts of the body in its structure, we may conclude, that it must resemble them in possessing a power of being excited into animal motion.—The subsequent experiments on the optic nerve, and on the colours remaining in the eye, are copied from a paper on ocular spectra published in the seventy-sixth volume of the Philos. Trans. by Dr. R. Darwin of Shrewsbury; which, as I shall have frequent occasion to refer to, is reprinted in this work, Sect. XL. The retina of an ox's eye was suspended in a glass of warm water, and forcibly torn in a few places; the edges of these parts appeared jagged and hairy, and did not contract and become smooth like simple mucus, when it is distended till it breaks; which evinced that it consisted of fibres. This fibrous construction became still more distinct to the light by adding some caustic alcali to the water; as the adhering mucus was first eroded, and the hair-like fibres remained floating in the vessel. Nor does the degree of transparency of the retina invalidate this evidence of its fibrous structure, since Leeuwenhoek has shewn, that the crystalline humour itself consists of fibres. Arc. Nat. V. I. 70.

Hence it appears, that as the muscles consist of larger fibres intermixed with a smaller quantity of nervous medulla, the organ of vision consists of a greater quantity of nervous medulla intermixed with smaller fibres. It is probable that the locomotive muscles of microscopic animals may have greater tenuity than these of the retina; and there is reason to conclude from analogy, that the other immediate organs of sense, as the portio mollis of the auditory nerve, and the rete mucosum of the skin, possess a similarity of structure with the retina, and a similar power of being excited into animal motion.

Therefore, it seems that while muscles are made up of larger fibers mixed with a smaller amount of nerve tissue, the organ of vision consists of a greater amount of nerve tissue mixed with smaller fibers. It's likely that the movement muscles of microscopic animals are thinner than those in the retina; and we can reasonably infer, based on comparisons, that other immediate sensory organs, like the soft part of the auditory nerve and the mucous network of the skin, have a similar structure to the retina and the same ability to trigger animal movement.

III. The subsequent articles shew, that neither mechanical impressions, nor chemical combinations of light, but that the animal activity of the retina constitutes vision.

III. The following articles show that vision comes not from mechanical impressions or chemical combinations of light, but from the active function of the retina.

1. Much has been conjectured by philosophers about the momentum of the rays of light; to subject this to experiment a very light horizontal balance was constructed by Mr. Michel, with about an inch square of thin leaf-copper suspended at each end of it, as described in Dr. Priestley's History of Light and Colours. The focus of a very large convex mirror was thrown by Dr. Powel, in his lectures on experimental philosophy, in my presence, on one wing of this delicate balance, and it receded from the light; thrown on the other wing, it approached towards the light, and this repeatedly; so that no sensible impulse could be observed, but what might well be ascribed to the ascent of heated air.

1. Many philosophers have speculated about the momentum of light rays; to test this, Mr. Michel created a lightweight horizontal balance with a thin square piece of copper hanging at each end, as mentioned in Dr. Priestley’s History of Light and Colours. During Dr. Powel's lectures on experimental philosophy, he directed the focus of a large convex mirror onto one side of this delicate balance, which moved away from the light. When aimed at the other side, it moved toward the light, and this happened repeatedly; thus, no noticeable movement could be detected other than that which could be attributed to the rising of warm air.

Whence it is reasonable to conclude, that the light of the day must be much too weak in its dilute state to make any mechanical impression on so tenacious a substance as the retina of the eye.—Add to this, that as the retina is nearly transparent, it could therefore make less resistance to the mechanical impulse of light; which, according, to the observations related by Mr. Melvil in the Edinburgh Literary Essays, only communicates heat, and should therefore only communicate momentum, where it is obstructed, reflected, or refracted.—From whence also may be collected the final cause of this degree of transparency of the retina, viz. left by the focus of stronger lights, heat and pain should have been produced in the retina, instead of that stimulus which excites it into animal motion.

It's reasonable to conclude that the daylight is too weak in its diluted state to have any mechanical effect on a substance as tough as the retina of the eye. Additionally, since the retina is almost transparent, it would offer even less resistance to the mechanical impact of light. According to observations mentioned by Mr. Melvil in the Edinburgh Literary Essays, light only transfers heat and should, therefore, only transfer momentum when it is blocked, reflected, or refracted. This also suggests the purpose of the retina's transparency: if stronger light were focused on it, it would cause heat and pain instead of the stimulus that triggers animal movement.

2. On looking long on an area of scarlet silk of about an inch in diameter laid on white paper, as in Plate I. the scarlet colour becomes fainter, till at length it entirely vanishes, though the eye is kept uniformly and steadily upon it. Now if the change or motion of the retina was a mechanical impression, or a chemical tinge of coloured light, the perception would every minute become stronger and stronger,—whereas in this experiment it becomes every instant weaker and weaker. The same circumstance obtains in the continued application of sound, or of sapid bodies, or of odorous ones, or of tangible ones, to their adapted organs of sense.

2. If you stare for a long time at a spot of scarlet silk about an inch in diameter placed on white paper, like in Plate I, the scarlet color starts to fade until it completely disappears, even though your eyes stay focused on it. If the change or movement in your retina was just a mechanical impression or a chemical effect of colored light, the perception would actually get stronger with each passing moment—yet in this experiment, it gets weaker and weaker every instant. The same thing happens with continuous exposure to sound, flavors, smells, or touch applied to their corresponding sense organs.

Plate II. Plate II.

Thus when a circular coin, as a shilling, is pressed on the palm of the hand, the sense of touch is mechanically compressed; but it is the stimulus of this pressure that excites the organ of touch into animal action, which constitutes the perception of hardness and of figure; for in some minutes the perception ceases, though the mechanical pressure of the object remains.

Thus, when a round coin, like a shilling, is pressed against the palm of the hand, the sense of touch feels the mechanical pressure; however, it's the stimulus from this pressure that triggers the sense of touch into physical response, which creates the perception of hardness and shape. After a few minutes, the perception fades, even though the mechanical pressure from the object continues.

3. Make with ink on white paper a very black spot about half an inch in diameter, with a tail about an inch in length, so as to resemble a tadpole, as in Plate II.; look steadfastly for a minute on the center of this spot, and, on moving the eye a little, the figure of the tadpole will be seen on the white part of the paper; which figure of the tadpole will appear more luminous than the other part of the white paper; which can only be explained by supposing that a part of the retina, on which the tadpole was delineated, to have become more sensible to light than the other parts of it, which were exposed to the white paper; and not from any idea of mechanical impression or chemical combination of light with the retina.

3. Using ink on white paper, create a very black spot about half an inch in diameter with a tail about an inch long, to resemble a tadpole, as shown in Plate II. Stare at the center of this spot for a minute, and when you move your eyes slightly, you’ll see the image of the tadpole on the white part of the paper; this tadpole image will appear brighter than the surrounding white paper. This effect can only be explained by suggesting that a part of the retina where the tadpole was drawn has become more sensitive to light than the other parts that were exposed to the white paper, and not due to any mechanical impression or chemical reaction between light and the retina.

4. When any one turns round rapidly, till he becomes dizzy, and falls upon the ground, the spectra of the ambient objects continue to present themselves in rotation, and he seems to behold the objects still in motion. Now if these spectra were impressions on a passive organ, they either must continue as they were received last, or not continue at all.

4. When someone spins around quickly until they get dizzy and falls to the ground, the images of the surrounding objects keep appearing to rotate, and it feels like the objects are still moving. If these images were just marks on a passive organ, they would either remain as they were last perceived or not persist at all.

5. Place a piece of red silk about an inch in diameter on a sheet of white paper in a strong light, as in Plate I; look steadily upon it from the distance of about half a yard for a minute; then closing your eye-lids, cover them with your hands and handkerchief, and a green spectrum will be seen in your eyes resembling in form the piece of red silk. After some seconds of time the spectrum will disappear, and in a few more seconds will reappear; and thus alternately three or four times, if the experiment be well made, till at length it vanishes entirely.

5. Place a piece of red silk about an inch in diameter on a sheet of white paper under strong light, like in Plate I; look at it steadily from about three feet away for one minute. After that, close your eyes and cover them with your hands and a handkerchief, and you’ll see a green afterimage in the shape of the red silk. After a few seconds, the afterimage will fade, and then it will reappear after a little while; this will happen three or four times if you do the experiment correctly, until it eventually disappears completely.

Plate III. Plate III.

6. Place a circular piece of white paper, about four inches in diameter, in the sunshine, cover the center of this with a circular piece of black silk, about three inches in diameter; and the center of the black silk with a circle of pink silk, about two inches in diameter; and the center of the pink silk with a circle of yellow silk, about one inch in diameter; and the center of this with a circle of blue silk, about half an inch in diameter; make a small spot with ink in the center of the blue silk, as in Plate III.; look steadily for a minute on this central spot, and then closing your eyes, and applying your hand at about an inch distance before them, so as to prevent too much or too little light from passing through the eye-lids, and you will see the most beautiful circles of colours that imagination can conceive; which are most resembled by the colours occasioned by pouring a drop or two of oil on a still lake in a bright day. But these circular irises of colours are not only different from the colours of the silks above mentioned, but are at the same time perpetually changing as long as they exist.

6. Place a round piece of white paper, about four inches wide, in the sunlight. Cover the center with a circular piece of black silk, about three inches wide; then place a circle of pink silk, about two inches wide, in the center of the black silk; followed by a small circle of yellow silk, about one inch wide, in the center of the pink silk; and a tiny circle of blue silk, about half an inch wide, in the center of the yellow silk. Put a small ink spot in the center of the blue silk, as shown in Plate III. Focus on this central spot for a minute, then close your eyes and position your hand about an inch away from them to block just the right amount of light from getting through your eyelids. You'll see the most beautiful circles of colors that your imagination can create, resembling the colors you get when you pour a drop or two of oil on a calm lake on a bright day. However, these colorful rings are not only different from the colors of the silks mentioned earlier but are also constantly changing as long as they’re visible.

From all these experiments it appears, that these spectra in the eye are not owing to the mechanical impulse of light impressed on the retina; nor to its chemical combination with that organ; nor to the absorption and emission of light, as is supposed, perhaps erroneously, to take place in calcined shells and other phosphorescent bodies, after having been exposed to the light: for in all these cases the spectra in the eye should either remain of the same colour, or gradually decay, when the object is withdrawn; and neither their evanescence during the presence of their object, as in the second experiment, nor their change from dark to luminous, as in the third experiment, nor their rotation, as in the fourth experiment, nor the alternate presence and evanescence of them, as in the fifth experiment, nor the perpetual change of colours of them, as in the last experiment, could exist.

From all these experiments, it seems that these afterimages in the eye aren't caused by the mechanical impact of light on the retina; nor by its chemical reaction with that organ; nor by the absorption and emission of light, which is thought—perhaps incorrectly—to happen in burned shells and other glowing materials after being exposed to light. In all these cases, the afterimages in the eye should either stay the same color or gradually fade away when the object is removed. Neither their fading while the object is still there, as seen in the second experiment, nor their shift from dark to bright, as in the third experiment, nor their spinning, as in the fourth experiment, nor their alternating presence and disappearance, as in the fifth experiment, nor the continuous change of colors, as in the last experiment, could happen.

IV. The subsequent articles shew, that these animal motions or configurations of our organs of sense constitute our ideas.

IV. The following articles show that these animal movements or arrangements of our sensory organs create our ideas.

1. If any one in the dark presses the ball of his eye, by applying his finger to the external corner of it, a luminous appearance is observed; and by a smart stroke on the eye great slashes of fire are perceived. (Newton's Optics.) So that when the arteries, that are near the auditory nerve, make stronger pulsations than usual, as in some fevers, an undulating sound is excited in the ears. Hence it is not the presence of the light and sound, but the motions of the organ, that are immediately necessary to constitute the perception or idea of light and sound.

1. If someone in the dark presses the ball of their eye by using a finger on the outer corner, a bright light is seen; and a quick tap on the eye reveals flashes of fire. (Newton's Optics.) So, when the arteries near the auditory nerve pulse harder than normal, like in certain fevers, it creates a vibrating sound in the ears. Therefore, it’s not the actual light and sound we detect, but rather the movements of the organ that are essential for the perception or idea of light and sound.

2. During the time of sleep, or in delirium, the ideas of imagination are mistaken for the perceptions of external objects; whence it appears, that these ideas of imagination, are no other than a reiteration of those motions of the organs of sense, which were originally excited by the stimulus of external objects: and in our waking hours the simple ideas, that we call up by recollection or by imagination, as the colour of red, or the smell of a rose, are exact resemblances of the same simple ideas from perception; and in consequence must be a repetition of those very motions.

2. When we sleep or experience delirium, the ideas created by our imagination can be confused with the actual perceptions of things around us. This suggests that these imaginative ideas are simply a repeat of the sensory movements that were triggered by external stimuli. During our waking hours, the simple ideas we recall or imagine, like the color red or the scent of a rose, closely resemble those we perceive directly. Therefore, they must be a repetition of the same sensory movements.

3. The disagreeable sensation called the tooth-edge is originally excited by the painful jarring of the teeth in biting the edge of the glass, or porcelain cup, in which our food was given us in our infancy, as is further explained in the Section XVI. 10, on Instinct.—This disagreeable sensation is afterwards excitable not only by a repetition of the sound, that was then produced, but by imagination alone, as I have myself frequently experienced; in this case the idea of biting a china cup, when I imagine it very distinctly, or when I see another person bite a cup or glass, excites an actual pain in the nerves of my teeth. So that this idea and pain seem to be nothing more than the reiterated motions of those nerves, that were formerly so disagreeably affected.

3. The disagreeable sensation called the tooth-edge is originally excited by the painful jarring of the teeth in biting the edge of the glass, or porcelain cup, in which our food was given us in our infancy, as is further explained in the Section XVI. 10, on Instinct.—This disagreeable sensation is afterwards excitable not only by a repetition of the sound, that was then produced, but by imagination alone, as I have myself frequently experienced; in this case the idea of biting a china cup, when I imagine it very distinctly, or when I see another person bite a cup or glass, excites an actual pain in the nerves of my teeth. So that this idea and pain seem to be nothing more than the reiterated motions of those nerves, that were formerly so disagreeably affected.

Other ideas that are excited by imagination or recollection in many instances produce similar effects on the constitution, as our perceptions had formerly produced, and are therefore undoubtedly a repetition of the same motions. A story which the celebrated Baron Van Swieton relates of himself is to this purpose. He was present when the putrid carcase of a dead dog exploded with prodigious stench; and some years afterwards, accidentally riding along the same road, he was thrown into the same sickness and vomiting by the idea of the stench, as he had before experienced from the perception of it.

Other ideas sparked by imagination or memory often create similar effects on the body as our earlier perceptions did, and are clearly just a repetition of the same reactions. A story that the famous Baron Van Swieton tells about himself illustrates this point. He was present when the decomposing body of a dead dog burst with a terrible smell; and years later, while riding along the same road, the mere thought of that foul odor made him feel the same nausea and vomiting he had experienced before when he actually encountered it.

4. Where the organ of sense is totally destroyed, the ideas which were received by that organ seem to perish along with it, as well as the power of perception. Of this a satisfactory instance has fallen under my observation. A gentleman about sixty years of age had been totally deaf for near thirty years: he appeared to be a man of good understanding, and amused himself with reading, and by conversing either by the use of the pen, or by signs made with his fingers, to represent letters. I observed that he had so far forgot the pronunciation of the language, that when he attempted to speak, none of his words had distinct articulation, though his relations could sometimes understand his meaning. But, which is much to the point, he assured me, that in his dreams he always imagined that people conversed with him by signs or writing, and never that he heard any one speak to him. From hence it appears, that with the perceptions of sounds he has also lost the ideas of them; though the organs of speech still retain somewhat of their usual habits of articulation.

4. When a sense organ is completely destroyed, the ideas received by that organ seem to vanish along with it, as well as the ability to perceive. I observed a clear example of this. A man around sixty years old had been completely deaf for nearly thirty years. He seemed to be intelligent and kept himself occupied with reading and communicating either by writing or using sign language with his fingers to represent letters. I noticed that he had forgotten how to pronounce the language so much that when he tried to speak, his words lacked clear articulation, although his family could occasionally understand his intent. Importantly, he told me that in his dreams, he only imagined people communicating with him through signs or writing and never that he heard anyone speaking to him. This indicates that along with his ability to hear sounds, he has also lost the concepts associated with them, even though the speech organs still retain some of their usual articulation habits.

This observation may throw some light on the medical treatment of deaf people; as it may be learnt from their dreams whether the auditory nerve be paralytic, or their deafness be owing to some defect of the external organ.

This observation may shed some light on the medical treatment of deaf individuals; as it may be understood from their dreams whether the auditory nerve is paralyzed or if their deafness is due to some issue with the external ear.

It rarely happens that the immediate organ of vision is perfectly destroyed. The most frequent causes of blindness are occasioned by defects of the external organ, as in cataracts and obfuscations of the cornea. But I have had the opportunity of conversing with two men, who had been some years blind; one of them had a complete gutta serena, and the other had lost the whole substance of his eyes. They both told me that they did not remember to have ever dreamt of visible objects, since the total loss of their sight.

It’s uncommon for the eye itself to be completely destroyed. The most common causes of blindness come from issues with the external part of the eye, like cataracts and corneal opacities. However, I’ve spoken with two men who had been blind for several years; one had complete gutta serena, and the other had lost all the substance of his eyes. Both of them told me they couldn’t remember dreaming of visible objects since they completely lost their sight.

V. Another method of discovering that our ideas are animal motions of the organs of sense, is from considering the great analogy they bear to the motions of the larger muscles of the body. In the following articles it will appear that they are originally excited into action by the irritation of external objects like our muscles; are associated together like our muscular motions; act in similar time with them; are fatigued by continued exertion like them; and that the organs of sense are subject to inflammation, numbness, palsy, convulsion, and the defects of old age, in the same manner as the muscular fibres.

V. Another way to understand that our ideas are just physical responses from our senses is by looking at how similar they are to the movements of the body's larger muscles. In the following sections, it will be shown that these ideas are initially triggered by the stimulation of external objects, just like our muscles; they are linked together like our muscle movements; they happen in similar timeframes; they get tired from prolonged use like muscles do; and the sensory organs can experience inflammation, numbness, paralysis, convulsions, and the decline that comes with old age, just like muscle fibers do.

1. All our perceptions or ideas of external objects are universally allowed to have been originally excited by the stimulus of those external objects; and it will be shewn in a succeeding section, that it is probable that all our muscular motions, as well those that are become voluntary as those of the heart and glandular system, were originally in like manner excited by the stimulus of something external to the organ of motion.

1. All our perceptions or ideas about external objects are generally accepted to have been initially triggered by the influence of those external objects. In a later section, it will be demonstrated that it’s likely all our muscular movements—both voluntary ones and those controlled by the heart and glandular system—were similarly triggered by the influence of something outside the organ of movement.

2. Our ideas are also associated together after their production precisely in the same manner as our muscular motions; which will likewise be fully explained in the succeeding section.

2. Our thoughts are connected in the same way our physical movements are after we create them; this will also be thoroughly explained in the following section.

3. The time taken up in performing an idea is likewise much the same as that taken up in performing a muscular motion. A musician can press the keys of an harpsichord with his fingers in the order of a tune he has been accustomed to play, in as little time as he can run over those notes in his mind. So we many times in an hour cover our eye-balls with our eye-lids without perceiving that we are in the dark; hence the perception or idea of light is not changed for that of darkness in so small a time as the twinkling of an eye; so that in this case the muscular motion of the eye-lid is performed quicker than the perception of light can be changed for that of darkness.—So if a fire-stick be whirled round in the dark, a luminous circle appears to the observer; if it be whirled somewhat slower, this circle becomes interrupted in one part; and then the time taken up in such a revolution of the stick is the same that the observer uses in changing his ideas: thus the δολικοσκοτον εγκος of Homer, the long shadow of the flying javelin, is elegantly designed to give us an idea of its velocity, and not of its length.

3. The time it takes to execute an idea is pretty much the same as the time it takes to make a physical movement. A musician can hit the keys of a harpsichord in the order of a melody he knows by heart just as quickly as he can think through those notes in his mind. Similarly, we often shut our eyes with our eyelids without realizing that we're in the dark; thus, the perception of light doesn’t change to that of darkness in the short time it takes for our eye to blink. In this case, the movement of the eyelid happens faster than the perception of light can switch to darkness. If a fire stick is spun around in the dark, the observer sees a glowing circle; if it’s spun a bit slower, that circle gets interrupted in one spot. The time it takes for the stick to make that rotation is the same amount of time the observer takes to shift their ideas: hence, the δολικοσκοτον εγκος of Homer, the long shadow of the flying javelin, is cleverly meant to convey its speed rather than its length.

4. The fatigue that follows a continued attention of the mind to one object is relieved by changing the subject of our thoughts; as the continued movement of one limb is relieved by moving another in its stead. Whereas a due exercise of the faculties of the mind strengthens and improves those faculties, whether of imagination or recollection; as the exercise of our limbs in dancing or fencing increases the strength and agility of the muscles thus employed.

4. The tiredness that comes from focusing on one thing for too long is eased by shifting our thoughts to something else; just like the strain on one limb is eased by using another. However, properly exercising our mental abilities strengthens and enhances those abilities, whether it's imagination or memory, just as moving our bodies in dancing or fencing builds the strength and agility of the muscles we use.

5. If the muscles of any limb are inflamed, they do not move without pain; so when the retina is inflamed, its motions also are painful. Hence light is as intolerable in this kind of ophthalmia, as pressure is to the finger in the paronychia. In this disease the patients frequently dream of having their eyes painfully dazzled; hence the idea of strong light is painful as well as the reality. The first of these facts evinces that our perceptions are motions of the organs of sense; and the latter, that our imaginations are also motions of the same organs.

5. When the muscles in any limb are inflamed, they hurt when you try to move; similarly, when the retina is inflamed, its movements also cause pain. Because of this, light is just as unbearable in this type of eye inflammation as pressure is to a finger with a nail infection. In this condition, patients often dream of their eyes being painfully dazzled; thus, the thought of bright light is as painful as the actual experience. The first observation shows that our perceptions involve movements of our sensory organs; the latter demonstrates that our imaginations also involve movements of the same organs.

6. The organs of sense, like the moving muscles, are liable to become benumbed, or less sensible, from compression. Thus, if any person on a light day looks on a white wall, he may perceive the ramifications of the optic artery, at every pulsation of it, represented by darker branches on the white wall; which is evidently owing to its compressing the retina during the diastole of the artery. Savage Nosolog.

6. Our senses, similar to our muscles, can become numb or less responsive when pressed. For instance, on a bright day, if someone looks at a white wall, they might notice the patterns of the optic artery, appearing as darker branches on the white surface, which clearly happens because it compresses the retina during the artery's relaxation phase. Savage Nosolog.

7. The organs of sense and the moving muscles are alike liable to be affected with palsy, as in the gutta serena, and in some cases of deafness; and one side of the face has sometimes lost its power of sensation, but retained its power of motion; other parts of the body have lost their motions but retained their sensation, as in the common hemiplagia; and in other instances both these powers have perished together.

7. The sensory organs and the muscles that enable movement can both be affected by paralysis, like in cases of gutta serena and some types of deafness. Sometimes, one side of the face might lose its sense of feeling but still be able to move; in other cases, different body parts may be unable to move while still feeling sensations, as seen in common hemiplegia. In other situations, both sensations and movement can be completely lost together.

8. In some convulsive diseases a delirium or insanity supervenes, and the convulsions cease; and conversely the convulsions shall supervene, and the delirium cease. Of this I have been a witness many times in a day in the paroxysms of violent epilepsies; which evinces that one kind of delirium is a convulsion of the organs of sense, and that our ideas are the motions of these organs: the subsequent cases will illustrate this observation.

8. In some seizure disorders, delirium or madness can occur, causing the seizures to stop; likewise, the seizures can start, and the delirium can end. I've seen this happen many times in just one day during severe episodes of epilepsy; this shows that one type of delirium is a disruption of the sensory organs, and that our thoughts are the movements of these organs: the following cases will illustrate this observation.

Miss G——, a fair young lady, with light eyes and hair, was seized with most violent convulsions of her limbs, with outrageous hiccough, and most vehement efforts to vomit: after near an hour was elapsed this tragedy ceased, and a calm talkative delirium supervened for about another hour; and these relieved each other at intervals during the greatest part of three or four days. After having carefully considered this disease, I thought the convulsions of her ideas less dangerous than those of her muscles; and having in vain attempted to make any opiate continue in her stomach, an ounce of laudanum was rubbed along the spine of her back, and a dram of it was used as an enema; by this medicine a kind of drunken delirium was continued many hours; and when it ceased the convulsions did not return; and the lady continued well many years, except some lighter relapses, which were relieved in the same manner.

Miss G——, a fair young woman with light eyes and hair, experienced severe muscle spasms, uncontrollable hiccups, and strong efforts to vomit. After about an hour, this episode stopped, and she entered a calm but talkative delirium for roughly another hour; these two states alternated throughout a significant part of three or four days. After closely examining her condition, I determined that the spasms in her mind seemed less dangerous than those in her body. I struggled to keep any opiate in her stomach, so I applied an ounce of laudanum along her spine and used a dram of it as an enema. This treatment induced a sort of drunken delirium that lasted several hours; when it ended, the spasms did not return, and she remained well for many years, aside from some minor relapses, which were treated in the same way.

Miss H——, an accomplished young lady, with light eyes and hair, was seized with convulsions of her limbs, with hiccough, and efforts to vomit, more violent than words can express; these continued near an hour, and were succeeded with a cataleptic spasm of one arm, with the hand applied to her head; and after about twenty minutes these spasms ceased, and a talkative reverie supervened for near an other hour, from which no violence, which it was proper to use, could awaken her. These periods of convulsions, first of the muscles, and then of the ideas, returned twice a day for several weeks; and were at length removed by great doses of opium, after a great variety of other medicines and applications had been in vain experienced. This lady was subject to frequent relapses, once or twice a year for many years, and was as frequently relieved by the same method.

Miss H——, a talented young woman with light eyes and hair, experienced intense convulsions in her limbs, along with hiccups and efforts to vomit that were more extreme than words can describe; these episodes lasted for almost an hour and were followed by a cataleptic spasm in one arm, with her hand against her head. After about twenty minutes, the spasms stopped, and she entered a talkative reverie that lasted nearly another hour, from which no amount of appropriate force could bring her out. These convulsive episodes, first affecting her muscles and then her thoughts, occurred twice a day for several weeks and were eventually alleviated by large doses of opium, after trying a wide range of other medications and treatments in vain. This lady often had relapses, once or twice a year for many years, and was similarly treated with the same method each time.

Miss W——, an elegant young lady, with black eyes and hair, had sometimes a violent pain of her side, at other times a most painful strangury, which were every day succeeded by delirium; which gave a temporary relief to the painful spasms. After the vain exhibition of variety of medicines and applications by different physicians, for more than a twelvemonth, she was directed to take some doses of opium, which were gradually increased, by which a drunken delirium was kept up for a day or two, and the pains prevented from returning. A flesh diet, with a little wine or beer, instead of the low regimen she had previously used, in a few weeks completely established her health; which, except a few relapses, has continued for many years.

Miss W——, an elegant young woman with black eyes and hair, occasionally experienced severe pain in her side and at other times suffered from intense urinary discomfort, which was followed daily by delirium that provided temporary relief from the painful spasms. After a year of unsuccessful treatments with various medicines and applications from different doctors, she was advised to take doses of opium, which were gradually increased. This resulted in a drunken delirium that lasted for a day or two, keeping the pain at bay. Switching to a meat-based diet, with a bit of wine or beer, instead of the low diet she had been on, restored her health completely within a few weeks, which, aside from a few relapses, has lasted for many years.

9. Lastly, as we advance in life all the parts of the body become more rigid, and are rendered less susceptible of new habits of motion, though they retain those that were before established. This is sensibly observed by those who apply themselves late in life to music, fencing, or any of the mechanic arts. In the same manner many elderly people retain the ideas they had learned early in life, but find great difficulty in acquiring new trains of memory; insomuch that in extreme old age we frequently see a forgetfulness of the business of yesterday, and at the same time a circumstantial remembrance of the amusements of their youth; till at length the ideas of recollection and activity of the body gradually cease together,—such is the condition of humanity!—and nothing remains but the vital motions and sensations.

9. Lastly, as we grow older, all parts of the body become stiffer and are less able to adapt to new movements, although they still remember the habits that were formed earlier. This is clearly noticeable for those who take up music, fencing, or any of the mechanical arts later in life. Similarly, many older adults hold onto the knowledge they gained in their youth but struggle to learn new things; in extreme old age, we often see them forgetting what happened yesterday, while still vividly recalling the joys of their youth. Eventually, the memories and physical activity both start to fade away—such is the human condition!—leaving only the essential life functions and sensations.

VI. 1. In opposition to this doctrine of the production of our ideas, it may be asked, if some of our ideas, like other animal motions, are voluntary, why can we not invent new ones, that have not been received by perception? The answer will be better understood after having perused the succeeding section, where it will be explained, that the muscular motions likewise are originally excited by the stimulus of bodies external to the moving organ; and that the will has only the power of repeating the motions thus excited.

VI. 1. In contrast to this idea of how we generate our thoughts, one might wonder if some of our thoughts, like other movements of animals, are voluntary. If that’s the case, why can’t we create new thoughts that we haven't already perceived? The answer will be clearer after reading the next section, where it will be explained that muscular movements are also initially triggered by external bodies acting on the moving part, and that our will can only repeat those movements that have already been triggered.

2. Another objector may ask, Can the motion of an organ of sense resemble an odour or a colour? To which I can only answer, that it has not been demonstrated that any of our ideas resemble the objects that excite them; it has generally been believed that they do not; but this shall be discussed at large in Sect. XIV.

2. Another objector may ask, Can the motion of an organ of sense resemble an odour or a colour? To which I can only answer, that it has not been demonstrated that any of our ideas resemble the objects that excite them; it has generally been believed that they do not; but this shall be discussed at large in Sect. XIV.

3. There is another objection that at first view would seem less easy to surmount. After the amputation, of a foot or a finger, it has frequently happened, that an injury being offered to the stump of the amputated limb, whether from cold air, too great pressure, or other accidents, the patient has complained, of a sensation of pain in the foot or finger, that was cut off. Does not this evince that all our ideas are excited in the brain, and not in the organs of sense? This objection is answered, by observing that our ideas of the shape, place, and solidity of our limbs, are acquired by our organs of touch and of sight, which are situated in our fingers and eyes, and not by any sensations in the limb itself.

3. There’s another objection that might initially seem harder to overcome. After a foot or finger is amputated, it often happens that if the stump of the amputated limb is injured—due to cold air, excessive pressure, or other accidents—the patient reports feeling pain in the foot or finger that was removed. Doesn’t this suggest that all our sensations come from the brain, not from the sensory organs? This objection is addressed by noting that our perceptions of the shape, position, and solidity of our limbs are learned through our sense of touch and sight, which are located in our fingers and eyes, rather than from any sensations in the limb itself.

In this case the pain or sensation, which formerly has arisen in the foot or toes, and been propagated along the nerves to the central part of the sensorium, was at the same time accompanied with a visible idea of the shape and place, and with a tangible idea of the solidity of the affected limb: now when these nerves are afterwards affected by any injury done to the remaining stump with a similar degree or kind of pain, the ideas of the shape, place, or solidity of the lost limb, return by association; as these ideas belong to the organs of sight and touch, on which they were first excited.

In this case, the pain or sensation that used to occur in the foot or toes and was transmitted through the nerves to the central part of the brain was also connected to a clear mental picture of the shape and location, along with a physical sense of the heaviness of the affected limb. Now, when these nerves are later impacted by an injury to the remaining stump that causes similar pain, the mental images of the shape, location, or heaviness of the lost limb come back through association, since these images are linked to the senses of sight and touch that originally triggered them.

4. If you wonder what organs of sense can be excited into motion, when you call up the ideas of wisdom or benevolence, which Mr. Locke has termed abstracted ideas; I ask you by what organs of sense you first became acquainted with these ideas? And the answer will be reciprocal; for it is certain that all our ideas were originally acquired by our organs of sense; for whatever excites our perception must be external to the organ that perceives it, and we have no other inlets to knowledge but by our perceptions: as will be further explained in Section XIV. and XV. on the Productions and Classes of Ideas.

4. If you wonder what organs of sense can be excited into motion, when you call up the ideas of wisdom or benevolence, which Mr. Locke has termed abstracted ideas; I ask you by what organs of sense you first became acquainted with these ideas? And the answer will be reciprocal; for it is certain that all our ideas were originally acquired by our organs of sense; for whatever excites our perception must be external to the organ that perceives it, and we have no other inlets to knowledge but by our perceptions: as will be further explained in Section XIV. and XV. on the Productions and Classes of Ideas.

VII. If our recollection or imagination be not a repetition of animal movements, I ask, in my turn, What is it? You tell me it consists of images or pictures of things. Where is this extensive canvas hung up? or where are the numerous receptacles in which those are deposited? or to what else in the animal system have they any similitude?

VII. If our memory or imagination isn't just a replay of animal actions, then I have to ask, what is it? You say it's made up of images or pictures of things. So, where is this large canvas displayed? Or where are all the various places where these are stored? Or what else in the animal body do they resemble?

That pleasing picture of objects, represented in miniature on the retina of the eye, seems to have given rise to this illusive oratory! It was forgot that this representation belongs rather to the laws of light, than to those of life; and may with equal elegance be seen in the camera obscura as in the eye; and that the picture vanishes for ever, when the object is withdrawn.

That nice image of things, shown in small form on the retina of the eye, seems to have inspired this misleading talk! It was overlooked that this image is more about the laws of light than the laws of life; it can be just as beautifully viewed in a camera obscura as in the eye, and that the image disappears completely when the object is taken away.



SECT. IV.

LAWS OF ANIMAL CAUSATION.

LAWS OF ANIMAL CAUSATION.

I. The fibres, which constitute the muscles and organs of sense, possess a power of contraction. The circumstances attending the exertion of this power of CONTRACTION constitute the laws of animal motion, as the circumstances attending the exertion of the power of ATTRACTION constitute the laws of motion of inanimate matter.

I. The fibers that make up the muscles and sensory organs have the ability to contract. The conditions that influence this contraction power define the laws of animal movement, just as the conditions influencing the attraction power define the laws of motion for inanimate objects.

II. The spirit of animation is the immediate cause of the contraction of animal fibres, it resides in the brain and nerves, and is liable to general or partial diminution or accumulation.

II. The essence of animation is the direct cause of the tightening of animal fibers; it exists in the brain and nerves and can decrease or increase in intensity, either overall or in specific areas.

III. The stimulus of bodies external to the moving organ is the remote cause of the original contractions of animal fibres.

III. The influence of external bodies on the moving organ is the distant cause of the initial contractions of animal fibers.

IV. A certain quantity of stimulus produces irritation, which is an exertion of the spirit of animation exciting the fibres into contraction.

IV. A certain amount of stimulus creates irritation, which is an effort of the spirit of animation causing the fibers to contract.

V. A certain quantity of contraction of animal fibres, if it be perceived at all, produces pleasure; a greater or less quantity of contraction, if it be perceived at all, produces pain; these constitute sensation.

V. A certain amount of contraction in animal fibers, if noticed, brings pleasure; a greater or lesser amount of contraction, if noticed, brings pain; these experiences make up sensation.

VI. A certain quantity of sensation produces desire or aversion; these constitute volition.

VI. A certain amount of sensation creates desire or dislike; these make up our will.

VII. All animal motions which have occurred at the same time, or in immediate succession, become so connected, that when one of them is reproduced, the other has a tendency to accompany or succeed it. When fibrous contractions succeed or accompany other fibrous contractions, the connection is termed association; when fibrous contractions succeed sensorial motions, the connexion is termed causation; when fibrous and sensorial motions reciprocally introduce each other, it is termed catenation of animal motions. All these connections are said to be produced by habit, that is, by frequent repetition. These laws of animal causation will be evinced by numerous facts, which occur in our daily exertions; and will afterwards be employed to explain the more recondite phænomena of the production, growth, diseases, and decay of the animal system.

VII. All animal movements that happen at the same time or closely one after the other become connected in such a way that when one is triggered, the other tends to happen alongside it or follow it. When muscle contractions happen after or together with other muscle contractions, this connection is called association; when muscle contractions follow sensory actions, the connection is referred to as causation; when muscle and sensory motions introduce each other alternately, it’s called catenation of animal movements. All these connections are said to arise from habit, which means frequent repetition. These laws of animal causation will be demonstrated by many facts that occur in our everyday activities and will later be used to explain the more obscure phenomena of production, growth, diseases, and decay in the animal system.



SECT. V.

OF THE FOUR FACULTIES OR MOTIONS OF THE SENSORIUM.

OF THE FOUR FACULTIES OR MOTIONS OF THE SENSORIUM.

1. Four sensorial powers. 2. Irritation, sensation, volition, association defined. 3. Sensorial motions distinguished from fibrous motions.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Four sensory abilities. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Irritation, sensation, will, and association defined. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Distinguishing sensory movements from muscular movements.

1. The spirit of animation has four different modes of action, or in other words the animal sensorium possesses four different faculties, which are occasionally exerted, and cause all the contractions of the fibrous parts of the body. These are the faculty of causing fibrous contractions in consequence of the irritations excited by external bodies, in consequence of the sensations of pleasure or pain, in consequence of volition, and in consequence of the associations of fibrous contractions with other fibrous contractions, which precede or accompany them.

1. The essence of animation has four different ways it can act, or in simpler terms, the animal nervous system has four distinct abilities that can be activated at different times, leading to all the contractions of the body's fibrous parts. These abilities include causing fibrous contractions due to irritations from external sources, as a result of feelings of pleasure or pain, because of willpower, and due to the connections between fibrous contractions that happen before or alongside each other.

These four faculties of the sensorium during their inactive state are termed irritability, sensibility, voluntarity, and associability; in their active state they are termed as above, irritation, sensation, volition, association.

These four faculties of the sensory system, when they are inactive, are called irritability, sensibility, voluntarity, and associability; when they are active, they are referred to as irritation, sensation, volition, and association.

2. IRRITATION is an exertion or change of some extreme part of the sensorium residing in the muscles or organs of sense, in consequence of the appulses of external bodies.

2. IIRRITATION is a strain or shift of a sensitive area in the muscles or sensory organs due to the impact of outside forces.

SENSATION is an exertion or change of the central parts of the sensorium, or of the whole of it, beginning at some of those extreme parts of it, which reside in the muscles or organs of sense.

SSensation is an effort or shift in the main areas of perception, or in all of them, starting from some of those outer areas, which are found in the muscles or sense organs.

VOLITION is an exertion or change of the central parts of the sensorium, or of the whole of it, terminating in some of those extreme parts of it, which reside in the muscles or organs of sense.

VOLITION is a process or shift in the main areas of the sensory system, or in all of it, resulting in activity in some of the outer parts, which are located in the muscles or sensory organs.

ASSOCIATION is an exertion or change of some extreme part of the sensorium residing in the muscles or organs of sense, in consequence of some antecedent or attendant fibrous contractions.

AASSOCIATION is a effort or change in a specific area of the sensory system located in the muscles or sensory organs, as a result of some prior or simultaneous muscular contractions.

3. These four faculties of the animal sensorium may at the time of their exertions be termed motions without impropriety of language; for we cannot pass from a state of insensibility or inaction to a state of sensibility or of exertion without some change of the sensorium, and every change includes motion. We shall therefore sometimes term the above described faculties sensorial motions to distinguish them from fibrous motions; which latter expression includes the motions of the muscles and organs of sense.

3. These four functions of the animal senses can be called movements during their use without misusing language; because we can't shift from a state of unresponsiveness or inactivity to one of awareness or effort without some change in the senses, and every change involves movement. We will therefore occasionally refer to the faculties described above as sensorial motions to differentiate them from fibrous motions; the latter term refers to the movements of the muscles and sensory organs.

The active motions of the fibres, whether those of the muscles or organs of sense, are probably simple contractions; the fibres being again elongated by antagonist muscles, by circulating fluids, or sometimes by elastic ligaments, as in the necks of quadrupeds. The sensorial motions, which constitute the sensations of pleasure or pain, and which constitute volition, and which cause the fibrous contractions in consequence of irritation or of association, are not here supposed to be fluctuations or refluctuations of the spirit of animation; nor are they supposed to be vibrations or revibrations, nor condensations or equilibrations of it; but to be changes or motions of it peculiar to life.

The active movements of the fibers, whether from muscles or sensory organs, are probably just simple contractions; the fibers are then stretched again by opposing muscles, circulating fluids, or sometimes by elastic ligaments, like those in the necks of four-legged animals. The sensory movements, which make up feelings of pleasure or pain, and which lead to willful actions and cause fiber contractions due to irritation or association, are not thought to be fluctuations or returns of the spirit of life; nor are they considered vibrations or reverberations, or compressions or balances of it; rather, they are seen as changes or movements of it that are unique to living beings.



SECT. VI.

OF THE FOUR CLASSES OF FIBROUS MOTIONS.

OF THE FOUR TYPES OF FIBROUS MOVEMENTS.

I. Origin of fibrous contractions. II. Distribution of them into four classes, irritative motions, sensitive motions, voluntary motions, and associate motions, defined.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Source of fibrous contractions. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Classification into four types: irritative, sensitive, voluntary, and associative movements, explained.

I. All the fibrous contractions of animal bodies originate from the sensorium, and resolve themselves into four classes, correspondent with the four powers or motions of the sensorium above described, and from which they have their causation.

I. All the muscle contractions in animal bodies start from the sensory system and fall into four categories, corresponding to the four functions or movements of the sensory system mentioned earlier, which are their source of origin.

1. These fibrous contractions were originally caused by the irritations excited by objects, which are external to the moving organ. As the pulsations of the heart are owing to the irritations excited by the stimulus of the blood; and the ideas of perception are owing to the irritations excited by external bodies.

1. These fibrous contractions were originally triggered by irritations caused by objects outside the moving organ. Just as the heart's pulsations are due to irritations caused by the stimulus of the blood, and our perceptions are due to irritations caused by external bodies.

2. But as painful or pleasurable sensations frequently accompanied those irritations, by habit these fibrous contractions became causeable by the sensations, and the irritations ceased to be necessary to their production. As the secretion of tears in grief is caused by the sensation of pain; and the ideas of imagination, as in dreams or delirium, are excited by the pleasure or pain, with which they were formerly accompanied.

2. But since painful or pleasurable sensations often accompanied those irritations, these fibrous contractions became triggered by the sensations over time, making the irritations no longer necessary for their occurrence. Just as the secretion of tears in grief is triggered by the feeling of pain; the ideas of imagination, as seen in dreams or delirium, are stirred by the pleasure or pain they were once associated with.

3. But as the efforts of the will frequently accompanied these painful or pleasureable sensations, by habit the fibrous contractions became causable by volition; and both the irritations and sensations ceased to be necessary to their production. As the deliberate locomotions of the body, and the ideas of recollection, as when we will to repeat the alphabet backwards.

3. But as our willpower often went along with these painful or pleasurable feelings, the muscle contractions became controllable by choice over time; and both the irritations and sensations were no longer needed to make them happen. Just like the intentional movements of the body, and the memories we call on, like when we decide to recite the alphabet backwards.

4. But as many of these fibrous contractions frequently accompanied other fibrous contractions, by habit they became causable by their associations with them; and the irritations, sensations, and volition, ceased to be necessary to their production. As the actions of the muscles of the lower limbs in fencing are associated with those of the arms; and the ideas of suggestion are associated with other ideas, which precede or accompany them; as in repeating carelessly the alphabet in its usual order after having began it.

4. But since many of these muscle contractions often occurred alongside other muscle contractions, they became triggers for each other by habit; and the irritations, sensations, and intentions were no longer needed for their occurrence. Just as the movements of the legs in fencing connect with those of the arms; and how suggestions relate to other thoughts that come before or alongside them; similar to casually reciting the alphabet in the usual order after starting it.

II. We shall give the following names to these four classes of fibrous motions, and subjoin their definitions.

II. We'll assign the following names to these four types of fibrous movements and provide their definitions.

1. Irritative motions. That exertion or change of the sensorium, which is caused by the appulses of external bodies, either simply subsides, or is succeeded by sensation, or it produces fibrous motions; it is termed irritation, and irritative motions are those contractions of the muscular fibres, or of the organs of sense, that are immediately consequent to this exertion or change of the sensorium.

1. Irritative movements. The effort or change in perception caused by external stimuli either simply fades away, leads to sensation, or triggers muscular contractions; this is called irritation. Irritative movements are the contractions of muscle fibers or sensory organs that occur immediately after this effort or change in perception.

2. Sensitive motions. That exertion or change of the sensorium, which constitutes pleasure or pain, either simply subsides, or is succeeded by volition, or it produces fibrous motions; it is termed sensation, and the sensitive motions are those contractions of the muscular fibres, or of the organs of sense, that are immediately consequent to this exertion or change of the sensorium.

2. Sensitive motions. The effort or change in the senses that creates pleasure or pain either just fades away, leads to a decision, or results in muscular contractions. This is called sensation, and the sensitive motions are the contractions of the muscle fibers or sensory organs that immediately follow this effort or change in the senses.

3. Voluntary motions. That exertion or change of the sensorium, which constitutes desire or aversion, either simply subsides, or is succeeded by fibrous motions; it is then termed volition, and voluntary motions are those contractions of the muscular fibres, or of the organs of sense, that are immediately consequent to this exertion or change of the sensorium.

3. Voluntary movements. The effort or change in the senses that creates desire or dislike either just fades away or leads to muscle movements; this is called volition, and voluntary movements are those muscle contractions or reactions of the sensory organs that happen right after this effort or change in the senses.

4. Associate motions. That exertion or change of the sensorium, which accompanies fibrous motions, either simply subsides, or is succeeded by sensation or volition, or it produces other fibrous motions; it is then termed association, and the associate motions are those contractions of the muscular fibres, or of the organs of sense, that are immediately consequent to this exertion or change of the sensorium.

4. Associate motions. The effort or change in the sensory system that comes with muscle movements either simply fades away, is followed by feeling or action, or leads to other muscle movements. This is called association, and the associated motions are the contractions of the muscle fibers or sensory organs that happen right after this effort or change in the sensory system.



SECT. VII.

OF IRRITATIVE MOTIONS.

OF IRRITATING MOVEMENTS.

I. 1. Some muscular motions are excited by perpetual irritations. 2. Others more frequently by sensations. 3. Others by volition. Case of involuntary stretchings in paralytic limbs. 4. Some sensual motions are excited by perpetual irritations. 5. Others more frequently by sensation or volition.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Some muscle movements are triggered by constant irritation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Others are more often influenced by sensations. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Some are initiated by will, like involuntary stretching in paralyzed limbs. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Some sensory movements are triggered by ongoing irritation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Others are more often influenced by sensation or will.

II. 1. Muscular motions excited by perpetual irritations occasionally become obedient sensation and to volition. 2. And the sensual motions.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Muscle movements triggered by constant irritation can sometimes respond to sensations and will. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. And the sensory movements.

III. 1. Other muscular motions are associated with the irritative ones. 2. And other ideas with irritative ones. Of letters, language, hieroglyphics. Irritative ideas exist without our attention to them.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Other muscle movements are linked with the irritative ones. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. And other ideas are tied to irritative ones, including letters, language, and hieroglyphics. Irritative ideas can exist without our awareness of them.

I. 1. Many of our muscular motions are excited by perpetual irritations, as those of the heart and arterial system by the circumfluent blood. Many other of them are excited by intermitted irritations, as those of the stomach and bowels by the aliment we swallow; of the bile-ducts by the bile; of the kidneys, pancreas, and many other glands, by the peculiar fluids they separate from the blood; and those of the lacteal and other absorbent vessels by the chyle, lymph, and moisture of the atmosphere. These motions are accelerated or retarded, as their correspondent irritations are increased or diminished, without our attention or consciousness, in the same manner as the various secretions of fruit, gum, resin, wax, and, honey, are produced in the vegetable world, and as the juices of the earth and the moisture of the atmosphere are absorbed by their roots and foliage.

I. 1. Many of our muscle movements are triggered by constant irritations, similar to how the heart and arteries react to flowing blood. Other movements are triggered by intermittent irritations, like how the stomach and intestines respond to the food we eat; how the bile ducts react to bile; how the kidneys, pancreas, and various glands respond to the specific fluids they extract from the blood; and how the lacteal and other absorbent vessels are influenced by chyle, lymph, and moisture in the air. These movements speed up or slow down as their corresponding irritations increase or decrease, without us even noticing, just like how different secretions of fruit, gum, resin, wax, and honey are created in the plant world, and how the earth's juices and atmospheric moisture are absorbed by their roots and leaves.

2. Other muscular motions, that are most frequently connected with our sensations, as those of the sphincters of the bladder and anus, and the musculi erectores penis, were originally excited into motion by irritation, for young children make water, and have other evacuations without attention to these circumstances; "et primis etiam ab incunabulis tenduntur sæpius puerorum penes, amore nondum expergefacto." So the nipples of young women are liable to become turgid by irritation, long before they are in a situation to be excited by the pleasure of giving milk to the lips of a child.

2. Other muscle movements that are most often linked to our sensations, like those of the bladder and anal sphincters, as well as the muscles that raise the penis, were initially triggered by irritation. Young children urinate and have other bodily functions without paying attention to these factors; "and even from their earliest days, the penises of boys often become erect before they experience the feelings of love." Similarly, the nipples of young women can become swollen due to irritation, even before they are in a situation where they feel the joy of breastfeeding a child.

3. The contractions of the larger muscles of our bodies, that are most frequently connected with volition, were originally excited into action by internal irritations: as appears from the stretching or yawning of all animals after long sleep. In the beginning of some fevers this irritation of the muscles produces perpetual stretching and yawning; in other periods of fever an universal restlessness arises from the same cause, the patient changing the attitude of his body every minute. The repeated struggles of the fœtus in the uterus must be owing to this internal irritation: for the fœtus can have no other inducement to move its limbs but the tædium or irksomeness of a continued posture.

3. The contractions of the larger muscles in our bodies, which are usually linked to our will, were initially triggered by internal stimuli: as seen with the stretching or yawning of all animals after a long sleep. At the start of some fevers, this irritation of the muscles leads to constant stretching and yawning; during other phases of fever, a general restlessness occurs for the same reason, causing the patient to shift their body position every minute. The repeated movements of the fetus in the uterus must be due to this internal irritation, as the fetus has no other reason to move its limbs except the discomfort or annoyance of staying in the same position for too long.

The following case evinces, that the motions of stretching the limbs after a continued attitude are not always owing to the power of the will. Mr. Dean, a mason, of Austry in Leicestershire, had the spine of the third vertebra of the back enlarged; in some weeks his lower extremities became feeble, and at length quite paralytic: neither the pain of blisters, the heat of fomentations, nor the utmost efforts of the will could produce the least motion in these limbs; yet twice or thrice a day for many months his feet, legs, and thighs, were affected for many minutes with forceable stretchings, attended with the sensation of fatigue; and he at length recovered the use of his limbs, though the spine continued protuberant. The same circumstance is frequently seen in a less degree in the common hemiplagia; and when this happens, I have believed repeated and strong shocks of electricity to have been of great advantage.

The following case shows that the movements of stretching the limbs after being in a fixed position aren’t always caused by willpower. Mr. Dean, a mason from Austry in Leicestershire, had his third vertebra in the back enlarged. After a few weeks, his lower limbs became weak and eventually completely paralyzed: neither the pain from blisters, the heat from wraps, nor his strongest efforts could make any movement happen in those limbs. However, two or three times a day, for many months, his feet, legs, and thighs experienced involuntary stretching for several minutes, accompanied by a sense of fatigue. Ultimately, he regained the use of his limbs, even though his spine remained swollen. This same situation is often observed in less severe cases of common hemiplegia, and when it occurs, I have found that repeated and strong electrical shocks can be very beneficial.

4. In like manner the various organs of sense are originally excited into motion by various external stimuli adapted to this purpose, which motions are termed perceptions or ideas; and many of these motions during our waking hours are excited by perpetual irritation, as those of the organs of hearing and of touch. The former by the constant low indistinct noises that murmur around us, and the latter by the weight of our bodies on the parts which support them; and by the unceasing variations of the heat, moisture, and pressure of the atmosphere; and these sensual motions, precisely as the muscular ones above mentioned, obey their correspondent irritations without our attention or consciousness.

4. Similarly, our various senses are initially triggered by different external stimuli designed for this purpose, and these triggers are called perceptions or ideas. Many of these triggers during our waking hours come from constant irritation, like those of our hearing and touch. Hearing deals with the steady, low, indistinct noises around us, while touch is affected by the weight of our bodies on the parts that support them, along with the continuous changes in heat, moisture, and atmospheric pressure. These sensory activities, just like the muscular ones mentioned earlier, respond to their corresponding irritations without needing our focus or awareness.

5. Other classes of our ideas are more frequently excited by our sensations of pleasure or pain, and others by volition: but that these have all been originally excited by stimuli from external objects, and only vary in their combinations or reparations, has been fully evinced by Mr. Locke: and are by him termed the ideas of perception in contradistinction to those, which he calls the ideas of reflection.

5. Other types of our ideas are often triggered by our feelings of pleasure or pain, while some are triggered by willpower. However, all of these ideas were initially stimulated by external factors, and they only differ in how they are combined or repaired, as Mr. Locke has clearly demonstrated. He refers to these as the ideas of perception, in contrast to those he calls the ideas of reflection.

II. 1. These muscular motions, that are excited by perpetual irritation, are nevertheless occasionally excitable by the sensations of pleasure or pain, or by volition; as appears by the palpitation of the heart from fear, the increased secretion of saliva at the sight of agreeable food, and the glow on the skin of those who are ashamed. There is an instance told in the Philosophical Transactions of a man, who could for a time stop the motion of his heart when he pleased; and Mr. D. has often told me, be could so far increase the peristaltic motion of his bowels by voluntary efforts, as to produce an evacuation by stool at any time in half an hour.

II. 1. These muscular movements, triggered by constant irritation, can also be influenced by feelings of pleasure or pain, or by a conscious decision; for example, when our heart races from fear, we salivate more at the sight of delicious food, and our skin flushes when we feel embarrassed. There's an example mentioned in the Philosophical Transactions about a man who could temporarily stop his heart from beating whenever he wanted; and Mr. D. has often told me that he could increase the movement in his intestines through willpower enough to have a bowel movement at any given time within half an hour.

2. In like manner the sensual motions, or ideas, that are excited by perpetual irritation, are nevertheless occasionally excited by sensation or volition; as in the night, when we listen under the influence of fear, or from voluntary attention, the motions excited in the organ of hearing by the whispering of the air in our room, the pulsation of our own arteries, or the faint beating of a distant watch, become objects of perception.

2. Similarly, the physical sensations or thoughts triggered by constant irritation can also be sparked by feeling or intention; for example, at night when we pay attention out of fear or conscious choice, the noises caused by the air whispering in our room, the beating of our own heart, or the faint ticking of a distant watch become noticeable to us.

III. 1. Innumerable trains or tribes of other motions are associated with these muscular motions which are excited by irritation; as by the stimulus of the blood in the right chamber of the heart, the lungs are induced to expand themselves; and the pectoral and intercostal muscles, and the diaphragm, act at the same time by their associations with them. And when the pharinx is irritated by agreeable food, the muscles of deglutition are brought into action by association. Thus when a greater light falls on the eye, the iris is brought into action without our attention; and the ciliary process, when the focus is formed before or behind the retina, by their associations with the increased irritative motions of the organ of vision. Many common actions of life are produced in a similar manner. If a fly settle on my forehead, whilst I am intent on my present occupation, I dislodge it with my finger, without exciting my attention or breaking the train of my ideas.

III. 1. Countless other movements are connected to these muscle movements that are triggered by irritation; for example, when blood flows into the right chamber of the heart, the lungs expand as a result. The chest and intercostal muscles, along with the diaphragm, work together at the same time. Similarly, when the throat is stimulated by tasty food, the swallowing muscles activate through their connections. So, when brighter light hits the eye, the iris reacts without us noticing; and the ciliary muscles adjust the focus depending on whether the image is in front of or behind the retina, responding to the increased stimulation of the visual system. Many everyday actions happen in the same way. If a fly lands on my forehead while I’m focused on something else, I brush it away with my finger without even thinking about it or interrupting my train of thought.

2. In like manner the irritative ideas suggest to us many other trains or tribes of ideas that are associated with them. On this kind of connection, language, letters, hieroglyphics, and every kind of symbol, depend. The symbols themselves produce irritative ideas, or sensual motions, which we do not attend to; and other ideas, that are succeeded by sensation, are excited by their association with them. And as these irritative ideas make up a part of the chain of our waking thoughts, introducing other ideas that engage our attention, though themselves are unattended to, we find it very difficult to investigate by what steps many of our hourly trains of ideas gain their admittance.

2. Similarly, the trigger ideas lead us to many other thoughts or groups of ideas that are connected to them. This kind of connection is what language, letters, hieroglyphics, and all sorts of symbols rely on. The symbols themselves generate trigger ideas or sensory reactions that we often overlook; other ideas that follow sensation are prompted by their association with these symbols. And since these trigger ideas form part of the stream of our conscious thoughts, bringing in other ideas that capture our focus, even though they go unnoticed, we find it really challenging to figure out the steps through which many of our daily streams of ideas come to mind.

It may appear paradoxical, that ideas can exist, and not be attended to; but all our perceptions are ideas excited by irritation, and succeeded by sensation. Now when these ideas excited by irritation give us neither pleasure nor pain, we cease to attend to them. Thus whilst I am walking through that grove before my window, I do not run against the trees or the benches, though my thoughts are strenuously exerted on some other object. This leads us to a distinct knowledge of irritative ideas, for the idea of the tree or bench, which I avoid, exists on my retina, and induces by association the action of certain locomotive muscles; though neither itself nor the actions of those muscles engage my attention.

It might seem contradictory that ideas can exist without being noticed, but all our perceptions are ideas triggered by stimulation, followed by sensation. When these ideas stimulated by irritation don't bring us pleasure or pain, we stop paying attention to them. So, while I'm walking through that grove outside my window, I don't bump into the trees or benches, even though my thoughts are focused on something else. This helps us understand irritative ideas more clearly, as the idea of the tree or bench that I avoid is present on my retina and triggers certain muscle movements, even though neither the idea itself nor those muscle actions capture my attention.

Thus whilst we are conversing on this subject, the tone, note, and articulation of every individual word forms its correspondent irritative idea on the organ of hearing; but we only attend to the associated ideas, that are attached by habit to these irritative ones, and are succeeded by sensation; thus when we read the words "PRINTING-PRESS" we do not attend to the shape, size, or existence of the letters which compose these words, though each of them excites a correspondent irritative motion of our organ of vision, but they introduce by association our idea of the most useful of modern inventions; the capacious reservoir of human knowledge, whose branching streams diffuse sciences, arts, and morality, through all nations and all ages.

Thus, while we’re discussing this topic, the tone, pitch, and clarity of every single word create its own corresponding sensation in our ears; however, we only focus on the connected ideas that we've learned to associate with these sensations, which then lead to feelings. So, when we read the words "Printing press", we don’t pay attention to the shape, size, or presence of the letters that make up these words, even though each one triggers a similar reaction in our eyes. Instead, they bring to mind our idea of one of the most valuable modern inventions: the vast source of human knowledge, whose various branches spread sciences, arts, and ethics across all nations and throughout history.



SECT. VIII.

OF SENSITIVE MOTIONS.

OF SENSITIVE ACTIONS.

I. 1. Sensitive muscular motions were originally excited into action by irritation. 2. And sensitive sensual motions, ideas of imagination, dreams. II. 1. Sensitive muscular motions are occasionally obedient to volition. 2. And sensitive sensual motions. III. 1. Other muscular motions are associated with the sensitive ones. 2. And other sensual motions.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Sensitive muscle movements were initially triggered by irritation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. And sensitive sensations, imaginative ideas, dreams. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Sensitive muscle movements can sometimes be controlled by will. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. And sensitive sensations. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Other muscle movements are linked to the sensitive ones. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. And other sensory movements.

I. 1. Many of the motions of our muscles, that are excited into action by irritation, are at the same time accompanied with painful or pleasurable sensations; and at length become by habit causable by the sensations. Thus the motions of the sphincters of the bladder and anus were originally excited into action by irritation; for young children give no attention to these evacuations; but as soon as they become sensible of the inconvenience of obeying these irritations, they suffer the water or excrement to accumulate, till it disagreeably affects them; and the action of those sphincters is then in consequence of this disagreeable sensation. So the secretion of saliva, which in young children is copiously produced by irritation, and drops from their mouths, is frequently attended with the agreeable sensation produced by the mastication of tasteful food;, till at length the sight of such food to a hungry person excites into action these salival glands; as is seen in the slavering of hungry dogs.

I. 1. Many of the movements of our muscles, triggered by irritation, are also associated with painful or pleasurable feelings; over time, these movements can become a result of those feelings. For example, the movements of the sphincters of the bladder and anus are initially triggered by irritation; young children typically don't pay much attention to these needs. However, once they realize the discomfort caused by ignoring these irritations, they hold in urine or feces until it becomes unpleasant for them, and the action of those sphincters is then a reaction to that unpleasant feeling. Similarly, the production of saliva in young children is greatly stimulated by irritation, causing it to dribble from their mouths, and it often becomes associated with the pleasurable sensation of chewing tasty food. Eventually, just seeing appetizing food can trigger those salivary glands in a hungry person, much like the drooling seen in hungry dogs.

The motions of those muscles, which are affected by lascivious ideas, and those which are exerted in smiling, weeping, starting from fear, and winking at the approach of danger to the eye, and at times the actions of every large muscle of the body become causable by our sensations. And all these motions are performed with strength and velocity in proportion to the energy of the sensation that excites them, and the quantity of sensorial power.

The movements of those muscles, influenced by lustful thoughts, as well as those used for smiling, crying, flinching from fear, and winking when danger approaches the eye, can sometimes activate every major muscle in the body based on our feelings. All these movements happen with strength and speed that correspond to the intensity of the sensation that triggers them and the amount of sensory energy available.

2. Many of the motions of our organs of sense, or ideas, that were originally excited into action by irritation, become in like manner more frequently causable by our sensations of pleasure or pain. These motions are then termed the ideas of imagination, and make up all the scenery and transactions of our dreams. Thus when any painful or pleasurable sensations possess us, as of love, anger, fear; whether in our sleep or waking hours, the ideas, that have been formerly excited by the objects of these sensations, now vividly recur before us by their connection with these sensations themselves. So the fair smiling virgin, that excited your love by her presence, whenever that sensation recurs, rises before you in imagination; and that with all the pleasing circumstances, that had before engaged your attention. And in sleep, when you dream under the influence of fear, all the robbers, fires, and precipices, that you formerly have seen or heard of, arise before you with terrible vivacity. All these sensual motions, like the muscular ones above mentioned, are performed with strength and velocity in proportion to the energy of the sensation of pleasure or pain, which excites them, and the quantity of sensorial power.

2. Many of the movements of our senses or thoughts, which were originally triggered by irritation, become more frequently caused by our feelings of pleasure or pain. These movements are then called imaginative ideas and make up all the scenes and events in our dreams. So, when we experience any painful or pleasurable sensations, like love, anger, or fear; whether we are sleeping or awake, the ideas that were previously triggered by the objects of these sensations now vividly come back to us because of their connection to those sensations. For instance, the beautiful smiling woman who sparked your love with her presence will appear in your imagination whenever that feeling returns, along with all the delightful details that had caught your interest before. Similarly, in sleep, when you dream under the influence of fear, all the robbers, fires, and cliffs you've seen or heard about come back to you with terrifying clarity. All these sensory movements, much like the muscular ones mentioned earlier, are carried out with strength and speed in proportion to the intensity of the sensation of pleasure or pain that triggers them, along with the amount of sensory power.

II. 1. Many of these muscular motions above described, that are most frequently excited by our sensations, are nevertheless occasionally causable by volition; for we can smile or frown spontaneously, can make water before the quantity or acrimony of the urine produces a disagreeable sensation, and can voluntarily masticate a nauseous drug, or swallow a bitter draught, though our sensation would strongly dissuade us.

II. 1. Many of the muscle movements mentioned above, which are often triggered by our senses, can also sometimes be controlled by our will; for we can smile or frown on our own, can urinate before the amount or harshness of the urine causes discomfort, and can choose to chew a disgusting medicine or drink a bitter liquid, even though our senses strongly discourage us from doing so.

2. In like manner the sensual motions, or ideas, that are most frequently excited by our sensations, are nevertheless occasionally causeable by volition, as we can spontaneously call up our last night's dream before us, tracing it industriously step by step through all its variety of scenery and transaction; or can voluntarily examine or repeat the ideas, that have been excited by out disgust or admiration.

2. Similarly, the feelings or ideas that our senses often trigger can also sometimes be influenced by our will. We can actively bring to mind our dream from the previous night, recalling it in detail as we navigate through its different scenes and events. We can also consciously reflect on or revisit the thoughts that our feelings of disgust or admiration have stirred up.

III. 1. Innumerable trains or tribes of motions are associated with these sensitive muscular motions above mentioned; as when a drop of water falling into the wind-pipe disagreeably affects the air-vessels of the lungs, they are excited into violent action; and with these sensitive motions are associated the actions of the pectoral and intercostal muscles, and the diaphragm; till by their united and repeated succussions the drop is returned through the larinx. The same occurs when any thing disagreeably affects the nostrils, or the stomach, or the uterus; variety of muscles are excited by association into forcible action, not to be suppressed by the utmost efforts of the will; as in sneezing, vomiting, and parturition.

III. 1. Countless movements or groups of motions are connected with these sensitive muscle movements mentioned earlier; for example, when a drop of water falls into the windpipe, it irritates the air vessels in the lungs, causing them to react violently. Along with these sensitive movements, the actions of the pectoral and intercostal muscles and the diaphragm are involved; through their combined and repeated contractions, the drop is expelled through the larynx. The same happens when something irritates the nostrils, stomach, or uterus; a variety of muscles are activated by association into strong actions that cannot be controlled by the strongest willpower, as seen in sneezing, vomiting, and childbirth.

2. In like manner with these sensitive sensual motions, or ideas of imagination, are associated many other trains or tribes of ideas, which by some writers of metaphysics have been classed under the terms of resemblance, causation, and contiguity; and will be more fully treated of hereafter.

2. Similarly, alongside these delicate sensual movements or imaginative ideas, there are many other groups or sets of ideas that some metaphysical writers have categorized as resemblance, causation, and contiguity; these will be discussed in more detail later.



SECT. IX.

OF VOLUNTARY MOTIONS.

Voluntary motions.

I. 1. Voluntary muscular motions are originally excited by irritations. 2. And voluntary ideas. Of reason. II. 1. Voluntary muscular motions are occasionally causable by sensations. 2. And voluntary ideas. III. 1. Voluntary muscular motions are occasionally obedient to irritations. 2. And voluntary ideas. IV. 1. Voluntary muscular motions are associated with other muscular motions. 2. And voluntary ideas.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Voluntary muscle movements are initially triggered by irritations. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. And voluntary thoughts. Of reason. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Voluntary muscle movements can sometimes be caused by sensations. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. And voluntary thoughts. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Voluntary muscle movements can sometimes respond to irritations. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. And voluntary thoughts. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Voluntary muscle movements are connected to other muscle movements. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. And voluntary thoughts.

When pleasure or pain affect the animal system, many of its motions both muscular and sensual are brought into action; as was shewn in the preceding section, and were called sensitive motions. The general tendency of these motions is to arrest and to possess the pleasure, or to dislodge or avoid the pain: but if this cannot immediately be accomplished, desire or aversion are produced, and the motions in consequence of this new faculty of the sensorium are called voluntary.

When pleasure or pain impact the animal system, many of its movements, both muscular and sensory, come into play; as demonstrated in the previous section, these are referred to as sensitive movements. The primary goal of these movements is to seize and enjoy the pleasure or to escape or avoid the pain. However, if this can't be achieved right away, desire or aversion arise, and the movements resulting from this new capability of the sensory system are called voluntary.

I. 1. Those muscles of the body that are attached to bones, have in general their principal connections with volition, as I move my pen or raise my body. These motions were originally excited by irritation, as was explained in the section on that subject, afterwards the sensations of pleasure or pain, that accompanied the motions thus excited, induced a repetition of them; and at length many of them were voluntarily practised in succession or in combination for the common purposes of life, as in learning to walk, or to speak; and are performed with strength and velocity in proportion to the energy of the volition, that excites them, and the quantity of sensorial power.

I. 1. The muscles in the body that connect to bones are generally linked to our ability to decide, like when I write with my pen or lift my body. These movements initially started from a response to stimuli, as explained in the previous section. Later, the feelings of pleasure or pain associated with these movements prompted us to do them again. Eventually, many of these actions were practiced voluntarily in order or in combination for everyday activities, like learning to walk or talk, and they are performed with strength and speed based on the intensity of the decision behind them and the amount of energy available.

2. Another great class of voluntary motions consists of the ideas of recollection. We will to repeat a certain train of ideas, as of the alphabet backwards; and if any ideas, that do not belong to this intended train, intrude themselves by other connections, we will to reject them, and voluntarily persist in the determined train. So at my approach to a house which I have but once visited, and that at the distance of many months, I will to recollect the names of the numerous family I expect to see there, and I do recollect them.

2. Another great type of voluntary motions involves the ideas of remembering. We choose to repeat a specific sequence of thoughts, like reciting the alphabet backwards; and if any unrelated thoughts try to pop up through other associations, we decide to ignore them and deliberately stick to the original sequence. For instance, as I approach a house I've only visited once, many months ago, I choose to remember the names of the many family members I expect to see there, and I do remember them.

On this voluntary recollection of ideas our faculty of reason depends, as it enables us to acquire an idea of the dissimilitude of any two ideas. Thus if you voluntarily produce the idea of a right-angled triangle, and then of a square; and after having excited these ideas repeatedly, you excite the idea of their difference, which is that of another right-angled triangle inverted over the former; you are said to reason upon this subject, or to compare your ideas.

On this voluntary recall of ideas, our ability to think relies, as it allows us to understand the differences between any two ideas. So, if you consciously bring to mind the idea of a right-angled triangle and then a square; and after repeatedly thinking about these ideas, you then spark the idea of their difference, which is that of another right-angled triangle flipped over the first one; you are said to be reasoning about this topic, or to be comparing your ideas.

These ideas of recollection, like the muscular motions above mentioned, were originally excited by the irritation of external bodies, and were termed ideas of perception: afterwards the pleasure or pain, that accompanied these motions, induced a repetition of them in the absence of the external body, by which they were first excited; and then they were termed ideas of imagination. At length they become voluntarily practised in succession or in combination for the common purposes of life; as when we make ourselves masters of the history of mankind, or of the sciences they have investigated; and are then called ideas of recollection; and are performed with strength and velocity in proportion to the energy of the volition that excites them, and the quantity of sensorial power.

These ideas of remembering, like the physical movements previously mentioned, were originally triggered by the irritation of external things, and were called ideas of perception. Later, the pleasure or pain that came with these movements led to their repetition even when the external trigger was absent; this is when they became known as ideas of imagination. Eventually, they are practiced voluntarily in sequence or combination for everyday purposes, such as when we learn about human history or the sciences they have explored. At this point, they are referred to as ideas of recollection, and they are executed with strength and speed based on the intensity of the will that activates them and the amount of mental energy available.

II. 1. The muscular motions above described, that are most frequently obedient to the will are nevertheless occasionally causable by painful or pleasurable sensation, as in the starting from fear, and the contraction of the calf of the leg in the cramp.

II. 1. The muscle movements mentioned above, which usually respond to our will, can also happen due to painful or pleasurable feelings, like jumping from fear or the tightening of the calf muscle during a cramp.

2. In like manner the sensual motions, or ideas, that are most frequently connected with volition, are nevertheless occasionally causable by painful or pleasurable sensation. As the histories of men, or the description of places, which we have voluntarily taken pains to remember, sometimes occur to us in our dreams.

2. Similarly, the physical urges or thoughts that are most often linked to our decisions can sometimes be triggered by feelings of pain or pleasure. Just like how the stories about people or descriptions of places that we’ve consciously tried to remember can sometimes come to us in our dreams.

III. 1. The muscular motions that are generally subservient to volition, are also occasionally causable by irritation, as in stretching the limbs after sleep, and yawning. In this manner a contraction of the arm is produced by passing the electric fluid from the Leyden phial along its muscles; and that even though the limb is paralytic. The sudden motion of the arm produces a disagreeable sensation in the joint, but the muscles seem to be brought into action simply by irritation.

III. 1. The muscle movements that normally respond to our will can also be triggered by irritation, like when we stretch our limbs after sleeping or yawn. For example, moving an arm can be caused by sending an electric charge from a Leyden jar through its muscles, even if the limb is paralyzed. This sudden movement can create an unpleasant feeling in the joint, but it seems that the muscles activate solely due to irritation.

2. The ideas, that are generally subservient to the will, are in like manner occasionally excited by irritation; as when we view again an object, we have before well studied, and often recollected.

2. The ideas that usually follow our will can also be stirred up by irritation; for example, when we look at an object again that we've previously studied and often remembered.

IV. 1. Innumerable trains or tribes of motions are associated with these voluntary muscular motions above mentioned; as when I will to extend my arm to a distant object, some other muscles are brought into action, and preserve the balance of my body. And when I wish to perform any steady exertion, as in threading a needle, or chopping with an ax, the pectoral muscles are at the same time brought into action to preserve the trunk of the body motionless, and we cease to respire for a time.

IV. 1. Countless trains or groups of movements are linked to the voluntary muscle actions mentioned earlier; for example, when I want to reach for something far away, other muscles engage to keep my body balanced. And when I need to make a steady effort, like when I'm threading a needle or chopping wood, the chest muscles also come into play to keep my torso still, and we hold our breath for a moment.

2. In like manner the voluntary sensual motions, or ideas of recollection, are associated with many other trains or tribes of ideas. As when I voluntarily recollect a gothic window, that I saw some time ago, the whole front of the cathedral occurs to me at the same time.

2. Similarly, when I think back to a sensual experience or an image, it connects with many other thoughts or groups of ideas. For example, when I intentionally remember a gothic window I saw a while ago, the entire façade of the cathedral comes to mind at the same time.



SECT. X.

OF ASSOCIATE MOTIONS.

ASSOCIATE MOVEMENTS.

I. 1. Many muscular motions excited by irritations in trains or tribes become associated. 2. And many ideas. II. 1. Many sensitive muscular motions become associated. 2. And many sensitive ideas. III. 1. Many voluntary muscular motions become associated. 2. And then become obedient to sensation or irritation. 3. And many voluntary ideas become associated.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Many muscular movements triggered by stimuli in groups or communities become linked. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. And many concepts. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Many sensitive muscular movements become linked. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. And many sensitive concepts. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Many intentional muscular movements become linked. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. And then respond to sensation or stimulus. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. And many intentional concepts become linked.

All the fibrous motions, whether muscular or sensual, which are frequently brought into action together, either in combined tribes, or in successive trains, become so connected by habit, that when one of them is reproduced the others have a tendency to succeed or accompany it.

All the fibrous movements, whether they're muscular or sensual, which often occur together, either in groups or in sequences, become so linked through repetition that when one happens, the others tend to follow or go along with it.

I. 1. Many of our muscular motions were originally excited in successive trains, as the contractions of the auricles and of the ventricles of the heart; and others in combined tribes, as the various divisions of the muscles which compose the calf of the leg, which were originally irritated into synchronous action by the tædium or irksomeness of a continued posture. By frequent repetitions these motions acquire associations, which continue during our lives, and even after the destruction of the greatest part of the sensorium; for the heart of a viper or frog will continue to pulsate long after it is taken from the body; and when it has entirely ceased to move, if any part of it is goaded with a pin, the whole heart will again renew its pulsations. This kind of connection we shall term irritative association, to distinguish it from sensitive and voluntary associations.

I. 1. Many of our muscle movements were initially triggered in sequential patterns, like the contractions of the heart's atria and ventricles; others were activated in groups, such as the various muscle sections in the calf, which were originally prompted into synchronized action by the discomfort of maintaining a certain position. Through repeated actions, these movements develop associations that persist throughout our lives, even after most of the sensory system is gone. For instance, the heart of a viper or frog continues to beat long after it's removed from the body; and even when it has completely stopped, if any part of it is poked with a pin, the entire heart will start beating again. We’ll call this type of connection irritative association, to differentiate it from sensitive and voluntary associations.

2. In like manner many of our ideas are originally excited in tribes; as all the objects of sight, after we become so well acquainted with the laws of vision, as to distinguish figure and distance as well as colour; or in trains, as while we pass along the objects that surround us. The tribes thus received by irritation become associated by habit, and have been termed complex ideas by the writers of metaphysics, as this book, or that orange. The trains have received no particular name, but these are alike associations of ideas, and frequently continue during our lives. So the taste of a pine-apple, though we eat it blindfold, recalls the colour and shape of it; and we can scarcely think on solidity without figure.

2. Similarly, many of our ideas initially come from groups; just like how we recognize different aspects of sight—like shape, distance, and color—once we understand the rules of vision. Or in sequences, as we move past the things around us. The ideas we pick up through stimulation become linked through habit, and thinkers in philosophy have called these complex ideas, like this book or that orange. The sequences don’t have a specific name, but they also represent associations of ideas and often last throughout our lives. For example, the taste of a pineapple, even when we eat it blindfolded, brings to mind its color and shape; and we can hardly think of solidity without also considering shape.

II. 1. By the various efforts of our sensations to acquire or avoid their objects, many muscles are daily brought into successive or synchronous actions; these become associated by habit, and are then excited together with great facility, and in many instances gain indissoluble connections. So the play of puppies and kittens is a representation of their mode of fighting or of taking their prey; and the motions of the muscles necessary for those purposes become associated by habit, and gain a great adroitness of action by these early repetitions: so the motions of the abdominal muscles, which were originally brought into concurrent action, with the protrusive motion of the rectum or bladder by sensation, become so conjoined with them by habit, that they not only easily obey these sensations occasioned by the stimulus of the excrement and urine, but are brought into violent and unrestrainable action in the strangury and tenesmus. This kind of connection we shall term sensitive association.

II. 1. Through our ongoing attempts to seek out or avoid certain things, many muscles are used in a sequence or at the same time each day; over time, these actions become linked through habit, making it easier for us to engage them together, and in many cases, they form strong, lasting connections. For example, the way puppies and kittens play mimics how they fight or hunt; the muscle movements needed for these activities become linked through these early habits, making them very skilled in their actions. Similarly, the abdominal muscles, which initially worked together with the movements of the rectum or bladder due to sensations, become so intertwined with these functions through habit that they not only respond easily to the sensations triggered by waste and urine but also go into uncontrollable and forceful action during conditions like strangury and tenesmus. We'll refer to this type of connection as sensitive association.

2. So many of our ideas, that have been excited together or in succession by our sensations, gain synchronous or successive associations, that are sometimes indissoluble but with life. Hence the idea of an inhuman or dishonourable action perpetually calls up before us the idea of the wretch that was guilty of it. And hence those unconquerable antipathies are formed, which some people have to the sight of peculiar kinds of food, of which in their infancy they have eaten to excess or by constraint.

2. Many of our thoughts, sparked together or one after another by our experiences, develop connections that are sometimes inseparable from life itself. This is why the thought of a cruel or dishonorable act constantly brings to mind the person who committed it. This is also why some people develop strong aversions to certain types of food, which they were forced to eat or overindulged in during childhood.

III. 1. In learning any mechanic art, as music, dancing, or the use of the sword, we teach many of our muscles to act together or in succession by repeated voluntary efforts; which by habit become formed into tribes or trains of association, and serve all our purposes with great facility, and in some instances acquire an indissoluble union. These motions are gradually formed into a habit of acting together by a multitude of repetitions, whilst they are yet separately causable by the will, as is evident from the long time that is taken up by children in learning to walk and to speak; and is experienced by every one, when he first attempts to skate upon the ice or to swim: these we shall term voluntary associations.

III. 1. When learning any skill, like music, dancing, or swordsmanship, we train many of our muscles to work together or in sequence through repeated voluntary practice. This repetition creates habits that form groups or patterns of movement, allowing us to perform tasks with ease, and in some cases, these movements become closely linked. These actions gradually develop into a coordinated habit through many repetitions, even while they can still be triggered separately by our will, which is clear from how long it takes children to learn to walk and talk. Everyone experiences this when they first try skating on ice or swimming: we can call these voluntary associations.

2. All these muscular movements, when they are thus associated into tribes or trains, become afterwards not only obedient to volition, but to the sensations and irritations; and the same movement composes a part of many different tribes or trains of motion. Thus a single muscle, when it acts in consort with its neighbours on one side, assists to move the limb in one direction; and in another, when, it acts with those in its neighbourhood on the other side; and in other directions, when it acts separately or jointly with those that lie immediately under or above it; and all these with equal facility after their associations have been well established.

2. All these muscle movements, when grouped into clusters or chains, eventually become not just responsive to our will but also to sensations and triggers; and the same movement can be part of many different clusters or chains of motion. So, a single muscle, when it works together with those next to it on one side, helps move the limb in one direction; and when it works with those on the opposite side, it moves in another direction; and in other directions, when it operates either alone or together with those directly above or below it; and all of these happen just as easily once their connections have been well established.

The facility, with which each muscle changes from one associated tribe to another, and that either backwards or forwards, is well observable in the muscles of the arm in moving the windlass of an air-pump; and the slowness of those muscular movements, that have not been associated by habit, may be experienced by any one, who shall attempt to saw the air quick perpendicularly with one hand, and horizontally with the other at the same time.

The ability for each muscle to switch between related tasks, either forwards or backwards, is clearly seen in the arm muscles when operating the windlass of an air pump. Anyone can notice how slow these muscle movements are when they haven't been practiced together by trying to saw the air quickly—one hand moving up and down and the other going side to side at the same time.

3. In learning every kind of science we voluntarily associate many tribes and trains of ideas, which afterwards are ready for all the purposes either of volition, sensation, or irritation; and in some instances acquire indissoluble habits of acting together, so as to affect our reasoning, and influence our actions. Hence the necessity of a good education.

3. When we study any type of science, we intentionally connect various groups and sequences of ideas, which then become available for all sorts of actions, feelings, or reactions. In some cases, these connections develop into strong habits of working together, influencing our reasoning and shaping our actions. This is why a good education is essential.

These associate ideas are gradually formed into habits of acting together by frequent repetition, while they are yet separately obedient to the will; as is evident from the difficulty we experience in gaining so exact an idea of the front of St. Paul's church, as to be able to delineate it with accuracy, or in recollecting a poem of a few pages.

These related ideas slowly become habits of acting together through frequent repetition, while still separately following our intentions; as is clear from the challenge we face in getting such a precise image of the front of St. Paul's church that we can描绘it accurately, or in remembering a poem that’s just a few pages long.

And these ideas, thus associated into tribes, not only make up the parts of the trains of volition, sensation, and irritation; but the same idea composes a part of many different tribes and trains of ideas. So the simple idea of whiteness composes a part of the complex idea of snow, milk, ivory; and the complex idea of the letter A composes a part of the several associated trains of ideas that make up the variety of words, in which this letter enters.

And these concepts, grouped into categories, not only contribute to the streams of will, sensation, and irritation, but the same concept can be part of many different categories and streams of ideas. For example, the simple idea of whiteness is a part of the complex idea of snow, milk, and ivory; and the complex idea of the letter A is part of various related streams of ideas that form the different words in which this letter appears.

The numerous trains of these associated ideas are divided by Mr. Hume into three classes, which he has termed contiguity, causation, and resemblance. Nor should we wonder to find them thus connected together, since it is the business of our lives to dispose them into those three classes; and we become valuable to ourselves and our friends, as we succeed in it. Those who have combined an extensive class of ideas by the contiguity of time or place, are men learned in the history of mankind, and of the sciences they have cultivated. Those who have connected a great class of ideas of resemblances, possess the source of the ornaments of poetry and oratory, and of all rational analogy. While those who have connected great classes of ideas of causation, are furnished with the powers of producing effects. These are the men of active wisdom, who lead armies to victory, and kingdoms to prosperity; or discover and improve the sciences, which meliorate and adorn the condition of humanity.

The many streams of these related ideas are categorized by Mr. Hume into three groups, which he calls contiguity, causation, and resemblance. We shouldn’t be surprised to see them linked this way, as it’s our task in life to organize them into these three categories; we become valuable to ourselves and our friends by doing so successfully. Those who have brought together a wide range of ideas based on the proximity of time or place are knowledgeable about human history and the sciences they study. Those who have linked a wide range of similar ideas hold the key to the beauty of poetry and speech, as well as all logical comparisons. Meanwhile, those who have connected significant categories of causal ideas are equipped with the ability to create outcomes. These are the people of practical wisdom who lead armies to victory and nations to prosperity, or who discover and enhance the sciences that improve and enrich the human condition.



SECT. XI.

ADDITIONAL OBSERVATIONS ON THE SENSORIAL POWERS.

ADDITIONAL OBSERVATIONS ON THE SENSORY POWERS.

I. Stimulation is of various kinds adapted to the organs of sense, to the muscles, to hollow membranes, and glands. Some objects irritate our senses by repeated impulses. II. 1. Sensation and volition frequently affect the whole sensorium. 2. Emotions, passions, appetites. 3. Origin of desire and aversion. Criterion of voluntary actions, difference of brutes and men. 4. Sensibility and voluntarity. III. Associations formed before nativity, irritative motions mistaken for officiated ones.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. There are various types of stimulation that are suited to our senses, muscles, hollow membranes, and glands. Some objects provoke our senses through repeated exposure. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Both sensation and will often influence the entire sensory system. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Emotions, passions, and desires. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. The origin of likes and dislikes. The basis of voluntary actions and the difference between animals and humans. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Sensitivity and voluntary action. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Connections formed prior to birth, reactive movements mistaken for intentional ones.

Irritation.

Irritation.

I. The various organs of sense require various kinds of stimulation to excite them into action; the particles of light penetrate the cornea and humours of the eye, and then irritate the naked retina; rapid particles, dissolved or diffused in water or saliva, and odorous ones, mixed or combined with the air, irritate the extremities of the nerves of taste and smell; which either penetrate, or are expanded on the membranes of the tongue and nostrils; the auditory nerves are stimulated by the vibrations of the atmosphere communicated by means of the tympanum and of the fluid, whether of air or of water, behind it; and the nerves of touch by the hardness of surrounding bodies, though the cuticle is interposed between these bodies and the medulla of the nerve.

I. The different sensory organs need different types of stimulation to activate them; light particles pass through the cornea and the fluids of the eye, then irritate the exposed retina; fast particles dissolved in water or saliva, and those that are mixed with air, stimulate the nerve endings for taste and smell, which either penetrate or spread out on the membranes of the tongue and nostrils; the auditory nerves are triggered by the vibrations in the air transmitted through the eardrum and the fluid behind it, whether air or water; and the touch nerves respond to the hardness of objects around them, even though the skin is between these objects and the nerves.

As the nerves of the senses have each their appropriated objects, which stimulate them into activity; so the muscular fibres, which are the terminations of other sets of nerves, have their peculiar objects, which excite them into action; the longitudinal muscles are stimulated into contraction by extension, whence the stretching or pandiculation after a long continued posture, during which they have been kept in a state of extension; and the hollow muscles are excited into action by distention, as those of the rectum and bladder are induced to protrude their contents from their sense of the distention rather than of the acrimony of those contents.

As the sensory nerves each have specific objects that activate them, the muscle fibers, which are the endpoints of other nerves, also respond to particular stimuli that trigger their movement. The long muscles contract when they are stretched, which is why we often stretch or yawn after being in the same position for a long time. Similarly, the hollow muscles, like those in the rectum and bladder, are prompted to push out their contents due to the feeling of being full rather than the irritation of what they contain.

There are other objects adapted to stimulate the nerves, which terminate in variety of membranes, and those especially which form the terminations of canals; thus the preparations of mercury particularly affect the salivary glands, ipecacuanha the stomach, aloe the sphincter of the anus, cantharides that of the bladder, and lastly every gland of the body appears to be indued with a kind of taste, by which it selects or forms each its peculiar fluid from the blood; and by which it is irritated into activity.

There are other objects designed to stimulate the nerves, which end in various membranes, especially those that form the endings of canals. For instance, mercury preparations particularly affect the salivary glands, ipecacuanha impacts the stomach, aloe affects the sphincter of the anus, and cantharides influence the bladder. Additionally, every gland in the body seems to have a sort of taste, allowing it to choose or create its specific fluid from the blood, and this taste also triggers its activity.

Many of these external properties of bodies, which stimulate our organs of sense, do not seem to effect this by a single impulse, but by repeated impulses; as the nerve of the ear is probably not excitable by a single vibration of air, nor the optic nerve by a single particle of light; which circumstance produces some analogy between those two senses, at the same time the solidity of bodies is perceived by a single application of a solid body to the nerves of touch, and that even through the cuticle; and we are probably possessed of a peculiar sense to distinguish the nice degrees of heat and cold.

Many of these external characteristics of objects, which stimulate our senses, don’t seem to affect us with just one impulse, but rather with repeated impulses. For example, the ear nerve likely isn’t triggered by a single vibration of air, nor is the optic nerve by a single particle of light. This creates some similarity between the two senses. In contrast, we can perceive the solidity of objects through a single contact of a solid object with our touch nerves, even through the skin. Additionally, we probably have a specific sense to recognize subtle differences in temperature.

The senses of touch and of hearing acquaint us with the mechanical impact and vibration of bodies, those of smell and taste seem to acquaint us with some of their chemical properties, while the sense of vision and of heat acquaint us with the existence of their peculiar fluids.

The senses of touch and hearing let us feel the mechanical impact and vibrations of objects, while smell and taste help us understand some of their chemical properties. Vision and the sense of heat inform us about the presence of their unique fluids.

Sensation and Volition.

Feeling and Choice.

II. Many motions are produced by pleasure or pain, and that even in contradiction to the power of volition, as in laughing, or in the strangury; but as no name has been given to pleasure or pain, at the time it is exerted so as to cause fibrous motions, we have used the term sensation for this purpose; and mean it to bear the same analogy to pleasure and pain, that the word volition does to desire and aversion.

II. Many movements happen because of pleasure or pain, even against our will, like in laughing or when experiencing a painful condition. Since there isn't a specific name for pleasure or pain when they trigger bodily movements, we've decided to use the term sensation for this reason. We intend for it to have a similar relationship to pleasure and pain as the word volition does to desire and aversion.

1. It was mentioned in the fifth Section, that, what we have termed sensation is a motion of the central parts, or of the whole sensorium, beginning at some of the extremities of it. This appears first, because our pains and pleasures are always caused by our ideas or muscular motions, which are the motions of the extremities of the sensorium. And, secondly, because the sensation of pleasure or pain frequently continues some time after the ideas or muscular motions which excited it have ceased: for we often feel a glow of pleasure from an agreeable reverie, for many minutes after the ideas, that were the subject of it, have escaped our memory; and frequently experience a dejection of spirits without being able to assign the cause of it but by much recollection.

1. It was mentioned in the fifth Section, that what we call sensation is a movement of the central parts, or of the entire sensory system, starting at some of its outer areas. This is evident for two reasons: first, because our feelings of pain and pleasure are always triggered by our thoughts or muscle movements, which originate from the outer parts of the sensory system. Secondly, because the feeling of pleasure or pain often lingers for a while after the thoughts or muscle movements that caused it have ended. For instance, we can feel a sense of pleasure from a pleasant daydream for several minutes after the thoughts that inspired it have faded from our minds, and we frequently find ourselves feeling down without being able to identify the reason for it without considerable reflection.

When the sensorial faculty of desire or aversion is exerted so as to cause fibrous motions, it is termed volition; which is said in Sect. V. to be a motion of the central parts, or of the whole sensorium, terminating in some of the extremities of it. This appears, first, because our desires and aversions always terminate in recollecting and comparing our ideas, or in exerting our muscles; which are the motions of the extremities of the sensorium. And, secondly, because desire or aversion begins, and frequently continues for a time in the central parts of the sensorium, before it is peculiarly exerted at the extremities of it; for we sometimes feel desire or aversion without immediately knowing their objects, and in consequence without immediately exerting any of our muscular or sensual motions to attain them: as in the beginning of the passion of love, and perhaps of hunger, or in the ennui of indolent people.

When the sensorial faculty of desire or aversion is exerted so as to cause fibrous motions, it is termed volition; which is said in Sect. V. to be a motion of the central parts, or of the whole sensorium, terminating in some of the extremities of it. This appears, first, because our desires and aversions always terminate in recollecting and comparing our ideas, or in exerting our muscles; which are the motions of the extremities of the sensorium. And, secondly, because desire or aversion begins, and frequently continues for a time in the central parts of the sensorium, before it is peculiarly exerted at the extremities of it; for we sometimes feel desire or aversion without immediately knowing their objects, and in consequence without immediately exerting any of our muscular or sensual motions to attain them: as in the beginning of the passion of love, and perhaps of hunger, or in the ennui of indolent people.

Though sensation and volition begin or terminate at the extremities or central parts of the sensorium, yet the whole of it is frequently influenced by the exertion of these faculties, as appears from their effects on the external habit: for the whole skin is reddened by shame, and an universal trembling is produced by fear: and every muscle of the body is agitated in angry people by the desire of revenge.

Though feeling and will start or end at the edges or central areas of the brain, the entire system is often affected by these abilities, as shown by their impact on outward behavior: the entire skin flushes with shame, and a general trembling occurs with fear; every muscle in angry people is stirred by the urge for revenge.

There is another very curious circumstance, which shews that sensation and volition are movements of the sensorium in contrary directions; that is, that volition begins at the central parts of it, and proceeds to the extremities; and that sensation begins at the extremities, and proceeds to the central parts: I mean that these two sensorial faculties cannot be strongly exerted at the same time; for when we exert our volition strongly, we do not attend to pleasure or pain; and conversely, when we are strongly affected with the sensation of pleasure or pain, we use no volition. As will be further explained in Section XVIII. on sleep, and Section XXXIV. on volition.

There is another very curious circumstance, which shews that sensation and volition are movements of the sensorium in contrary directions; that is, that volition begins at the central parts of it, and proceeds to the extremities; and that sensation begins at the extremities, and proceeds to the central parts: I mean that these two sensorial faculties cannot be strongly exerted at the same time; for when we exert our volition strongly, we do not attend to pleasure or pain; and conversely, when we are strongly affected with the sensation of pleasure or pain, we use no volition. As will be further explained in Section XVIII. on sleep, and Section XXXIV. on volition.

2. All our emotions and passions seem to arise out of the exertions of these two faculties of the animal sensorium. Pride, hope, joy, are the names of particular pleasures: shame, despair, sorrow, are the names of peculiar pains: and love, ambition, avarice, of particular desires: hatred, disgust, fear, anxiety, of particular aversions. Whilst the passion of anger includes the pain from a recent injury, and the aversion to the adversary that occasioned it. And compassion is the pain we experience at the sight of misery, and the desire of relieving it.

2. All our feelings and desires seem to come from the efforts of these two parts of our animal senses. Pride, hope, and joy are names for certain pleasures; shame, despair, and sorrow are names for specific pains; and love, ambition, and greed are names for certain desires. Hatred, disgust, fear, and anxiety are names for specific aversions. Anger includes the pain from a recent injury and the dislike for the person who caused it. Compassion is the pain we feel when we see suffering and the urge to help.

There is another tribe of desires, which are commonly termed appetites, and are the immediate consequences of the absence of some irritative motions. Those, which arise from defect of internal irritations, have proper names conferred upon them, as hunger, thirst, lust, and the desire of air, when our respiration is impaired by noxious vapours; and of warmth, when we are exposed to too great a degree of cold. But those, whose stimuli are external to the body, are named from the objects, which are by nature constituted to excite them; these desires originate from our past experience of the pleasurable sensations they occasion, as the smell of an hyacinth, or the taste of a pine-apple.

There’s another group of desires, typically referred to as appetites, which are direct results of the lack of certain stimuli. Those that come from the absence of internal urges have specific names, such as hunger, thirst, sexual desire, and the need for air when our breathing is affected by harmful fumes; and the need for warmth when we’re exposed to extreme cold. However, those whose triggers come from outside the body are named after the objects that naturally provoke them; these desires stem from our past experiences with the pleasurable feelings they produce, like the scent of a hyacinth or the taste of a pineapple.

Whence it appears, that our pleasures and pains are at least as various and as numerous as our irritations; and that our desires and aversions must be as numerous as our pleasures and pains. And that as sensation is here used as a general term for our numerous pleasures and pains, when they produce the contractions of our fibres; so volition is the general name for our desires and aversions, when they produce fibrous contractions. Thus when a motion of the central parts, or of the whole sensorium, terminates in the exertion of our muscles, it is generally called voluntary action; when it terminates in the exertion of our ideas, it is termed recollection, reasoning, determining.

It seems that our pleasures and pains are at least as diverse and plentiful as our irritations, and our desires and dislikes must be as numerous as our pleasures and pains. Sensation is used here as a broad term for our many pleasures and pains when they cause contractions in our fibers; similarly, volition is the general term for our desires and dislikes when they lead to those contractions. So, when a movement of the central parts, or the entire sensory system, results in the use of our muscles, it’s usually called voluntary action; when it results in the use of our thoughts, it’s referred to as recollection, reasoning, or decision-making.

3. As the sensations of pleasure and pain are originally introduced by the irritations of external objects: so our desires and aversions are originally introduced by those sensations; for when the objects of our pleasures or pains are at a distance, and we cannot instantaneously possess the one, or avoid the other, then desire or aversion is produced, and a voluntary exertion of our ideas or muscles succeeds.

3. Just as our experiences of pleasure and pain come from the irritation of outside objects, our wants and dislikes also start with those sensations. When the things that bring us pleasure or pain are far away, and we can’t instantly enjoy one or get away from the other, that’s when desire or aversion kicks in, leading to a conscious effort with our thoughts or actions.

The pain of hunger excites you to look out for food, the tree, that shades you, presents its odoriferous fruit before your eyes, you approach, pluck, and eat.

The hunger pain drives you to search for food; the tree that provides your shade shows off its fragrant fruit right in front of you. You go closer, pick it, and eat.

The various movements of walking to the tree, gathering the fruit, and masticating it, are associated motions introduced by their connection with sensation; but if from the uncommon height of the tree, the fruit be inaccessible, and you are prevented from quickly possessing the intended pleasure, desire is produced. The consequence of this desire is, first, a deliberation about the means to gain the object of pleasure in process of time, as it cannot be procured immediately; and, secondly, the muscular action necessary for this purpose.

The different actions of walking to the tree, picking the fruit, and chewing it are all linked motions related to our senses. However, if the tree is too tall and the fruit is out of reach, you can’t quickly enjoy what you want, leading to desire. This desire causes two things: first, you think about how to eventually obtain the fruit since you can't get it right away; and second, you begin to engage in the physical effort needed to achieve this goal.

You voluntarily call up all your ideas of causation, that are related to the effect you desire, and voluntarily examine and compare them, and at length determine whether to ascend the tree, or to gather stones from the neighbouring brook, is easier to practise, or more promising of success; and, finally, you gather the stones, and repeatedly fling them to dislodge the fruit.

You consciously bring to mind all your thoughts about what causes the effect you want, and you look at them closely and compare them. In the end, you decide whether it's easier to climb the tree or collect stones from the nearby stream, which seems more likely to work. Ultimately, you choose to gather the stones and throw them repeatedly to knock the fruit down.

Hence then we gain a criterion to distinguish voluntary acts or thoughts from those caused by sensation. As the former are always employed about the means to acquire pleasurable objects, or the means to avoid painful ones; while the latter are employed in the possession of those, which are already in our power.

Hence, we now have a way to distinguish between voluntary actions or thoughts and those triggered by sensation. The former always focus on the means to gain enjoyable things or the means to avoid painful ones, while the latter are about what we already have.

Hence the activity of this power of volition produces the great difference between the human and the brute creation. The ideas and the actions of brutes are almost perpetually employed about their present pleasures, or their present pains; and, except in the few instances which are mentioned in Section XVI, on instinct, they seldom busy themselves about the means of procuring future bliss, or of avoiding future misery; so that the acquiring of languages, the making of tools, and labouring for money, which are all only the means to procure pleasures; and the praying to the Deity, as another means to procure happiness, are characteristic of human nature.

Hence the activity of this power of volition produces the great difference between the human and the brute creation. The ideas and the actions of brutes are almost perpetually employed about their present pleasures, or their present pains; and, except in the few instances which are mentioned in Section XVI, on instinct, they seldom busy themselves about the means of procuring future bliss, or of avoiding future misery; so that the acquiring of languages, the making of tools, and labouring for money, which are all only the means to procure pleasures; and the praying to the Deity, as another means to procure happiness, are characteristic of human nature.

4. As there are many diseases produced by the quantity of the sensation of pain or pleasure being too great or too little; so are there diseases produced by the susceptibility of the constitution to motions causable by these sensations being too dull or too vivid. This susceptibility of the system to sensitive motions is termed sensibility, to distinguish it from sensation, which is the actual existence or exertion of pain or pleasure.

4. Just as many illnesses can arise from feeling too much or too little pain or pleasure, there are also illnesses caused by how sensitive the body is to movements triggered by these sensations being too subdued or too intense. This sensitivity of the system to emotional movements is called sensibility, to differentiate it from sensation, which is the direct experience of pain or pleasure.

Other classes of diseases are owing to the excessive promptitude, or sluggishness of the constitution to voluntary exertions, as well as to the quantity of desire or of aversion. This susceptibility of the system to voluntary motions is termed voluntarity, to distinguish it from volition, which is the exertion of desire or aversion; these diseases will be treated of at length in the progress of the work.

Other types of diseases are due to either the excessive readiness or the sluggishness of the body to engage in voluntary actions, as well as the amount of desire or aversion involved. This sensitivity of the body to voluntary movements is called voluntarity, to differentiate it from volition, which is the act of feeling desire or aversion. These diseases will be discussed in detail as the work progresses.

Association.

Group.

III. 1. It is not easy to assign a cause, why those animal movements, that have once occurred in succession, or in combination, should afterwards have a tendency to succeed or accompany each other. It is a property of animation, and distinguishes this order of being from the other productions of nature.

III. 1. It's not simple to pinpoint why certain animal behaviors, which have happened one after the other or together, tend to follow or accompany each other later on. This is a characteristic of living things and sets this category of existence apart from other creations in nature.

When a child first wrote the word man, it was distinguished in his mind into three letters, and those letters into many parts of letters; but by repeated use the word man becomes to his hand in writing it, as to his organs of speech in pronouncing it, but one movement without any deliberation, or sensation, or irritation, interposed between the parts of it. And as many separate motions of our muscles thus become united, and form, as it were, one motion; so each separate motion before such union may be conceived to consist of many parts or spaces moved through; and perhaps even the individual fibres of our muscles have thus gradually been brought to act in concert, which habits began to be acquired as early as the very formation of the moving organs, long before the nativity of the animal; as explained in the Section XVI. 2. on instinct.

When a child first wrote the word man, it was distinguished in his mind into three letters, and those letters into many parts of letters; but by repeated use the word man becomes to his hand in writing it, as to his organs of speech in pronouncing it, but one movement without any deliberation, or sensation, or irritation, interposed between the parts of it. And as many separate motions of our muscles thus become united, and form, as it were, one motion; so each separate motion before such union may be conceived to consist of many parts or spaces moved through; and perhaps even the individual fibres of our muscles have thus gradually been brought to act in concert, which habits began to be acquired as early as the very formation of the moving organs, long before the nativity of the animal; as explained in the Section XVI. 2. on instinct.

2. There are many motions of the body, belonging to the irritative class, which might by a hasty observer be mistaken for associated ones; as the peristaltic motion of the stomach and intestines, and the contractions of the heart and arteries, might be supposed to be associated with the irritative motions of their nerves of sense, rather than to be excited by the irritation of their muscular fibres by the distention, acrimony, or momentum of the blood. So the distention or elongation of muscles by objects external to them irritates them into contraction, though the cuticle or other parts may intervene between the stimulating body and the contracting muscle. Thus a horse voids his excrement when its weight or bulk irritates the rectum or sphincter ani. These muscles act from the irritation of distention, when he excludes his excrement, but the muscles of the abdomen and diaphragm are brought into motion by association with those of the sphincter and rectum.

2. There are many movements of the body that belong to the irritative category, which a quick observer might confuse with associated movements; for example, the peristaltic motion of the stomach and intestines, as well as the contractions of the heart and arteries, might be thought to be linked to the irritative movements of their sensory nerves, rather than being triggered by irritation of their muscle fibers caused by stretching, acidity, or the force of the blood. Similarly, the stretching or elongation of muscles by external objects irritates them into contracting, even if the skin or other parts are between the stimulating object and the contracting muscle. For instance, a horse excretes feces when its weight or bulk irritates the rectum or anal sphincter. These muscles respond to the irritation from the stretching when the horse expels its feces, but the muscles of the abdomen and diaphragm become active through association with those of the sphincter and rectum.



SECT. XII.

OF STIMULUS, SENSORIAL EXERTION, AND FIBROUS CONTRACTION.

OF STIMULUS, SENSORIAL EXERTION, AND FIBROUS CONTRACTION.

I. Of fibrous contraction. 1. Two particles of a fibre cannot approach without the intervention of something, as in magnetism, electricity, elasticity. Spirit of life is not electric ether. Galvani's experiments. 2. Contraction of a fibre. 3. Relaxation succeeds. 4. Successive contractions, with intervals. Quick pulse from debility, from paucity of blood. Weak contractions performed in less time, and with shorter intervals. 5. Last situation of the fibres continues after contraction. 6. Contraction greater than usual induces pleasure or pain. 7. Mobility of the fibres uniform. Quantity of sensorial power fluctuates. Constitutes excitability. II. Of sensorial exertion. 1. Animal motion includes stimulus, sensorial power, and contractile fibres. The sensorial faculties act separately or conjointly. Stimulus of four kinds. Strength and weakness defined. Sensorial power perpetually exhausted and renewed. Weakness from defect of stimulus. From defect of sensorial power, the direct and indirect debility of Dr. Brown. Why we become warm in Buxton bath after a time, and see well after a time in a darkish room. Fibres may act violently, or with their whole force, and yet feebly. Great exertion in inflammation explained. Great muscular force of some insane people. 2. Occasional accumulation of sensorial power in muscles subject to constant stimulus. In animals sleeping in winter. In eggs, seeds, schirrous tumours, tendons, bones. 3. Great exertion introduces pleasure or pain. Inflammation. Libration of the system between torpor and activity. Fever-fits. 4. Desire and aversion introduced. Excess of volition cures fevers. III. Of repeated stimulus. 1. A stimulus repeated too frequently looses effect. As opium, wine, grief. Hence old age. Opium and aloes in small doses. 2. A stimulus not repeated too frequently does not lose effect. Perpetual movement of the vital organs. 3. A stimulus repeated at uniform times produces greater effect. Irritation combined with association. 4. A stimulus repeated frequently and uniformly may be withdrawn, and the action of the organ will continue. Hence the bark cures agues, and strengthens weak constitutions. 5. Defect of stimulus repeated at certain intervals causes fever-fits. 6. Stimulus long applied ceases to act a second time. 7. If a stimulus excites sensation in an organ not usually excited into sensation, inflammation is produced. IV. Of stimulus greater than natural. 1. A stimulus greater than natural diminishes the quantity of sensorial power in general. 2. In particular organs. 3. Induces the organ into spasmodic actions. 4. Induces the antagonist fibres into action. 5. Induces the organ into convulsive or fixed spasms. 6. Produces paralysis of the organ. V. Of stimulus less than natural. 1. Stimulus less than natural occasions accumulation of sensorial power in general. 2. In particular organs, flushing of the face in a frosty morning. In fibres subject to perpetual stimulus only. Quantity of sensorial power inversely as the stimulus. 3. Induces pain. As of cold, hunger, head-ach. 4. Induces more feeble and frequent contraction. As in low fevers. Which are frequently owing to deficiency of sensorial power rather than to deficiency of stimulus. 5. Inverts successive trains of motion. Inverts ideas. 6. Induces paralysis and death. VI. Cure of increased exertion. 1. Natural cure of exhaustion of sensorial power. 2. Decrease the irritations. Venesection. Cold. Abstinence. 3. Prevent the previous cold fit. Opium. Bark. Warmth. Anger. Surprise. 4. Excite some other part of the system. Opium and warm bath relieve pains both from defect and from excess of stimulus. 5. First increase the stimulus above, and then decrease it beneath the natural quantity. VII. Cure of decreased exertion. 1. Natural cure by accumulation of sensorial power. Ague-fits. Syncope. 2. Increase the stimulation, by wine, opium, given so as not to intoxicate. Cheerful ideas. 3. Change the kinds of stimulus. 4. Stimulate the associated organs. Blisters of use in heart-burn, and cold extremities. 5. Decrease the stimulation for a time, cold bath. 6. Decrease the stimulation below natural, and then increase it above natural. Bark after emetics. Opium after venesection. Practice of Sydenham in chlorosis. 7. Prevent unnecessary expenditure of sensorial power. Decumbent posture, silence, darkness. Pulse quickened by rising out of bed. 8. To the greatest degree of quiescence apply the least stimulus. Otherwise paralysis or inflammation of the organ ensues. Gin, wine, blisters, destroy by too great stimulation in fevers with debility. Intoxication in the slightest degree succeeded by debility. Golden rule for determining the best degree of stimulus in low fevers. Another golden rule for determining the quantity of spirit which those, who are debilitated by drinking it, may safely omit.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Regarding fibrous contraction. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Two fiber particles can’t get close without something intervening, like magnetism, electricity, or elasticity. The spirit of life isn’t the same as electric ether. See Galvani's experiments. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Contraction of a fiber. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Relaxation follows. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Repeated contractions occur with intervals. A rapid pulse can come from weakness or low blood volume. Weak contractions happen faster and with shorter breaks. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. The fibers' last state remains after contraction. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. More contraction than usual can cause either pleasure or pain. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. The fibers' mobility is consistent. The amount of sensorial power varies. This creates excitability. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. About sensorial exertion. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Animal motion includes stimulus, sensorial power, and contractile fibers. The sensorial faculties may operate separately or together. There are four types of stimulus. Strength and weakness are defined. Sensorial power is continuously depleted and renewed. Weakness comes from a lack of stimulus. A deficiency in sensorial power causes the direct and indirect weakness described by Dr. Brown. This explains why we warm up in a Buxton bath over time and start seeing well in a dim room after a while. Fibers can act intensely but still feel weak. Excessive effort in inflammation is clarified. Some insane people can show great muscle strength. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. There can be occasional buildup of sensorial power in muscles exposed to constant stimulus. This can be seen in animals hibernating in winter or in eggs, seeds, tumor-like masses, tendons, and bones. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. Significant exertion results in pleasure or pain. This includes inflammation. The body fluctuates between inactivity and activity, leading to fever symptoms. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. Feelings of desire and aversion arise. Excessive willpower can help cure fevers. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. About repeated stimulus. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. A stimulus that is repeated too often loses its effect. This is like opium, wine, or grief. This is a factor in aging. Small doses of opium and aloes. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. A stimulus that isn’t repeated too often retains its effectiveness. This ensures the continual movement of vital organs. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. A stimulus repeated at regular intervals has a greater effect. Irritation combined with association. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. A stimulus repeated frequently and uniformly may be removed, and the organ will keep functioning. Therefore, quinine can cure fevers and strengthen weak bodies. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_18__. A lack of stimulus repeated at specific intervals causes fever symptoms. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_19__. A stimulus that is applied for too long loses its effect the next time. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_20__. If a stimulus ignites sensation in an organ that normally doesn’t feel sensation, it can cause inflammation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_21__. About stimulus greater than normal. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_22__. A stimulus that is stronger than normal reduces the overall amount of sensorial power. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_23__. In specific organs. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_24__. This can cause chaotic actions in the organ. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_25__. This activates opposing fibers. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_26__. This can cause the organ to undergo convulsive or fixed spasms. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_27__. This can lead to paralysis of the organ. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_28__. About stimulus less than normal. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_29__. A stimulus that is less than normal leads to a buildup of sensorial power overall. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_30__. In specific organs, such as flushing in the face on a cold morning. In fibers exposed to constant stimulus only. The amount of sensorial power inversely relates to the stimulus. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_31__. This causes pain, like from cold, hunger, or headaches. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_32__. This leads to weaker and more frequent contractions, as seen in low fevers. These often stem from a lack of sensorial power rather than a lack of stimulus. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_33__. This can reverse chains of motion. This can also reverse thoughts. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_34__. This can cause paralysis and death. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_35__. A solution for increased exertion. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_36__. A natural remedy for the exhaustion of sensorial power. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_37__. Reduce irritations. Bloodletting. Cold. Abstaining. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_38__. Prevent the prior cold symptoms. Opium. Quinine. Warmth. Anger. Surprise. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_39__. Stimulate another part of the body. Opium and a warm bath can alleviate pain from both a lack and an excess of stimulus. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_40__. First increase the stimulus above normal, then decrease it below the standard amount. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_41__. Solutions for decreased exertion. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_42__. A natural remedy through the buildup of sensorial power. Fever symptoms. Fainting. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_43__. Enhance stimulation with wine or opium, given in amounts that won't cause intoxication. Positive thoughts. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_44__. Change up the types of stimulus. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_45__. Stimulate related organs. Blisters can be helpful for heartburn and cold extremities. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_46__. Temporarily reduce stimulation with a cold bath. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_47__. Lower the stimulation below normal, then increase it above normal. Use quinine after emetics. Opium after bloodletting. Follow Sydenham’s practice in chlorosis. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_48__. Prevent unnecessary depletion of sensorial power. Resting, silence, and darkness. Pulse increases when getting out of bed. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_49__. Apply the least stimulation to achieve the highest state of rest. Otherwise, inflammation or paralysis of the organ can occur. Alcohol, wine, and blisters can cause too much stimulation in fevers with weakness. Even mild intoxication can lead to weakness. A key guideline for determining the optimal amount of stimulus in low fevers. Another important guideline for identifying how much alcohol someone, who has been weakened by its consumption, can safely skip.

I. Of fibrous contraction.

I. Of fibrous contraction.

1. If two particles of iron lie near each other without motion, and afterwards approach each other; it is reasonable to conclude that something besides the iron particles is the cause of their approximation; this invisible something is termed magnetism. In the same manner, if the particles, which compose an animal muscle, do not touch each other in the relaxed state of the muscle, and are brought into contact during the contraction of the muscle, it is reasonable to conclude, that some other agent is the cause of this new approximation. For nothing can act, where it does not exist; for to act includes to exist; and therefore the particles of the muscular fibre (which in its state of relaxation are supposed not to touch) cannot affect each other without the influence of some intermediate agent; this agent is here termed the spirit of animation, or sensorial power, but may with equal propriety be termed the power, which causes contraction; or may be called by any other name, which the reader may choose to affix to it.

1. If two iron particles are sitting next to each other without moving, and then they move closer, it's reasonable to think that something other than the iron is causing them to come together; this unseen force is called magnetism. Similarly, if the particles that make up a muscle are not touching when the muscle is relaxed but come into contact when the muscle contracts, it makes sense to believe that some other factor is responsible for this new closeness. Nothing can act where it doesn’t exist, as to act means to exist; therefore, the particles of muscle fiber (which are thought not to touch in a relaxed state) can't influence each other without the help of some intermediary force. This force is referred to as the spirit of animation or sensorial power, but it can just as easily be called the force that causes contraction, or any other name the reader wishes to use.

The contraction of a muscular fibre may be compared to the following electric experiment, which is here mentioned not as a philosophical analogy, but as an illustration or simile to facilitate the conception of a difficult subject. Let twenty very small Leyden phials properly coated be hung in a row by fine silk threads at a small distance from each other; let the internal charge of one phial be positive, and of the other negative alternately, if a communication be made from the internal surface of the first to the external surface of the last in the row, they will all of them instantly approach each other, and thus shorten a line that might connect them like a muscular fibre. See Botanic Garden, p. 1. Canto I. 1. 202, note on Gymnotus.

The contraction of a muscle fiber can be compared to the following electric experiment, mentioned here not as a philosophical analogy, but as an illustration to help understand a complex topic. Imagine twenty very small Leyden jars, properly coated, hanging in a row by fine silk threads, spaced slightly apart. If the internal charge of one jar is positive and the next one is negative alternately, then if a connection is made from the inner surface of the first jar to the outer surface of the last in the row, they will all instantly pull towards each other, effectively shortening the line that connects them, similar to how a muscle fiber works. See Botanic Garden, p. 1. Canto I. 1. 202, note on Gymnotus.

The attractions of electricity or of magnetism do not apply philosophically to the illustration of the contraction of animal fibres, since the force of those attractions increases in some proportion inversely as the distance, but in muscular motion there appears no difference in velocity or strength during the beginning or end of the contraction, but what may be clearly ascribed to the varying mechanic advantage in the approximation of one bone to another. Nor can muscular motion be assimilated with greater plausibility to the attraction of cohesion or elasticity; for in bending a steel spring, as a small sword, a less force is required to bend it the first inch than the second; and the second than the third; the particles of steel on the convex side of the bent spring endeavouring to restore themselves more powerfully the further they are drawn from each other. See Botanic Garden, P. I. addit. Note XVIII.

The attractions of electricity or magnetism don't really apply when discussing the contraction of animal fibers. This is because the strength of those attractions increases inversely with distance. However, with muscle movement, there’s no noticeable difference in speed or strength at the beginning or end of the contraction; any variation can be clearly linked to the mechanical advantage of one bone getting closer to another. Additionally, muscle movement can’t be compared to the attraction of cohesion or elasticity in a convincing way. For example, when bending a steel spring, like a small sword, it takes less force to bend it the first inch than it does for the second inch, and less force for the second than for the third. The particles of steel on the outside of the bent spring try to return to their original positions more forcefully the further apart they are pulled. See Botanic Garden, P. I. addit. Note XVIII.

I am aware that this may be explained another way, by supposing the elasticity of the spring to depend more on the compression of the particles on the concave side than on the extension of them on the convex side; and by supposing the elasticity of the elastic gum to depend more on the resistance to the lateral compression of its particles than to the longitudinal extension of them. Nevertheless in muscular contraction, as above observed, there appears no difference in the velocity or force of it at its commencement or at its termination; from whence we must conclude that animal contraction is governed by laws of its own, and not by those of mechanics, chemistry, magnetism, or electricity.

I understand that this could be explained differently by suggesting that the elasticity of the spring relies more on the compression of the particles on the inward curve than on the stretching of them on the outward curve. Similarly, the elasticity of the elastic gum might be attributed more to the resistance against the sideways compression of its particles than to their lengthwise stretching. However, in muscular contraction, as mentioned earlier, there seems to be no difference in the speed or strength at the beginning or end; therefore, we must conclude that animal contraction follows its own set of laws, distinct from those of mechanics, chemistry, magnetism, or electricity.

On these accounts I do not think the experiments conclusive, which were lately published by Galvani, Volta, and others, to shew a similitude between the spirit of animation, which contracts the muscular fibres, and the electric fluid. Since the electric fluid may act only as a more potent stimulus exciting the muscular fibres into action, and not by supplying them with a new quantity of the spirit of life. Thus in a recent hemiplegia I have frequently observed, when the patient yawned and stretched himself, that the paralytic limbs moved also, though they were totally disobedient to the will. And when he was electrified by passing shocks from the affected hand to the affected foot, a motion of the paralytic limbs was also produced. Now as in the act of yawning the muscles of the paralytic limbs were excited into action by the stimulus of the irksomeness of a continued posture, and not by any additional quantity of the spirit of life; so we may conclude, that the passage of the electric fluid, which produced a similar effect, acted only as a stimulus, and not by supplying any addition of sensorial power.

For these reasons, I don’t believe the experiments recently published by Galvani, Volta, and others are conclusive in showing a similarity between the vital spirit that contracts muscle fibers and the electric fluid. The electric fluid might simply serve as a stronger stimulus that excites the muscle fibers into action, rather than providing them with a new supply of life force. In a recent case of hemiplegia, I often noticed that when the patient yawned and stretched, the paralyzed limbs also moved, even though they completely ignored the will. Additionally, when he received electric shocks from the affected hand to the affected foot, it also caused movement in the paralyzed limbs. Just as the act of yawning excited the muscles of the paralyzed limbs into action due to the discomfort of staying in one position, rather than from any extra life force, we can conclude that the passage of the electric fluid, which produced a similar effect, acted merely as a stimulus and didn’t provide any additional sensory power.

If nevertheless this theory should ever become established, a stimulus must be called an eductor of vital ether; which stimulus may consist of sensation or volition, as in the electric eel, as well as in the appulses of external bodies; and by drawing off the charges of vital fluid may occasion the contraction or motions of the muscular fibres, and organs of sense.

If this theory ever gets accepted, a stimulus needs to be referred to as a provider of vital energy; this stimulus could be based on sensation or will, like in the electric eel, as well as from the impacts of external objects; and by draining the charges of vital fluid, it might cause the contraction or movement of muscle fibers and sense organs.

2. The immediate effect of the action of the spirit of animation or sensorial power on the fibrous parts of the body, whether it acts in the mode of irritation, sensation, volition, or association, is a contraction of the animal fibre, according to the second law of animal causation. Sect. IV. Thus the stimulus of the blood induces the contraction of the heart; the agreeable taste of a strawberry produces the contraction of the muscles of deglutition; the effort of the will contracts the muscles, which move the limbs in walking; and by association other muscles of the trunk are brought into contraction to preserve the balance of the body. The fibrous extremities of the organs of sense have been shewn, by the ocular spectra in Sect. III. to suffer similar contraction by each of the above modes of excitation; and by their configurations to constitute our ideas.

2. The immediate effect of the action of the spirit of animation or sensorial power on the fibrous parts of the body, whether it acts in the mode of irritation, sensation, volition, or association, is a contraction of the animal fibre, according to the second law of animal causation. Sect. IV. Thus the stimulus of the blood induces the contraction of the heart; the agreeable taste of a strawberry produces the contraction of the muscles of deglutition; the effort of the will contracts the muscles, which move the limbs in walking; and by association other muscles of the trunk are brought into contraction to preserve the balance of the body. The fibrous extremities of the organs of sense have been shewn, by the ocular spectra in Sect. III. to suffer similar contraction by each of the above modes of excitation; and by their configurations to constitute our ideas.

3. After animal fibres have for some time been excited into contraction, a relaxation succeeds, even though the exciting cause continues to act. In respect to the irritative motions this is exemplified in the peristaltic contractions of the bowels; which cease and are renewed alternately, though the stimulus of the aliment continues to be uniformly applied; in the sensitive motions, as in strangury, tenesmus, and parturition, the alternate contractions and relaxations of the muscles exist, though the stimulus is perpetual. In our voluntary exertions it is experienced, as no one can hang long by the hands, however vehemently he wills so to do; and in the associate motions the constant change of our attitudes evinces the necessity of relaxation to those muscles, which have been long in action.

3. After animal fibers have been stimulated into contraction for a while, they relax, even if the stimulating factor is still present. This can be seen in the rhythmic contractions of the intestines, which alternately stop and start, even though the food stimulus remains constant. In sensitive movements, like those experienced during painful urination, straining, and childbirth, muscles contract and relax in alternation, even though the stimulus keeps coming. In our voluntary actions, we feel this too, as no one can hang from their hands for long, no matter how strongly they want to. Additionally, the constant changes in our positions show that muscles need to relax after being active for a prolonged period.

This relaxation of a muscle after its contraction, even though the stimulus continues to be applied, appears to arise from the expenditure or diminution of the spirit of animation previously resident in the muscle, according to the second law of animal causation in Sect. IV. In those constitutions, which are termed weak, the spirit of animation becomes sooner exhausted, and tremulous motions are produced, as in the hands of infirm people, when they lift a cup to their mouths. This quicker exhaustion of the spirit of animation is probably owing to a less quantity of it residing in the acting fibres, which therefore more frequently require a supply from the nerves, which belong to them.

This relaxation of a muscle after its contraction, even though the stimulus continues to be applied, appears to arise from the expenditure or diminution of the spirit of animation previously resident in the muscle, according to the second law of animal causation in Sect. IV. In those constitutions, which are termed weak, the spirit of animation becomes sooner exhausted, and tremulous motions are produced, as in the hands of infirm people, when they lift a cup to their mouths. This quicker exhaustion of the spirit of animation is probably owing to a less quantity of it residing in the acting fibres, which therefore more frequently require a supply from the nerves, which belong to them.

4. If the sensorial power continues to act, whether it acts in the mode of irritation, sensation, volition, or association, a new contraction of the animal fibre succeeds after a certain interval; which interval is of shorter continuance in weak people than in strong ones. This is exemplified in the shaking of the hands of weak people, when they attempt to write. In a manuscript epistle of one of my correspondents, which is written in a small hand, I observed from four to six zigzags in the perpendicular stroke of every letter, which shews that both the contractions of the fingers, and intervals between them, must have been performed in very short periods of time.

4. If the sensory system continues to function, whether it's through irritation, sensation, will, or association, a new contraction of the muscle fibers happens after a certain amount of time; this time is shorter for weaker individuals than for stronger ones. This is evident in the shaking hands of weak people when they try to write. In a handwritten letter from one of my correspondents, which is written in a small script, I noticed four to six zigzags in the vertical stroke of each letter, indicating that both the contractions of the fingers and the pauses between them occurred in very short intervals.

The times of contraction of the muscles of enfeebled people being less, and the intervals between those contractions being less also, accounts for the quick pulse in fevers with debility, and in dying animals. The shortness of the intervals between one contraction and another in weak constitutions, is probably owing to the general deficiency of the quantity of the spirit of animation, and that therefore there is a less quantity of it to be received at each interval of the activity of the fibres. Hence in repeated motions, as of the fingers in performing on the harpsichord, it would at first sight appear, that swiftness and strength were incompatible; nevertheless the single contraction of a muscle is performed with greater velocity as well as with greater force by vigorous constitutions, as in throwing a javelin.

The shorter duration of muscle contractions in weak individuals, along with the reduced time between those contractions, explains the rapid pulse seen during fevers and in dying animals. The brief intervals between contractions in fragile bodies likely stem from a general lack of vital energy, meaning there's less energy available during each cycle of muscle activity. Thus, in repetitive tasks, like playing the harpsichord, it might seem that speed and strength can't coexist; however, a single muscle contraction is executed with more speed and greater power in stronger bodies, as seen in activities like javelin throwing.

There is however another circumstance, which may often contribute to cause the quickness of the pulse in nervous fevers, as in animals bleeding to death in the slaughter-house; which is the deficient quantity of blood; whence the heart is but half distended, and in consequence sooner contracts. See Sect. XXXII. 2. 1.

There is however another circumstance, which may often contribute to cause the quickness of the pulse in nervous fevers, as in animals bleeding to death in the slaughter-house; which is the deficient quantity of blood; whence the heart is but half distended, and in consequence sooner contracts. See Sect. XXXII. 2. 1.

For we must not confound frequency of repetition with quickness of motion, or the number of pulsations with the velocity, with which the fibres, which constitute the coats of the arteries, contract themselves. For where the frequency of the pulsations is but seventy-five in a minute, as in health; the contracting fibres, which constitute the sides of the arteries, may move through a greater space in a given time, than where the frequency of pulsation is one hundred and fifty in a minute, as in some fevers with great debility. For if in those fevers the arteries do not expand themselves in their diastole to more than half the usual diameter of their diastole in health, the fibres which constitute their coats, will move through a less space in a minute than in health, though they make two pulsations for one.

We shouldn't confuse how often something happens with how fast it happens, or the number of beats with the speed at which the fibers that make up the artery walls contract. For instance, when the pulse is seventy-five beats per minute, as it is when healthy, the contracting fibers may move a greater distance in a given time than if the pulse is one hundred and fifty beats per minute, which can happen in some fevers with significant weakness. In those fevers, if the arteries don’t expand to more than half their usual diameter during relaxation, the fibers that make up their walls will move a shorter distance in a minute than when healthy, even though there are twice as many beats.

Suppose the diameter of the artery during its systole to be one line, and that the diameter of the same artery during its diastole is in health is four lines, and in a fever with, great debility only two lines. It follows, that the arterial fibres contract in health from a circle of twelve lines in circumference to a circle of three lines in circumference, that is they move through a space of nine lines in length. While the arterial fibres in the fever with debility would twice contract from a circle of six lines to a circle of three lines; that is while they move through a space equal to six lines. Hence though the frequency of pulsation in fever be greater as two to one, yet the velocity of contraction in health is greater as nine to six, or as three to two.

Suppose the diameter of the artery during its systole is one line, and the diameter of the same artery during its diastole, when healthy, is four lines. In a fever with significant weaknes, it is only two lines. It follows that the arterial fibers contract in health from a circle with a circumference of twelve lines to a circle with a circumference of three lines, which means they move through a distance of nine lines. Meanwhile, the arterial fibers during fever with weakness contract from a circle of six lines to a circle of three lines, moving through a distance of six lines. Therefore, even though the frequency of pulsation in fever is greater, as two to one, the velocity of contraction in health is greater, as nine to six, or three to two.

On the contrary in inflammatory diseases with strength, as in the pleurisy, the velocity of the contracting sides of the arteries is much greater than in health, for if we suppose the number of pulsations in a pleurisy to be half as much more than in health, that is as one hundred and twenty to eighty, (which is about what generally happens in inflammatory diseases) and if the diameter of the artery in diastole be one third greater than in health, which I believe is near the truth, the result will be, that the velocity of the contractile sides of the arteries will be in a pleurisy as two and a half to one, compared to the velocity of their contraction in a state of health, for if the circumference of the systole of the artery be three lines, and the diastole in health be twelve lines in circumference, and in a pleurisy eighteen lines; and secondly, if the artery pulsates thrice in the diseased state for twice in the healthy one, it follows, that the velocity of contraction in the diseased state to that in the healthy state will be forty-five to eighteen, or as two and a half to one.

In inflammatory diseases like pleurisy, the speed of the contracting sides of the arteries is much higher than in a healthy state. If we assume that the number of heartbeats during pleurisy is about fifty percent more than in a healthy condition—say one hundred and twenty beats compared to eighty—which is typical in inflammatory diseases, and if the artery's diameter during diastole is about one-third larger than normal, which seems accurate, then the contraction speed of the arteries during pleurisy is two and a half times greater than in a healthy state. For example, if the artery's circumference during systole is three units, and during diastole in a healthy state is twelve units, and during pleurisy it is eighteen units; if the artery beats three times in the diseased state for every two beats in the healthy state, then the contraction speed during disease compared to health will be forty-five to eighteen, or about two and a half to one.

From hence it would appear, that if we had a criterion to determine the velocity of the arterial contractions, it would at the same time give us their strength, and thus be of more service in distinguishing diseases, than the knowledge of their frequency. As such a criterion cannot be had, the frequency of pulsation, the age of the patient being allowed for, will in some measure assist us to distinguish arterial strength from arterial debility, since in inflammatory diseases with strength the frequency seldom exceeds one hundred and eighteen or one hundred and twenty pulsations in a minute; unless under peculiar circumstance, as the great additional stimuli of wine or of external heat.

It seems that if we had a way to measure the speed of arterial contractions, it would also indicate their strength, making it more useful for distinguishing diseases than just knowing how often they happen. Since we can't get that kind of measurement, the frequency of the pulse, adjusted for the patient's age, can help us differentiate between strong arteries and weak ones. In inflammatory diseases with strength, the pulse rarely exceeds one hundred and eighteen or one hundred and twenty beats per minute, unless there are special circumstances, like the stimulating effects of alcohol or external heat.

5. After a muscle or organ of sense has been excited into contraction, and the sensorial power ceases to act, the last situation or configuration of it continues; unless it be disturbed by the action of some antagonist fibres, or other extraneous power. Thus in weak or languid people, wherever they throw their limbs on their bed or sofa, there they lie, till another exertion changes their attitude; hence one kind of ocular spectra seems to be produced after looking at bright objects; thus when a fire-stick is whirled round in the night, there appears in the eye a complete circle of fire; the action or configuration of one part of the retina not ceasing before the return of the whirling fire.

5. After a muscle or sense organ gets stimulated and contracts, and the nervous signal stops, it retains its last position or shape; unless interrupted by opposing muscles or some other outside force. So, in people who are weak or lethargic, wherever they drop their limbs on a bed or sofa, they stay there until some effort changes their position; this is how a certain type of visual afterimage appears after looking at bright objects; for example, when a burning stick is spun around at night, a complete circle of fire seems to appear in the eye, because the action or shape of one part of the retina doesn’t stop until the whirling fire returns.

Thus if any one looks at the setting sun for a short time, and then covers his closed eyes with his hand, he will for many seconds of time perceive the image of the sun on his retina. A similar image of all other bodies would remain some time in the eye, but is effaced by the eternal change of the motions of the extremity of this nerve in our attention to other objects. See Sect. XVIII. 5. on Sleep. Hence the dark spots, and other ocular spectra, are more frequently attended to, and remain longer in the eyes of weak people, as after violent exercise, intoxication, or want of sleep.

Thus if any one looks at the setting sun for a short time, and then covers his closed eyes with his hand, he will for many seconds of time perceive the image of the sun on his retina. A similar image of all other bodies would remain some time in the eye, but is effaced by the eternal change of the motions of the extremity of this nerve in our attention to other objects. See Sect. XVIII. 5. on Sleep. Hence the dark spots, and other ocular spectra, are more frequently attended to, and remain longer in the eyes of weak people, as after violent exercise, intoxication, or want of sleep.

6. A contraction of the fibres somewhat greater than usual introduces pleasurable sensation into the system, according to the fourth law of animal causation. Hence the pleasure in the beginning of drunkenness is owing to the increased action of the system from the stimulus of vinous spirit or of opium. If the contractions be still greater in energy or duration, painful sensations are introduced, as in consequence of great heat, or caustic applications, or fatigue.

6. A slight tightening of the fibers, more intense than normal, brings a pleasurable feeling to the body, following the fourth law of animal causation. Therefore, the enjoyment at the onset of drunkenness comes from the heightened activity of the body due to the effects of alcoholic drinks or opium. If the contractions are even stronger or last longer, they can lead to painful sensations, similar to what happens with excessive heat, irritating substances, or physical exhaustion.

If any part of the system, which is used to perpetual activity, as the stomach, or heart, or the fine vessels of the skin, acts for a time with less energy, another kind of painful sensation ensues, which is called hunger, or faintness, or cold. This occurs in a less degree in the locomotive muscles, and is called wearysomeness. In the two former kinds of sensation there is an expenditure of sensorial power, in these latter there is an accumulation of it.

If any part of the system that is used for continuous activity, like the stomach, heart, or the fine blood vessels in the skin, works less efficiently for a while, it leads to a painful sensation known as hunger, faintness, or cold. This happens to a smaller extent in the muscles that help us move, and it's referred to as weariness. In the first two types of sensations, there is a loss of sensory energy, whereas in the latter, there is a buildup of it.

7. We have used the words exertion of sensorial power as a general term to express either irritation, sensation, volition, or association; that is, to express the activity or motion of the spirit of animation, at the time it produces the contractions of the fibrous parts of the system. It may be supposed that there may exist a greater or less mobility of the fibrous parts of our system, or a propensity to be stimulated into contraction by the greater or less quantity or energy of the spirit of animation; and that hence if the exertion of the sensorial power be in its natural state, and the mobility of the fibres be increased, the same quantity of fibrous contraction will be caused, as if the mobility of the fibres continues in its natural state, and the sensorial exertion be increased.

7. We have used the term "exertion of sensorial power" as a broad way to describe irritation, sensation, will, or association; in other words, to express the activity or movement of our spirit of animation when it causes the fibrous parts of our system to contract. It's possible that there can be varying degrees of flexibility in the fibrous parts of our system, or a tendency to contract in response to different amounts or levels of energy from the spirit of animation. Therefore, if the exertion of sensorial power is in its normal state and the mobility of the fibers is increased, the same amount of fibrous contraction will occur as if the mobility of the fibers remains normal while the sensorial exertion is increased.

Thus it may be conceived, that in diseases accompanied with strength, as in inflammatory fevers with arterial strength, that the cause of greater fibrous contraction, may exist in the increased mobility of the fibres, whose contractions are thence both more forceable and more frequent. And that in diseases attended with debility, as in nervous fevers, where the fibrous contractions are weaker, and more frequent, it may be conceived that the cause consists in a decrease of mobility of the fibres; and that those weak constitutions, which are attended with cold extremities and large pupils of the eyes, may possess less mobility of the contractile fibres, as well as less quantity of exertion of the spirit of animation.

Thus, it can be understood that in diseases characterized by strength, like inflammatory fevers with strong pulses, the reason for greater fibrous contraction might lie in the increased flexibility of the fibers, which makes their contractions both stronger and more frequent. Conversely, in diseases marked by weakness, such as nervous fevers, where the fibrous contractions are weaker and more frequent, it may be thought that the cause is a decrease in the flexibility of the fibers. Furthermore, those with weak constitutions, who experience cold extremities and large pupils, may have less flexibility in their contractile fibers as well as a reduced ability to exert the spirit of animation.

In answer to this mode of reasoning it may be sufficient to observe, that the contractile fibres consist of inert matter, and when the sensorial power is withdrawn, as in death, they possess no power of motion at all, but remain in their last state, whether of contraction or relaxation, and must thence derive the whole of this property from the spirit of animation. At the same time it is not improbable, that the moving fibres of strong people may possess a capability of receiving or containing a greater quantity of the spirit of animation than those of weak people.

In response to this line of thinking, it’s enough to point out that the contractile fibers are made of inert material, and when the sensory power is gone, like in death, they have no ability to move at all. They stay in whatever state they were in, whether contracted or relaxed, and must get all their movement ability from the vital spirit. Additionally, it’s likely that the moving fibers in strong individuals can hold more of this vital spirit than those in weaker individuals.

In every contraction of a fibre there is an expenditure of the sensorial power, or spirit of animation; and where the exertion of this sensorial power has been for some time increased, and the muscles or organs of sense have in consequence acted with greater energy, its propensity to activity is proportionally lessened; which is to be ascribed to the exhaustion or diminution of its quantity. On the contrary, where there has been less fibrous contraction than usual for a certain time, the sensorial power or spirit of animation becomes accumulated in the inactive part of the system. Hence vigour succeeds rest, and hence the propensity to action of all our organs of sense and muscles is in a state of perpetual fluctuation. The irritability for instance of the retina, that is, its quantity of sensorial power, varies every moment according to the brightness or obscurity of the object last beheld compared with the present one. The same occurs to our sense of heat, and to every part of our system, which is capable of being excited into action.

In every contraction of a muscle, there’s a use of sensory energy, or the spirit of animation. When this sensory energy is used more than normal for some time, the muscles or senses act with more intensity, but this leads to a decrease in their capacity for action, due to exhaustion or a reduction in its supply. On the other hand, if there’s been less muscle contraction than usual for a while, the sensory energy builds up in the inactive parts of the body. This is why energy follows rest, and it explains the constant ebb and flow of our senses and muscle activity. For example, the sensitivity of the retina, or its amount of sensory energy, changes constantly based on the brightness or darkness of what was last seen compared to what is currently being observed. The same principle applies to our sense of heat and every part of our system that can be activated.

When this variation of the exertion of the sensorial power becomes much and permanently above or beneath the natural quantity, it becomes a disease. If the irritative motions be too great or too little, it shews that the stimulus of external things affect this sensorial power too violently or too inertly. If the sensitive motions be too great or too little, the cause arises from the deficient or exuberant quantity of sensation produced in consequence of the motions of the muscular fibres or organs of sense; if the voluntary actions are diseased the cause is to be looked for in the quantity of volition produced in consequence of the desire or aversion occasioned by the painful or pleasurable sensations above mentioned. And the diseases of associations probably depend on the greater or less quantity of the other three sensorial powers by which they were formed.

When the way we use our sensory abilities is consistently above or below the normal range, it becomes a disorder. If the responsive movements are too intense or too weak, it indicates that external stimuli are impacting our sensory abilities either too harshly or too passively. If the sensitive responses are excessive or insufficient, it is due to either a lack or surplus of sensation resulting from the movements of muscle fibers or sensory organs. If voluntary actions are impaired, the issue lies in the amount of willpower generated by the desires or aversions created by the previously mentioned painful or pleasurable sensations. Moreover, disorders in associations likely stem from the varying levels of the other three sensory powers that contributed to their formation.

From whence it appears that the propensity to action, whether it be called irritability, sensibility, voluntarity, or associability, is only another mode of expression for the quantity of sensorial power residing in the organ to be excited. And that on the contrary the words inirritability and insensibility, together with inaptitude to voluntary and associate motions, are synonymous with deficiency of the quantity of sensorial power, or of the spirit of animation, residing in the organs to be excited.

From this, it seems that the tendency to act, whether referred to as irritability, sensitivity, willfulness, or associativity, is just another way of saying the amount of sensory power present in the organ that gets stimulated. Conversely, the terms irritability and insensitivity, along with the inability to perform voluntary and associative movements, mean a lack of sensory power or the spirit of activity in the organs that are meant to be stimulated.

II. Of sensorial Exertion.

II. *Of sensory Exertion.*

1. There are three circumstances to be attended to in the production of animal motions, 1st. The stimulus. 2d. The sensorial power. 3d. The contractile fibre. 1st. A stimulus, external to the organ, originally induces into action the sensorial faculty termed irritation; this produces the contraction of the fibres, which, if it be perceived at all, introduces pleasure or pain; which in their active state are termed sensation; which is another sensorial faculty, and occasionally produces contraction of the fibres; this pleasure or pain is therefore to be considered as another stimulus, which may either act alone or in conjunction with the former faculty of the sensorium termed irritation.

1. There are three factors to consider in the production of animal movements: 1st. The stimulus. 2nd. The sensory power. 3rd. The contractile fiber. 1st. A stimulus, coming from outside the organ, initially triggers the sensory function known as irritation; this causes the fibers to contract, which leads to either pleasure or pain if perceived at all; these states, when active, are referred to as sensation; sensation is another sensory function, and it can sometimes cause the fibers to contract as well; thus, this pleasure or pain should be seen as another stimulus, which can act either independently or together with the earlier sensory function known as irritation.

This new stimulus of pleasure or pain either induces into action the sensorial faculty termed sensation, which then produces the contraction of the fibres; or it introduces desire or aversion, which excite into action another sensorial faculty, termed volition, and may therefore be considered as another stimulus, which either alone or in conjunction with one or both of the two former faculties of the sensorium produces the contraction of animal fibres. There is another sensorial power, that of association, which perpetually, in conjunction with one or more of the above, and frequently singly, produces the contraction of animal fibres, and which is itself excited into action by the previous motions of contracting fibres.

This new stimulus of pleasure or pain either triggers the sensory ability known as sensation, which then causes the muscle fibers to contract; or it leads to desire or aversion, which activate another sensory ability called volition, and can therefore be seen as another stimulus. This stimulus, whether on its own or combined with one or both of the previous sensory faculties, leads to the contraction of muscle fibers. There's also another sensory power, which is association, that consistently, together with one or more of the aforementioned faculties, and often on its own, causes the contraction of muscle fibers, and is itself activated by the previous movements of contracting fibers.

Now as the sensorial power, termed irritation, residing in any particular fibres, is excited into exertion by the stimulus of external bodies acting on those fibres; the sensorial power, termed sensation, residing in any particular fibres is excited into exertion by the stimulus of pleasure or pain acting on those fibres; the sensorial power, termed volition, residing in any particular fibres is excited into exertion by the stimulus of desire or aversion; and the sensorial power, termed association, residing in any particular fibres, is excited into action by the stimulus of other fibrous motions, which had frequently preceded them. The word stimulus may therefore be used without impropriety of language, for any of these four causes, which excite the four sensorial powers into exertion. For though the immediate cause of volition has generally been termed a motive; and that of irritation only has generally obtained the name of stimulus; yet as the immediate cause, which excites the sensorial powers of sensation, or of association into exertion, have obtained no general name, we shall use the word stimulus for them all.

Now, the sensory power known as irritation, located in specific fibers, is activated by the stimulus of external objects acting on those fibers; the sensory power called sensation, also in specific fibers, is activated by the stimulus of pleasure or pain affecting those fibers; the sensory power referred to as volition, found in specific fibers, is triggered by the stimulus of desire or aversion; and the sensory power called association, located in particular fibers, is prompted into action by the stimulus of other fibrous movements that have frequently occurred before. The term stimulus can therefore be used appropriately for any of these four causes that activate the four sensory powers. Although the immediate cause of volition is generally called a motive, and that of irritation is mainly termed stimulus, the immediate causes that stimulate the sensory powers of sensation or association have not received a common name, so we will use the word stimulus for all of them.

Hence the quantity of motion produced in any particular part of the animal system will be as the quantity of stimulus and the quantity of sensorial power, or spirit of animation, residing in the contracting fibres. Where both these quantities are great, strength is produced, when that word is applied to the motions of animal bodies. Where either of them is deficient, weakness is produced, as applied to the motions of animal bodies.

Therefore, the amount of movement generated in any specific part of the animal body depends on the level of stimulus and the amount of sensory power, or life force, present in the contracting fibers. When both of these factors are high, it results in strength when referring to the movements of animal bodies. Conversely, when either factor is lacking, it leads to weakness as it relates to the movements of animal bodies.

Now as the sensorial power, or spirit of animation, is perpetually exhausted by the expenditure of it in fibrous contractions, and is perpetually renewed by the secretion or production of it in the brain and spinal marrow, the quantity of animal strength must be in a perpetual state of fluctuation on this account; and if to this be added the unceasing variation of all the four kinds of stimulus above described, which produce the exertions of the sensorial powers, the ceaseless vicissitude of animal strength becomes easily comprehended.

Now, since the sensory power, or the spirit of animation, is constantly drained by its use in muscle contractions and is continuously replenished by its production in the brain and spinal cord, the amount of physical strength must always be fluctuating for this reason. When you also consider the ongoing changes from the four types of stimuli mentioned earlier, which trigger the use of these sensory powers, it's easy to understand the constant ups and downs of physical strength.

If the quantity of sensorial power remains the same, and the quantity of stimulus be lessened, a weakness of the fibrous contractions ensues, which may be denominated debility from defect of stimulus. If the quantity of stimulus remains the same, and the quantity of sensorial power be lessened, another kind of weakness ensues, which may be termed debility from defect of sensorial power; the former of these is called by Dr. Brown, in his Elements of Medicine, direct debility, and the latter indirect debility. The coincidence of some parts of this work with correspondent deductions in the Brunonian Elementa Medicina, a work (with some exceptions) of great genius, must be considered as confirmations of the truth of the theory, as they were probably arrived at by different trains of reasoning.

If the amount of sensory power stays the same, but the amount of stimulus decreases, it leads to a weakness in the fibrous contractions, which can be called debility from defect of stimulus. If the amount of stimulus stays the same, but the amount of sensory power decreases, another type of weakness occurs, which can be referred to as debility from defect of sensorial power; the first is referred to by Dr. Brown in his Elements of Medicine as direct debility, while the latter is called indirect debility. The overlap between some parts of this work and similar conclusions in the Brunonian Elementa Medicina, a work (with some exceptions) of great intelligence, should be seen as validations of the theory's accuracy, as they were likely reached through different lines of reasoning.

Thus in those who have been exposed to cold and hunger there is a deficiency of stimulus. While in nervous fever there is a deficiency of sensorial power. And in habitual drunkards, in a morning before their usual potation, there is a deficiency both of stimulus and of sensorial power. While, on the other hand, in the beginning of intoxication there is an excess of stimulus; in the hot-ach, after the hands have been immersed in snow, there is a redundancy of sensorial power; and in inflammatory diseases with arterial strength, there is an excess of both.

So, in those who have faced cold and hunger, there's a lack of stimulation. In cases of nervous fever, there's a lack of sensory power. For habitual drinkers, on mornings before their usual drink, there's a shortage of both stimulation and sensory power. Conversely, at the start of intoxication, there's an overflow of stimulation; in the case of a hot ache after hands have been plunged in snow, there's an excess of sensory power; and in inflammatory diseases with strong arteries, there's an excess of both.

Hence if the sensorial power be lessened, while the quantity of stimulus remains the same as in nervous fever, the frequency of repetition of the arterial contractions may continue, but their force in respect to removing obstacles, as in promoting the circulation of the blood, or the velocity of each contraction, will be diminished, that is, the animal strength will be lessened. And secondly, if the quantity of sensorial power be lessened, and the stimulus be increased to a certain degree, as in giving opium in nervous fevers, the arterial contractions may be performed more frequently than natural, yet with less strength.

If the sensory power is reduced while the amount of stimulus stays the same, like in nervous fever, the heart may continue to contract often, but the effectiveness in overcoming challenges, such as helping blood circulation or the speed of each contraction, will be weakened; in other words, the animal strength will decrease. Additionally, if the sensory power is reduced and the stimulus is increased to some extent, as seen when administering opium in nervous fevers, the heart may contract more frequently than usual, but with less force.

And thirdly, if the sensorial power continues the same in respect to quantity, and the stimulus be somewhat diminished, as in going into a darkish room, or into a coldish bath, suppose of about eighty degrees of heat, as Buxton-bath, a temporary weakness of the affected fibres is induced, till an accumulation of sensorial power gradually succeeds, and counterbalances the deficiency of stimulus, and then the bath ceases to feel cold, and the room ceases to appear dark; because the fibres of the subcutaneous vessels, or of the organs of sense, act with their usual energy.

And thirdly, if the sensory power remains the same in terms of quantity, and the stimulus is slightly reduced, like when entering a dimly lit room or a cooler bath, say around eighty degrees Fahrenheit, like Buxton bath, a temporary weakness in the affected fibers occurs until a buildup of sensory power gradually kicks in to balance out the lack of stimulus, and then the bath stops feeling cold, and the room stops appearing dark; because the fibers of the subcutaneous vessels or the sensory organs work with their usual energy.

A set of muscular fibres may thus be stimulated into violent exertion, that is, they may act frequently, and with their whole sensorial power, but may nevertheless not act strongly; because the quantity of their sensorial power was originally small, or was previously exhausted. Hence a stimulus may be great, and the irritation in consequence act with its full force, as in the hot paroxysms of nervous fever; but if the sensorial power, termed irritation, be small in quantity, the force of the fibrous contractions, and the times of their continuance in their contracted state, will be proportionally small.

A group of muscle fibers can be triggered into intense action, meaning they can fire frequently and use all their sensory power, but they may not be very strong because their sensory power was originally low or has already been depleted. So, a stimulus can be intense, and the irritation can react at full intensity, such as during the hot episodes of a nervous fever; however, if the sensory power, known as irritation, is low in quantity, the strength of the muscle contractions and the duration they stay contracted will be correspondingly weak.

In the same manner in the hot paroxysm of putrid fevers, which are shewn in Sect. XXXIII. to be inflammatory fevers with arterial debility, the sensorial power termed sensation is exerted with great activity, yet the fibrous contractions, which produce the circulation of the blood, are performed without strength, because the quantity of sensorial power then residing in that part of the system is small.

In the same manner in the hot paroxysm of putrid fevers, which are shewn in Sect. XXXIII. to be inflammatory fevers with arterial debility, the sensorial power termed sensation is exerted with great activity, yet the fibrous contractions, which produce the circulation of the blood, are performed without strength, because the quantity of sensorial power then residing in that part of the system is small.

Thus in irritative fever with arterial strength, that is, with excess of spirit of animation, the quantity of exertion during the hot part of the paroxysm is to be estimated from the quantity of stimulus, and the quantity of sensorial power. While in sensitive (or inflammatory) fever with arterial strength, that is, with excess of spirit of animation, the violent and forcible actions of the vascular system during the hot part of the paroxysm are induced by the exertions of two sensorial powers, which are excited by two kinds of stimulus. These are the sensorial power of irritation excited by the stimulus of bodies external to the moving fibres, and the sensorial power of sensation excited by the pain in consequence of the increased contractions of those moving fibres.

In irritative fever with strong arteries, meaning an excess of energy, the amount of effort during the hot phase of the episode is determined by the level of stimulus and the amount of sensory power. In sensitive (or inflammatory) fever with strong arteries, also indicating an excess of energy, the intense and forceful actions of the vascular system during the hot phase of the episode are triggered by the efforts of two sensory powers, which are stimulated by two types of inputs. These inputs are the sensory power of irritation caused by external stimuli affecting the moving fibers, and the sensory power of sensation triggered by pain due to the increased contractions of those moving fibers.

And in insane people in some cases the force of their muscular actions will be in proportion to the quantity of sensorial power, which they possess, and the quantity of the stimulus of desire or aversion, which excites their volition into action. At the same time in other cases the stimulus of pain or pleasure, and the stimulus of external bodies, may excite into action the sensorial powers of sensation and irritation, and thus add greater force to their muscular actions.

And in some cases, the physical strength of insane people will match the level of sensory power they have, along with the intensity of their desires or aversions that drive them to act. Meanwhile, in other situations, the sensations of pain or pleasure, as well as external stimuli, can activate their sensory powers of feeling and agitation, thereby increasing the strength of their physical actions.

2. The application of the stimulus, whether that stimulus be some quality of external bodies, or pleasure or pain, or desire or aversion, or a link of association, excites the correspondent sensorial power into action, and this causes the contraction of the fibre. On the contraction of the fibre a part of the spirit of animation becomes expended, and the fibre ceases to contract, though the stimulus continues to be applied; till in a certain time the fibre having received a supply of sensorial power is ready to contract again, if the stimulus continues to be applied. If the stimulus on the contrary be withdrawn, the same quantity of quiescent sensorial power becomes resident in the fibre as before its contraction; as appears from the readiness for action of the large locomotive muscles of the body in a short time after common exertion.

2. When a stimulus is applied, whether it's an external quality, pleasure, pain, desire, aversion, or an association, it activates the relevant sensory power, leading to the contraction of the fiber. With the contraction of the fiber, a portion of the vital spirit is used up, causing the fiber to stop contracting, even though the stimulus is still present; however, after some time, the fiber, having received a new supply of sensory power, can contract again if the stimulus continues. Conversely, if the stimulus is removed, the same amount of resting sensory power remains in the fiber as it had before contracting, which is evident from how quickly the large muscles of the body can respond after normal exertion.

But in those muscular fibres, which are subject to constant stimulus, as the arteries, glands, and capillary vessels, another phenomenon occurs, if their accustomed stimulus be withdrawn; which is, that the sensorial power becomes accumulated in the contractile fibres, owing to the want of its being perpetually expended, or carried away, by their usual unremitted contractions. And on this account those muscular fibres become afterwards excitable into their natural actions by a much weaker stimulus; or into unnatural violence of action by their accustomed stimulus, as is seen in the hot fits of intermittent fevers, which are in consequence of the previous cold ones. Thus the minute vessels of the skin are constantly stimulated by the fluid matter of heat; if the quantity of this stimulus of heat be a while diminished, as in covering the hands with snow, the vessels cease to act, as appears from the paleness of the skin; if this cold application of snow be continued but a short time, the sensorial power, which had habitually been supplied to the fibres, becomes now accumulated in them, owing to the want of its being expended by their accustomed contractions. And thence a less stimulus of heat will now excite them into violent contractions.

But in those muscle fibers that are constantly stimulated, like the arteries, glands, and capillary vessels, another phenomenon happens when their normal stimulus is removed. The sensory power builds up in the contractile fibers because it isn’t being continuously used or drained by their usual constant contractions. As a result, those muscle fibers can later respond to their natural actions with a much weaker stimulus, or to extreme actions with their regular stimulus, as seen in the hot episodes of intermittent fevers that follow the cold ones. The tiny blood vessels in the skin are continuously stimulated by heat. If the amount of this heat stimulus is temporarily reduced, like when covering the hands with snow, the vessels stop working, which is evident from the skin turning pale. If this cold application of snow lasts for only a short time, the sensory power that was normally supplied to the fibers accumulates in them because it hasn’t been used up by their usual contractions. Consequently, a lesser stimulus of heat will now cause them to contract violently.

If the quiescence of fibres, which had previously been subject to perpetual stimulus, continues a longer time; or their accustomed stimulus be more completely withdrawn; the accumulation of sensorial power becomes still greater, as in those exposed to cold and hunger; pain is produced, and the organ gradually dies from the chemical changes, which take place in it; or it is at a great distance of time restored to action by stimulus applied with great caution in small quantity, as happens to some larger animals and to many insects, which during the winter months lie benumbed with cold, and are said to sleep, and to persons apparently drowned, or apparently frozen to death. Snails have been said to revive by throwing them into water after having been many years shut up in the cabinets of the curious; and eggs and seeds in general are restored to life after many months of torpor by the stimulus of warmth and moisture.

If the inactivity of fibers that were previously under constant stimulation lasts for a longer time, or if their usual stimulation is completely removed, the buildup of sensory power increases even more, similar to those exposed to cold and hunger. This causes pain, and the organ gradually deteriorates due to the chemical changes that occur within it; or, after a long time, it can be cautiously reactivated with a small amount of stimulus, as seen in some larger animals and many insects that lie dormant from the cold during the winter months, which is often described as sleep, as well as in people who appear to be drowned or frozen to death. It's said that snails can be revived by placing them in water after being locked away for many years in collectors' cabinets; eggs and seeds, in general, can return to life after many months of inactivity when exposed to warmth and moisture.

The inflammation of schirrous tumours, which have long existed in a state of inaction, is a process of this kind; as well as the sensibility acquired by inflamed tendons and bones, which had at their formation a similar sensibility, which had so long lain dormant in their uninflamed state.

The inflammation of hard tumors that have been inactive for a long time is a process like this; as well as the sensitivity gained by inflamed tendons and bones, which had a similar sensitivity when they formed but had remained dormant in their uninflamed state.

3. If after long quiescence from defect of stimulus the fibres, which had previously been habituated to perpetual stimulus, are again exposed to but their usual quantity of it; as in those who have suffered the extremes of cold or hunger; a violent exertion of the affected organ commences, owing, as above explained, to the great accumulation of sensorial power. This violent exertion not only diminishes the accumulated spirit of animation, but at the same time induces pleasure or pain into the system, which, whether it be succeeded by inflammation or not, becomes an additional stimulus, and acting along with the former one, produces still greater exertions; and thus reduces the sensorial power in the contracting fibres beneath its natural quantity.

3. If, after a long period of inactivity due to lack of stimulation, the fibers that were once used to constant stimulation are exposed again to just the usual amount; as seen in those who have experienced extreme cold or hunger; a strong response from the affected organ begins, due to the significant buildup of sensory energy. This intense response not only decreases the accumulated energy but also brings pleasure or pain to the system, which, whether or not it leads to inflammation, serves as an additional stimulus and works alongside the original one, resulting in even greater responses; thus lowering the sensory energy in the contracting fibers below its normal level.

When the spirit of animation is thus exhausted by useless exertions, the organ becomes torpid or unexcitable into action, and a second fit of quiescence succeeds that of abundant activity. During this second fit of quiescence the sensorial power becomes again accumulated, and another fit of exertion follows in train. These vicissitudes of exertion and inertion of the arterial system constitute the paroxysms of remittent fevers; or intermittent ones, when there is an interval of the natural action of the arteries between the exacerbations.

When the energy of movement is drained by pointless effort, the body becomes sluggish or unable to react, and a second phase of inactivity follows a period of high activity. During this second phase of rest, the sensory power builds up again, leading to another period of effort. These cycles of activity and inactivity in the circulatory system make up the episodes of recurring fevers; or intermittent ones, when there is a break of normal arterial function between the flare-ups.

In these paroxysms of fevers, which consist of the libration of the arterial system between the extremes of exertion and quiescence, either the fits become less and less violent from the contractile fibres becoming coming less excitable to the stimulus by habit, that is, by becoming accustomed to it, as explained below XII. 3. 1. or the whole sensorial power becomes exhausted, and the arteries cease to beat, and the patient dies in the cold part of the paroxysm. Or secondly, so much pain is introduced into the system by the violent contractions of the fibres, that inflammation arises, which prevents future cold fits by expending a part of the sensorial power in the extension of old vessels or the production of new ones; and thus preventing the too great accumulation or exertion of it in other parts of the system; or which by the great increase of stimulus excites into great action the whole glandular system as well as the arterial, and thence a greater quantity of sensorial power is produced in the brain, and thus its exhaustion in any peculiar part of the system ceases to be affected.

In these paroxysms of fevers, which consist of the libration of the arterial system between the extremes of exertion and quiescence, either the fits become less and less violent from the contractile fibres becoming coming less excitable to the stimulus by habit, that is, by becoming accustomed to it, as explained below XII. 3. 1. or the whole sensorial power becomes exhausted, and the arteries cease to beat, and the patient dies in the cold part of the paroxysm. Or secondly, so much pain is introduced into the system by the violent contractions of the fibres, that inflammation arises, which prevents future cold fits by expending a part of the sensorial power in the extension of old vessels or the production of new ones; and thus preventing the too great accumulation or exertion of it in other parts of the system; or which by the great increase of stimulus excites into great action the whole glandular system as well as the arterial, and thence a greater quantity of sensorial power is produced in the brain, and thus its exhaustion in any peculiar part of the system ceases to be affected.

4. Or thirdly, in consequence of the painful or pleasurable sensation above mentioned, desire and aversion are introduced, and inordinate volition succeeds; which by its own exertions expends so much of the spirit of animation, that the two other sensorial faculties, or irritation and sensation, act so much more feebly; that the paroxysms of fever, or that libration between the extremes of exertion and inactivity of the arterial system, gradually subsides. On this account a temporary insanity is a favourable sign in fevers, as I have had some opportunities of observing.

4. Alternatively, as a result of the painful or pleasurable feelings mentioned earlier, desire and aversion come into play, leading to excessive willpower; this, in turn, uses up so much of our energy that the other two senses—irritation and sensation—start to work much weaker. As a result, the episodes of fever, or the fluctuation between extreme activity and inactivity in the arterial system, slowly lessen. For this reason, a temporary insanity can be a good sign during fevers, as I have had some chances to notice.

III. Of repeated Stimulus.

III. Of repeated stimulation.

1. When a stimulus is repeated more frequently than the expenditure of sensorial power can be renewed in the acting organ, the effect of the stimulus becomes gradually diminished. Thus if two grains of opium be swallowed by a person unused to so strong a stimulus, all the vascular systems in the body act with greater energy, all the secretions and the absorption from those secreted fluids are increased in quantity; and pleasure or pain are introduced into the system, which adds an additional stimulus to that already too great. After some hours the sensorial power becomes diminished in quantity, expended by the great activity of the system; and thence, when the stimulus of the opium is withdrawn, the fibres will not obey their usual degree of natural stimulus, and a consequent torpor or quiescence succeeds, as is experienced by drunkards, who on the day after a great excess of spirituous potation feel indigestion, head-ach, and general debility.

1. When a stimulus is repeated more often than the body can regenerate the necessary sensory energy, the impact of that stimulus steadily decreases. For example, if someone who isn't used to strong stimuli takes two grains of opium, all the vascular systems in their body respond with increased energy, and both secretion and absorption from those secreted fluids ramp up. This introduces sensations of pleasure or pain, adding an extra layer of stimulus on top of an already overwhelming one. After a few hours, the sensory energy depletes due to the high level of activity in the body; as a result, when the effect of the opium wears off, the body’s fibers won't respond to their usual natural stimuli. This leads to a state of sluggishness or inactivity, similar to what people experience after drinking too much alcohol, where they might feel indigestion, headaches, and overall weakness the next day.

In this fit of torpor or quiescence of a part or of the whole of the system, an accumulation of the sensorial power in the affected fibres is formed, and occasions a second paroxysm of exertion by the application only of the natural stimulus, and thus a libration of the sensorial exertion between one excess and the other continues for two or three days, where the stimulus was violent in degree; and for weeks in some fevers, from the stimulus of contagious matter.

In this state of sluggishness or stillness of part or all of the system, an accumulation of sensory energy in the affected fibers builds up and leads to a second outburst of activity simply by applying the natural stimulus. This back-and-forth of sensory exertion between one extreme and another goes on for two or three days if the stimulus was intense, and can last for weeks in some fevers caused by contagious materials.

But if a second dose of opium be exhibited before the fibres have regained their natural quantity of sensorial power, its effect will be much less than the former, because the spirit of animation or sensorial power is in part exhausted by the previous excess of exertion. Hence all medicines repeated too frequently gradually lose their effect, as opium and wine. Many things of disagreeable taste at first cease to be disagreeable by frequent repetition, as tobacco; grief and pain gradually diminish, and at length cease altogether, and hence life itself becomes tolerable.

But if a second dose of opium is given before the nerves have regained their normal level of sensitivity, its effect will be much weaker than the first because the energy and sensitivity are partially drained from the previous overload. That's why all medications used too often slowly lose their effectiveness, just like opium and wine. Many unpleasant tastes eventually become tolerable with repeated exposure, like tobacco; sadness and pain gradually lessen and eventually disappear, making life itself bearable.

Besides the temporary diminution of the spirit of animation or sensorial power, which is naturally stationary or resident in every living fibre, by a single exhibition of a powerful stimulus, the contractile fibres themselves, by the perpetual application of a new quantity of stimulus, before they have regained their natural quantity of sensorial power, appear to suffer in their capability of receiving so much as the natural quantity of sensorial power; and hence a permanent deficiency of spirit of animation takes place, however long the stimulus may have been withdrawn. On this cause depends the permanent debility of those, who have been addicted to intoxication, the general weakness of old age, and the natural debility or inirritability of those, who have pale skins and large pupils of their eyes.

Apart from the temporary decrease in energy or sensory power, which is naturally stable in every living fiber, a single intense stimulus can affect it. The contractile fibers themselves, subjected to continuous new stimuli before they've had a chance to regain their usual sensory power, seem to struggle with even receiving their natural amount of sensory power. As a result, there’s a lasting decline in energy, no matter how long the stimulus is removed. This phenomenon explains the lasting weakness in those who abuse alcohol, the general frailty that comes with old age, and the natural weakness or lack of responsiveness in individuals with pale skin and large pupils.

There is a curious phenomenon belongs to this place, which has always appeared difficult of solution; and that is, that opium or aloes may be exhibited in small doses at first, and gradually increased to very large ones without producing stupor or diarrhœa. In this case, though the opium and aloes are given in such small doses as not to produce intoxication or catharsis, yet they are exhibited in quantities sufficient in some degree to exhaust the sensorial power, and hence a stronger and a stronger dose is required; otherwise the medicine would soon cease to act at all.

There’s an interesting phenomenon in this place that has always been hard to explain: opium or aloes can be given in small doses at first and then gradually increased to very large amounts without causing drowsiness or diarrhea. In this instance, even though the opium and aloes are given in small amounts that don’t lead to intoxication or a laxative effect, they are still given in quantities that somewhat deplete the body's sensory power. As a result, progressively larger doses are needed; otherwise, the medicine would stop working altogether.

On the contrary, if the opium or aloes be exhibited in a large dose at first, so as to produce intoxication or diarrhœa; after a few repetitions the quantity of either of them may be diminished, and they will still produce this effect. For the more powerful stimulus dissevers the progressive catenations of animal motions, described in Sect. XVII. and introduces a new link between them; whence every repetition strengthens this new association or catenation, and the stimulus may be gradually decreased, or be nearly withdrawn, and yet the effect shall continue; because the sensorial power of association or catenation being united with the stimulus, increases in energy with every repetition of the catenated circle; and it is by these means that all the irritative associations of motions are originally produced.

On the contrary, if the opium or aloes be exhibited in a large dose at first, so as to produce intoxication or diarrhœa; after a few repetitions the quantity of either of them may be diminished, and they will still produce this effect. For the more powerful stimulus dissevers the progressive catenations of animal motions, described in Sect. XVII. and introduces a new link between them; whence every repetition strengthens this new association or catenation, and the stimulus may be gradually decreased, or be nearly withdrawn, and yet the effect shall continue; because the sensorial power of association or catenation being united with the stimulus, increases in energy with every repetition of the catenated circle; and it is by these means that all the irritative associations of motions are originally produced.

2. When a stimulus is repeated at such distant intervals of time, that the natural quantity of sensorial power becomes completely restored in the acting fibres, it will act with the same energy as when first applied. Hence those who have lately accustomed themselves to large doses of opium by beginning with small ones, and gradually increasing them, and repeating them frequently, as mentioned in the preceding paragraph; if they intermit the use of it for a few days only, must begin again with as small doses as they took at first, otherwise they will experience the inconveniences of intoxication.

2. When a stimulus is repeated after long gaps of time, allowing the body's sensory power to fully recover in the active fibers, it will have the same effect as when it was first applied. Therefore, those who have recently trained themselves to take large doses of opium by starting with small amounts and gradually increasing the dosage while frequently repeating it, as mentioned in the previous paragraph; if they stop using it for just a few days, must start again with the same small doses they began with, or they will face the negative effects of intoxication.

On this circumstance depend the constant unfailing effects of the various kinds of stimulus, which excite into action all the vascular systems in the body; the arterial, venous, absorbent, and glandular vessels, are brought into perpetual unwearied action by the fluids, which are adapted to stimulate them; but these have the sensorial power of association added to that of irritation, and even in some degree that of sensation, and even of volition, as will be spoken of in their places; and life itself is thus carried on by the production of sensorial power being equal to its waste or expenditure in the perpetual movement of the vascular organization.

On this situation depend the constant, reliable effects of the different types of stimuli, which activate all the vascular systems in the body; the arterial, venous, absorbent, and glandular vessels are put into constant, tireless action by the fluids that are meant to stimulate them. However, these fluids also have the ability to associate with irritation, and to some extent, with sensation and even willpower, as will be discussed later. Life itself continues because the production of sensory power matches its consumption in the ongoing movement of the vascular system.

3. When a stimulus is repeated at uniform intervals of time with such distances between them, that the expenditure of sensorial power in the acting fibres becomes completely renewed, the effect is produced with greater facility or energy. For the sensorial power of association is combined with the sensorial power of irritation, or, in common language, the acquired habit assists the power of the stimulus.

3. When a stimulus is repeated at regular time intervals, and the breaks between them allow for the complete renewal of sensory energy in the responding fibers, the effect is produced more easily or with more intensity. This is because the sensory power of association works together with the sensory power of irritation, or, in simpler terms, the habit that's been formed supports the effect of the stimulus.

This circumstance not only obtains in the annual and diurnal catenations of animal motions explained in Sect. XXXVI. but in every less circle of actions or ideas, as in the burthen of a song, or the iterations of a dance; and constitutes the pleasure we receive from repetition and imitation; as treated of in Sect. XXII. 2.

This circumstance not only obtains in the annual and diurnal catenations of animal motions explained in Sect. XXXVI. but in every less circle of actions or ideas, as in the burthen of a song, or the iterations of a dance; and constitutes the pleasure we receive from repetition and imitation; as treated of in Sect. XXII. 2.

4. When a stimulus has been many times repeated at uniform intervals, so as to produce the complete action of the organ, it may then be gradually diminished, or totally withdrawn, and the action of the organ will continue. For the sensorial power of association becomes united with that of irritation, and by frequent repetition becomes at length of sufficient energy to carry on the new link in the circle of actions, without the irritation which at first introduced it.

4. When a stimulus has been repeated many times at consistent intervals to fully engage the organ's function, it can then be slowly reduced or completely removed, and the organ's action will still persist. This is because the power of association becomes intertwined with irritation, and through frequent repetition, it eventually gains enough strength to maintain the new connection in the cycle of actions without the initial irritation that triggered it.

Hence, when the bark is given at stated intervals for the cure of intermittent fevers, if sixty grains of it be given every three hours for the twenty-four hours preceding the expected paroxysm, so as to stimulate the defective part of the system into action, and by that means to prevent the torpor or quiescence of the fibres, which constitutes the cold fit; much less than half the quantity, given before the time at which another paroxysm of quiescence would have taken place, will be sufficient to prevent it; because now the sensorial power, termed association, acts in a twofold manner. First, in respect to the period of the catenation in which the cold fit was produced, which is now dissevered by the stronger stimulus of the first doses of the bark; and, secondly, because each dose of bark being repeated at periodical times, has its effect increased by the sensorial faculty of association being combined with that of irritation.

Therefore, when the bark is administered at regular intervals to treat intermittent fevers, taking sixty grains every three hours for the twenty-four hours before the expected fever attack helps stimulate the affected part of the body into action. This approach prevents the sluggishness or inactivity of the fibers that causes the cold phase. Much less than half that amount, given just before when another cold phase would typically occur, will be enough to stop it. This happens because the mind's ability to make connections, called association, works in two ways. First, it relates to the time during which the cold phase was triggered, which is now broken by the stronger effects of the initial doses of bark. Second, since each dose of bark is given at regular intervals, its impact is amplified by the combination of the mind's associative power and the irritation caused by the medicine.

Now, when sixty grains of Peruvian bark are taken twice a day, suppose at ten o'clock and at six, for a fortnight, the irritation excited by this additional stimulus becomes a part of the diurnal circle of actions, and will at length carry on the increased action of the system without the assistance of the stimulus of the bark. On this theory the bitter medicines, chalybeates, and opiates in appropriated doses, exhibited for a fortnight, give permanent strength to pale feeble children, and other weak constitutions.

Now, when sixty grains of Peruvian bark are taken twice a day, say at ten o'clock and six, for two weeks, the irritation caused by this extra stimulus becomes part of the daily routine of actions and will eventually maintain the increased activity of the body without needing the bark’s stimulus. Based on this idea, bitter medicines, iron supplements, and opioids in suitable doses, administered for two weeks, provide lasting strength to pale, weak children and other fragile individuals.

5. When a defect of stimulus, as of heat, recurs at certain diurnal intervals, which induces some torpor or quiescence of a part of the system, the diurnal catenation of actions becomes disordered, and a new association with this link of torpid action is formed; on the next period the quantity of quiescence will be increased, suppose the same defect of stimulus to recur, because now the new association conspires with the defective irritation in introducing the torpid action of this part of the diurnal catenation. In this manner many fever-fits commence, where the patient is for some days indisposed at certain hours, before the cold paroxysm of fever is completely formed. See Sect. XVII. 3. 3. on Catenation of Animal Motions.

5. When a defect of stimulus, as of heat, recurs at certain diurnal intervals, which induces some torpor or quiescence of a part of the system, the diurnal catenation of actions becomes disordered, and a new association with this link of torpid action is formed; on the next period the quantity of quiescence will be increased, suppose the same defect of stimulus to recur, because now the new association conspires with the defective irritation in introducing the torpid action of this part of the diurnal catenation. In this manner many fever-fits commence, where the patient is for some days indisposed at certain hours, before the cold paroxysm of fever is completely formed. See Sect. XVII. 3. 3. on Catenation of Animal Motions.

6. If a stimulus, which at first excited the affected organ into so great exertion as to produce sensation, be continued for a certain time, it will cease to produce sensation both then and when repeated, though the irritative motions in consequence of it may continue or be re-excited.

6. If a stimulus that initially causes the affected organ to work hard enough to create sensation is applied for a certain period, it will stop producing sensation both while it's applied and when it's repeated, even though the reactive movements as a result of it may continue or be triggered again.

Many catenations of irritative motions were at first succeeded by sensation, as the apparent motions of objects when we walk past them, and probably the vital motions themselves in the early state of our existence. But as those sensations were followed by no movements of the system in consequence of them, they gradually ceased to be produced, not being joined to any succeeding link of catenation. Hence contagious matter, which has for some weeks stimulated the system into great and permanent sensation, ceases afterwards to produce general sensation, or inflammation, though it may still induce topical irritations. See Sect. XXXIII. 2. 8. XIX. 9.

Many catenations of irritative motions were at first succeeded by sensation, as the apparent motions of objects when we walk past them, and probably the vital motions themselves in the early state of our existence. But as those sensations were followed by no movements of the system in consequence of them, they gradually ceased to be produced, not being joined to any succeeding link of catenation. Hence contagious matter, which has for some weeks stimulated the system into great and permanent sensation, ceases afterwards to produce general sensation, or inflammation, though it may still induce topical irritations. See Sect. XXXIII. 2. 8. XIX. 9.

Our absorbent system then seems to receive those contagious matters, which it has before experienced, in the same manner as it imbibes common moisture or other fluids; that is, without being thrown into so violent action as to produce sensation; the consequence of which is an increase of daily energy or activity, till inflammation and its consequences succeed.

Our absorbent system appears to take in those infectious substances, just like it absorbs regular moisture or other liquids; that is, without reacting so strongly that it causes sensation. As a result, there is an increase in daily energy or activity until inflammation and its effects set in.

7. If a stimulus excites an organ into such violent contractions as to produce sensation, the motions of which organ had not usually produced sensation, this new sensorial power, added to the irritation occasioned by the stimulus, increases the activity of the organ. And if this activity be catenated with the diurnal circle of actions, an increasing inflammation is produced; as in the evening paroxysms of small-pox, and other fevers with inflammation. And hence schirrous tumours, tendons and membranes, and probably the arteries themselves become inflamed, when they are strongly stimulated.

7. If a stimulus causes an organ to contract violently enough to create sensation, even when it typically wouldn’t, this new sensory response, combined with the irritation from the stimulus, boosts the organ's activity. If this heightened activity aligns with the daily rhythm of bodily functions, it can lead to increased inflammation, like in the evening flare-ups of smallpox and other inflammatory fevers. As a result, hard tumors, tendons, membranes, and likely even the arteries themselves can become inflamed when they are intensely stimulated.

IV. Of Stimulus greater than natural.

IV. Of Stimulus greater than natural.

1. A quantity of stimulus greater than natural, producing an increased exertion of sensorial power, whether that exertion be in the mode of irritation, sensation, volition, or association, diminishes the general quantity of it. This fact is observable in the progress of intoxication, as the increased quantity or energy of the irritative motions, owing to the stimulus of vinous spirit, introduces much pleasurable sensation into the system, and much exertion of muscular or sensual motions in consequence of this increased sensation; the voluntary motions, and even the associate ones, become much impaired or diminished; and delirium and staggering succeed. See Sect. XXI. on Drunkenness. And hence the great prostration of the strength of the locomotive muscles in some fevers, is owing to the exhaustion of sensorial power by the increased action of the arterial system.

1. A quantity of stimulus greater than natural, producing an increased exertion of sensorial power, whether that exertion be in the mode of irritation, sensation, volition, or association, diminishes the general quantity of it. This fact is observable in the progress of intoxication, as the increased quantity or energy of the irritative motions, owing to the stimulus of vinous spirit, introduces much pleasurable sensation into the system, and much exertion of muscular or sensual motions in consequence of this increased sensation; the voluntary motions, and even the associate ones, become much impaired or diminished; and delirium and staggering succeed. See Sect. XXI. on Drunkenness. And hence the great prostration of the strength of the locomotive muscles in some fevers, is owing to the exhaustion of sensorial power by the increased action of the arterial system.

In like manner a stimulus greater than natural, applied to a part of the system, increases the exertion of sensorial power in that part, and diminishes it in some other part. As in the commencement of scarlet fever, it is usual to see great redness and heat on the faces and breasts of children, while at the same time their feet are colder than natural; partial heats are observable in other fevers with debility, and are generally attended with torpor or quiescence of some other part of the system. But these partial exertions of sensorial power are sometimes attended with increased partial exertions in other parts of the system, which sympathize with them, as the flushing of the face after a full meal. Both these therefore are to be ascribed to sympathetic associations, explained in Sect. XXXV. and not to general exhaustion or accumulation of sensorial power.

In like manner a stimulus greater than natural, applied to a part of the system, increases the exertion of sensorial power in that part, and diminishes it in some other part. As in the commencement of scarlet fever, it is usual to see great redness and heat on the faces and breasts of children, while at the same time their feet are colder than natural; partial heats are observable in other fevers with debility, and are generally attended with torpor or quiescence of some other part of the system. But these partial exertions of sensorial power are sometimes attended with increased partial exertions in other parts of the system, which sympathize with them, as the flushing of the face after a full meal. Both these therefore are to be ascribed to sympathetic associations, explained in Sect. XXXV. and not to general exhaustion or accumulation of sensorial power.

2. A quantity of stimulus greater than natural, producing an increased exertion of sensorial power in any particular organ, diminishes the quantity of it in that organ. This appears from the contractions of animal fibres being not so easily excited by a less stimulus after the organ has been subjected to a greater. Thus after looking at any luminous object of a small size, as at the setting sun, for a short time, so as not much to fatigue the eye, this part of the retina becomes less sensible to smaller quantities of light; hence when the eyes are turned on other less luminous parts of the sky, a dark spot is seen resembling the shape of the sun, or other luminous object which we last behold. See Sect. XL. No. 2.

2. A quantity of stimulus greater than natural, producing an increased exertion of sensorial power in any particular organ, diminishes the quantity of it in that organ. This appears from the contractions of animal fibres being not so easily excited by a less stimulus after the organ has been subjected to a greater. Thus after looking at any luminous object of a small size, as at the setting sun, for a short time, so as not much to fatigue the eye, this part of the retina becomes less sensible to smaller quantities of light; hence when the eyes are turned on other less luminous parts of the sky, a dark spot is seen resembling the shape of the sun, or other luminous object which we last behold. See Sect. XL. No. 2.

Thus we are some time before we can distinguish objects in an obscure room after coming from bright day-light, though the iris presently contracts itself. We are not able to hear weak sounds after loud ones. And the stomachs of those who have been much habituated to the stronger stimulus of fermented or spirituous liquors, are not excited into due action by weaker ones.

So it takes a while for us to see objects in a dim room after coming in from bright daylight, even though our pupils quickly adjust. We also can't hear faint sounds after being exposed to loud ones. And people who are used to stronger drinks, like beer or spirits, don't react properly to lighter ones.

3. A quantity of stimulus something greater than the last mentioned, or longer continued, induces the organ into spasmodic action, which ceases and recurs alternately. Thus on looking for a time on the setting sun, so as not greatly to fatigue the sight, a yellow spectrum is seen when the eyes are closed and covered, which continues for a time, and then disappears and recurs repeatedly before it entirely vanishes. See Sect. XL. No. 5. Thus the action of vomiting ceases and is renewed by intervals, although the emetic drug is thrown up with the first effort. A tenesmus continues by intervals some time after the exclusion of acrid excrement; and the pulsations of the heart of a viper are said to continue some time after it is cleared from its blood.

3. A quantity of stimulus something greater than the last mentioned, or longer continued, induces the organ into spasmodic action, which ceases and recurs alternately. Thus on looking for a time on the setting sun, so as not greatly to fatigue the sight, a yellow spectrum is seen when the eyes are closed and covered, which continues for a time, and then disappears and recurs repeatedly before it entirely vanishes. See Sect. XL. No. 5. Thus the action of vomiting ceases and is renewed by intervals, although the emetic drug is thrown up with the first effort. A tenesmus continues by intervals some time after the exclusion of acrid excrement; and the pulsations of the heart of a viper are said to continue some time after it is cleared from its blood.

In these cases the violent contractions of the fibres produce pain according to law 4; and this pain constitutes an additional kind or quantity of excitement, which again induces the fibres into contraction, and which painful excitement is again renewed, and again induces contractions of the fibres with gradually diminishing effect.

In these situations, the intense tightening of the fibers causes pain in line with law 4; this pain creates an extra type or level of stimulation, which once more triggers the fibers to contract, and this painful stimulation is renewed again, causing the fibers to contract with gradually decreasing intensity.

4. A quantity of stimulus greater than that last mentioned, or longer continued, induces the antagonist muscles into spasmodic action. This is beautifully illustrated by the ocular spectra described in Sect. XL. No. 6. to which the reader is referred. From those experiments there is reason to conclude that the fatigued part of the retina throws itself into a contrary mode of action like oscitation or pandiculation, as soon as the stimulus, which has fatigued it, is withdrawn; but that it still remains liable to be excited into action by any other colours except the colour with which it has been fatigued. Thus the yawning and stretching the limbs after a continued action or attitude seems occasioned by the antagonist muscles being stimulated by their extension during the contractions of those in action, or in the situation in which that action last left them.

4. A quantity of stimulus greater than that last mentioned, or longer continued, induces the antagonist muscles into spasmodic action. This is beautifully illustrated by the ocular spectra described in Sect. XL. No. 6. to which the reader is referred. From those experiments there is reason to conclude that the fatigued part of the retina throws itself into a contrary mode of action like oscitation or pandiculation, as soon as the stimulus, which has fatigued it, is withdrawn; but that it still remains liable to be excited into action by any other colours except the colour with which it has been fatigued. Thus the yawning and stretching the limbs after a continued action or attitude seems occasioned by the antagonist muscles being stimulated by their extension during the contractions of those in action, or in the situation in which that action last left them.

5. A quantity of stimulus greater than the last, or longer continued, induces variety of convulsions or fixed spasms either of the affected organ or of the moving fibres in the other parts of the body. In respect to the spectra in the eye, this is well illustrated in No. 7 and 8, of Sect. XL. Epileptic convulsions, as the emprosthotonos and opisthotonos, with the cramp of the calf of the leg, locked jaw, and other cataleptic fits, appear to originate from pain, as some of these patients scream aloud before the convulsion takes place; which seems at first to be an effort to relieve painful sensation, and afterwards an effort to prevent it.

5. A quantity of stimulus greater than the last, or longer continued, induces variety of convulsions or fixed spasms either of the affected organ or of the moving fibres in the other parts of the body. In respect to the spectra in the eye, this is well illustrated in No. 7 and 8, of Sect. XL. Epileptic convulsions, as the emprosthotonos and opisthotonos, with the cramp of the calf of the leg, locked jaw, and other cataleptic fits, appear to originate from pain, as some of these patients scream aloud before the convulsion takes place; which seems at first to be an effort to relieve painful sensation, and afterwards an effort to prevent it.

In these cases the violent contractions of the fibres produce so much pain, as to constitute a perpetual excitement; and that in so great a degree as to allow but small intervals of relaxation of the contracting fibres as in convulsions, or no intervals at all as in fixed spasms.

In these situations, the intense tightening of the fibers causes so much pain that it creates constant agitation; this occurs to such an extent that there are only brief moments of relaxation for the contracting fibers, like in convulsions, or none at all, as seen in fixed spasms.

6. A quantity of stimulus greater than the last, or longer continued, produces a paralysis of the organ. In many cases this paralysis is only a temporary effect, as on looking long on a small area of bright red silk placed on a sheet of white paper on the floor in a strong light, the red silk gradually becomes paler, and at length disappears; which evinces that a part of the retina, by being violently excited, becomes for a time unaffected by the stimulus of that colour. Thus cathartic medicines, opiates, poisons, contagious matter, cease to influence our system after it has been habituated to the use of them, except by the exhibition of increased quantities of them; our fibres not only become unaffected by stimuli, by which they have previously been violently irritated, as by the matter of the small-pox or measles; but they also become unaffected by sensation, where the violent exertions, which disabled them, were in consequence of too great quantity of sensation. And lastly the fibres, which become disobedient to volition, are probably disabled by their too violent exertions in consequence of too great a quantity of volition.

6. A stronger or longer-lasting stimulus leads to the paralysis of the organ. In many instances, this paralysis is just a temporary effect; for example, if you stare at a small patch of bright red silk on a sheet of white paper under strong light, the red silk gradually looks lighter and eventually disappears. This shows that a part of the retina, after being intensely stimulated, becomes temporarily unresponsive to that color's stimulus. Similarly, cathartic medicines, opiates, poisons, and contagious materials stop affecting our system once we get used to them, except when larger amounts are used; our fibers not only become unresponsive to stimuli that previously irritated them, like those from smallpox or measles, but they also become unresponsive to sensations when the intense reactions came from too much sensory input. Lastly, the fibers that stop responding to our will are likely impaired by their excessive activity due to too much exertion of will.

After every exertion of our fibres a temporary paralysis succeeds, whence the intervals of all muscular contractions, as mentioned in No. 3 and 4 of this Section; the immediate cause of these more permanent kinds of paralysis is probably owing in the same manner to the too great exhaustion of the spirit of animation in the affected part; so that a stronger stimulus is required, or one of a different kind from that, which occasioned those too violent contractions, to again excite the affected organ into activity; and if a stronger stimulus could be applied, it must again induce paralysis.

After every strain on our muscles, there’s a temporary paralysis that follows, which explains the breaks in all muscular contractions, as noted in No. 3 and 4 of this Section. The immediate cause of these more lasting types of paralysis is likely due to the excessive fatigue of the energy in the affected area; which means a stronger stimulus, or one that's different from what caused those intense contractions, is needed to reactivate the affected organ. However, if a stronger stimulus could be applied, it would once again lead to paralysis.

For these powerful stimuli excite pain at the same time, that they produce irritation; and this pain not only excites fibrous motions by its stimulus, but it also produces volition; and thus all these stimuli acting at the same time, and sometimes with the addition of their associations, produce so great exertion as to expend the whole of the sensorial power in the affected fibres.

For these strong stimuli trigger pain at the same time that they cause irritation; and this pain not only stimulates nerve activity but also creates a desire to act; thus, all these stimuli working together, sometimes accompanied by their associations, generate such intense effort that it drains all the sensory energy from the affected fibers.

V. Of Stimulus less than natural.

V. Of Stimuli less than natural.

1. A quantity of stimulus less than natural, producing a decreased exertion of sensorial power, occasions an accumulation of the general quantity of it. This circumstance is observable in the hemiplagia, in which the patients are perpetually moving the muscles, which are unaffected. On this account we awake with greater vigour after sleep, because during so many hours, the great usual expenditure of sensorial power in the performance of voluntary actions, and in the exertions of our organs of sense, in consequence of the irritations occasioned by external objects had been suspended, and a consequent accumulation had taken place.

1. A lower-than-normal amount of stimulus, which leads to reduced use of our sensory abilities, results in a buildup of those abilities. This can be seen in hemiplegia, where patients constantly move the unaffected muscles. This is why we wake up feeling more energized after sleep; during those hours, the usual high demand for our sensory power—used for voluntary actions and the stimulation of our senses from external objects—was paused, allowing for a buildup to occur.

In like manner the exertion of the sensorial power less than natural in one part of the system, is liable to produce an increase of the exertion of it in some other part. Thus by the action of vomiting, in which the natural exertion of the motions of the stomach are destroyed or diminished, an increased absorption of the pulmonary and cellular lymphatics is produced, as is known by the increased absorption of the fluid deposited in them in dropsical cases. But these partial quiescences of sensorial power are also sometimes attended with other partial quiescences, which sympathize with them, as cold and pale extremities from hunger. These therefore are to be ascribed to the associations of sympathy explained in Sect. XXXV. and not to the general accumulation of sensorial power.

In like manner the exertion of the sensorial power less than natural in one part of the system, is liable to produce an increase of the exertion of it in some other part. Thus by the action of vomiting, in which the natural exertion of the motions of the stomach are destroyed or diminished, an increased absorption of the pulmonary and cellular lymphatics is produced, as is known by the increased absorption of the fluid deposited in them in dropsical cases. But these partial quiescences of sensorial power are also sometimes attended with other partial quiescences, which sympathize with them, as cold and pale extremities from hunger. These therefore are to be ascribed to the associations of sympathy explained in Sect. XXXV. and not to the general accumulation of sensorial power.

2. A quantity of stimulus less than natural, applied to fibres previously accustomed to perpetual stimulus, is succeeded by accumulation of sensorial power in the affected organ. The truth of this proposition is evinced, because a stimulus less than natural, if it be somewhat greater than that above mentioned, will excite the organ so circumstanced into violent activity. Thus on a frosty day with wind, the face of a person exposed to the wind is at first pale and shrunk; but on turning the face from the wind, it becomes soon of a glow with warmth and flushing. The glow of the skin in emerging from the cold-bath is owing to the same cause.

2. A stimulus that's less intense than normal, applied to fibers that are used to constant stimulation, leads to a buildup of sensory power in the affected area. This idea is clear because a stimulus that's still below normal, but slightly stronger than the one mentioned earlier, can trigger the organ into intense activity. For example, on a cold, windy day, a person’s face exposed to the wind is initially pale and drawn; however, when they turn away from the wind, their face quickly becomes warm and flushed. The warmth and color of the skin after coming out of a cold bath are due to the same reason.

It does not appear, that an accumulation of sensorial power above the natural quantity is acquired by those muscles, which are not subject to perpetual stimulus, as the locomotive muscles: these, after the greatest fatigue, only acquire by rest their usual aptitude to motion; whereas the vascular system, as the heart and arteries, after a short quiescence, are thrown into violent action by their natural quantity of stimulus.

It doesn’t seem that muscles not constantly stimulated, like the locomotor muscles, can gain more sensory power than what’s natural. These muscles, after intense fatigue, regain their usual ability to move only after resting. In contrast, the vascular system, like the heart and arteries, jumps into intense action quickly after a brief period of rest due to their natural level of stimulation.

Nevertheless by this accumulation of sensorial power during the application of decreased stimulus, and by the exhaustion of it during the action of increased stimulus, it is wisely provided, that the actions of the vascular muscles and organs of sense are not much deranged by small variations of stimulus; as the quantity of sensorial power becomes in some measure inversely as the quantity of stimulus.

Nevertheless, through the buildup of sensory power during reduced stimulus and its depletion during heightened stimulus, it is smartly arranged that the actions of the vascular muscles and sensory organs are not significantly disturbed by minor changes in stimulus; this is because the amount of sensory power is somewhat inversely related to the amount of stimulus.

3. A quantity of stimulus less than that mentioned above, and continued for some time, induces pain in the affected organ, as the pain of cold in the hands, when they are immersed in snow, is owing to a deficiency of the stimulation of heat. Hunger is a pain from the deficiency of the stimulation of food. Pain in the back at the commencement of ague-fits, and the head-achs which attend feeble people, are pains from defect of stimulus, and are hence relieved by opium, essential oils, spirit of wine.

3. A level of stimulation that is less than what’s mentioned above, and lasts for a while, causes pain in the affected area, similar to how the cold pain in your hands when they’re in snow is due to not enough heat stimulation. Hunger is a type of pain caused by a lack of food stimulation. Back pain at the start of chills, and headaches that affect weak individuals, are pains caused by insufficient stimulation, and can be relieved by opium, essential oils, or alcohol.

As the pains, which originate from defect of stimulus, only occur in those parts of the system, which have been previously subjected to perpetual stimulus; and as an accumulation of sensorial power is produced in the quiescent organ along with the pain, as in cold or hunger, there is reason to believe, that the pain is owing to the accumulation of sensorial power. For, in the locomotive muscles, in the retina of the eye, and other organs of senses, no pain occurs from the absence of stimulus, nor any great accumulation of sensorial power beyond their natural quantity, since these organs have not been used to a perpetual supply of it. There is indeed a greater accumulation occurs in the organ of vision after its quiescence, because it is subject to more constant stimulus.

Since the pain that comes from a lack of stimulation only happens in parts of the system that have been consistently stimulated before, and since there’s a buildup of sensory power in the inactive organ along with the pain, similar to feelings of cold or hunger, we can assume that the pain is due to this buildup of sensory power. In the muscles used for movement, in the retina of the eye, and in other sensory organs, pain doesn't occur from the absence of stimulation, nor is there a significant buildup of sensory power beyond their usual amount, because these organs aren’t accustomed to constant stimulation. However, a greater buildup does happen in the organ of vision after it has been inactive because it is exposed to more constant stimulation.

4. A certain quantity of stimulus less than natural induces the moving organ into feebler and more frequent contractions, as mentioned in No. I. 4. of this Section. For each contraction moving through a less space, or with less force, that is, with less expenditure of the spirit of animation, is sooner relaxed, and the spirit of animation derived at each interval into the acting fibres being less, these intervals likewise become shorter. Hence the tremours of the hands of people accustomed to vinous spirit, till they take their usual stimulus; hence the quick pulse in fevers attended with debility, which is greater than in fevers attended with strength; in the latter the pulse seldom beats above 120 times in a minute, in the former it frequently exceeds 140.

4. A certain quantity of stimulus less than natural induces the moving organ into feebler and more frequent contractions, as mentioned in No. I. 4. of this Section. For each contraction moving through a less space, or with less force, that is, with less expenditure of the spirit of animation, is sooner relaxed, and the spirit of animation derived at each interval into the acting fibres being less, these intervals likewise become shorter. Hence the tremours of the hands of people accustomed to vinous spirit, till they take their usual stimulus; hence the quick pulse in fevers attended with debility, which is greater than in fevers attended with strength; in the latter the pulse seldom beats above 120 times in a minute, in the former it frequently exceeds 140.

It must be observed, that in this and the two following articles the decreased action of the system is probably more frequently occasioned by deficiency in the quantity of sensorial power, than in the quantity of stimulus. Thus those feeble constitutions which have large pupils of their eyes, and all who labour under nervous fevers, seem to owe their want of natural quantity of activity in the system to the deficiency of sensorial power; since, as far as can be seen, they frequently possess the natural quantity of stimulus.

It should be noted that in this and the next two articles, the reduced function of the system is likely caused more often by a lack of sensorial power than by a lack of stimulus. For example, those with weak constitutions who have large pupils and anyone suffering from nervous fevers appear to lack the natural level of activity in their systems due to insufficient sensorial power, since they often seem to have the normal amount of stimulus.

5. A certain quantity of stimulus, less than that above mentioned, inverts the order of successive fibrous contractions; as in vomiting the vermicular motions of the stomach and duodenum are inverted, and their contents ejected, which is probably owing to the exhaustion of the spirit of animation in the acting muscles by a previous excessive stimulus, as by the root of ipecacuanha, and the consequent defect of sensorial power. The same retrograde motions affect the whole intestinal canal in ileus; and the œsophagus in globus hystericus. See this further explained in Sect. XXIX. No. 11. on Retrograde Motions.

5. A certain quantity of stimulus, less than that above mentioned, inverts the order of successive fibrous contractions; as in vomiting the vermicular motions of the stomach and duodenum are inverted, and their contents ejected, which is probably owing to the exhaustion of the spirit of animation in the acting muscles by a previous excessive stimulus, as by the root of ipecacuanha, and the consequent defect of sensorial power. The same retrograde motions affect the whole intestinal canal in ileus; and the œsophagus in globus hystericus. See this further explained in Sect. XXIX. No. 11. on Retrograde Motions.

I must observe, also, that something similar happens in the production of our ideas, or sensual motions, when they are too weakly excited; when any one is thinking intensely about one thing, and carelessly conversing about another, he is liable to use the word of a contrary meaning to that which he designed, as cold weather for hot weather, summer for winter.

I should point out that a similar thing occurs in how we come up with our ideas or sensory reactions when they are not strongly stimulated. When someone is deeply focused on one topic and casually discussing another, they might mistakenly use a word that means the opposite of what they intended, like saying "cold weather" when they meant "hot weather," or "summer" when they meant "winter."

6. A certain quantity of stimulus, less than that above mentioned, is succeeded by paralysis, first of the voluntary and sensitive motions, and afterwards of those of irritation, and of association, which constitutes death.

6. A certain amount of stimulation, less than what was mentioned above, leads to paralysis, first affecting voluntary and sensory movements, and later impacting those related to irritation and association, which results in death.

VI. Cure of increased Exertion.

VI. Treatment for increased effort.

1. The cure, which nature has provided for the increased exertion of any part of the system, consists in the consequent expenditure of the sensorial power. But as a greater torpor follows this exhaustion of sensorial power, as explained in the next paragraph, and a greater exertion succeeds this torpor, the constitution frequently sinks under these increasing librations between exertion and quiescence; till at length complete quiescence, that is, death, closes the scene.

1. The remedy that nature offers for the increased activity of any part of the body is the resulting use of sensory energy. However, since a deeper fatigue follows this depletion of sensory energy, as explained in the next paragraph, and a greater effort comes after this fatigue, the body often struggles under these increasing swings between activity and rest; ultimately, complete rest, which is death, ends the chapter.

For, during the great exertion of the system in the hot fit of fever, an increase of stimulus is produced from the greater momentum of the blood, the greater distention of the heart and arteries, and the increased production of heat, by the violent actions of the system occasioned by this augmentation of stimulus, the sensorial power becomes diminished in a few hours much beneath its natural quantity, the vessels at length cease to obey even these great degrees of stimulus, as shewn in Sect. XL. 9. 1. and a torpor of the whole or of a part of the system ensues.

For, during the great exertion of the system in the hot fit of fever, an increase of stimulus is produced from the greater momentum of the blood, the greater distention of the heart and arteries, and the increased production of heat, by the violent actions of the system occasioned by this augmentation of stimulus, the sensorial power becomes diminished in a few hours much beneath its natural quantity, the vessels at length cease to obey even these great degrees of stimulus, as shewn in Sect. XL. 9. 1. and a torpor of the whole or of a part of the system ensues.

Now as this second cold fit commences with a greater deficiency of sensorial power, it is also attended with a greater deficiency of stimulus than in the preceding cold fit, that is, with less momentum of blood, less distention of the heart. On this account the second cold fit becomes more violent and of longer duration than the first; and as a greater accumulation of sensorial power must be produced before the system of vessels will again obey the diminished stimulus, it follows, that the second hot fit of fever will be more violent than the former one. And that unless some other causes counteract either the violent exertions in the hot fit, or the great torpor in the cold fit, life will at length be extinguished by the expenditure of the whole of the sensorial power. And from hence it appears, that the true means of curing fevers must be such as decrease the action of the system in the hot fit, and increase it in the cold fit; that is, such as prevent the too great diminution of sensorial power in the hot fit, and the too great accumulation of it in the cold one.

Now, as the second cold fit begins with an even greater lack of sensory power, it also comes with a larger decrease in stimulus than the first cold fit, meaning there is less blood flow and less pressure on the heart. Because of this, the second cold fit is more intense and lasts longer than the first; and since more sensory power must build up before the vascular system responds to the reduced stimulus again, it follows that the second hot fit of fever will be more severe than the previous one. Unless other factors counteract the intense activity during the hot fit or the significant lethargy during the cold fit, life will eventually be extinguished due to the complete depletion of sensory power. Thus, it seems that the proper way to treat fevers should involve methods that reduce the body's activity in the hot fit and increase it in the cold fit; that is, methods that prevent a severe drop in sensory power during the hot fit and a major buildup of it during the cold fit.

2. Where the exertion of the sensorial powers is much increased, as in the hot fits of fever or inflammation, the following are the usual means of relieving it. Decrease the irritations by blood-letting, and other evacuations; by cold water taken into the stomach, or injected as an enema, or used externally; by cold air breathed into the lungs, and diffused over the skin; with food of less stimulus than the patient has been accustomed to.

2. When the use of the senses is greatly heightened, as during intense fever or inflammation, the typical methods to relieve it are as follows. Reduce irritations through bloodletting and other forms of evacuation; by drinking cold water, using it as an enema, or applying it externally; by inhaling cold air into the lungs and allowing it to circulate over the skin; and by consuming less stimulating food than what the patient is used to.

3. As a cold fit, or paroxysm of inactivity of some parts of the system, generally precedes the hot fit, or paroxysm of exertion, by which the sensorial power becomes accumulated, this cold paroxysm should be prevented by stimulant medicines and diet, as wine, opium, bark, warmth, cheerfulness, anger, surprise.

3. A cold fit, or pause in activity in certain parts of the body, usually comes before the hot fit, or burst of energy, during which the sensory power builds up. To avoid this cold spell, stimulant medicines and foods like wine, opium, bark, warmth, cheerfulness, anger, and surprise should be used.

4. Excite into greater action some other part of the system, by which means the spirit of animation may be in part expended, and thence the inordinate actions of the diseased part may be lessened. Hence when a part of the skin acts violently, as of the face in the eruption of the small-pox, if the feet be cold they should be covered. Hence the use of a blister applied near a topical inflammation. Hence opium and warm bath relieve pains both from excess and defect of stimulus.

4. Encourage more activity in another area of the body, so that the energy can be redirected, which can help reduce the excessive actions of the affected area. For example, when there's intense activity in a part of the skin, like the face during a smallpox rash, if the feet are cold, they should be kept warm. This is why a blister is used near an area of inflammation. Similarly, opium and warm baths can relieve pain caused by either too much or too little stimulation.

5. First increase the general stimulation above its natural quantity, which may in some degree exhaust the spirit of animation, and then decrease the stimulation beneath its natural quantity. Hence after sudorific medicines and warm air, the application of refrigerants may have greater effect, if they could be administered without danger of producing too great torpor of some part of the system; as frequently happens to people in health from coming out of a warm room into the cold air, by which a topical inflammation in consequence of torpor of the mucous membrane of the nostril is produced, and is termed a cold in the head.

5. First, increase the overall stimulation beyond its normal level, which may somewhat drain the energy of vibrancy, and then reduce the stimulation below its normal level. Therefore, after using sweat-inducing medicines and warm air, applying cooling agents might have a greater impact, as long as they can be given without the risk of causing excessive sluggishness in some part of the body; this often occurs to healthy individuals when they go from a warm room into cold air, leading to localized inflammation due to the reduced activity of the nasal membrane, commonly referred to as a cold in the head.

VII. Cure of decreased Exertion.

VII. Treatment for low energy.

1. Where the exertion of the sensorial powers is much decreased, as in the cold fits of fever, a gradual accumulation of the spirit of animation takes place; as occurs in all cases where inactivity or torpor of a part of the system exists; this accumulation of sensorial power increases, till stimuli less than natural are sufficient to throw it into action, then the cold fit ceases; and from the action of the natural stimuli a hot one succeeds with increased activity of the whole system.

1. When the activity of the senses is greatly reduced, as in the cold phases of a fever, there is a gradual buildup of vitality; this happens in all situations where a part of the body is inactive or sluggish. This buildup of sensory energy continues until less intense stimuli become enough to trigger a response, at which point the cold phase ends; then, fueled by normal stimuli, a hot phase follows with increased activity throughout the entire body.

So in fainting fits, or syncope, there is a temporary deficiency of sensorial exertion, and a consequent quiescence of a great part of the system. This quiescence continues, till the sensorial power becomes again accumulated in the torpid organs; and then the usual diurnal stimuli excite the revivescent parts again into action; but as this kind of quiescence continues but a short time compared to the cold paroxysm of an ague, and less affects the circulatory system, a less superabundancy of exertion succeeds in the organs previously torpid, and a less excess of arterial activity. See Sect. XXXIV. 1. 6.

So in fainting fits, or syncope, there is a temporary deficiency of sensorial exertion, and a consequent quiescence of a great part of the system. This quiescence continues, till the sensorial power becomes again accumulated in the torpid organs; and then the usual diurnal stimuli excite the revivescent parts again into action; but as this kind of quiescence continues but a short time compared to the cold paroxysm of an ague, and less affects the circulatory system, a less superabundancy of exertion succeeds in the organs previously torpid, and a less excess of arterial activity. See Sect. XXXIV. 1. 6.

2. In the diseases occasioned by a defect of sensorial exertion, as in cold fits of ague, hysteric complaint, and nervous fever, the following means are those commonly used. 1. Increase the stimulation above its natural quantity for some weeks, till a new habit of more energetic contraction of the fibres is established. This is to be done by wine, opium, bark, steel, given at exact periods, and in appropriate quantities; for if these medicines be given in such quantity, as to induce the least degree of intoxication, a debility succeeds from the useless exhaustion of spirit of animation in consequence of too great exertion of the muscles or organs of sense. To these irritative stimuli should be added the sensitive ones of cheerful ideas, hope, affection.

2. In illnesses caused by a lack of sensory activity, like cold sweats from fever, hysterical conditions, and nervous fever, the following methods are typically used. 1. Increase stimulation above its normal level for a few weeks until the body develops a new habit of stronger muscle contractions. This should be done with wine, opium, quinine, and iron, administered at specific times and in suitable amounts; if these substances are given in amounts that cause even slight intoxication, it can lead to weakness due to the unnecessary draining of vitality from overexertion of the muscles or sensory organs. Along with these stimulating treatments, it's important to include uplifting thoughts, hope, and love.

3. Change the kinds of stimulus. The habits acquired by the constitution depend on such nice circumstances, that when one kind of stimulus ceases to excite the sensorial power into the quantity of exertion necessary to health, it is often sufficient to change the stimulus for another apparently similar in quantity and quality. Thus when wine ceases to stimulate the constitution, opium in appropriate doses supplies the defect; and the contrary. This is also observed in the effects of cathartic medicines, when one loses its power, another, apparently less efficacious, will succeed. Hence a change of diet, drink, and stimulating medicines, is often advantageous in diseases of debility.

3. Change the types of stimuli. The habits developed by the body rely on such specific circumstances that when one type of stimulus stops activating the body's sensory response at the level needed for good health, it's often enough to switch to another stimulus that seems similar in both amount and quality. For example, when wine stops stimulating the body, opium in proper doses can fill the gap, and vice versa. This is also seen with laxative medicines; when one loses its effectiveness, another, which may seem less effective, can take its place. Therefore, changing one's diet, beverages, and stimulating medications is often beneficial in cases of weakness or illness.

4. Stimulate the organs, whose motions are associated with the torpid parts of the system. The actions of the minute vessels of the various parts of the external skin are not only associated with each other, but are strongly associated with those of some of the internal membranes, and particularly of the stomach. Hence when the exertion of the stomach is less than natural, and indigestion and heartburn succeed, nothing so certainly removes these symptoms as the stimulus of a blister on the back. The coldness of the extremities, as of the nose, ears, or fingers, are hence the best indication for the successful application of blisters.

4. Stimulate the organs that are linked to the sluggish parts of the body. The functions of the tiny blood vessels in different areas of the skin are not only connected to each other, but also closely tied to some of the internal membranes, especially those of the stomach. Therefore, when the stomach isn’t working as it should and problems like indigestion and heartburn occur, nothing clears these symptoms more effectively than applying a blister to the back. Coldness in the extremities, like the nose, ears, or fingers, are the best signs for the effective use of blisters.

5. Decrease the stimulus for a time. By lessening the quantity of heat for a minute or two by going into the cold bath, a great accumulation of sensorial power is produced; for not only the minute vessels of the whole external skin for a time become inactive, as appears by their paleness; but the minute vessels of the lungs lose much of their activity also by concert with those of the skin, as appears from the difficulty of breathing at first going into cold water. On emerging from the bath the sensorial power is thrown into great exertion by the stimulus of the common degree of the warmth of the atmosphere, and a great production of animal heat is the consequence. The longer a person continues in the cold bath the greater must be the present inertion of a great part of the system, and in consequence a greater accumulation of sensorial power. Whence M. Pomè recommends some melancholy patients to be kept from two to six hours in spring-water, and in baths still colder.

5. Reduce the stimulation for a while. By lowering the temperature for a minute or two by stepping into a cold bath, you create a significant buildup of sensory energy; not only do the tiny blood vessels in the skin temporarily become inactive, as shown by their paleness, but the small blood vessels in the lungs also decrease their activity in sync with those in the skin, which is evident from the initial difficulty in breathing when entering cold water. When you come out of the bath, the sensory energy kicks into high gear due to the usual warmth of the surrounding air, leading to a substantial increase in body heat. The longer someone stays in the cold bath, the greater the current inactivity of a large part of the body, resulting in an even higher accumulation of sensory energy. Therefore, M. Pomè suggests keeping some patients with depression in spring water for two to six hours or in even colder baths.

6. Decrease the stimulus for a time below the natural, and then increase it above natural. The effect of this process, improperly used, is seen in giving much food, or applying much warmth, to those who have been previously exposed to great hunger, or to great cold. The accumulated sensorial power is thrown into so violent exertion, that inflammations and mortifications supervene, and death closes the catastrophe. In many diseases this method is the most successful; hence the bark in agues produces more certain effect after the previous exhibition of emetics. In diseases attended with violent pain, opium has double the effect, if venesection and a cathartic have been previously used. On this seems to have been founded the successful practice of Sydenham, who used venesection and a cathartic in chlorosis before the exhibition of the bark, steel, and opiates.

6. Temporarily lower the stimulus below its natural level, then increase it above that level. If this method is misapplied, we see issues like giving too much food or applying excessive warmth to those who have suffered from severe hunger or extreme cold. The built-up sensory response can lead to such intense exertion that inflammations and tissue damage occur, potentially resulting in death. In many illnesses, this technique is quite effective; for example, the use of bark for fevers often works better after giving emetics first. In conditions involving severe pain, opium is much more effective if bloodletting and a laxative have been used beforehand. This approach appears to be the basis of Sydenham's successful treatment, where he used bloodletting and a laxative in cases of chlorosis before administering bark, iron, and opiates.

7. Prevent any unnecessary expenditure of sensorial power. Hence in fevers with debility, a decumbent posture is preferred, with silence, little light, and such a quantity of heat as may prevent any chill sensation, or any coldness of the extremities. The pulse of patients in fevers with debility increases in frequency above ten pulsations in a minute on their rising out of bed. For the expenditure of sensorial power to preserve an erect posture of the body adds to the general deficiency of it, and thus affects the circulation.

7. Avoid any unnecessary use of energy. In cases of fever with weakness, lying down is recommended, along with silence, minimal light, and enough warmth to prevent chills or cold extremities. The heart rate of patients with fevers and weakness increases by more than ten beats per minute when they get out of bed. The effort required to maintain an upright position depletes energy further, which negatively impacts circulation.

8. The longer in time and the greater in degree the quiescence or inertion of an organ has been, so that it still retains life or excitability, the less stimulus should at first be applied to it. The quantity of stimulation is a matter of great nicety to determine, where the torpor or quiescence of the fibres has been experienced in a great degree, or for a considerable time, as in cold fits of the ague, in continued fevers with great debility, or in people famished at sea, or perishing with cold. In the two last cases, very minute quantities of food should be first supplied, and very few additional degrees of heat. In the two former cases, but little stimulus of wine or medicine, above what they had been lately accustomed to, should be exhibited, and this at frequent and stated intervals, so that the effect of one quantity may be observed before the exhibition of another.

8. The longer an organ has been inactive or unresponsive while still showing signs of life or sensitivity, the less stimulus should be applied initially. Determining the right amount of stimulation is very delicate, especially when the inactivity of the fibers has been severe or prolonged, as seen in cold episodes of fever, prolonged illnesses with significant weakness, or among people who are starving at sea or suffering from extreme cold. In the latter two cases, only very small amounts of food should be provided at first, along with minimal additional heat. In the former cases, only a small amount of wine or medicine should be given, slightly above what they have recently tolerated, and this should be administered at frequent, regular intervals so the effects of one dose can be evaluated before the next is given.

If these circumstances are not attended to, as the sensorial power becomes accumulated in the quiescent fibres, an inordinate exertion takes place by the increase of stimulus acting on the accumulated quantity of sensorial power, and either the paralysis, or death of the contractile fibres ensues, from the total expenditure of the sensorial power in the affected organ, owing to this increase of exertion, like the debility after intoxication. Or, secondly, the violent exertions above mentioned produce painful sensation, which becomes a new stimulus, and by thus producing inflammation, and increasing the activity of the fibres already too great, sooner exhausts the whole of the sensorial power in the acting organ, and mortification, that is, the death of the part, supervenes.

If these conditions are not addressed, as the sensory energy builds up in the calm fibers, excessive strain occurs due to the increased stimulus acting on this accumulated sensory energy. This can lead to either paralysis or death of the contractile fibers, as the total use of sensory energy in the affected area is depleted because of this increased effort, similar to the weakness felt after intoxication. Alternatively, the intense efforts mentioned above can cause painful sensations, which create a new stimulus. This, in turn, can lead to inflammation and further increase the already excessive activity of the fibers, quickly exhausting all sensory energy in the active organ, resulting in necrosis, or the death of the tissue.

Hence there have been many instances of people, whose limbs have been long benumbed by exposure to cold, who have lost them by mortification on their being too hastily brought to the fire; and of others, who were nearly famished at sea, who have died soon after having taken not more than an usual meal of food. I have heard of two well-attested instances of patients in the cold fit of ague, who have died from the exhibition of gin and vinegar, by the inflammation which ensued. And in many fevers attended with debility, the unlimited use of wine, and the wanton application of blisters, I believe, has destroyed numbers by the debility consequent to too great stimulation, that is, by the exhaustion of the sensorial power by its inordinate exertion.

As a result, there have been many cases of people whose limbs were completely numb from exposure to the cold, who lost those limbs because they were warmed too quickly by the fire. There are also reports of others who were nearly starving at sea and died shortly after eating just a regular meal. I've heard of two well-documented cases of patients experiencing severe chills from ague who died after being given gin and vinegar because of the resulting inflammation. Additionally, in many fevers characterized by weakness, I believe the excessive use of wine and the reckless use of blisters have led to the deaths of many due to the weakness that follows from too much stimulation, which results in the exhaustion of vital energy from overexertion.

Wherever the least degree of intoxication exists, a proportional debility is the consequence; but there is a golden rule by which the necessary and useful quantity of stimulus in fevers with debility may be ascertained. When wine or beer are exhibited either alone or diluted with water, if the pulse becomes slower the stimulus is of a proper quantity; and should be repeated every two or three hours, or when the pulse again becomes quicker.

Wherever there is even a slight level of intoxication, there will be some degree of weakness as a result; however, there is a golden rule for determining the appropriate and beneficial amount of stimulation needed in fevers with weakness. When wine or beer is given, either on its own or mixed with water, if the pulse slows down, then the amount of stimulation is just right; this should be repeated every two or three hours, or whenever the pulse speeds up again.

In the chronical debility brought on by drinking spirituous or fermented liquors, there is another golden rule by which I have successfully directed the quantity of spirit which they may safely lessen, for there is no other means by which they can recover their health. It should be premised, that where the power of digestion in these patients is totally destroyed, there is not much reason to expect a return to healthful vigour.

In the ongoing weakness caused by drinking strong or fermented alcoholic beverages, there’s a key guideline I've used to help people safely reduce their alcohol intake, as this is the only way for them to regain their health. It should be noted that if these individuals have completely lost their ability to digest, we shouldn't expect a significant return to good health.

I have directed several of these patients to omit one fourth part of the quantity of vinous spirit they have been lately accustomed to, and if in a fortnight their appetite increases, they are advised to omit another fourth part; but if they perceive that their digestion becomes impaired from the want of this quantity of spirituous potation, they are advised to continue as they are, and rather bear the ills they have, than risk the encounter of greater. At the same time flesh-meat with or without spice is recommended, with Peruvian bark and steel in small quantities between their meals, and half a grain of opium or a grain, with five or eight grains of rhubarb at night.

I have advised several of these patients to cut back by a quarter of the amount of alcoholic beverage they have been used to recently. If their appetite improves after two weeks, they should cut back another quarter. However, if they notice that their digestion worsens without this amount of alcohol, they should stick to what they're doing and tolerate their current issues instead of risking worse problems. Meanwhile, it's suggested they eat meat, with or without spices, along with small amounts of Peruvian bark and iron between meals, and take half a grain of opium or a grain, along with five or eight grains of rhubarb at night.



SECT. XIII.

OF VEGETABLE ANIMATION.

VEGETABLE ANIMATION.

I. 1. Vegetables are irritable; mimosa, dionæa muscipula. Vegetable secretions. 2. Vegetable buds are inferior animals, are liable to greater or less irritability. II. Stamens and pistils of plants shew marks of sensibility. III. Vegetables possess some degree of volition. IV. Motions of plants are associated like those of animals. V. 1. Vegetable structure like that of animals, their anthers and stigmas are living creatures. Male-flowers of Vallisneria. 2. Whether vegetables, possess ideas? They have organs of sense as of touch and smell, and ideas of external things?

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Plants can be sensitive; mimosa and Venus flytrap. Plant secretions. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Plant buds are simpler organisms and can show varying levels of sensitivity. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. The stamens and pistils of plants exhibit signs of responsiveness. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Plants have some level of will. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. The movements of plants are similar to those of animals. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. The structure of plants resembles that of animals, with their anthers and stigmas being living entities. Male flowers of Vallisneria. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Do plants have thoughts? They possess sensory organs for touch and smell, and could they have perceptions of the outside world?

I. 1. The fibres of the vegetable world, as well as those of the animal, are excitable into a variety of motion by irritations of external objects. This appears particularly in the mimosa or sensitive plant, whose leaves contract on the slightest injury; the dionæa muscipula, which was lately brought over from the marshes of America, presents us with another curious instance of vegetable irritability; its leaves are armed with spines on their upper edge, and are spread on the ground around the stem; when an insect creeps on any of them in its passage to the flower or seed, the leaf shuts up like a steel rat-trap, and destroys its enemy. See Botanic Garden, Part II. note on Silene.

I. 1. The fibers of plants, just like those of animals, can react with different motions when triggered by external stimuli. This is especially visible in the mimosa or sensitive plant, whose leaves curl up at the slightest touch. The dionaea muscipula, recently brought over from the marshes of America, provides another fascinating example of plant sensitivity; its leaves have spines along the top edge and lie flat on the ground around the stem. When an insect crawls across any of these leaves on its way to the flower or seed, the leaf snaps shut like a steel rat trap, capturing its prey. See Botanic Garden, Part II. note on Silene.

The various secretions of vegetables, as of odour, fruit, gum, resin, wax, honey, seem brought about in the same manner as in the glands of animals; the tasteless moisture of the earth is converted by the hop-plant into a bitter juice; as by the caterpillar in the nut-shell the sweet kernel is converted into a bitter powder. While the power of absorption in the roots and barks of vegetables is excited into action by the fluids applied to their mouths like the lacteals and lymphatics of animals.

The different secretions from plants, like scents, fruit, gum, resin, wax, and honey, seem to happen in a similar way as in animal glands. The tasteless moisture in the soil is turned by the hop plant into a bitter juice, just as the caterpillar transforms the sweet kernel in a nut shell into a bitter powder. The ability of plant roots and barks to absorb substances is triggered by the fluids that come into contact with them, similar to the lacteals and lymphatic systems in animals.

2. The individuals of the vegetable world may be considered as inferior or less perfect animals; a tree is a congeries of many living buds, and in this respect resembles the branches of coralline, which are a congeries of a multitude of animals. Each of these buds of a tree has its proper leaves or petals for lungs, produces its viviparous or its oviparous offspring in buds or seeds; has its own roots, which extending down the stem of the tree are interwoven with the roots of the other buds, and form the bark, which is the only living part of the stem, is annually renewed, and is superinduced upon the former bark, which then dies, and with its stagnated juices gradually hardening into wood forms the concentric circles, which we see in blocks of timber.

2. The beings of the plant world can be seen as simpler or less advanced than animals; a tree is a collection of many living buds, and in this way, it’s similar to coral branches, which are made up of numerous animals. Each bud of a tree has its own leaves or petals that function like lungs, produces its young either by live birth or through seeds, and has its own roots. These roots extend down the tree’s stem and intertwine with the roots of the other buds, forming the bark. The bark is the only living part of the stem, renews itself each year, and layers over the old bark, which then dies and, with its stagnant juices, gradually hardens into wood, creating the rings we see in timber.

The following circumstances evince the individuality of the buds of trees. First, there are many trees, whose whole internal wood is perished, and yet the branches are vegete and healthy. Secondly, the fibres of the barks of trees are chiefly longitudinal, resembling roots, as is beautifully seen in those prepared barks, that were lately brought from Otaheita. Thirdly, in horizontal wounds of the bark of trees, the fibres of the upper lip are always elongated downwards like roots, but those of the lower lip do not approach to meet them. Fourthly, if you wrap wet moss round any joint of a vine, or cover it with moist earth, roots will shoot out from it. Fifthly, by the inoculation or engrafting of trees many fruits are produced from one stem. Sixthly, a new tree is produced from a branch plucked from an old one, and set in the ground. Whence it appears that the buds of deciduous trees are so many annual plants, that the bark is a contexture of the roots of each individual bud; and that the internal wood is of no other use but to support them in the air, and that thus they resemble the animal world in their individuality.

The following circumstances demonstrate the individuality of tree buds. First, there are many trees whose entire inner wood is dead, yet their branches remain alive and healthy. Second, the fibers in tree bark are mostly arranged lengthwise, resembling roots, as is beautifully shown in the prepared barks recently brought from Otaheita. Third, in horizontal injuries to the bark of trees, the fibers on the upper side always extend downward like roots, while those on the lower side do not come close to meeting them. Fourth, if you wrap wet moss around any joint of a vine or cover it with moist soil, roots will sprout from it. Fifth, through grafting or inoculating trees, multiple fruits can grow from a single stem. Sixth, a new tree can grow from a branch taken from an old one and planted in the ground. From this, it appears that the buds of deciduous trees function like annual plants, and the bark serves as a network of the roots for each individual bud; the internal wood primarily exists to support them in the air, resembling the animal world in their individuality.

The irritability of plants, like that of animals, appears liable to be increased or decreased by habit; for those trees or shrubs, which are brought from a colder climate to a warmer, put out their leaves and blossoms a fortnight sooner than the indigenous ones.

The sensitivity of plants, similar to that of animals, seems to be affected by their habits; for trees or shrubs that are moved from a colder climate to a warmer one, they sprout their leaves and flowers about two weeks earlier than the local ones.

Professor Kalm, in his Travels in New York, observes that the apple-trees brought from England blossom a fortnight sooner than the native ones. In our country the shrubs, that are brought a degree or two from the north, are observed to flourish better than those, which come from the south. The Siberian barley and cabbage are said to grow larger in this climate than the similar more southern vegetables. And our hoards of roots, as of potatoes and onions, germinate with less heat in spring, after they have been accustomed to the winter's cold, than in autumn after the summer's heat.

Professor Kalm, in his Travels in New York, notes that the apple trees brought from England bloom two weeks earlier than the local ones. In our country, shrubs that come from a bit further north tend to do better than those that come from the south. It’s said that Siberian barley and cabbage grow bigger in this climate than similar vegetables from the south. Also, our stored roots, like potatoes and onions, sprout with less warmth in the spring, after they've experienced the winter's cold, than they do in the fall after the summer's heat.

II. The stamens and pistils of flowers shew evident marks of sensibility, not only from many of the stamens and some pistils approaching towards each other at the season of impregnation, but from many of them closing their petals and calyxes during the cold parts of the day. For this cannot be ascribed to irritation, because cold means a defect of the stimulus of heat; but as the want of accustomed stimuli produces pain, as in coldness, hunger, and thirst of animals, these motions of vegetables in closing up their flowers must be ascribed to the disgreeable sensation, and not to the irritation of cold. Others close up their leaves during darkness, which, like the former, cannot be owing to irritation, as the irritating material is withdrawn.

II. The stamens and pistils of flowers show clear signs of sensitivity, not only because many stamens and some pistils move closer together during the fertilization period, but also because many of them close their petals and calyxes during the cooler parts of the day. This behavior can't be attributed to irritation, since cold represents a lack of the warmth stimulus; however, just as the absence of familiar stimuli causes discomfort in animals, such as cold, hunger, and thirst, the movements of plants in closing their flowers must be linked to an unpleasant sensation, rather than simply to the irritation of cold. Other plants close their leaves at night, which also, like the previous example, cannot be due to irritation since the irritating factor is absent.

The approach of the anthers in many flowers to the stigmas, and of the pistils of some flowers to the anthers, must be ascribed to the passion of love, and hence belongs to sensation, not to irritation.

The way the anthers in many flowers move toward the stigmas, and how the pistils of some flowers reach out to the anthers, should be attributed to the passion of love, and therefore it relates to sensation, not irritation.

III. That the vegetable world possesses some degree of voluntary powers, appears from their necessity to sleep, which we have shewn in Sect. XVIII. to consist in the temporary abolition of voluntary power. This voluntary power seems to be exerted in the circular movement of the tendrils of vines, and other climbing vegetables; or in the efforts to turn the upper surface of their leaves, or their flowers to the light.

III. That the vegetable world possesses some degree of voluntary powers, appears from their necessity to sleep, which we have shewn in Sect. XVIII. to consist in the temporary abolition of voluntary power. This voluntary power seems to be exerted in the circular movement of the tendrils of vines, and other climbing vegetables; or in the efforts to turn the upper surface of their leaves, or their flowers to the light.

IV. The associations of fibrous motions are observable in the vegetable world, as well as in the animal. The divisions of the leaves of the sensitive plant have been accustomed to contract at the same time from the absence of light; hence if by any other circumstance, as a slight stroke or injury, one division is irritated into contraction, the neighbouring ones contract also, from their motions being associated with those of the irritated part. So the various stamina of the class of syngenesia have been accustomed to contract together in the evening, and thence if you stimulate one of them with a pin, according to the experiment of M. Colvolo, they all contract from their acquired associations.

IV. You can see the connections of fibrous movements in both plants and animals. The leaves of the sensitive plant usually close up when there’s no light. So, if one part is triggered to close by something like a light touch or injury, the nearby parts also close because they're linked to the movements of the affected part. Similarly, the stamens of the syngenesia class tend to close together in the evening, and if you poke one with a pin, as shown in M. Colvolo's experiment, they all close due to their learned connections.

To evince that the collapsing of the sensitive plant is not owing to any mechanical vibrations propagated along the whole branch, when a single leaf is struck with the finger, a leaf of it was slit with sharp scissors, and some seconds of time passed before the plant seemed sensible of the injury; and then the whole branch collapsed as far as the principal stem: this experiment was repeated several times with the least possible impulse to the plant.

To show that the wilting of the sensitive plant isn't caused by mechanical vibrations spreading through the entire branch, a single leaf was tapped with a finger while another leaf was cut with sharp scissors. Several seconds passed before the plant reacted to the damage, and then the whole branch drooped down to the main stem. This experiment was repeated multiple times with minimal impact on the plant.

V. 1. For the numerous circumstances in which vegetable buds are analogous to animals, the reader is referred to the additional notes at the end of the Botanic Garden, Part I. It is there shewn, that the roots of vegetables resemble the lacteal system of animals; the sap-vessels in the early spring, before their leaves expand, are analogous to the placental vessels of the fœtus; that the leaves of land-plants resemble lungs, and those of aquatic plants the gills of fish; that there are other systems of vessels resembling the vena portarum of quadrupeds, or the aorta of fish; that the digestive power of vegetables is similar to that of animals converting the fluids, which they absorb, into sugar; that their seeds resemble the eggs of animals, and their buds and bulbs their viviparous offspring. And, lastly, that the anthers and stigmas are real animals, attached indeed to their parent tree like polypi or coral insects, but capable of spontaneous motion; that they are affected with the passion of love, and furnished with powers of reproducing their species, and are fed with honey like the moths and butterflies, which plunder their nectaries. See Botanic Garden, Part I. add. note XXXIX.

V. 1. For the many situations where plant buds are similar to animals, the reader can check the additional notes at the end of the Botanic Garden, Part I. It shows that the roots of plants are like the lymphatic system of animals; the sap vessels in early spring, before their leaves open, are comparable to the placental vessels of a fetus; the leaves of land plants function like lungs, while those of aquatic plants are similar to fish gills; there are other systems of vessels resembling the portal vein of quadrupeds or the aorta of fish; the digestive abilities of plants are akin to those of animals, converting the fluids they take in into sugar; their seeds are like animal eggs, and their buds and bulbs are like their live-born young. Finally, the anthers and stigmas are real small creatures, attached to their parent tree like polyps or coral insects, yet capable of movement; they experience a form of love and have the ability to reproduce their kind, being nourished by nectar like the moths and butterflies that steal from their flowers. See Botanic Garden, Part I. add. note XXXIX.

The male flowers of vallisneria approach still nearer to apparent animality, as they detach themselves from the parent plant, and float on the surface of the water to the female ones. Botanic Garden, Part II. Art. Vallisneria. Other flowers of the classes of monecia and diecia, and polygamia, discharge the fecundating farina, which floating in the air is carried to the stigma of the female flowers, and that at considerable distances. Can this be effected by any specific attraction? or, like the diffusion of the odorous particles of flowers, is it left to the currents of winds, and the accidental miscarriages of it counteracted by the quantity of its production?

The male flowers of vallisneria come even closer to resembling animals as they detach from the parent plant and float on the water's surface to reach the female ones. Botanic Garden, Part II. Art. Vallisneria. Other flowers from the categories of monecia, diecia, and polygamia release their fertilizing pollen, which floats through the air and can travel considerable distances to reach the stigma of the female flowers. Is this possible due to some specific attraction? Or, similar to the way fragrant particles from flowers spread, is it left to the wind currents, with any misdirection balanced out by the sheer amount produced?

2. This leads us to a curious enquiry, whether vegetables have ideas of external things? As all our ideas are originally received by our senses, the question may be changed to, whether vegetables possess any organs of sense? Certain it is, that they possess a sense of heat and cold, another of moisture and dryness, and another of light and darkness; for they close their petals occasionally from the presence of cold, moisture, or darkness. And it has been already shewn, that these actions cannot be performed simply from irritation, because cold and darkness are negative quantities, and on that account sensation or volition are implied, and in consequence a sensorium or union of their nerves. So when we go into the light, we contract the iris; not from any stimulus of the light on the fine muscles of the iris, but from its motions being associated with the sensation of too much light on the retina: which could not take place without a sensorium or center of union of the nerves of the iris with those of vision. See Botanic Garden, Part I. Canto 3. l. 440. note.

2. This raises an interesting question: do plants have perceptions of the outside world? Since all our perceptions come from our senses, we can rephrase the question to ask whether plants have any sensory organs. It's clear that they can sense heat and cold, moisture and dryness, as well as light and darkness; for example, they sometimes close their petals in response to cold, wetness, or darkness. It's already been demonstrated that these actions can't be explained just by irritation, since cold and darkness are negative states. Therefore, sensation and will are implied, which means there must be a nervous system or a connection among their nerves. For instance, when we move into the light, our iris constricts—not just due to the light stimulating the tiny muscles in the iris, but because that movement is linked to the sensation of too much light hitting the retina. This wouldn't happen without a nervous system or a central connection between the iris's nerves and those involved in vision. See Botanic Garden, Part I. Canto 3. l. 440. note.

Besides these organs of sense, which distinguish cold, moisture, and darkness, the leaves of mimosa, and of dionæa, and of drosera, and the stamens of many flowers, as of the berbery, and the numerous class of syngenesia, are sensible to mechanic impact, that is, they possess a sense of touch, as well as a common sensorium; by the medium of which their muscles are excited into action. Lastly, in many flowers the anthers, when mature, approach the stigma, in others the female organ approaches to the male. In a plant of collinsonia, a branch of which is now before me, the two yellow stamens are about three eights of an inch high, and diverge from each other, at an angle of about fifteen degrees, the purple style is half an inch high, and in some flowers is now applied to the stamen on the right hand, and in others to that of the left; and will, I suppose, change place to-morrow in those, where the anthers have not yet effused their powder.

Besides these sense organs, which detect cold, moisture, and darkness, the leaves of mimosa, dionaea, and drosera, as well as the stamens of many flowers like barberry and the wide group of syngenesia, are sensitive to physical contact, meaning they have a sense of touch, along with a general sensory system; through this, their muscles are activated. Lastly, in many flowers, the anthers, when ripe, move closer to the stigma, while in others, the female organ moves towards the male. In a collinsonia plant, with a branch in front of me, the two yellow stamens are about three-eighths of an inch tall and spread out at an angle of about fifteen degrees. The purple style stands half an inch tall and in some flowers leans toward the right stamen, while in others it leans toward the left; I expect it will swap places tomorrow in those where the anthers haven’t yet released their pollen.

I ask, by what means are the anthers in many flowers, and stigmas in other flowers, directed to find their paramours? How do either of them know, that the other exists in their vicinity? Is this curious kind of storge produced by mechanic attraction, or by the sensation of love? The latter opinion is supported by the strongest analogy, because a reproduction of the species is the consequence; and then another organ of sense must be wanted to direct these vegetable amourettes to find each other, one probably analogous to our sense of smell, which in the animal world directs the new-born infant to its source of nourishment, and they may thus possess a faculty of perceiving as well as of producing odours.

I wonder, how do the anthers in many flowers and the stigmas in other flowers manage to find their partners? How do they know the other is nearby? Is this intriguing kind of connection caused by mechanical attraction or by the feeling of love? The latter view seems stronger since it leads to the reproduction of the species; and another sense would be needed for these plant lovers to locate each other, possibly something like our sense of smell, which guides newborn animals to their source of food. They might therefore have the ability to perceive as well as produce scents.

Thus, besides a kind of taste at the extremities of their roots, similar to that of the extremities of our lacteal vessels, for the purpose of selecting their proper food: and besides different kinds of irritability residing in the various glands, which separate honey, wax, resin, and other juices from their blood; vegetable life seems to possess an organ of sense to distinguish the variations of heat, another to distinguish the varying degrees of moisture, another of light, another of touch, and probably another analogous to our sense of smell. To these must be added the indubitable evidence of their passion of love, and I think we may truly conclude, that they are furnished with a common sensorium belonging to each bud and that they must occasionally repeat those perceptions either in their dreams or waking hours, and consequently possess ideas of so many of the properties of the external world, and of their own existence.

Thus, in addition to having a type of taste at the ends of their roots, similar to the ends of our lacteal vessels, which helps them choose the right food, and different types of sensitivity present in various glands that extract honey, wax, resin, and other substances from their fluids; plant life seems to have sensory organs to detect changes in heat, moisture, light, touch, and likely another sense similar to our smell. Additionally, we must recognize the clear signs of their ability to experience love, and I think we can genuinely conclude that they have a shared sensory experience linked to each bud and that they may occasionally revisit those sensations either in their dreams or while awake, which means they possess awareness of many properties of the external world and their own existence.



SECT. XIV.

OF THE PRODUCTION OF IDEAS.

IDEA GENERATION.

I. Of material and immaterial beings. Doctrine of St. Paul. II. 1. Of the sense of touch. Of solidity. 2. Of figure. Motion. Time. Place. Space. Number. 3. Of the penetrability of matter. 4. Spirit of animation possesses solidity, figure, visibility, &c. Of Spirits and angels. 5. The existence of external things. III. Of vision. IV. Of hearing. V. Of smell and taste. VI. Of the organ of sense by which we perceive heat and cold, not by the sense of touch. VII. Of the sense of extension, the whole of the locomotive muscles may be considered as one organ of sense. VIII. Of the senses of hunger, thirst, want of fresh air, suckling children, and lust. IX. Of many other organs of sense belonging to the glands. Of painful sensations from the excess of light, pressure, heat, itching, caustics, and electricity.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. About material and immaterial beings. Doctrine of St. Paul. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. About the sense of touch. About solidity. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. About shape. Motion. Time. Place. Space. Number. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. About the penetrability of matter. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. The spirit of animation has solidity, shape, visibility, etc. About spirits and angels. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. The existence of external things. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. About vision. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. About hearing. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. About smell and taste. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. About the sense through which we perceive heat and cold, not through the sense of touch. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. About the sense of extension; all of the locomotive muscles can be seen as one sense organ. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. About the senses of hunger, thirst, lack of fresh air, nursing infants, and desire. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. About many other sense organs related to glands. About painful sensations from excessive light, pressure, heat, itching, caustics, and electricity.

I. Philosophers have been much perplexed to understand, in what manner we become acquainted with the external world; insomuch that Dr. Berkly even doubted its existence, from having observed (as he thought) that none of our ideas resemble their correspondent objects. Mr. Hume asserts, that our belief depends on the greater distinctness or energy of our ideas from perception; and Mr. Reid has lately contended, that our belief of external objects is an innate principle necessarily joined with our perceptions.

I. Philosophers have long been confused about how we come to know the outside world; so much so that Dr. Berkeley even questioned its existence after noticing (as he believed) that none of our ideas truly reflect their corresponding objects. Mr. Hume argues that our belief relies on the clarity or intensity of our ideas based on perception; while Mr. Reid has recently claimed that our belief in external objects is an inherent principle that is naturally linked to our perceptions.

So true is the observation of the famous Malbranch, "that our senses are not given us to discover the essences of things, but to acquaint us with the means of preserving our existence," (L. I. ch. v.) a melancholy reflection to philosophers!

So true is the observation of the famous Malbranch, "that our senses are not given to us to discover the essences of things, but to help us understand how to preserve our existence," (L. I. ch. v.) a sad thought for philosophers!

Some philosophers have divided all created beings into material and immaterial: the former including all that part of being, which obeys the mechanic laws of action and reaction, but which can begin no motion of itself; the other is the cause of all motion, and is either termed the power of gravity, or of specific attraction, or the spirit of animation. This immaterial agent is supposed to exist in or with matter, but to be quite distinct from it, and to be equally capable of existence, after the matter, which now possesses it, is decomposed.

Some philosophers categorize all created beings into two groups: material and immaterial. The material includes everything that follows the mechanical laws of action and reaction but cannot initiate movement on its own. The immaterial, on the other hand, is the source of all motion and is referred to as the power of gravity, specific attraction, or the spirit of animation. This immaterial agent is thought to exist in or alongside matter, yet it is completely separate from it and is equally capable of existing even after the matter it currently inhabits is broken down.

Nor is this theory ill supported by analogy, since heat, electricity, and magnetism, can be given to or taken from a piece of iron; and must therefore exist, whether separated from the metal, or combined with it. From a parity of reasoning, the spirit of animation, would appear to be capable of existing as well separately from the body as with it.

Nor is this theory unsupported by analogy, since heat, electricity, and magnetism can be added to or removed from a piece of iron; they must therefore exist, whether separate from the metal or combined with it. By the same logic, the spirit of animation seems capable of existing both separately from the body and alongside it.

I beg to be understood, that I do not wish to dispute about words, and am ready to allow, that the powers of gravity, specific attraction, electricity, magnetism, and even the spirit of animation, may consist of matter of a finer kind; and to believe, with St. Paul and Malbranch, that the ultimate cause only of all motion is immaterial, that is God. St. Paul says, "in him we live and move, and have our being;" and, in the 15th chapter to the Corinthians, distinguishes between the psyche or living spirit, and the pneuma or reviving spirit. By the words spirit of animation or sensorial power, I mean only that animal life, which mankind possesses in common with brutes, and in some degree even with vegetables, and leave the consideration of the immortal part of us, which is the object of religion, to those who treat of revelation.

I hope to be clear that I'm not looking to argue about wording. I'm open to the idea that gravity, specific attraction, electricity, magnetism, and even the life force may be made up of a finer kind of matter. I believe, like St. Paul and Malebranche, that the ultimate cause of all motion is immaterial—meaning God. St. Paul says, "in him we live and move, and have our being," and in the 15th chapter to the Corinthians, he highlights the difference between the psyche, or living spirit, and the pneuma, or reviving spirit. When I refer to the life force or sensory power, I mean the animal life that humans share with animals and, to some extent, plants. I’ll leave the discussion about the immortal part of us—which is the focus of religion—to those who study revelation.

II. 1. Of the Sense of Touch.

II. 1. The Sense of Touch.

The first idea we become acquainted with, are those of the sense of touch; for the fœtus must experience some varieties of agitation, and exert some muscular action, in the womb; and may with great probability be supposed thus to gain some ideas of its own figure, of that of the uterus, and of the tenacity of the fluid, that surrounds it, (as appears from the facts mentioned in the succeeding Section upon Instinct.)

The first concepts we encounter are those related to the sense of touch; the fetus must go through various movements and exert some muscle activity in the womb. It's reasonable to assume that this allows the fetus to form some ideas about its own shape, the shape of the uterus, and the consistency of the fluid surrounding it, as discussed in the following section on Instinct.

Many of the organs of sense are confined to a small part of the body, as the nostrils, ear, or eye, whilst the sense of touch is diffused over the whole skin, but exists with a more exquisite degree of delicacy at the extremities of the fingers and thumbs, and in the lips. The sense of touch is thus very commodiously disposed for the purpose of encompassing smaller bodies, and for adapting itself to the inequalities of larger ones. The figure of small bodies seems to be learnt by children by their lips as much as by their fingers; on which account they put every new object to their mouths, when they are satiated with food, as well as when they are hungry. And puppies seem to learn their ideas of figure principally by the lips in their mode of play.

Many of our senses are limited to specific parts of the body, like the nostrils, ears, or eyes, while the sense of touch is spread across the entire skin. However, it is especially sensitive at the tips of our fingers, thumbs, and lips. This sensitivity of touch is really useful for grasping small objects and adjusting to the different shapes of larger ones. Children appear to learn about the shapes of small objects as much through their lips as through their fingers, which is why they tend to put every new item in their mouths, whether they're full or hungry. Likewise, puppies seem to understand the shapes of things mainly through their lips while playing.

We acquire our tangible ideas of objects either by the simple pressure of this organ of touch against a solid body, or by moving our organ of touch along the surface of it. In the former case we learn the length and breadth of the object by the quantity of our organ of touch, that is impressed by it: in the latter case we learn the length and breadth of objects by the continuance of their pressure on our moving organ of touch.

We get our concrete ideas of objects either by simply pressing our sense of touch against a solid surface or by running our sense of touch along that surface. In the first case, we understand the length and width of the object based on how much of our sense of touch is in contact with it; in the second case, we grasp the length and width of objects through the ongoing pressure they exert on our moving sense of touch.

It is hence, that we are very slow in acquiring our tangible ideas, and very slow in recollecting them; for if I now think of the tangible idea of a cube, that is, if I think of its figure, and of the solidity of every part of that figure, I must conceive myself as passing my fingers over it, and seem in some measure to feel the idea, as I formerly did the impression, at the ends of them, and am thus very slow in distinctly recollecting it.

It’s because of this that we take a long time to grasp our concrete ideas and to remember them. If I think about the concrete concept of a cube—its shape and the solidity of every part of that shape—I have to imagine myself running my fingers over it, almost feeling the idea like I used to feel the impression on my fingertips. Because of that, I’m really slow in clearly recalling it.

When a body compresses any part of our sense of touch, what happens? First, this part of our sensorium undergoes a mechanical compression, which is termed a stimulus; secondly, an idea, or contraction of a part of the organ of sense is excited; thirdly, a motion of the central parts, or of the whole sensorium, which is termed sensation, is produced; and these three constitute the perception of solidity.

When a body presses on any part of our sense of touch, what happens? First, that part of our sensory system gets mechanically pressed, which is called a stimulus. Next, an idea or a contraction of a part of the sensory organ is triggered. Then, there’s movement in the central areas or in the entire sensory system, which is referred to as sensation. These three steps together create the perception of solidity.

2. Of Figure, Motion, Time, Place, Space, Number.

2. Of Figure, Motion, Time, Place, Space, Number.

No one will deny, that the medulla of the brain and nerves has a certain figure; which, as it is diffused through nearly the whole of the body, must have nearly the figure of that body. Now it follows, that the spirit of animation, or living principle, as it occupies this medulla, and no other part, (which is evinced by a great variety of cruel experiments on living animals,) it follows, that this spirit of animation has also the same figure as the medulla above described. I appeal to common sense! the spirit of animation acts, Where does it act? It acts wherever there is the medulla above mentioned; and that whether the limb is yet joined to a living animal, or whether it be recently detached from it; as the heart of a viper or frog will renew its contractions, when pricked with a pin, for many minutes of time after its exsection from the body.—Does it act any where else?—No; then it certainly exists in this part of space, and no where else; that is, it hath figure; namely, the figure of the nervous system, which is nearly the figure of the body. When the idea of solidity is excited, as above explained, a part of the extensive organ of touch is compressed by some external body, and this part of the sensorium so compressed exactly resembles in figure the figure of the body that compressed it. Hence, when we acquire the idea of solidity, we acquire at the same time the idea of FIGURE; and this idea of figure, or motion of a part of the organ of touch, exactly resembles in its figure the figure of the body that occasions it; and thus exactly acquaints us with this property of the external world.

No one can deny that the brainstem and nerves have a particular shape, which, since it stretches throughout nearly the entire body, must roughly match the body's shape. Therefore, since the life force, or principle of life, occupies this brainstem and no other part (which is shown through various cruel experiments on living animals), it follows that this life force also shares the same shape as the brainstem described above. I appeal to common sense! The life force acts—where does it act? It acts wherever the brainstem is present, whether the limb is still connected to a living animal or has just been detached from it; for example, the heart of a viper or frog will continue to contract for several minutes after being pricked with a pin, even after being removed from the body. Does it act anywhere else? No; therefore, it certainly exists in this specific area and nowhere else; that is, it has a shape—specifically, the shape of the nervous system, which closely resembles the shape of the body. When the idea of solidity arises, as previously explained, a part of the large touch organ is compressed by some external object, and this part of the sensory system that is compressed precisely resembles in shape the shape of the object that compressed it. Thus, when we develop the idea of solidity, we simultaneously gain the idea of FIGURE; and this idea of figure, or movement of a part of the touch organ, closely resembles in its shape the shape of the object that causes it; and thus accurately informs us about this property of the external world.

Now, as the whole universe with all its parts possesses a certain form or figure, if any part of it moves, that form or figure of the whole is varied: hence, as MOTION is no other than a perpetual variation of figure, our idea of motion is also a real resemblance of the motion that produced it.

Now, since the entire universe and all its parts have a specific shape or form, whenever any part moves, the shape or form of the whole changes. Therefore, since MOVEMENT is essentially a constant change in shape, our understanding of motion actually reflects the motion that caused it.

It may be said in objection to this definition of motion, that an ivory globe may revolve on its axis, and that here will be a motion without change of figure. But the figure of the particle x on one side of this globe is not the same figure as the figure of y on the other side, any more than the particles themselves are the same, though they are similar figures; and hence they cannot change place with each other without disturbing or changing the figure of the whole.

It could be argued against this definition of motion that an ivory ball can spin on its axis, which would mean there’s motion without any change in shape. However, the shape of particle x on one side of the ball is not the same shape as particle y on the other side, just as the particles themselves are not the same, even though they are similar shapes. Therefore, they can't switch places without disrupting or altering the shape of the entire ball.

Our idea of TIME is from the same source, but is more abstracted, as it includes only the comparative velocities of these variations of figure; hence if it be asked, How long was this book in printing? it may be answered, Whilst the sun was passing through Aries.

Our concept of TIME comes from the same source, but it's more abstract because it only considers the relative speeds of these changes in figures. So, if someone asks, How long did it take to print this book? you could say, While the sun was in Aries.

Our idea of PLACE includes only the figure of a group of bodies, not the figures of the bodies themselves. If it be asked where is Nottinghamshire, the answer is, it is surrounded by Derbyshire, Lincolnshire and Leicestershire; hence place is our idea of the figure of one body surrounded by the figures of other bodies.

Our concept of PLACE only includes the shape of a group of bodies, not the shapes of the bodies themselves. If someone asks where Nottinghamshire is, the answer is that it's enclosed by Derbyshire, Lincolnshire, and Leicestershire; thus, place is our idea of one body's shape surrounded by the shapes of other bodies.

The idea of SPACE is a more abstracted idea of place excluding the group of bodies.

The concept of SPACE represents a more abstract notion of place that omits the collection of objects.

The idea of NUMBER includes only the particular arrangements, or distributions of a group of bodies, and is therefore only a more abstracted idea of the parts of the figure of the group of bodies; thus when I say England is divided into forty counties, I only speak of certain divisions of its figure.

The concept of NUMBER only pertains to the specific arrangements or distributions of a collection of entities, making it a more abstract idea about the components of the group's shape. So, when I mention that England is divided into forty counties, I'm just referring to specific divisions of its shape.

Hence arises the certainty of the mathematical sciences, as they explain these properties of bodies, which are exactly resembled by our ideas of them, whilst we are obliged to collect almost all our other knowledge from experiment; that is, by observing the effects exerted by one body upon another.

Thus comes the certainty of mathematics, as it explains the properties of objects that perfectly match our ideas about them, while we have to gather nearly all our other knowledge from experiments; that is, by observing the effects one object has on another.

3. Of the Penetrability of Matter.

3. *Of the Penetrability of Matter.*

The impossibility of two bodies existing together in the same space cannot be deduced from our idea of solidity, or of figure. As soon as we perceive the motions of objects that surround us, and learn that we possess a power to move our own bodies, we experience, that those objects, which excite in us the idea of solidity and of figure, oppose this voluntary movement of our own organs; as whilst I endeavour to compress between my hands an ivory ball into a spheroid. And we are hence taught by experience, that our own body and those, which we touch, cannot exist in the same part of space.

The impossibility of two objects existing in the same space can't be figured out just from our understanding of solidity or shape. As soon as we notice the movement of things around us and realize that we can move our own bodies, we find that those objects, which make us think of solidity and shape, resist our voluntary movements. For example, when I try to squeeze an ivory ball into a different shape between my hands. This teaches us through experience that our body and the objects we touch can't occupy the same space at the same time.

But this by no means demonstrates, that no two bodies can exist together in the same part of space. Galilæo in the preface to his works seems to be of opinion, that matter is not impenetrable; Mr. Michel, and Mr. Boscowich in his Theoria. Philos. Natur. have espoused this hypothesis: which has been lately published by Dr. Priestley, to whom the world is much indebted for so many important discoveries in science. (Hist. of Light and Colours, p. 391.) The uninterrupted passage of light through transparent bodies, of the electric æther through metallic and aqueous bodies, and of the magnetic effluvia through all bodies, would seem to give some probability to this opinion. Hence it appears, that beings may exist without possessing the property of solidity, as well as they can exist without possessing the properties, which excite our smell or taste, and can thence occupy space without detruding other bodies from it; but we cannot become acquainted with such beings by our sense of touch, any more than we can with odours or flavours without our senses of smell and taste.

But this doesn't prove that no two bodies can exist in the same space. Galileo, in the preface to his works, seems to believe that matter is not impenetrable. Mr. Michel and Mr. Boscowich in his Theoria. Philos. Natur. have supported this idea, which has recently been published by Dr. Priestley, to whom the world owes many significant scientific discoveries. (Hist. of Light and Colours, p. 391.) The continuous passage of light through transparent objects, the flow of electric ether through metal and water, and the presence of magnetic fields through all materials seem to lend some weight to this theory. Thus, it appears that entities can exist without knowing the quality of solidity, just as they can exist without properties that stimulate our sense of smell or taste, allowing them to occupy space without pushing other bodies out of it. However, we cannot perceive such entities through our sense of touch, just as we cannot recognize odors or flavors without our senses of smell and taste.

But that any being can exist without existing in space, is to my ideas utterly incomprehensible. My appeal is to common sense. To be implies a when and a where; the one is comparing it with the motions of other beings, and the other with their situations.

But the idea that any being can exist without existing in space is completely incomprehensible to me. I’m appealing to common sense. To be implies a time and a place; one involves comparing it with the movements of other beings, and the other with their locations.

If there was but one object, as the whole creation may be considered as one object, then I cannot ask where it exists? for there are no other objects to compare its situation with. Hence if any one denies, that a being exists in space, he denies, that there are any other beings but that one; for to answer the question, "Where does it exist?" is only to mention the situation of the objects that surround it.

If there is only one thing, since all of creation can be seen as one thing, then I can't ask where it exists because there are no other things to compare its location with. So, if someone claims that a being doesn't exist in space, they are also saying that no other beings exist aside from that one. To answer the question, "Where does it exist?" is simply to describe the position of the things around it.

In the same manner if it be asked—"When does a being exist?" The answer only specifies the successive motions either of itself, or of other bodies; hence to say, a body exists not in time, is to say, that there is, or was, no motion in the world.

In the same way, if someone asks—"When does a being exist?" The answer just points out the ongoing movements either of itself or of other objects; therefore, to say that a body doesn't exist in time means that there is, or was, no movement in the world.

4. Of the Spirit of Animation.

4. *Of the Spirit of Animation.*

But though there may exist beings in the universe, that have not the property of solidity; that is, which can possess any part of space, at the same time that it is occupied by other bodies; yet there may be other beings, that can assume this property of solidity, or disrobe themselves of it occasionally, as we are taught of spirits, and of angels; and it would seem, that THE SPIRIT OF ANIMATION must be endued with this property, otherwise how could it occasionally give motion to the limbs of animals?—or be itself stimulated into motion by the obtrusions of surrounding bodies, as of light, or odour?

But even though there might be beings in the universe that don’t have the quality of solidity—meaning they can occupy space at the same time as other objects—there could also be other beings that can take on this solid property or shed it from time to time, like we hear about with spirits and angels. It seems that THE ART OF ANIMATION must have this property; otherwise, how could it occasionally cause movement in the limbs of animals? Or be stimulated into motion by the presence of surrounding objects, like light or smell?

If the spirit of animation was always necessarily penetrable, it could not influence or be influenced by the solidity of common matter; they would exist together, but could not detrude each other from the part of space, where they exist; that is, they could not communicate motion to each other. No two things can influence or affect each other, which have not some property common to both of them; for to influence or affect another body is to give or communicate some property to it, that it had not before; but how can one body give that to another, which it does not possess itself?—The words imply, that they must agree in having the power or faculty of possessing some common property. Thus if one body removes another from the part of space, that it possesses, it must have the power of occupying that space itself: and if one body communicates heat or motion to another, it follows, that they have alike the property of possessing heat or motion.

If the essence of animation could always be penetrated, it wouldn’t be able to influence or be influenced by the solidity of ordinary matter; they would coexist, but couldn’t push each other out of the space they occupy; that is, they couldn’t transfer motion to each other. No two things can influence or affect each other unless they share some common property; to influence or affect another body means to give it a property it didn’t have before; but how can one body give something to another that it doesn’t have itself?—The words suggest that they must share the ability or capacity to possess some common property. So, if one body displaces another from the space it occupies, it must have the ability to occupy that space itself: and if one body transfers heat or motion to another, it means they both possess the property of having heat or motion.

Hence the spirit of animation at the time it communicates or receives motion from solid bodies, must itself possess some property of solidity. And in consequence at the time it receives other kinds of motion from light, it must possess that property, which light possesses, to communicate that kind of motion; and for which no language has a name, unless it may be termed Visibility. And at the time it is stimulated into other kinds of animal motion by the particles of sapid and odorous bodies affecting the senses of taste and smell, it must resemble these particles of flavour, and of odour, in possessing some similar or correspondent property; and for which language has no name, unless we may use the words Saporosity and Odorosity for those common properties, which are possessed by our organs of taste and smell, and by the particles of sapid and odorous bodies; as the words Tangibility and Audibility may express the common property possessed by our organs of touch, and of hearing, and by the solid bodies, or their vibrations, which affect those organs.

Therefore, the essence of animation, when it communicates or receives motion from solid objects, must itself have some quality of solidity. As a result, when it receives other types of motion from light, it must have that property which light possesses to convey that kind of motion; a property for which there is no name in language, unless we call it Visibility. Furthermore, when it is triggered into other forms of animal motion by the particles of flavorful and fragrant bodies that engage our senses of taste and smell, it must be similar to these flavor and scent particles, sharing some related property; a property for which language lacks a name, unless we use the terms Saporosity and Odorosity for those common qualities found in our taste and smell organs and the particles from flavorful and fragrant bodies; just as the terms Tangibility and Audibility express the common property found in our organs of touch and hearing, and in the solid objects or their vibrations that affect those organs.

5. Finally, though the figures of bodies are in truth resembled by the figure of the part of the organ of touch, which is stimulated into motion; and that organ resembles the solid body, which stimulates it, in its property of solidity; and though the sense of hearing resembles the vibrations of external bodies in its capability of being stimulated into motion by those vibrations; and though our other organs of sense resemble the bodies, that stimulate them, in their capability of being stimulated by them; and we hence become acquainted with these properties of the external world; yet as we can repeat all these motions of our organs of sense by the efforts of volition, or in consequence of the sensation of pleasure or pain, or by their association with other fibrous motions, as happens in our reveries or in sleep, there would still appear to be some difficulty in demonstrating the existence of any thing external to us.

5. In the end, even though the shapes of our bodies are actually reflected in the shape of the part of the touch organ that gets moved, and that organ shares the solid nature of the object that stimulates it; and even though our sense of hearing mirrors the vibrations of outside objects by being able to respond to those vibrations; and although our other senses also reflect the properties of the objects that affect them, allowing us to learn about these traits of the outside world; there still seems to be some challenge in proving that anything exists outside of ourselves, especially since we can recreate all these sensory actions through our will, or as a result of feelings of pleasure or pain, or by their connection with other nerve movements, like what happens in our dreams or when we sleep.

In our dreams we cannot determine this circumstance, because our power of volition is suspended, and the stimuli of external objects are excluded; but in our waking hours we can compare our ideas belonging to one sense with those belonging to another, and can thus distinguish the ideas occasioned by irritation from those excited by sensation, volition, or association. Thus if the idea of the sweetness of sugar should be excited in our dreams, the whiteness and hardness of it occur at the same time by association; and we believe a material lump of sugar present before us. But if, in our waking hours, the idea of the sweetness of sugar occurs to us, the stimuli of surrounding objects, as the edge of the table, on which we press, or green colour of the grass, on which we tread, prevent the other ideas of the hardness and whiteness of the sugar from being exerted by association. Or if they should occur, we voluntarily compare them with the irritative ideas of the table or grass above mentioned, and detect their fallacy. We can thus distinguish the ideas caused by the stimuli of external objects from those, which are introduced by association, sensation, or volition; and during our waking hours can thus acquire a knowledge of the external world. Which nevertheless we cannot do in our dreams, because we have neither perceptions of external bodies, nor the power of volition to enable us to compare them with the ideas of imagination.

In our dreams, we can't figure out this situation because our ability to make choices is paused, and external stimuli are shut out. However, when we're awake, we can compare our ideas from one sense to those from another, allowing us to differentiate between ideas caused by irritation and those triggered by sensation, choice, or association. For example, if we dream about the sweetness of sugar, the whiteness and hardness of it might come to mind at the same time through association, leading us to believe there’s a real piece of sugar in front of us. But when we're awake and think about the sweetness of sugar, the things around us—like the edge of the table we're leaning on or the green grass we're walking on—stop us from pulling up the other ideas of the sugar's hardness and whiteness through association. If those ideas do come up, we can actively compare them with the disruptive ideas of the table or grass mentioned earlier and realize they're not accurate. This way, we can tell apart the ideas created by external stimuli from those introduced by association, sensation, or choice, which helps us understand the outside world while we are awake. In dreams, however, we can't do this because we lack perceptions of the physical world and the ability to compare them with our imagined ideas.

III. Of Vision.

III. About Vision.

Our eyes observe a difference of colour, or of shade, in the prominences and depressions of objects, and that those shades uniformly vary, when the sense of touch observes any variation. Hence when the retina becomes stimulated by colours or shades of light in a certain form, as in a circular spot; we know by experience, that this is a sign, that a tangible body is before us; and that its figure is resembled by the miniature figure of the part of the organ of vision, that is thus stimulated.

Our eyes notice differences in color or shade in the bumps and indentations of objects, and these shades change consistently when our sense of touch detects any variation. So, when the retina gets stimulated by colors or shades of light in a specific shape, like a circular spot, we know from experience that this indicates a physical object is in front of us, and that its shape is mirrored by the small area of our visual organ that is being stimulated.

Here whilst the stimulated part of the retina resembles exactly the visible figure of the whole in miniature, the various kinds of stimuli from different colours mark the visible figures of the minuter parts; and by habit we instantly recall the tangible figures.

Here, while the activated part of the retina looks just like a small version of the whole visible image, the different types of stimuli from various colors represent the visible figures of the smaller parts; and through habit, we quickly remember the physical shapes.

Thus when a tree is the object of sight, a part of the retina resembling a flat branching figure is stimulated by various shades of colours; but it is by suggestion, that the gibbosity of the tree, and the moss, that fringes its trunk, appear before us. These are ideas of suggestion, which we feel or attend to, associated with the motions of the retina, or irritative ideas, which we do not attend to.

Thus, when we see a tree, a section of the retina that looks like a flat branching shape is activated by different shades of color. However, it's through suggestion that we perceive the bulges of the tree and the moss that edges its trunk. These are suggested ideas that we focus on, linked to the movements of the retina, or reactive ideas that we don’t pay attention to.

So that though our visible ideas resemble in miniature the outline of the figure of coloured bodies, in other respects they serve only as a language, which by acquired associations introduce the tangible ideas of bodies. Hence it is, that this sense is so readily deceived by the art of the painter to our amusement and instruction. The reader will find much very curious knowledge on this subject in Bishop Berkley's Essay on Vision, a work of great ingenuity.

So even though our visible ideas look like a small version of the shapes of colored objects, in other ways they only act as a language that, through learned connections, brings in the physical ideas of objects. This is why our sense of sight can be easily tricked by the painter's skill, entertaining and teaching us at the same time. The reader will find a lot of fascinating information on this topic in Bishop Berkeley's Essay on Vision, which is a very clever work.

The immediate object however of the sense of vision is light; this fluid, though its velocity is so great, appears to have no perceptible mechanical impulse, as was mentioned in the third Section, but seems to stimulate the retina into animal motion by its transmission through this part of the sensorium: for though the eyes of cats or other animals appear luminous in obscure places; yet it is probable, that none of the light, which falls on the retina, is reflected from it, but adheres to or enters into combination with the choroide coat behind it.

The main focus of vision is light; this phenomenon, despite its incredible speed, doesn't seem to have any noticeable mechanical force, as mentioned in the third section. Instead, it seems to activate the retina to produce movement by passing through this part of the sensory system. While the eyes of cats and other animals may appear to glow in low light, it's likely that none of the light hitting the retina is actually reflected back, but rather sticks to or merges with the choroid layer behind it.

The combination of the particles of light with opake bodies, and therefore with the choroide coat of the eye, is evinced from the heat, which is given out, as in other chemical combinations. For the sunbeams communicate no heat in their passage through transparent bodies, with which they do not combine, as the air continues cool even in the focus of the largest burning-glasses, which in a moment vitrifies a particle of opaque matter.

The way light particles combine with opaque objects, like the choroid layer of the eye, is shown by the heat produced during these chemical reactions. Sunlight doesn’t give off heat as it passes through transparent materials, which is why the air stays cool even at the focus of the largest magnifying glasses, while those same glasses can quickly turn a piece of opaque material into glass.

IV. Of the Organ of Hearing.

IV. About the Ear.

It is generally believed, that the tympanum of the ear vibrates mechanically, when exposed to audible sounds, like the strings of one musical instrument, when the same notes are struck upon another. Nor is this opinion improbable, as the muscles and cartilages of the larynx are employed in producing variety of tones by mechanical vibration: so the muscles and bones of the ear seem adapted to increase or diminish the tension of the tympanum for the purposes of similar mechanical vibrations.

It is commonly thought that the eardrum vibrates mechanically when it comes into contact with sounds we can hear, similar to how the strings of a musical instrument resonate when the same notes are played on another instrument. This idea makes sense, as the muscles and cartilage in the larynx are used to create different tones through mechanical vibration; likewise, the muscles and bones in the ear appear to adjust the tension of the eardrum to facilitate similar mechanical vibrations.

But it appears from dissection, that the tympanum is not the immediate organ of hearing, but that like the humours and cornea of the eye, it is only of use to prepare the object for the immediate organ. For the portio mollis of the auditory nerve is not spread upon the tympanum, but upon the vestibulum, and cochlea, and semicircular canals of the ear; while between the tympanum and the expansion of the auditory nerve the cavity is said by Dr. Cotunnus and Dr. Meckel to be filled with water; as they had frequently observed by freezing the heads of dead animals before they dissected them; and water being a more dense fluid than air is much better adapted to the propagation of vibrations. We may add, that even the external opening of the ear is not absolutely necessary for the perception of sound: for some people, who from these defects would have been completely deaf, have distinguished acute or grave sounds by the tremours of a stick held between their teeth propagated along the bones of the head, (Haller. Phys. T. V. p. 295).

But dissection shows that the eardrum isn't the direct organ of hearing. Instead, like the fluids and cornea of the eye, it's only there to prepare the sound for the actual hearing organ. The soft part of the auditory nerve doesn’t cover the eardrum but rather the vestibule, cochlea, and semicircular canals of the ear. Dr. Cotunnus and Dr. Meckel noted that the space between the eardrum and the auditory nerve is filled with water, which they observed by freezing the heads of animals before dissection. Water, being denser than air, is much better for transmitting vibrations. Additionally, even the external opening of the ear isn't strictly necessary for hearing: some people who would otherwise be completely deaf have been able to recognize high or low sounds through the vibrations of a stick held between their teeth, which travel along the bones of their head (Haller. Phys. T. V. p. 295).

Hence it appears, that the immediate organ of hearing is not affected by the particles of the air themselves, but is stimulated into animal motion by the vibrations of them. And it is probable from the loose bones, which are found in the heads of some fishes, that the vibrations of water are sensible to the inhabitants of that element by a similar organ.

Hence, it seems that the main organ for hearing isn’t directly influenced by the air particles themselves, but instead is triggered into movement by their vibrations. It’s likely, based on the loose bones found in the heads of certain fish, that water vibrations are detectable by those creatures through a similar organ.

The motions of the atmosphere, which we become acquainted with by the sense of touch, are combined with its solidity, weight, or vis intertiæ; whereas those, that are perceived by this organ, depend alone on its elasticity. But though the vibration of the air is the immediate object of the sense of hearing, yet the ideas, we receive by this sense, like those received from light, are only as a language, which by acquired associations acquaints us with those motions of tangible bodies, which depend on their elasticity; and which we had before learned by our sense of touch.

The movements in the atmosphere that we feel through touch are mixed with its solidity, weight, or inertia; while those we perceive with this sense rely solely on its elasticity. Although the vibration of air is what we directly hear, the concepts we gain through this sense, similar to those we get from light, act like a language that, through learned associations, helps us understand the movements of solid objects, which are based on their elasticity, and which we previously learned through our sense of touch.

V. Of Smell and of Taste.

V. About Smell and Taste.

The objects of smell are dissolved in the fluid atmosphere, and those of taste in the saliva, or other aqueous fluid, for the better diffusing them on their respective organs, which seem to be stimulated into animal motion perhaps by the chemical affinities of these particles, which constitute the sapidity and odorosity of bodies with the nerves of sense, which perceive them.

The things we smell dissolve in the air, while the ones we taste dissolve in our saliva or other watery fluids to help spread them across our sensory organs. These organs seem to trigger reactions in our bodies, likely due to the chemical properties of these particles that give taste and smell to substances, which our sensory nerves detect.

Mr. Volta has lately observed a curious circumstance relative to our sense of taste. If a bit of clean lead and a bit of clean silver be separately applied to the tongue and palate no taste is perceived; but by applying them in contact in respect to the parts out of the mouth, and nearly so in respect to the parts, which are immediately applied to the tongue and palate, a saline or acidulous taste is perceived, as of a fluid like a stream of electricity passing from one of them to the other. This new application of the sense of taste deserves further investigation, as it may acquaint us with new properties of matter.

Mr. Volta has recently noticed something interesting about our sense of taste. When a piece of clean lead and a piece of clean silver are placed separately on the tongue and palate, there’s no taste. However, when they are brought into contact in relation to the parts outside the mouth, and almost so for the parts that touch the tongue and palate, a salty or tangy taste is detected, similar to a stream of electricity flowing from one to the other. This new way of experiencing taste deserves more research, as it could reveal new properties of matter.

From the experiments above mentioned of Galvani, Volta, Fowler, and others, it appears, that a plate of zinc and a plate of silver have greater effect than lead and silver. If one edge of a plate of silver about the size of half a crown-piece be placed upon the tongue, and one edge of a plate of zinc about the same size beneath the tongue, and if their opposite edges are then brought into contact before the point of the tongue, a taste is perceived at the moment of their coming into contact; secondly, if one of the above plates be put between the upper lip and the gum of the fore-teeth, and the other be placed under the tongue, and their exterior edges be then brought into contact in a darkish room, a flash of light is perceived in the eyes.

From the experiments mentioned above by Galvani, Volta, Fowler, and others, it seems that a zinc plate and a silver plate have a stronger effect than lead and silver. If you place one edge of a silver plate about the size of a half-crown coin on your tongue and one edge of a zinc plate of the same size underneath your tongue, and then bring their opposite edges into contact in front of the tip of your tongue, you'll feel a taste at the moment they touch. Additionally, if you position one of these plates between your upper lip and the gum of your front teeth, while placing the other under your tongue, and then bring their outer edges into contact in a dimly lit room, you will see a flash of light in your eyes.

These effects I imagine only shew the sensibility of our nerves of sense to very small quantities of the electric fluid, as it passes through them; for I suppose these sensations are occasioned by slight electric shocks produced in the following manner. By the experiments published by Mr. Bennet, with his ingenious doubler of electricity, which is the greatest discovery made in that science since the coated jar, and the eduction of lightning from the skies, it appears that zinc was always found minus, and silver was always found plus, when both of them were in their separate state. Hence, when they are placed in the manner above described, as soon as their exterior edges come nearly into contact, so near as to have an extremely thin plate of air between them, that plate of air becomes charged in the same manner as a plate of coated glass; and is at the same instant discharged through the nerves of taste or of sight, and gives the sensations, as above described, of light or of saporocity; and only shews the great sensibility of these organs of sense to the stimulus of the electric fluid in suddenly passing through them.

These effects I think just show how sensitive our sensory nerves are to very small amounts of electric energy as it moves through them. I believe these sensations are caused by tiny electric shocks happening in the following way. According to the experiments published by Mr. Bennet with his clever electricity doubler, which is the biggest breakthrough in that field since the coated jar and extracting lightning from the sky, zinc was always found to be negative and silver was always found to be positive when they were in their separate states. Therefore, when they are arranged as described above, as soon as their outer edges come close enough to touch—just leaving a very thin layer of air between them—that layer of air gets charged like a coated glass plate; and at the same moment, it discharges through the nerves of taste or sight, producing the sensations of light or flavor as mentioned above. This only highlights how sensitive these sensory organs are to the electric energy stimulus as it quickly passes through them.

VI. Of the Sense of Heat.

VI. On the Feeling of Heat.

There are many experiments in chemical writers, that evince the existence of heat as a fluid element, which covers and pervades all bodies, and is attracted by the solutions of some of them, and is detruded from the combination of others. Thus from the combinations of metals with acids, and from those combinations of animal fluids, which are termed secretions, this fluid matter of heat is given out amongst the neighbouring bodies; and in the solutions of salts in water, or of water in air, it is absorbed from the bodies, that surround them; whilst in its facility in passing through metallic bodies, and its difficulty in pervading resins and glass, it resembles the properties of the electric aura; and is like that excited by friction, and seems like that to gravitate amongst other bodies in its uncombined state, and to find its equilibrium.

There are many experiments with chemical substances that show heat exists as a fluid element that surrounds and fills all things. It is attracted by some solutions and pushed away by others. For example, when metals combine with acids or when animal fluids, known as secretions, combine, this fluid heat is released into nearby bodies. In the case of salts dissolving in water or water mixing with air, it is drawn from the surrounding materials. Heat easily moves through metals but has a harder time passing through resins and glass. This behavior is similar to the properties of electric aura; it resembles what is produced by friction and seems to gravitate towards other bodies in its uncombined state, trying to find balance.

There is no circumstance of more consequence in the animal economy than a due proportion of this fluid of heat; for the digestion of our nutriment in the stomach and bowels, and the proper qualities of all our secreted fluids, as they are produced or prepared partly by animal and partly by chemical processes, depend much on the quantity of heat; the excess of which, or its deficiency, alike gives us pain, and induces us to avoid the circumstances that occasion them. And in this the perception of heat essentially differs from the perceptions of the sense of touch, as we receive pain from too great pressure of solid bodies, but none from the absence of it. It is hence probable, that nature has provided us with a set of nerves for the perception of this fluid, which anatomists have not yet attended to.

There is no situation more important in the animal body than having the right balance of heat. This balance affects how we digest food in our stomach and intestines, as well as the characteristics of all the fluids our body produces, which come from both biological and chemical processes. The amount of heat we have plays a significant role; too much or too little can cause us pain and lead us to avoid situations that create that discomfort. This is where our perception of heat differs from our sense of touch: we feel pain from excessive pressure from solid objects, but we don’t feel anything from the lack of pressure. Therefore, it seems likely that nature has equipped us with a set of nerves specifically for sensing this heat, which anatomists have yet to fully explore.

There may be some difficulty in the proof of this assertion; if we look at a hot fire, we experience no pain of the optic nerve, though the heat along with the light must be concentrated upon it. Nor does warm water or warm oil poured into the ear give pain to the organ of hearing; and hence as these organs of sense do not perceive small excesses or deficiences of heat; and as heat has no greater analogy to the solidity or to the figures of bodies, than it has to their colours or vibrations; there seems no sufficient reason for our ascribing the perception of heat and cold to the sense of touch; to which it has generally been attributed, either because it is diffused beneath the whole skin like the sense of touch, or owing to the inaccuracy of our observations, or the defect of our languages.

There may be some difficulty in proving this claim; when we look at a hot fire, we feel no pain in our eyes, even though both heat and light are focused on them. Similarly, warm water or warm oil poured into our ears doesn’t hurt our hearing. This suggests that our sensory organs don’t detect slight changes in temperature. Since heat doesn’t relate to the solidity or shape of objects any more than it does to their colors or vibrations, there seems to be no good reason for linking the perception of heat and cold to the sense of touch, which is typically the case. This could be either because it spreads across the skin like the sense of touch or due to the flaws in our observations or the limitations of our language.

There is another circumstance would induce us to believe, that the perceptions of heat and cold do not belong to the organ of touch; since the teeth, which are the least adapted for the perceptions of solidity or figure, are the most sensible to heat or cold; whence we are forewarned from swallowing those materials, whose degree of coldness or of heat would injure our stomachs.

There’s another reason that makes us think that the feelings of heat and cold don’t come from the sense of touch; the teeth, which aren’t really suited for sensing solidity or shape, are actually the most sensitive to heat or cold. Because of this, we learn to be careful about swallowing things that are too hot or too cold, as they could harm our stomachs.

The following is an extract from a letter of Dr. R.W. Darwin, of Shrewsbury, when he was a student at Edinburgh. "I made an experiment yesterday in our hospital, which much favours your opinion, that the sensation of heat and of touch depend on different sets of nerves. A man who had lately recovered from a fever, and was still weak, was seized with violent cramps in his legs and feet; which were removed by opiates, except that one of his feet remained insensible. Mr. Ewart pricked him with a pin in five or six places, and the patient declared he did not feel it in the least, nor was he sensible of a very smart pinch. I then held a red-hot poker at some distance, and brought it gradually nearer till it came within three inches, when he asserted that he felt it quite distinctly. I suppose some violent irritation on the nerves of touch had caused the cramps, and had left them paralytic; while the nerves of heat, having suffered no increased stimulus, retained their irritability."

The following is an excerpt from a letter by Dr. R.W. Darwin of Shrewsbury, when he was studying in Edinburgh. "I ran an experiment yesterday in our hospital that supports your idea that the sensations of heat and touch rely on different sets of nerves. A man who had recently recovered from a fever and was still weak was struck with severe cramps in his legs and feet, which were relieved by opiates, except one of his feet remained numb. Mr. Ewart pricked him with a pin in five or six spots, and the patient said he didn't feel it at all, nor did he notice a sharp pinch. I then held a red-hot poker at a distance and brought it gradually closer until it was about three inches away, when he claimed he felt it quite clearly. I think some intense irritation on the touch nerves caused the cramps and left them paralyzed, while the heat nerves, not having experienced any increased stimulation, kept their responsiveness."

Add to this, that the lungs, though easily stimulated into inflammation, are not sensible to heat. See Class. III. 1. 1. 10.

Add to this that the lungs, although easily triggered into inflammation, are not sensitive to heat. See Class. III. 1. 1. 10.

VII. Of the Sense of Extension.

VII. On the Sense of Space.

The organ of touch is properly the sense of pressure, but the muscular fibres themselves constitute the organ of sense, that feels extension. The sense of pressure is always attended with the ideas of the figure and solidity of the object, neither of which accompany our perception of extension. The whole set of muscles, whether they are hollow ones, as the heart, arteries, and intestines, or longitudinal ones attached to bones, contract themselves, whenever they are stimulated by forcible elongation; and it is observable, that the white muscles, which constitute the arterial system, seem to be excited into contraction from no other kinds of stimulus, according to the experiments of Haller. And hence the violent pain in some inflammations, as in the paronychia, obtains immediate relief by cutting the membrane, that was stretched by the tumour of the subjacent parts.

The sense of touch is essentially about feeling pressure, but the muscle fibers themselves act as the sensory organs that perceive extension. Pressure sensation is always linked with the ideas of an object's shape and solidity, neither of which is present when we perceive extension. All types of muscles, whether they are hollow like the heart, arteries, and intestines, or long ones attached to bones, contract when they are stimulated by strong stretching. It's noticeable that the white muscles making up the arterial system seem to contract only when stimulated in this way, as shown by Haller's experiments. This is why intense pain in certain inflammations, like paronychia, can be immediately relieved by cutting into the membrane that’s been stretched by the tumor in the underlying areas.

Hence the whole muscular system may be considered as one organ of sense, and the various attitudes of the body, as ideas belonging to this organ, of many of which we are hourly conscious, while many others, like the irritative ideas of the other senses, are performed without our attention.

Hence, the entire muscular system can be viewed as a single sense organ, and the different positions of the body can be seen as ideas related to this organ. Many of these we are aware of constantly, while others, like the reflex actions of our other senses, happen without us noticing.

When the muscles of the heart cease to act, the refluent blood again distends or elongates them; and thus irritated they contract as before. The same happens to the arterial system, and I suppose to the capillaries, intestines, and various glands of the body.

When the heart muscles stop working, the returning blood stretches them out again; and feeling that irritation, they contract just like before. The same thing happens in the arterial system, and I think in the capillaries, intestines, and various glands of the body as well.

When the quantity of urine, or of excrement, distends the bladder, or rectum, those parts contract, and exclude their contents, and many other muscles by association act along with them; but if these evacuations are not soon complied with, pain is produced by a little further extension of the muscular fibres: a similar pain is caused in the muscles, when a limb is much extended for the reduction of dislocated bones; and in the punishment of the rack: and in the painful cramps of the calf of the leg, or of other muscles, for a greater degree of contraction of a muscle, than the movement of the two bones, to which its ends are affixed, will admit of, must give similar pain to that, which is produced by extending it beyond its due length. And the pain from punctures or incisions arises from the distention of the fibres, as the knife passes through them; for it nearly ceases as soon as the division is completed.

When the amount of urine or feces fills the bladder or rectum, those areas contract to expel their contents, and many other muscles work together in response; however, if these urges to relieve oneself aren't met quickly, pain occurs due to further stretching of the muscle fibers. A similar pain happens in the muscles when a limb is stretched too much to reset dislocated bones, during torture on the rack, or during painful cramps in the calf or other muscles. Greater contraction of a muscle than what the movement between its two attached bones allows causes pain similar to that experienced when it is stretched beyond its normal length. Additionally, the pain from cuts or punctures is due to the stretching of the fibers as the knife moves through them, and it nearly stops as soon as the cut is complete.

All these motions of the muscles, that are thus naturally excited by the stimulus of distending bodies, are also liable to be called into strong action by their catenation, with the irritations or sensations produced by the momentum of the progressive particles of blood in the arteries, as in inflammatory fevers, or by acrid substances on other sensible organs, as in the strangury, or tenesmus, or cholera.

All these muscle movements, which are naturally triggered by the stimulation of expanding bodies, can also be strongly activated by their connection to the irritations or sensations caused by the flow of blood particles in the arteries, as seen in inflammatory fevers, or by irritating substances affecting other sensitive organs, like in strangury, tenesmus, or cholera.

We shall conclude this account of the sense of extension by observing, that the want of its object is attended with a disagreeable sensation, as well as the excess of it. In those hollow muscles, which have been accustomed to it, this disagreeable sensation is called faintness, emptiness, and sinking; and, when it arises to a certain degree, is attended with syncope, or a total quiescence of all motions, but the internal irritative ones, as happens from sudden loss of blood, or in the operation of tapping in the dropsy.

We will wrap up this discussion on the sense of extension by noting that not having it causes an unpleasant feeling, just like having too much of it does. In those hollow muscles that are used to it, this unpleasant feeling is referred to as faintness, emptiness, or sinking. When it reaches a certain level, it can lead to syncope, which is a complete lack of motion except for internal movements, as seen in cases of sudden blood loss or during the procedure of tapping for dropsy.

VIII. Of the Appetites of Hunger, Thirst, Heat, Extension, the want of fresh Air, animal Love, and the Suckling of Children.

VIII. About the cravings of hunger, thirst, warmth, expansion, the need for fresh air, animal desire, and nursing children.

Hunger is most probably perceived by those numerous ramifications of nerves that are seen about the upper opening of the stomach; and thirst by the nerves about the fauces, and the top of the gula. The ideas of these senses are few in the generality of mankind, but are more numerous in those, who by disease, or indulgence, desire particular kinds of foods or liquids.

Hunger is likely sensed by the many nerve endings found near the upper opening of the stomach, while thirst is detected by the nerves around the throat and the back of the mouth. Most people have a limited understanding of these sensations, but those who have certain illnesses or indulge frequently tend to crave specific types of foods or drinks.

A sense of heat has already been spoken of, which may with propriety be called an appetite, as we painfully desire it, when it is deficient in quantity.

A sense of heat has already been mentioned, which can properly be called an appetite, as we feel a painful desire for it when there's not enough of it.

The sense of extension may be ranked amongst these appetites, since the deficiency of its object gives disagreeable sensation; when this happens in the arterial system, it is called faintness, and seems to bear some analogy to hunger and to cold; which like it are attended with emptiness of a part of the vascular system.

The feeling of extension can be grouped with these desires because a lack of its object creates an unpleasant sensation. When this occurs in the arterial system, it's referred to as faintness, and it seems to be similar to hunger and cold, which also involve a sense of emptiness in part of the vascular system.

The sense of want of fresh air has not been attended to, but is as distinct as the others, and the first perhaps that we experience after our nativity; from the want of the object of this sense many diseases are produced, as the jail-fever, plague, and other epidemic maladies. Animal love is another appetite, which occurs later in life, and the females of lactiferous animals have another natural inlet of pleasure or pain from the suckling their offspring. The want of which either owing to the death of their progeny, or to the fashion of their country, has been fatal to many of the sex. The males have also pectoral glands, which are frequently turgid with a thin milk at their nativity, and are furnished with nipples, which erect on titillation like those of the female; but which seem now to be of no further use, owing perhaps to some change which these animals have undergone in the gradual progression of the formation of the earth, and of all that it inhabit.

The feeling of needing fresh air hasn't been addressed, but it’s as clear as the others, and probably the first one we notice after we're born. The lack of this sense can lead to many diseases, like jail fever, plague, and other contagious illnesses. Another desire that comes later in life is animal love, and female mammals have another natural source of pleasure or pain from nursing their young. The absence of this, whether due to the death of their offspring or local customs, has led to the death of many females. Males also have breast glands that can sometimes swell with a thin milk shortly after birth, and they have nipples that can rise when stimulated, just like females. However, these seem to serve no further purpose, likely due to changes that these animals have gone through over time as the earth and everything living on it has evolved.

These seven last mentioned senses may properly be termed appetites, as they differ from those of touch, sight, hearing, taste, and smell, in this respect; that they are affected with pain as well by the defect of their objects as by the excess of them, which is not so in the latter. Thus cold and hunger give us pain, as well as an excess of heat or satiety; but it is not so with darkness and silence.

These last seven senses can be properly called appetites because they differ from touch, sight, hearing, taste, and smell in one important way: they can feel pain from both a lack of their objects and an abundance of them, which isn’t the case with the latter senses. For example, cold and hunger cause us pain, just like too much heat or being overly full does; but darkness and silence don't have the same effect.

IX. Before we conclude this Section on the organs of sense, we must observe, that, as far as we know, there are many more senses, than have been here mentioned, as every gland seems to be influenced to separate from the blood, or to absorb from the cavities of the body, or from the atmosphere, its appropriated fluid, by the stimulus of that fluid on the living gland; and not by mechanical capillary absorption, nor by chemical affinity. Hence it appears, that each of these glands must have a peculiar organ to perceive these irritations, but as these irritations are not succeeded by sensation, they have not acquired the names of senses.

IX. Before we finish this section on the senses, we should point out that, as far as we know, there are many more senses than those mentioned here. Every gland seems to be affected to either release a fluid from the blood or to absorb a fluid from the body’s cavities or the air, driven by the stimulus of that fluid on the living gland, rather than by mechanical capillary absorption or chemical attraction. Therefore, it seems that each of these glands must have a specific organ to detect these irritations, but since these irritations don't result in sensation, they haven’t been classified as senses.

However when these glands are excited into motions stronger than usual, either by the acrimony of their fluids, or by their own irritability being much increased, then the sensation of pain is produced in them as in all the other senses of the body; and these pains are all of different kinds, and hence the glands at this time really become each a different organ of sense, though these different kinds of pain have acquired no names.

However, when these glands are stimulated to work harder than normal, either by the sharpness of their fluids or by an increase in their own sensitivity, a sensation of pain is created in them just like in all the other senses of the body. These pains come in various forms, so at this moment, each gland essentially becomes a different sensory organ, even though these different types of pain haven't been given specific names.

Thus a great excess of light does not give the idea of light but of pain; as in forcibly opening the eye when it is much inflamed. The great excess of pressure or distention, as when the point of a pin is pressed upon our skin, produces pain, (and when this pain of the sense of distention is slighter, it is termed itching, or tickling), without any idea of solidity or of figure: an excess of heat produces smarting, of cold another kind of pain; it is probable by this sense of heat the pain produced by caustic bodies is perceived, and of electricity, as all these are fluids, that permeate, distend, or decompose the parts that feel them.

So, having too much light doesn’t really feel like light at all; it feels more like pain, similar to the discomfort of opening your eyes when they’re really irritated. Excessive pressure or stretching, like when a pin pokes our skin, causes pain. When this feeling of pressure is less intense, we call it itching or tickling, but it doesn’t give us any sense of hardness or shape. Too much heat causes a stinging sensation, while cold brings another type of pain. It’s likely that we sense the pain from caustic substances through our perception of heat, and the same goes for electricity, since all these things are fluids that can penetrate, stretch, or break down the parts that feel them.



SECT. XV.

OF THE CLASSES OF IDEAS.

OF THE TYPES OF IDEAS.

I. 1. Ideas received in tribes. 2. We combine them further, or abstract from these tribes. 3. Complex ideas. 4. Compounded ideas. 5. Simple ideas, modes, substances, relations, general ideas. 6. Ideas of reflexion. 7. Memory and imagination imperfectly defined. Ideal presence. Memorandum-rings. II. 1. Irritative ideas. Perception. 2. Sensitive ideas, imagination. 3. Voluntary ideas, recollection. 4. Associated ideas, suggestion. III. 1. Definitions of perception, memory. 2. Reasoning, judgment, doubting, distinguishing, comparing. 3. Invention. 4. Consciousness. 5. Identity. 6. Lapse of time. 7. Free-will.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Ideas gathered in groups. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. We further combine them or abstract from these groups. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Complex ideas. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Combined ideas. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Simple ideas, modes, substances, relationships, general ideas. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Reflexive ideas. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Memory and imagination are not clearly defined. Ideal presence. Reminder rings. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Irritative ideas. Perception. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Sensitive ideas, imagination. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. Voluntary ideas, recollection. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. Associated ideas, suggestion. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. Definitions of perception and memory. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. Reasoning, judgment, doubt, distinguishing, comparing. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. Invention. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. Consciousness. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_18__. Identity. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_19__. Passage of time. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_20__. Free will.

I. 1. As the constituent elements of the material world are only perceptible to our organs of sense in a state of combination; it follows, that the ideas or sensual motions excited by them, are never received singly, but ever with a greater or less degree of combination. So the colours of bodies or their hardnesses occur with their figures: every smell and taste has its degree of pungency as well as its peculiar flavour: and each note in music is combined with the tone of some instrument. It appears from hence, that we can be sensible of a number of ideas at the same time, such as the whiteness, hardness, and coldness, of a snow-ball, and can experience at the same time many irritative ideas of surrounding bodies, which we do not attend to, as mentioned in Section VII. 3. 2. But those ideas which belong to the same sense, seem to be more easily combined into synchronous tribes, than those which were not received by the same sense, as we can more easily think of the whiteness and figure of a lump of sugar at the same time, than the whiteness and sweetness of it.

I. 1. As the constituent elements of the material world are only perceptible to our organs of sense in a state of combination; it follows, that the ideas or sensual motions excited by them, are never received singly, but ever with a greater or less degree of combination. So the colours of bodies or their hardnesses occur with their figures: every smell and taste has its degree of pungency as well as its peculiar flavour: and each note in music is combined with the tone of some instrument. It appears from hence, that we can be sensible of a number of ideas at the same time, such as the whiteness, hardness, and coldness, of a snow-ball, and can experience at the same time many irritative ideas of surrounding bodies, which we do not attend to, as mentioned in Section VII. 3. 2. But those ideas which belong to the same sense, seem to be more easily combined into synchronous tribes, than those which were not received by the same sense, as we can more easily think of the whiteness and figure of a lump of sugar at the same time, than the whiteness and sweetness of it.

2. As these ideas, or sensual motions, are thus excited with greater or less degrees of combination; so we have a power, when we repeat them either by our volition or sensation, to increase or diminish this degree of combination, that is, to form compounded ideas from those, which were more simple; and abstract ones from those, which were more complex, when they were first excited; that is, we can repeat a part or the whole of those sensual motions, which did constitute our ideas of perception; and the repetition of which now constitutes our ideas of recollection, or of imagination.

2. As these thoughts, or sensory experiences, arise with varying degrees of combination, we have the ability, whether through our intentional choice or our senses, to increase or decrease this level of combination. This means we can create complex ideas from simpler ones and abstract ideas from those that were initially more detailed. In other words, we can recall part or all of the sensory experiences that made up our perception, and the act of recalling them now forms our ideas of memory or imagination.

3. Those ideas, which we repeat without change of the quantity of that combination, with which we first received them, are called complex ideas, as when you recollect Westminster Abbey, or the planet Saturn: but it must be observed, that these complex ideas, thus re-excited by volition, sensation, or association, are seldom perfect copies of their correspondent perceptions, except in our dreams, where other external objects do not detract our attention.

3. The ideas we repeat without changing the combination in which we first encountered them are known as complex ideas, like when you remember Westminster Abbey or the planet Saturn. However, it's important to note that these complex ideas, which are triggered by our will, sensations, or associations, are rarely exact replicas of the original perceptions, except in our dreams, where other external objects don't distract us.

4. Those ideas, which are more complex than the natural objects that first excited them, have been called compounded ideas, as when we think of a sphinx, or griffin.

4. Those ideas, which are more complicated than the natural objects that initially sparked our interest, are referred to as compounded ideas, like when we imagine a sphinx or griffin.

5. And those that are less complex than the correspondent natural objects, have been termed abstracted ideas: thus sweetness, and whiteness, and solidity, are received at the same time from a lump of sugar, yet I can recollect any of these qualities without thinking of the others, that were excited along with them.

5. The ones that are simpler than the corresponding natural objects are called abstract ideas: for example, I can experience sweetness, whiteness, and solidity from a piece of sugar, but I can also remember any of these qualities without thinking of the others that came to mind at the same time.

When ideas are so far abstracted as in the above example, they have been termed simple by the writers of metaphysics, and seem indeed to be more complete repetitions of the ideas or sensual motions, originally excited by external objects.

When ideas are as abstract as in the example above, they've been called simple by metaphysics writers, and they really do seem to be more complete repetitions of the ideas or sensory experiences initially triggered by external objects.

Other classes of these ideas, where the abstraction has not been so great, have been termed, by Mr. Locke, modes, substances, and relations, but they seem only to differ in their degree of abstraction from the complex ideas that were at first excited; for as these complex or natural ideas are themselves imperfect copies of their correspondent perceptions, so these abstract or general ideas are only still more imperfect copies of the same perceptions. Thus when I have seen an object but once, as a rhinoceros, my abstract idea of this animal is the same as my complex one. I may think more or less distinctly of a rhinoceros, but it is the very rhinoceros that I saw, or some part or property of him, which recurs to my mind.

Other categories of these ideas, where the abstraction isn’t as strong, have been called, by Mr. Locke, modes, substances, and relations. However, they seem to only differ in how abstract they are from the complex ideas that were initially triggered. Just as these complex or natural ideas are flawed representations of their related perceptions, these abstract or general ideas are even more flawed representations of the same perceptions. So, when I have seen an object just once, like a rhinoceros, my abstract idea of that animal is the same as my complex one. I might think about a rhinoceros more or less clearly, but it's always the same rhinoceros I saw, or some part or quality of it, that comes to my mind.

But when any class of complex objects becomes the subject of conversation, of which I have seen many individuals, as a castle or an army, some property or circumstance belonging to it is peculiarly alluded to; and then I feel in my own mind, that my abstract idea of this complex object is only an idea of that part, property, or attitude of it, that employs the present conversation, and varies with every sentence that is spoken concerning it. So if any one should say, "one may sit upon a horse safer than on a camel," my abstract idea of the two animals includes only an outline of the level back of the one, and the gibbosity on the back of the other. What noise is that in the street?—Some horses trotting over the pavement. Here my idea of the horses includes principally the shape and motion of their legs. So also the abstract ideas of goodness and courage are still more imperfect representations of the objects they were received from; for here we abstract the material parts, and recollect only the qualities.

But when any group of complex objects comes up in conversation, like a castle or an army, there's always a specific attribute or situation that gets highlighted; I realize, in my own mind, that my general idea of this complex object is just a notion of that part, quality, or aspect that’s being talked about, and it changes with every sentence spoken about it. So if someone says, "you can sit on a horse more safely than on a camel," my general idea of the two animals only includes a rough outline of the horse's flat back and the camel's hump. What’s that noise outside?—Some horses trotting on the pavement. In this case, my idea of the horses mainly focuses on the shape and movement of their legs. Similarly, the general ideas of goodness and courage are even less accurate representations of the original objects; here we strip away the physical components and remember only the qualities.

Thus we abstract so much from some of our complex ideas, that at length it becomes difficult to determine of what perception they partake; and in many instances our idea seems to be no other than of the sound or letters of the word, that stands for the collective tribe, of which we are said to have an abstracted idea, as noun, verb, chimæra, apparition.

Thus we simplify so much from some of our complex ideas that, eventually, it becomes hard to figure out what perception they come from; and in many cases, our idea seems to be nothing more than the sound or letters of the word that represents the whole group, of which we are said to have an abstract idea, like noun, verb, chimera, apparition.

6. Ideas have been divided into those of perception and those of reflection, but as whatever is perceived must be external to the organ that perceives it, all our ideas must originally be ideas of perception.

6. Ideas have been categorized into those based on perception and those based on reflection, but since everything we perceive must be separate from the perceiving mind, all our ideas must ultimately stem from perception.

7. Others have divided our ideas into those of memory, and those of imagination; they have said that a recollection of ideas in the order they were received constitutes memory, and without that order imagination; but all the ideas of imagination, excepting the few that are termed simple ideas, are parts of trains or tribes in the order they were received; as if I think of a sphinx, or a griffin, the fair face, bosom, wings, claws, tail, are all complex ideas in the order they were received: and it behoves the writers, who adhere to this definition, to determine, how small the trains must be, that shall be called imagination; and how great those, that shall be called memory.

7. Some people have divided our thoughts into two categories: memory and imagination. They've suggested that remembering ideas in the order we first encountered them is what we call memory, while anything that doesn't follow that order is considered imagination. However, all imaginative ideas, except for a few that are labeled as simple ideas, are actually parts of sequences or groups in the order they were received. For example, if I think of a sphinx or a griffin, the beautiful face, chest, wings, claws, and tail are all complex ideas following the order in which I received them. It is up to the writers who stick to this definition to figure out how small the sequences must be to be called imagination, and how large they must be to be considered memory.

Others have thought that the ideas of memory have a greater vivacity than those of imagination: but the ideas of a person in sleep, or in a waking reverie, where the trains connected with sensation are uninterrupted, are more vivid and distinct than those of memory, so that they cannot be distinguished by this criterion.

Others believe that memories are more vivid than imaginative ideas. However, the thoughts of someone sleeping or daydreaming, where sensations flow without interruption, are actually more intense and clear than memories, making it impossible to tell them apart using this standard.

The very ingenious author of the Elements of Criticism has described what he conceives to be a species of memory, and calls it ideal presence; but the instances he produces are the reveries of sensation, and are therefore in truth connections of the imagination, though they are recalled in the order they were received.

The clever author of the Elements of Criticism has described what he thinks is a type of memory and calls it ideal presence; however, the examples he gives are just daydreams from our senses and are actually connections of the imagination, even though they are remembered in the order they were experienced.

The ideas connected by association are in common discourse attributed to memory, as we talk of memorandum-rings, and tie a knot on our handkerchiefs to bring something into our minds at a distance of time. And a school-boy, who can repeat a thousand unmeaning lines in Lilly's Grammar, is said to have a good memory. But these have been already shewn to belong to the class of association; and are termed ideas of suggestion.

The ideas connected by association are often considered part of memory in everyday conversation. We refer to things like memory aids, and we tie knots in our handkerchiefs to remind ourselves of something later. A schoolboy who can recite countless meaningless lines from Lilly's Grammar is said to have a good memory. However, these examples have already been shown to belong to the category of association and are called ideas of suggestion.

II. Lastly, the method already explained of classing ideas into those excited by irritation, sensation, volition, or association, we hope will be found more convenient both for explaining the operations of the mind, and for comparing them with those of the body; and for the illustration and the cure of the diseases of both, and which we shall here recapitulate.

II. Finally, the method we've already discussed for categorizing ideas based on irritation, sensation, volition, or association should be more convenient for explaining how the mind works. It will also help us compare those processes with what's happening in the body, as well as for illustrating and treating the diseases affecting both, which we will summarize here.

1. Irritative ideas are those, which are preceded by irritation, which is excited by objects external to the organs of sense: as the idea of that tree, which either I attend to, or which I shun in walking near it without attention. In the former case it is termed perception, in the latter it is termed simply an irritative idea.

1. Irritative ideas are those that come after irritation caused by things outside the senses: like the idea of that tree, which I either focus on or avoid while walking near it without thinking. In the first case, it's called perception, and in the second, it's just called an irritative idea.

2. Sensitive ideas are those, which are preceded by the sensation of pleasure or pain; as the ideas, which constitute our dreams or reveries, this is called imagination.

2. Sensitive ideas are those that are associated with the feelings of pleasure or pain; like the ideas that make up our dreams or daydreams, this is referred to as imagination.

3. Voluntary ideas are those, which are preceded by voluntary exertion, as when I repeat the alphabet backwards: this is called recollection.

3. Voluntary ideas are those that come after a deliberate effort, like when I recite the alphabet backwards: this is known as recollection.

4. Associate ideas are those, which are preceded by other ideas or muscular motions, as when we think over or repeat the alphabet by rote in its usual order; or sing a tune we are accustomed to; this is called suggestion.

4. Associated ideas are those that come after other ideas or physical actions, like when we think about or recite the alphabet in the usual order, or sing a familiar tune; this is known as suggestion.

III. 1. Perceptions signify those ideas, which are preceded by irritation and succeeded by the sensation of pleasure or pain, for whatever excites our attention interests us; that is, it is accompanied with, pleasure or pain; however slight may be the degree or quantity of either of them.

III. 1. Perceptions signify the thoughts that come after annoyance and are followed by feelings of pleasure or pain, because anything that grabs our attention matters to us; that is, it comes with pleasure or pain, regardless of how mild either one may be.

The word memory includes two classes of ideas, either those which, are preceded by voluntary exertion, or those which are suggested by their associations with other ideas.

The word memory includes two types of ideas: those that come from conscious effort and those that are triggered by their connections to other ideas.

2. Reasoning is that operation of the sensorium, by which we excite two or many tribes of ideas; and then re-excite the ideas, in which they differ, or correspond. If we determine this difference, it is called judgment; if we in vain endeavour to determine it, it is called doubting.

2. The idea is that the way our senses work allows us to activate multiple groups of ideas; and then we can revisit the ideas to see how they are alike or different. If we manage to identify this difference, it’s called judgment; if we struggle to pinpoint it, it’s called doubt.

If we re-excited the ideas, in which they differ, it is called distinguishing. If we re-excite those in which they correspond, it is called comparing.

If we bring back the ideas where they differ, it’s called distinguishing. If we bring back the ones where they match, it’s called comparing.

3. Invention is an operation of the sensorium, by which we voluntarily continue to excite one train of ideas, suppose the design of raising water by a machine; and at the same time attend to all other ideas, which are connected with this by every kind of catenation; and combine or separate them voluntarily for the purpose of obtaining some end.

3. Invention is a process of the senses, where we intentionally keep stimulating one sequence of ideas, like the concept of using a machine to raise water; while simultaneously considering all other ideas linked to this in various ways; and we deliberately mix or separate them to achieve a specific goal.

For we can create nothing new, we can only combine or separate the ideas, which we have already received by our perceptions: thus if I wish to represent a monster, I call to my mind the ideas of every thing disagreeable and horrible, and combine the nastiness and gluttony of a hog, the stupidity and obstinacy of an ass, with the fur and awkwardness of a bear, and call the new combination Caliban. Yet such a monster may exist in nature, as all his attributes are parts of nature. So when I wish to represent every thing, that is excellent, and amiable; when I combine benevolence with cheerfulness, wisdom, knowledge, taste, wit, beauty of person, and elegance of manners, and associate them in one lady as a pattern to the world, it is called invention; yet such a person may exist,—such a person does exist!—It is —— ——, who is as much a monster as Caliban.

For we can't create anything truly new; we can only mix or separate the ideas we've already gotten from our experiences. So, if I want to represent a monster, I think of all the things that are unpleasant and terrifying, and I combine the grossness and greediness of a pig, the stupidity and stubbornness of a donkey, with the fur and clumsiness of a bear, and I name this new mix Caliban. However, such a monster could exist in nature since all its traits are part of the natural world. When I want to depict everything that is excellent and charming, I blend kindness with happiness, wisdom, knowledge, taste, wit, physical beauty, and graceful manners and group them together in one woman as an example for everyone, it's called invention. Yet such a person could exist—such a person does exist!—It is —— ——, who is as much a monster as Caliban.

4. In respect to consciousness, we are only conscious of our existence, when we think about it; as we only perceive the lapse of time, when we attend to it; when we are busied about other objects, neither the lapse of time nor the consciousness of our own existence can occupy our attention. Hence, when we think of our own existence, we only excite abstracted or reflex ideas (as they are termed), of our principal pleasures or pains, of our desires or aversions, or of the figure, solidity, colour, or other properties of our bodies, and call that act of the sensorium a consciousness of our existence. Some philosopher, I believe it is Des Cartes, has said, "I think, therefore I exist." But this is not right reasoning, because thinking is a mode of existence; and it is thence only saying, "I exist, therefore I exist." For there are three modes of existence, or in the language of grammarians three kinds of verbs. First, simply I am, or exist. Secondly, I am acting, or exist in a state of activity, as I move. Thirdly, I am suffering, or exist in a state of being acted upon, as I am moved. The when, and the where, as applicable to this existence, depends on the successive motions of our own or of other bodies; and on their respective situations, as spoken of Sect. XIV. 2. 5.

4. In respect to consciousness, we are only conscious of our existence, when we think about it; as we only perceive the lapse of time, when we attend to it; when we are busied about other objects, neither the lapse of time nor the consciousness of our own existence can occupy our attention. Hence, when we think of our own existence, we only excite abstracted or reflex ideas (as they are termed), of our principal pleasures or pains, of our desires or aversions, or of the figure, solidity, colour, or other properties of our bodies, and call that act of the sensorium a consciousness of our existence. Some philosopher, I believe it is Des Cartes, has said, "I think, therefore I exist." But this is not right reasoning, because thinking is a mode of existence; and it is thence only saying, "I exist, therefore I exist." For there are three modes of existence, or in the language of grammarians three kinds of verbs. First, simply I am, or exist. Secondly, I am acting, or exist in a state of activity, as I move. Thirdly, I am suffering, or exist in a state of being acted upon, as I am moved. The when, and the where, as applicable to this existence, depends on the successive motions of our own or of other bodies; and on their respective situations, as spoken of Sect. XIV. 2. 5.

5. Our identity is known by our acquired habits or catenated trains of ideas and muscular motions; and perhaps, when we compare infancy with old age, in those alone can our identity be supposed to exist. For what else is there of similitude between the first speck of living entity and the mature man?—every deduction of reasoning, every sentiment or passion, with every fibre of the corporeal part of our system, has been subject almost to annual mutation; while some catenations alone of our ideas and muscular actions have continued in part unchanged.

5. Our identity is shaped by our learned habits and connected thoughts and movements; and maybe, when we look at childhood compared to old age, that's where our identity is truly found. What else is similar between the first tiny spark of life and an adult?—every bit of reasoning, every feeling or emotion, along with every part of our physical being, has changed almost yearly; while only a few patterns of our thoughts and physical actions have remained somewhat the same.

By the facility, with which we can in our waking hours voluntarily produce certain successive trains of ideas, we know by experience, that we have before reproduced them; that is, we are conscious of a time of our existence previous to the present time; that is, of our identity now and heretofore. It is these habits of action, these catenations of ideas and muscular motions, which begin with life, and only terminate with it; and which we can in some measure deliver to our posterity; as explained in Sect. XXXIX.

By the facility, with which we can in our waking hours voluntarily produce certain successive trains of ideas, we know by experience, that we have before reproduced them; that is, we are conscious of a time of our existence previous to the present time; that is, of our identity now and heretofore. It is these habits of action, these catenations of ideas and muscular motions, which begin with life, and only terminate with it; and which we can in some measure deliver to our posterity; as explained in Sect. XXXIX.

6. When the progressive motions of external bodies make a part of our present catenation of ideas, we attend to the lapse of time; which appears the longer, the more frequently we thus attend to it; as when we expect something at a certain hour, which much interests us, whether it be an agreeable or disagreeable event; or when we count the passing seconds on a stop-watch.

6. When the ongoing movements of outside things are part of our current chain of thoughts, we notice the passing of time; it feels longer the more we focus on it. This happens when we're waiting for something to happen at a specific time that matters to us, whether it’s a pleasant or unpleasant event, or when we watch the seconds ticking away on a stopwatch.

When an idea of our own person, or a reflex idea of our pleasures and pains, desires and aversions, makes a part of this catenation, it is termed consciousness; and if this idea of consciousness makes a part of a catenation, which we excite by recollection, and know by the facility with which we excite it, that we have before experienced it, it is called identity, as explained above.

When an idea about ourselves, or a reflective idea about our joys and sorrows, wants and dislikes, is part of this chain, it's called consciousness. And if this idea of consciousness is part of a chain that we trigger by remembering, and we recognize how easily we can trigger it, we know we've experienced it before, which is referred to as identity, as explained above.

7. In respect to freewill, it is certain, that we cannot will to think of a new train of ideas, without previously thinking of the first link of it; as I cannot will to think of a black swan, without previously thinking of a black swan. But if I now think of a tail, I can voluntarily recollect all animals, which have tails; my will is so far free, that I can pursue the ideas linked to this idea of tail, as far as my knowledge of the subject extends; but to will without motive is to will without desire or aversion; which is as absurd as to feel without pleasure or pain; they are both solecisms in the terms. So far are we governed by the catenations of motions, which affect both the body and the mind of man, and which begin with our irritability, and end with it.

7. When it comes to free will, it’s clear that we can’t choose to think of a new chain of ideas without first considering the initial one; just like I can’t choose to think of a black swan without already having that thought in my mind. However, if I now think of a tail, I can voluntarily recall all animals that have tails; my will is free enough to explore the ideas connected to this concept of a tail, as far as my knowledge allows. But to will without a reason is to will without any desire or aversion; that’s as ridiculous as feeling without pleasure or pain; both situations are contradictions in terms. We are influenced by the connections of thoughts that affect both our body and mind, starting with our impulses and ending with them.



SECT. XVI.

OF INSTINCT.

OF INSTINCT.

Haud equidem credo, quia sit divinitus illis

Haud equidem credo, quia sit divinitus illis

Ingenium, aut rerum fato prudentia major.—Virg. Georg. L. I. 415.

Ingenium, or the wisdom of fate, is greater than all things. —Virg. Georg. L. I. 415.

I. Instinctive actions defined. Of connate passions. II. Of the sensations and motions of the fœtus in the womb. III. Some animals are more perfectly formed than others before nativity. Of learning to walk. IV. Of the swallowing, breathing, sucking, pecking, and lapping of young animals. V. Of the sense of smell, and its uses to animals. Why cats do not eat their kittens. VI. Of the accuracy of sight in mankind, and their sense of beauty. Of the sense of touch in elephants, monkies, beavers, men. VII. Of natural language. VIII. The origin of natural language; 1. the language of fear; 2. of grief; 3. of tender pleasure; 4. of serene pleasure; 5. of anger; 6. of attention. IX. Artificial language of turkies, hens, ducklings, wagtails, cuckoos, rabbits, dogs, and nightingales. X. Of music; of tooth-edge; of a good ear; of architecture. XI. Of acquired knowledge; of foxes, rooks, fieldfares, lapwings, dogs, cats, horses, crows, and pelicans. XII. Of birds of passage, dormice, snakes, bats, swallows, quails, ringdoves, stare, chaffinch, hoopoe, chatterer, hawfinch, crossbill, rails and cranes. XIII. Of birds nests; of the cuckoo; of swallows nests; of the taylor bird. XIV. Of the old soldier; of haddocks, cods, and dog fish; of the remora; of crabs, herrings, and salmon. XV. Of spiders, caterpillars, ants, and the ichneumon. XVI. 1. Of locusts, gnats; 2. bees; 3. dormice, flies, worms, ants, and wasps. XVII. Of the faculty that distinguishes man from the brutes.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Instinctive behaviors explained. About innate feelings. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Concerning the sensations and movements of the fetus in the womb. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Some animals are better formed than others before birth. On learning to walk. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. On the actions of swallowing, breathing, sucking, pecking, and lapping in young animals. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. About the sense of smell and its benefits for animals. Why cats don't eat their kittens. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Regarding the accuracy of sight in humans and their sense of beauty. Touch perception in elephants, monkeys, beavers, and people. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. On natural language. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. The origins of natural language; __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. the language of fear; __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. of sorrow; __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. of gentle happiness; __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. of peaceful joy; __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. of anger; __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. of focus. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. The artificial language of turkeys, hens, ducklings, wagtails, cuckoos, rabbits, dogs, and nightingales. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. About music; about hearing; about having a good ear; about architecture. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. Regarding learned knowledge; concerning foxes, rooks, fieldfares, lapwings, dogs, cats, horses, crows, and pelicans. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. On migratory birds, dormice, snakes, bats, swallows, quails, ringdoves, starlings, chaffinches, hoopoes, chatterers, hawfinches, crossbills, rails, and cranes. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_18__. About bird nests; of the cuckoo; of swallow nests; of the tailor bird. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_19__. Concerning old soldiers; about haddocks, cods, and dogfish; about the remora; about crabs, herrings, and salmon. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_20__. On spiders, caterpillars, ants, and the ichneumon. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_21__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_22__. About locusts, gnats; __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_23__. bees; __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_24__. dormice, flies, worms, ants, and wasps. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_25__. About the ability that sets humans apart from animals.

I. All those internal motions of animal bodies, which contribute to digest their aliment, produce their secretions, repair their injuries, or increase their growth, are performed without our attention or consciousness. They exist as well in our sleep, as in our waking hours, as well in the fœtus during the time of gestation, as in the infant after nativity, and proceed with equal regularity in the vegetable as in the animal system. These motions have been shewn in a former part of this work to depend on the irritations of peculiar fluids, and as they have never been classed amongst the instinctive actions of animals, are precluded from our present disquisition.

I. All those internal processes in animal bodies that help digest food, produce secretions, heal injuries, or promote growth happen without our awareness or consciousness. They occur during our sleep as well as when we’re awake, in the fetus during pregnancy, and in infants after birth, and they happen with the same consistency in plants as they do in animals. These processes have been discussed earlier in this work as depending on the stimulation from specific fluids, and since they haven’t been categorized as instinctive actions in animals, they are excluded from our current discussion.

But all those actions of men or animals, that are attended with consciousness, and seem neither to have been directed by their appetites, taught by their experience, nor deduced from observation or tradition, have been referred to the power of instinct. And this power has been explained to be a divine something, a kind of inspiration; whilst the poor animal, that possesses it, has been thought little better than a machine!

But all the actions of people or animals that involve awareness and don't seem to be driven by their desires, shaped by their experiences, or derived from observation or tradition, have been attributed to the force of instinct. This force has been described as a divine something, a kind of inspiration; meanwhile, the unfortunate animal that has it has been seen as little more than a machine!

The irksomeness, that attends a continued attitude of the body, or the pains, that we receive from heat, cold, hunger, or other injurious circumstances, excite us to general locomotion: and our senses are so formed and constituted by the hand of nature, that certain objects present us with pleasure, others with pain, and we are induced to approach and embrace these, to avoid and abhor those, as such sensations direct us.

The annoyance that comes from staying in the same position for too long, or the discomfort we feel from heat, cold, hunger, or other harmful situations, drives us to move around: and our senses are designed by nature in such a way that some things bring us pleasure, while others bring us pain, leading us to seek out and welcome the pleasurable, and to avoid and reject the painful, as these feelings guide us.

Thus the palates of some animals are gratefully affected by the mastication of fruits, others of grains, and others of flesh; and they are thence instigated to attain, and to consume those materials; and are furnished with powers of muscular motion, and of digestion proper for such purposes.

Thus, the taste preferences of some animals are positively influenced by chewing fruits, others by grains, and others by meat; and this motivates them to seek out and consume those foods; and they are equipped with the muscular abilities and digestive systems needed for such tasks.

These sensations and desires constitute a part of our system, as our muscles and bones constitute another part: and hence they may alike be termed natural or connate; but neither of them can properly be termed instinctive: as the word instinct in its usual acceptation refers only to the actions of animals, as above explained: the origin of these actions is the subject of our present enquiry.

These sensations and desires are part of our system, just like our muscles and bones are part of it too; therefore, they can both be called natural or innate. However, neither of them can be accurately referred to as instinctive: because the term instinct, in its common meaning, applies only to the actions of animals, as explained above. The origin of these actions is what we are currently investigating.

The reader is intreated carefully to attend to this definition of instinctive actions, lest by using the word instinct without adjoining any accurate idea to it, he may not only include the natural desires of love and hunger, and the natural sensations of pain or pleasure, but the figure and contexture of the body, and the faculty of reason itself under this general term.

The reader is asked to pay close attention to this definition of instinctive actions, so that by using the word instinct without attaching a precise meaning to it, they do not unintentionally include natural desires like love and hunger, as well as the natural feelings of pain or pleasure, the shape and structure of the body, and the ability to reason itself under this broad term.

II. We experience some sensations, and perform some actions before our nativity; the sensations of cold and warmth, agitation and rest, fulness and inanition, are instances of the former; and the repeated struggles of the limbs of the fœtus, which begin about the middle of gestation, and those motions by which it frequently wraps the umbilical chord around its neck or body, and even sometimes ties it on a knot; are instances of the latter. Smellie's Midwifery, (Vol. I. p. 182.)

II. We experience certain sensations and carry out specific actions before we are born; sensations of cold and warmth, movement and stillness, fullness and emptiness are examples of the former. The repeated movements of the fetus's limbs, which start around the middle of pregnancy, and the ways it often wraps the umbilical cord around its neck or body, and even sometimes ties it in a knot, are examples of the latter. Smellie's Midwifery, (Vol. I. p. 182.)

By a due attention to these circumstances many of the actions of young animals, which at first sight seemed only referable to an inexplicable instinct, will appear to have been acquired like all other animal actions, that are attended with consciousness, by the repeated efforts of our muscles under the conduct of our sensations or desires.

By paying close attention to these situations, many of the behaviors of young animals, which at first glance seemed only linked to some mysterious instinct, will actually seem to have been learned like all other animal behaviors that involve awareness, through the repeated efforts of our muscles guided by our sensations or desires.

The chick in the shell begins to move its feet and legs on the sixth day of incubation (Mattreican, p. 138); or on the seventh day, (Langley); afterwards they are seen to move themselves gently in the liquid that surrounds them, and to open and shut their mouths, (Harvei, de Generat. p. 62, and 197. Form de Poulet. ii. p. 129). Puppies before the membranes are broken, that involve them, are seen to move themselves, to put out their tongues, and to open and shut their mouths, (Harvey, Gipson, Riolan, Haller). And calves lick themselves and swallow many of their hairs before their nativity: which however puppies do not, (Swammerden, p. 319. Flemyng Phil. Trans. Ann. 1755. 42). And towards the end of gestation, the fœtus of all animals are proved to drink part of the liquid in which they swim, (Haller. Physiol. T. 8. 204). The white of egg is found in the mouth and gizzard of the chick, and is nearly or quite consumed before it is hatched, (Harvie de Generat. 58). And the liquor amnii is found in the mouth and stomach of the human fœtus, and of calves; and how else should that excrement be produced in the intestines of all animals, which is voided in great quantity soon after their birth; (Gipson, Med. Essays, Edinb. V. i. 13. Halleri Physiolog. T. 3. p. 318. and T. 8). In the stomach of a calf the quantity of this liquid amounted to about three pints, and the hairs amongst it were of the same colour with those on its skin, (Blasii Anat. Animal, p.m. 122). These facts are attested by many other writers of credit, besides those above mentioned.

The chick inside the shell starts moving its feet and legs on the sixth day of incubation (Mattreican, p. 138); or on the seventh day (Langley). After that, they can be seen gently moving in the fluid around them and opening and closing their mouths (Harvei, de Generat. p. 62, and 197. Form de Poulet. ii. p. 129). Puppies, before the membranes that surround them are broken, are observed to move, stick out their tongues, and open and close their mouths (Harvey, Gipson, Riolan, Haller). Calves lick themselves and swallow many of their hairs before they are born, although puppies do not do this (Swammerden, p. 319. Flemyng Phil. Trans. Ann. 1755. 42). Toward the end of pregnancy, the fetuses of all animals are shown to drink some of the liquid they are in (Haller. Physiol. T. 8. 204). The egg white is found in the mouth and gizzard of the chick, and it is mostly or completely consumed before hatching (Harvie de Generat. 58). The amniotic fluid is found in the mouth and stomach of human fetuses and calves; otherwise, how would the waste in the intestines of all animals be produced, which is expelled in large quantities soon after birth (Gipson, Med. Essays, Edinb. V. i. 13. Halleri Physiolog. T. 3. p. 318. and T. 8)? In the stomach of a calf, the amount of this liquid was about three pints, and the hairs mixed in with it were the same color as those on its skin (Blasii Anat. Animal, p.m. 122). Many other credible writers, in addition to those mentioned above, support these findings.

III. It has been deemed a surprising instance of instinct, that calves and chickens should be able to walk by a few efforts almost immediately after their nativity: whilst the human infant in those countries where he is not incumbered with clothes, as in India, is five or six months, and in our climate almost a twelvemonth, before he can safely stand upon his feet.

III. It's surprisingly instinctive that calves and chicks can start walking almost right after they’re born, while human infants in places like India, where they aren’t bundled up in clothes, take about five or six months, and in our climate, nearly a year, before they can safely stand on their own.

The struggles of all animals in the womb must resemble their mode of swimming, as by this kind of motion they can best change their attitude in water. But the swimming of the calf and chicken resembles their manner of walking, which they have thus in part acquired before their nativity, and hence accomplish it afterwards with very few efforts, whilst the swimming of the human creature resembles that of the frog, and totally differs from his mode of walking.

The struggles of all animals in the womb must be similar to how they swim, as this movement allows them to adjust their position in the water most effectively. However, the way calves and chicks swim is similar to how they walk, having learned this partly before they were born, which makes it easy for them to swim later on. In contrast, how humans swim is more like how frogs swim and is completely different from how they walk.

There is another circumstance to be attended to in this affair, that not only the growth of those peculiar parts of animals, which are first wanted to secure their subsistence, are in general furthest advanced before their nativity: but some animals come into the world more completely formed throughout their whole system than others: and are thence much forwarder in all their habits of motion. Thus the colt, and the lamb, are much more perfect animals than the blind puppy, and the naked rabbit; and the chick of the pheasant, and the partridge, has more perfect plumage, and more perfect eyes, as well as greater aptitude to locomotion, than the callow nestlings of the dove, and of the wren. The parents of the former only find it necessary to shew them their food, and to teach them to take it up; whilst those of the latter are obliged for many days to obtrude it into their gaping mouths.

There’s another aspect to consider in this situation: not only are the essential parts of animals that help them survive generally more developed before they are born, but some animals are also born more fully formed than others, which gives them an advantage in their movements. For example, a colt and a lamb are much more complete in their development than a blind puppy and a naked rabbit. Similarly, a pheasant or partridge chick has better feathers and more developed eyesight, as well as greater ability to move around, compared to the helpless baby doves and wrens. The parents of the former only need to show them their food and teach them how to eat, while the parents of the latter have to stuff food into their open mouths for several days.

IV. From the facts mentioned in No. 2. of this Section, it is evinced that the fœtus learns to swallow before its nativity; for it is seen to open its mouth, and its stomach is found filled with the liquid that surrounds it. It opens its mouth, either instigated by hunger, or by the irksomeness of a continued attitude of the muscles of its face; the liquor amnii, in which it swims, is agreeable to its palate, as it consists of a nourishing material, (Haller Phys. T. 8. p. 204). It is tempted to experience its taste further in the mouth, and by a few efforts learns to swallow, in the same manner as we learn all other animal actions, which are attended with consciousness, by the repeated efforts of our muscles under the conduct of our sensations or volitions.

IV. From the facts mentioned in No. 2. of this Section, it is evinced that the fœtus learns to swallow before its nativity; for it is seen to open its mouth, and its stomach is found filled with the liquid that surrounds it. It opens its mouth, either instigated by hunger, or by the irksomeness of a continued attitude of the muscles of its face; the liquor amnii, in which it swims, is agreeable to its palate, as it consists of a nourishing material, (Haller Phys. T. 8. p. 204). It is tempted to experience its taste further in the mouth, and by a few efforts learns to swallow, in the same manner as we learn all other animal actions, which are attended with consciousness, by the repeated efforts of our muscles under the conduct of our sensations or volitions.

The inspiration of air into the lungs is so totally different from that of swallowing a fluid in which we are immersed, that it cannot be acquired before our nativity. But at this time, when the circulation of the blood is no longer continued through the placenta, that suffocating sensation, which we feel about the precordia, when we are in want of fresh air, disagreeably affects the infant: and all the muscles of the body are excited into action to relieve this oppression; those of the breast, ribs, and diaphragm are found to answer this purpose, and thus respiration is discovered, and is continued throughout our lives, as often as the oppression begins to recur. Many infants, both of the human creature, and of quadrupeds, struggle for a minute after they are born before they begin to breathe, (Haller Phys. T. 8. p. 400. ib pt. 2. p. 1). Mr. Buffon thinks the action of the dry air upon the nerves of smell of new-born animals, by producing an endeavour to sneeze, may contribute to induce this first inspiration, and that the rarefaction of the air by the warmth of the lungs contributes to induce expiration, (Hist. Nat. Tom. 4. p. 174). Which latter it may effect by producing a disagreeable sensation by its delay, and a consequent effort to relieve it. Many children sneeze before they respire, but not all, as far as I have observed, or can learn from others.

The way we take air into our lungs is completely different from how we swallow a liquid we're surrounded by, and this can't be learned before we're born. However, once the blood flow through the placenta stops, the uncomfortable feeling in the chest that we experience when we need fresh air affects newborns. This leads all the muscles in their bodies to spring into action to relieve the pressure; the muscles in the chest, ribs, and diaphragm help with this, and that’s how breathing is discovered. It continues throughout our lives every time we feel that pressure again. Many newborns, both human and animal, struggle for a minute after birth before they begin to breathe. Mr. Buffon suggests that the dry air affects the smell nerves in newborn animals, prompting a sneeze that might help trigger that first breath, and that the warming of the lungs causes the air to expand, leading to exhaling. This last part could happen because the delay creates an uncomfortable sensation, prompting an effort to relieve it. Many babies sneeze before they take their first breath, but not all do, as far as I've seen or learned from others.

At length, by the direction of its sense of smell, or by the officious care of its mother, the young animal approaches the odoriferous rill of its future nourishment, already experienced to swallow. But in the act of swallowing, it is necessary nearly to close the mouth, whether the creature be immersed in the fluid it is about to drink, or not: hence, when the child first attempts to suck, it does not slightly compress the nipple between its lips, and suck as an adult person would do, by absorbing the milk; but it takes the whole nipple into its mouth for this purpose, compresses it between its gums, and thus repeatedly chewing (as it were) the nipple, presses out the milk, exactly in the same manner as it is drawn from the teats of cows by the hands of the milkmaid. The celebrated Harvey observes, that the fœtus in the womb must have sucked in a part of its nourishment, because it knows how to suck the minute it is born, as any one may experience by putting a finger between its lips, and because in a few days it forgets this art of sucking, and cannot without some difficulty again acquire it, (Exercit. de Gener. Anim. 48). The same observation is made by Hippocrates.

Eventually, guided by its sense of smell or the attentive care of its mother, the young animal approaches the fragrant stream of its future food, already familiar with how to swallow. However, when it swallows, it needs to almost close its mouth, whether it's in the liquid it's about to drink or not. So, when the baby first tries to suck, it doesn’t just gently squeeze the nipple between its lips and suck like an adult would, by drawing in the milk. Instead, it takes the whole nipple into its mouth, compresses it with its gums, and by repeatedly chewing on the nipple, it forces out the milk, just like how a milkmaid milks a cow. The famous Harvey notes that the fetus in the womb must have sucked some of its nourishment because it knows how to suck the moment it's born, as anyone can see by placing a finger between its lips. Additionally, it quickly forgets this ability to suck in just a few days and struggles to relearn it (Exercit. de Gener. Anim. 48). Hippocrates makes the same observation.

A little further experience teaches the young animal to suck by absorption, as well as by compression; that is, to open the chest as in the beginning of respiration, and thus to rarefy the air in the mouth, that the pressure of the denser external atmosphere may contribute to force out the milk.

A little more experience teaches the young animal to suck by both absorption and compression. In other words, it learns to open its chest like it does at the start of breathing, which allows it to create a vacuum in its mouth so that the higher pressure in the surrounding air helps push the milk out.

The chick yet in the shell has learnt to drink by swallowing a part of the white of the egg for its food; but not having experienced how to take up and swallow solid seeds, or grains, is either taught by the felicitous industry of its mother; or by many repeated attempts is enabled at length to distinguish and to swallow this kind of nutriment.

The chick still in the shell has figured out how to drink by consuming some of the egg white for food; however, since it hasn't yet learned how to pick up and swallow solid seeds or grains, it either learns from its mother's careful guidance or, after many attempts, finally manages to recognize and eat this type of food.

And puppies, though they know how to suck like other animals from their previous experience in swallowing, and in respiration; yet are they long in acquiring the art of lapping with their tongues, which from the flaccidity of their cheeks, and length of their mouths, is afterwards a more convenient way for them to take in water.

And puppies, even though they know how to suck like other animals from their past experiences with swallowing and breathing, take a while to learn how to lap water with their tongues. This method becomes more convenient for them later on because of the softness of their cheeks and the length of their mouths.

V. The senses of smell and taste in many other animals greatly excel those of mankind, for in civilized society, as our victuals are generally prepared by others, and are adulterated with salt, spice, oil, and empyreuma, we do not hesitate about eating whatever is set before us, and neglect to cultivate these senses: whereas other animals try every morsel by the smell, before they take it into their mouths, and by the taste before they swallow it: and are led not only each to his proper nourishment by this organ of sense, but it also at a maturer age directs them in the gratification of their appetite of love. Which may be further understood by considering the sympathies of these parts described in Class IV. 2. 1. 7. While the human animal is directed to the object of his love by his sense of beauty, as mentioned in No. VI. of this Section. Thus Virgil. Georg. III. 250.

V. The senses of smell and taste in many other animals greatly excel those of mankind, for in civilized society, as our victuals are generally prepared by others, and are adulterated with salt, spice, oil, and empyreuma, we do not hesitate about eating whatever is set before us, and neglect to cultivate these senses: whereas other animals try every morsel by the smell, before they take it into their mouths, and by the taste before they swallow it: and are led not only each to his proper nourishment by this organ of sense, but it also at a maturer age directs them in the gratification of their appetite of love. Which may be further understood by considering the sympathies of these parts described in Class IV. 2. 1. 7. While the human animal is directed to the object of his love by his sense of beauty, as mentioned in No. VI. of this Section. Thus Virgil. Georg. III. 250.

Nonne vides, ut tota tremor pertentat equorum

Nonne vides, how the whole tremor affects the horses

Corpora, si tantum notas odor attulit auras?

Corpora, if only the scent has brought the breezes?

Nonne canis nidum veneris nasutus odore

Nonne canis nidum veneris nasutus odore

Quærit, et erranti trahitur sublambere linguâ?

Quærit, et erranti trahitur sublambere linguâ?

Respuit at gustum cupidus, labiisque retractis

Respuit at gustum cupidus, labiisque retractis

Elevat os, trepidansque novis impellitur æstris

Elevator, trembling, is propelled by new stars.

Inserit et vivum felici vomere semen.—

Inserit et vivum felici vomere semen.—

Quam tenui filo cæcos adnectit amores

Quam tenui filo cæcos adnectit amores

Docta Venus, vitæque monet renovare favillam!—ANON.

Docta Venus, reminds us to renew the spark of life!—ANON.

The following curious experiment is related by Galen. "On dissecting a goat great with young I found a brisk embryon, and having detached it from the matrix, and snatching it away before it saw its dam, I brought it into a certain room, where there were many vessels, some filled with wine, others with oil, some with honey, others with milk, or some other liquor; and in others were grains and fruits; we first observed the young animal get upon its feet, and walk; then it shook itself, and afterwards scratched its side with one of its feet: then we saw it smelling to every one of these things, that were set in the room; and when it had smelt to them all, it drank up the milk." L. 6. de locis. cap. 6.

The following interesting experiment is reported by Galen. "When I dissected a pregnant goat, I found a lively embryo. After I separated it from the womb and took it away before it could see its mother, I brought it into a room filled with various containers, some with wine, others with oil, some with honey, others with milk, or other liquids; and in some were grains and fruits. We first watched the young animal get on its feet and walk; then it shook itself and scratched its side with one of its feet. Next, we saw it sniffing all the items placed in the room; and after it had smelled everything, it drank the milk." L. 6. de locis. cap. 6.

Parturient quadrupeds, as cats, and bitches, and sows, are led by their sense of smell to eat the placenta as other common food; why then do they not devour their whole progeny, as is represented in an antient emblem of TIME? This is said sometimes to happen in the unnatural state in which we confine sows; and indeed nature would seem to have endangered her offspring in this nice circumstance! But at this time the stimulus of the milk in the tumid teats of the mother excites her to look out for, and to desire some unknown circumstance to relieve her. At the same time the smell of the milk attracts the exertions of the young animals towards its source, and thus the delighted mother discovers a new appetite, as mentioned in Sect. XIV. 8. and her little progeny are led to receive and to communicate pleasure by this most beautiful contrivance.

Parturient quadrupeds, as cats, and bitches, and sows, are led by their sense of smell to eat the placenta as other common food; why then do they not devour their whole progeny, as is represented in an antient emblem of TIME? This is said sometimes to happen in the unnatural state in which we confine sows; and indeed nature would seem to have endangered her offspring in this nice circumstance! But at this time the stimulus of the milk in the tumid teats of the mother excites her to look out for, and to desire some unknown circumstance to relieve her. At the same time the smell of the milk attracts the exertions of the young animals towards its source, and thus the delighted mother discovers a new appetite, as mentioned in Sect. XIV. 8. and her little progeny are led to receive and to communicate pleasure by this most beautiful contrivance.

VI. But though the human species in some of their sensations are much inferior to other animals, yet the accuracy of the sense of touch, which they possess in so eminent a degree, gives them a great superiority of understanding; as is well observed by the ingenious Mr. Buffon. The extremities of other animals terminate in horns, and hoofs, and claws, very unfit for the sensation of touch; whilst the human hand is finely adapted to encompass its object with this organ of sense.

VI. Although humans may be less skilled in some sensations compared to other animals, their highly developed sense of touch provides them with a significant edge in understanding, as noted by the clever Mr. Buffon. Other animals have extremities that end in horns, hooves, and claws, which are not well-suited for the sense of touch; in contrast, the human hand is perfectly designed to grasp and feel its surroundings with this sensory organ.

The elephant is indeed endued with a fine sense of feeling at the extremity of his proboscis, and hence has acquired much more accurate ideas of touch and of sight than most other creatures. The two following instances of the sagacity of these animals may entertain the reader, as they were told me by some gentlemen of distinct observation, and undoubted veracity, who had been much conversant with our eastern settlements. First, the elephants that are used to carry the baggage of our armies, are put each under the care of one of the natives of Indostan, and whilst himself and his wife go into the woods to collect leaves and branches of trees for his food, they fix him to the ground by a length of chain, and frequently leave a child yet unable to walk, under his protection: and the intelligent animal not only defends it, but as it creeps about, when it arrives near the extremity of his chain, he wraps his trunk gently round its body, and brings it again into the centre of his circle. Secondly, the traitor elephants are taught to walk on a narrow path between two pit-falls, which are covered with turf, and then to go into the woods, and to seduce the wild elephants to come that way, who fall into these wells, whilst he passes safe between them: and it is universally observed, that those wild elephants that escape the snare, pursue the traitor with the utmost vehemence, and if they can overtake him, which sometimes happens, they always beat him to death.

The elephant has a remarkable sense of feel at the tip of its trunk, which gives it much better ideas of touch and sight than most other animals. The following two examples of these animals' intelligence might intrigue you, as they were shared with me by some observant and trustworthy gentlemen who have spent a lot of time in our eastern settlements. First, the elephants used to carry our army's baggage are each assigned to a native from Indostan. While that person and his wife go into the woods to gather leaves and branches for food, they anchor the elephant to the ground with a chain and often leave a child, too young to walk, in its care. The smart animal not only protects the child but also gently wraps its trunk around the child and pulls it back toward the center of its area when it wanders too close to the end of the chain. Secondly, the traitor elephants are trained to walk along a narrow path between two concealed pits, which are covered with grass, and then go into the woods to lure wild elephants into that direction, causing them to fall into these traps while the traitor safely passes by. It's commonly noted that the wild elephants that escape the trap pursue the traitor with great intensity, and if they catch up to him— which occasionally happens— they will always beat him to death.

The monkey has a hand well enough adapted for the sense of touch, which contributes to his great facility of imitation; but in taking objects with his hands, as a stick or an apple, he puts his thumb on the same side of them with his fingers, instead of counteracting the pressure of his fingers with it: from this neglect he is much slower in acquiring the figures of objects, as he is less able to determine the distances or diameters of their parts, or to distinguish their vis inertiæ from their hardness. Helvetius adds, that the shortness of his life, his being fugitive before mankind, and his not inhabiting all climates, combine to prevent his improvement. (De l'Esprit. T. 1. p.) There is however at this time an old monkey shewn in Exeter Change, London, who having lost his teeth, when nuts are given him, takes a stone into his hand, and cracks them with it one by one; thus using tools to effect his purpose like mankind.

The monkey has a hand that's well-suited for touch, which helps him imitate others easily. However, when he grabs objects like a stick or an apple, he places his thumb on the same side as his fingers instead of using it to counterbalance the pressure from his fingers. Because of this oversight, he takes longer to learn the shapes of objects, as he struggles with judging distances or sizes of their parts, and distinguishing their resistance to movement from their hardness. Helvetius points out that his short lifespan, tendency to flee from humans, and inability to live in all climates all limit his ability to improve. (De l'Esprit. T. 1. p.) Nevertheless, there is currently an old monkey displayed at Exeter Change in London who, after losing his teeth, takes a stone in his hand and cracks nuts one by one, using tools to achieve his goal like humans do.

The beaver is another animal that makes much use of his hands, and if we may credit the reports of travellers, is possessed of amazing ingenuity. This however, M. Buffon affirms, is only where they exist in large numbers, and in countries thinly peopled with men; while in France in their solitary state they shew no uncommon ingenuity.

The beaver is another animal that makes great use of its hands, and if we can trust the reports from travelers, it has incredible ingenuity. However, M. Buffon claims this is only true when they live in large groups and in sparsely populated areas; in France, when they are alone, they show no unusual cleverness.

Indeed all the quadrupeds, that have collar-bones, (claviculæ) use their fore-limbs in some measure as we use our hands, as the cat, squirrel, tyger, bear and lion; and as they exercise the sense of touch more universally than other animals, so are they more sagacious in watching and surprising their prey. All those birds, that use their claws for hands, as the hawk, parrot, and cuckoo, appear to be more docile and intelligent; though the gregarious tribes of birds have more acquired knowledge.

Indeed, all four-legged animals that have collar bones (clavicles) use their front limbs somewhat like we use our hands, such as cats, squirrels, tigers, bears, and lions. Since they rely on their sense of touch more than other animals, they tend to be more clever in observing and catching their prey. Birds that use their claws like hands, such as hawks, parrots, and cuckoos, seem to be more trainable and smart, although social groups of birds have a greater amount of learned knowledge.

Now as the images, that are painted on the retina of the eye, are no other than signs, which recall to our imaginations the objects we had before examined by the organ of touch, as is fully demonstrated by Dr. Berkley in his treatise on vision; it follows that the human creature has greatly more accurate and distinct sense of vision than that of any other animal. Whence as he advances to maturity he gradually acquires a sense of female beauty, which at this time directs him to the object of his new passion.

Now, the images that are projected onto the retina of the eye are just signs that trigger our imaginations to recall the objects we've previously examined with our sense of touch, as thoroughly explained by Dr. Berkeley in his work on vision. This means that humans have a much more accurate and distinct sense of sight than any other animal. As a person matures, they gradually develop a sense of female beauty, which at this point leads them to the object of their new passion.

Sentimental love, as distinguished from the animal passion of that name, with which it is frequently accompanied, consists in the desire or sensation of beholding, embracing, and saluting a beautiful object.

Sentimental love, unlike the raw passion often associated with it, is about the longing or feeling of seeing, holding, and greeting something beautiful.

The characteristic of beauty therefore is that it is the object of love; and though many other objects are in common language called beautiful, yet they are only called so metaphorically, and ought to be termed agreeable. A Grecian temple may give us the pleasurable idea of sublimity, a Gothic temple may give us the pleasurable idea of variety, and a modern house the pleasurable idea of utility; music and poetry may inspire our love by association of ideas; but none of these, except metaphorically, can be termed beautiful; as we have no wish to embrace or salute them.

The essence of beauty is that it represents something we love; and while many other things are commonly referred to as beautiful, it’s usually just a metaphor, and they should really be called agreeable. A Greek temple might evoke feelings of grandeur, a Gothic temple might inspire thoughts of diversity, and a modern house might represent practicality; music and poetry can stir our affection through associations; but none of these can truly be called beautiful, except in a metaphorical sense, since we don’t actually want to embrace or greet them.

Our perception of beauty consists in our recognition by the sense of vision of those objects, first, which have before inspired our love by the pleasure, which they have afforded to many of our senses: as to our sense of warmth, of touch, of smell, of taste, hunger and thirst; and, secondly, which bear any analogy of form to such objects.

Our perception of beauty comes from what we see and recognize, mainly those things that have previously evoked our affection by providing pleasure to many of our senses, like our sense of warmth, touch, smell, taste, hunger, and thirst. Additionally, it includes objects that have a similar shape to those that we find beautiful.

When the babe, soon after it is born into this cold world, is applied to its mother's bosom; its sense of perceiving warmth is first agreeably affected; next its sense of smell is delighted with the odour of her milk; then its taste is gratified by the flavour of it: afterwards the appetites of hunger and of thirst afford pleasure by the possession of their objects, and by the subsequent digestion of the aliment; and, lastly, the sense of touch is delighted by the softness and smoothness of the milky fountain, the source of such variety of happiness.

When a baby is born into this cold world and is placed against its mother's chest, it first feels the comforting warmth. Then, it enjoys the smell of her milk, followed by the taste of it. Next, the feelings of hunger and thirst bring pleasure as they are satisfied, and the process of digesting the food adds to that joy. Finally, the sense of touch delights in the softness and smoothness of the milk, the source of such a variety of happiness.

All these various kinds of pleasure at length become associated with the form of the mother's breast; which the infant embraces with its hands, presses with its lips, and watches with its eyes; and thus acquires more accurate ideas of the form of its mother's bosom, than of the odour and flavour or warmth, which it perceives by its other senses. And hence at our maturer years, when any object of vision is presented to us, which by its waving or spiral lines bears any similitude to the form of the female bosom, whether it be found in a landscape with soft gradations of rising and descending surface, or in the forms of some antique vases, or in other works of the pencil or the chissel, we feel a general glow of delight, which seems to influence all our senses; and, if the object be not too large, we experience an attraction to embrace it with our arms, and to salute it with our lips, as we did in our early infancy the bosom of our mother. And thus we find, according to the ingenious idea of Hogarth, that the waving lines of beauty were originally taken from the temple of Venus.

All these different kinds of pleasure eventually become linked to the shape of the mother's breast, which the baby hold with its hands, kisses with its lips, and gazes at with its eyes. This way, the baby gains a clearer understanding of the form of its mother’s bosom than of the scent, taste, or warmth it perceives through its other senses. As we grow older, when we encounter any visual object that has wavy or spiral lines resembling the female bosom—whether in a landscape with gentle slopes or in the shapes of old vases, or in other artistic works—we feel a warm sense of joy that seems to affect all our senses. If the object isn’t too large, we feel drawn to wrap our arms around it and kiss it, just like we did as infants with our mother’s bosom. Thus, as the clever Hogarth suggested, the flowing lines of beauty were originally inspired by the temple of Venus.

This animal attraction is love; which is a sensation, when the object is present; and a desire, when it is absent. Which constitutes the purest source of human felicity, the cordial drop in the otherwise vapid cup of life, and which overpays mankind for the care and labour, which are attached to the pre-eminence of his situation above other animals.

This animal attraction is love; it's a feeling when the person is around, and a longing when they’re not. It’s the purest source of human happiness, the sweet moment in an otherwise dull life, and it compensates people for the effort and trouble that come with being at the top of the animal kingdom.

It should have been observed, that colour as well as form sometimes enters into our idea of a beautiful object, as a good complexion for instance, because a fine or fair colour is in general a sign of health, and conveys to us an idea of the warmth of the object; and a pale countenance on the contrary gives an idea of its being cold to the touch.

It should be noted that color, along with shape, often influences our perception of beauty. For example, a good complexion typically indicates good health and gives us a sense of warmth from the object. In contrast, a pale face suggests that it would feel cold to the touch.

It was before remarked, that young animals use their lips to distinguish the forms of things, as well as their fingers, and hence we learn the origin of our inclination to salute beautiful objects with our lips. For a definition of Grace, see Class III. 1. 2. 4.

It was previously noted that young animals use their lips to recognize the shapes of things, just like they use their fingers, and from this, we understand why we have the tendency to greet beautiful objects with our lips. For a definition of Grace, see Class III. 1. 2. 4.

VII. There are two ways by which we become acquainted with the passions of others: first, by having observed the effects of them, as of fear or anger, on our own bodies, we know at sight when others are under the influence of these affections. So when two cocks are preparing to fight, each feels the feathers rise round his own neck, and knows from the same sign the disposition of his adversary: and children long before they can speak, or understand the language of their parents, may be frightened by an angry countenance, or soothed by smiles and blandishments.

VII. There are two ways we get to know the feelings of others: first, by observing how these feelings, like fear or anger, affect our own bodies, we can easily tell when someone else is experiencing them. For example, when two roosters are getting ready to fight, each one can feel their own feathers standing up, which tells them how their opponent is feeling. Similarly, children, even before they can talk or understand their parents' words, can be scared by an angry face or comforted by smiles and gentle words.

Secondly, when we put ourselves into the attitude that any passion naturally occasions, we soon in some degree acquire that passion; hence when those that scold indulge themselves in loud oaths, and violent actions of the arms, they increase their anger by the mode of expressing themselves: and on the contrary the counterfeited smile of pleasure in disagreeable company soon brings along with it a portion of the reality, as is well illustrated by Mr. Burke. (Essay on the Sublime and Beautiful.)

Secondly, when we adopt the mindset that any passion naturally brings, we quickly start to feel that passion to some extent; thus, when people who scold let loose with loud curses and aggressive gestures, they intensify their anger through their way of expressing it. Conversely, a fake smile of enjoyment in an unpleasant situation often leads to some genuine feelings of pleasure, as Mr. Burke illustrates well. (Essay on the Sublime and Beautiful.)

This latter method of entering into the passions of others is rendered of very extensive use by the pleasure we take in imitation, which is every day presented before our eyes, in the actions of children, and indeed in all the customs and fashions of the world. From this our aptitude to imitation, arises what is generally understood by the word sympathy so well explained by Dr. Smith of Glasgow. Thus the appearance of a cheerful countenance gives us pleasure, and of a melancholy one makes us sorrowful. Yawning and sometimes vomiting are thus propagated by sympathy, and some people of delicate fibres, at the presence of a spectacle of misery, have felt pain in the same parts of their own bodies, that were diseased or mangled in the other. Amongst the writers of antiquity Aristotle thought this aptitude to imitation an essential property of the human species, and calls man an imitative animal. Το ζωον μιμωμενον.

This later method of connecting with the emotions of others is widely used because we enjoy imitating what we see, which is evident in the behaviors of children and in all the social customs and trends around us. Our natural ability to imitate leads to what we commonly understand as sympathy, a concept thoroughly explained by Dr. Smith of Glasgow. For instance, seeing a happy face brings us joy, while seeing a sad one brings us sorrow. Yawning and even vomiting can spread through sympathy, and some sensitive individuals may feel pain in the same areas of their own bodies that are hurt or harmed in others when confronted with scenes of suffering. Among ancient writers, Aristotle believed that this tendency to imitate is a fundamental trait of humans, calling man an imitative creature. Το ζώο μιμώμενον.

These then are the natural signs by which we understand each other, and on this slender basis is built all human language. For without some natural signs, no artificial ones could have been invented or understood, as is very ingeniously observed by Dr. Reid. (Inquiry into the Human Mind.)

These are the natural signs that help us understand each other, and on this fragile foundation, all human language is built. Without some natural signs, no artificial ones could have been created or comprehended, as Dr. Reid cleverly points out. (Inquiry into the Human Mind.)

VIII. The origin of this universal language is a subject of the highest curiosity, the knowledge of which has always been thought utterly inaccessible. A part of which we shall however here attempt.

VIII. The origin of this universal language is a topic of great interest, and people have always believed that understanding it is completely out of reach. However, we will try to explore part of it here.

Light, sound, and odours, are unknown to the fœtus in the womb, which, except the few sensations and motions already mentioned, sleeps away its time insensible of the busy world. But the moment he arrives into day, he begins to experience many vivid pains and pleasures; these are at the same time attended with certain muscular motions, and from this their early, and individual association, they acquire habits of occurring together, that are afterwards indissoluble.

Light, sound, and smells are unknown to the fetus in the womb, which, aside from the few sensations and movements already mentioned, spends its time sleeping, unaware of the busy world. But the moment he is born, he starts to feel many intense pains and pleasures; these are accompanied by specific muscle movements, and from this initial, individual connection, they develop habits of happening together that become impossible to break later on.

1. Of Fear.

1. About Fear.

As soon as the young animal is born, the first important sensations, that occur to him, are occasioned by the oppression about his precordia for want of respiration, and by his sudden transition from ninety-eight degrees of heat into so cold a climate.—He trembles, that is, he exerts alternately all the muscles of his body, to enfranchise himself from the oppression about his bosom, and begins to breathe with frequent and short respirations; at the same time the cold contracts his red skin, gradually turning it pale; the contents of the bladder and of the bowels are evacuated: and from the experience of these first disagreeable sensations the passion of fear is excited, which is no other than the expectation of disagreeable sensations. This early association of motions and sensations persists throughout life; the passion of fear produces a cold and pale skin, with tremblings, quick respiration, and an evacuation of the bladder and bowels, and thus constitutes the natural or universal language of this passion.

As soon as the young animal is born, the first important feelings it experiences come from the pressure on its chest due to a lack of breath and the sudden shift from a warm environment of ninety-eight degrees to a much colder climate. It shivers, meaning it uses all its muscles alternately to free itself from the pressure on its chest and starts to breathe rapidly and shallowly. At the same time, the cold tightens its red skin, gradually turning it pale; it also empties its bladder and bowels. From these initial unpleasant sensations, the feeling of fear is triggered, which is simply the anticipation of discomfort. This early link between movements and feelings continues throughout life; the feeling of fear leads to cold, pale skin, shivering, fast breathing, and an emptying of the bladder and bowels, creating the natural or universal expression of this feeling.

On observing a Canary bird this morning, January 28, 1772, at the house of Mr. Harvey, near Tutbury, in Derbyshire, I was told it always fainted away, when its cage was cleaned, and desired to see the experiment. The cage being taken from the ceiling, and its bottom drawn out, the bird began to tremble, and turned quite white about the root of his bill: he then opened his mouth as if for breath, and respired quick, stood straighter up on his perch, hung his wings, spread his tail, closed his eyes, and appeared quite stiff and cataleptic for near half an hour, and at length with much trembling and deep respirations came gradually to himself.

This morning, January 28, 1772, while watching a Canary bird at Mr. Harvey's house near Tutbury in Derbyshire, I was informed that the bird always fainted when its cage was cleaned, and I wanted to see this for myself. When the cage was taken down from the ceiling and its bottom removed, the bird started to shake and its beak turned completely white. It then opened its mouth as if gasping for air, breathing rapidly, stood upright on its perch, hung its wings down, spread its tail, closed its eyes, and seemed completely stiff and unresponsive for nearly half an hour. Eventually, after a lot of trembling and deep breaths, it slowly began to recover.

2. Of Grief.

2. On Grief.

That the internal membrane of the nostrils may be kept always moist, for the better perception of odours, there are two canals, that conduct the tears after they have done their office in moistening and cleaning the ball of the eye into a sack, which is called the lacrymal sack; and from which there is a duct, that opens into the nostrils: the aperture of this duct is formed of exquisite sensibility, and when it is stimulated by odorous particles, or by the dryness or coldness of the air, the sack contracts itself, and pours more of its contained moisture on the organ of smell. By this contrivance the organ is rendered more fit for perceiving such odours, and is preserved from being injured by those that are more strong or corrosive. Many other receptacles of peculiar fluids disgorge their contents, when the ends of their ducts are stimulated; as the gall bladder, when the contents of the duodenum stimulate the extremity of the common bile duct: and the salivary glands, when the termination of their ducts in the mouth are excited by the stimulus of the food we masticate. Atque vesiculæ seminales suum exprimunt fluidum glande penis fricatâ.

To keep the inner membrane of the nostrils moist for better smelling, there are two canals that carry tears, after they've done their job of moistening and cleaning the eyeball, into a pouch called the lacrimal sac. From this sac, there’s a duct that opens into the nostrils. The opening of this duct is very sensitive, and when it’s triggered by smell particles or by dry or cold air, the sac contracts and releases more moisture onto the smell organ. This mechanism makes the organ more capable of detecting odors and protects it from stronger or corrosive smells. Many other glands that hold special fluids also release their contents when the ends of their ducts are stimulated; for example, the gallbladder releases bile when the contents of the duodenum trigger the end of the common bile duct, and the salivary glands release saliva when their duct endings in the mouth are stimulated by food we chew. Similarly, the seminal vesicles release their fluid when the penis is stimulated.

The coldness and dryness of the atmosphere, compared with the warmth and moisture, which the new-born infant had just before experienced, disagreeably affects the aperture of this lacrymal sack: the tears, that are contained in this sack, are poured into the nostrils, and a further supply is secreted by the lacrymal glands, and diffused upon the eye-balls; as is very visible in the eyes and nostrils of children soon after their nativity. The same happens to us at our maturer age, for in severe frosty weather, snivelling and tears are produced by the coldness and dryness of the air.

The cold and dry air, compared to the warmth and moisture the new-born baby just experienced, unpleasantly affects the opening of the tear duct. The tears in this duct drain into the nostrils, and the tear glands produce more tears, spreading them over the eyeballs; this is clearly seen in the eyes and nostrils of infants shortly after birth. The same occurs in us as adults; during harsh, cold weather, we tend to snivel and cry due to the cold and dry air.

But the lacrymal glands, which separate the tears from the blood, are situated on the upper external part of the globes of each eye; and, when a greater quantity of tears are wanted, we contract the forehead, and bring down the eye-brows, and use many other distortions of the face, to compress these glands.

But the tear glands, which separate tears from blood, are located on the upper outer part of each eyeball. When we need more tears, we frown, furrow our brows, and make various facial expressions to squeeze these glands.

Now as the suffocating sensation, that produces respiration, is removed almost as soon as perceived, and does not recur again: this disagreeable irritation of the lacrymal ducts, as it must frequently recur, till the tender organ becomes used to variety of odours, is one of the first pains that is repeatedly attended to: and hence throughout our infancy, and in many people throughout their lives, all disagreeable sensations are attended with snivelling at the nose, a profusion of tears, and some peculiar distortions of countenance: according to the laws of early association before mentioned, which constitutes the natural or universal language of grief.

Now, as the uncomfortable feeling that triggers breathing goes away almost as soon as we notice it, and doesn’t come back: this annoying irritation of the tear ducts, which often returns until the sensitive organ gets used to various smells, is one of the first pains we notice repeatedly. Because of this, throughout our childhood, and for many people throughout their lives, all unpleasant feelings are accompanied by a runny nose, plenty of tears, and some unique facial expressions. This follows the early associations mentioned earlier, which form the natural or universal language of grief.

You may assure yourself of the truth of this observation, if you will attend to what passes, when you read a distressful tale alone; before the tears overflow your eyes, you will invariably feel a titillation at that extremity of the lacrymal duct, which terminates in the nostril, then the compression of the eyes succeeds, and the profusion of tears.

You can confirm the truth of this observation by noticing what happens when you read a sad story by yourself; before the tears start to flow, you'll usually feel a tingling sensation at the point in your tear duct that connects to your nose, then comes the tightening of your eyes, followed by a flood of tears.

Linnæus asserts, that the female bear sheds tears in grief; the same has been said of the hind, and some other animals.

Linnæus claims that female bears cry out of grief; the same has been said about does and some other animals.

3. Of Tender Pleasure.

3. Of Sweet Pleasure.

The first most lively impression of pleasure, that the infant enjoys after its nativity, is excited by the odour of its mother's milk. The organ of smell is irritated by this perfume, and the lacrymal sack empties itself into the nostrils, as before explained, and an increase of tears is poured into the eyes. Any one may observe this, when very young infants are about to suck; for at those early periods of life, the sensation affects the organ of smell, much more powerfully, than after the repeated habits of smelling has inured it to odours of common strength: and in our adult years, the stronger smells, though they are at the same time agreeable to us, as of volatile spirits, continue to produce an increased secretion of tears.

The first vivid sense of pleasure that an infant experiences right after birth comes from the smell of its mother's milk. The sense of smell is triggered by this scent, leading to tears flowing into the eyes, as previously explained. Anyone can see this happening when very young infants are about to breastfeed; during these early stages of life, their sense of smell is much more intensely affected than it is later, after they've become used to common smells. Even as adults, strong scents, like those from fragrant spirits, still cause an increased tear production, even though we find them pleasant.

This pleasing sensation of smell is followed by the early affection of the infant to the mother that suckles it, and hence the tender feelings of gratitude and love, as well as of hopeless grief, are ever after joined with the titillation of the extremity of the lacrymal ducts, and a profusion of tears.

This pleasant feeling of smell is followed by the early bond between the baby and the mother who breastfeeds it, leading to feelings of gratitude and love, as well as deep sadness, forever linked with the tickling sensation in the tear ducts and an abundance of tears.

Nor is it singular, that the lacrymal sack should be influenced by pleasing ideas, as the sight of agreeable food produces the same effect on the salivary glands. Ac dum vidimus insomniis lascivæ puellæ simulacrum tenditur penis.

Nor is it unusual for the tear ducts to be affected by pleasant thoughts, just as the sight of appealing food triggers a response in the salivary glands. Ac dum vidimus insomniis lascivæ puellæ simulacrum tenditur penis.

Lambs shake or wriggle their tails, at the time when they first suck, to get free of the hard excrement, which had been long lodged in their bowels. Hence this becomes afterwards a mark of pleasure in them, and in dogs, and other tailed animals. But cats gently extend and contract their paws when they are pleased, and purr by drawing in their breath, both which resemble their manner of sucking, and thus become their language of pleasure, for these animals having collar-bones use their paws like hands when they suck, which dogs and sheep do not.

Lambs shake or wiggle their tails when they first suck to get rid of the hard waste that has been stuck in their intestines for a while. This becomes a sign of enjoyment for them, as well as for dogs and other tailed animals. However, cats gently stretch and retract their paws when they’re happy and purr by inhaling, both of which mimic their sucking behavior. This becomes their way of expressing pleasure, as these animals have collarbones and use their paws like hands when they suck, unlike dogs and sheep.

4. Of Serene Pleasure.

4. *Of Serene Pleasure.*

In the action of sucking, the lips of the infant are closed around the nipple of its mother, till he has filled his stomach, and the pleasure occasioned by the stimulus of this grateful food succeeds. Then the sphincter of the mouth, fatigued by the continued action of sucking, is relaxed; and the antagonist muscles of the face gently acting, produce the smile of pleasure: as cannot but be seen by all who are conversant with children.

In the act of sucking, the baby’s lips wrap around the mother’s nipple until he’s full, and the pleasure from this satisfying food follows. Then, the mouth's sphincter, tired from the constant sucking, relaxes; and the opposing facial muscles gently work, creating a smile of enjoyment, which anyone who knows children can easily recognize.

Hence this smile during our lives is associated with gentle pleasure; it is visible in kittens, and puppies, when they are played with, and tickled; but more particularly marks the human features. For in children this expression of pleasure is much encouraged, by their imitation of their parents, or friends; who generally address them with a smiling countenance: and hence some nations are more remarkable for the gaiety, and others for the gravity of their looks.

Thus, this smile in our lives is linked to gentle pleasure; it's seen in kittens and puppies when they're played with and tickled; but it especially defines human features. For children, this expression of happiness is often encouraged by mimicking their parents or friends, who usually interact with them with a smiling face. This is why some cultures are known for their cheerful demeanor, while others are noted for their serious expressions.

5. Of Anger.

5. About Anger.

The actions that constitute the mode of fighting, are the immediate language of anger in all animals; and a preparation for these actions is the natural language of threatening. Hence the human creature clenches his fist, and sternly surveys his adversary, as if meditating where to make the attack; the ram, and the bull, draws himself some steps backwards, and levels his horns; and the horse, as he most frequently fights by striking with his hinder feet, turns his heels to his foe, and bends back his ears, to listen out the place of his adversary, that the threatened blow may not be ineffectual.

The actions involved in fighting are the immediate expression of anger in all animals, and getting ready for these actions is the natural way to show a threat. So, a human clenches their fist and stares at their opponent, as if thinking about where to strike. A ram or a bull steps back and points their horns forward, while a horse, which usually fights by kicking with its back legs, turns its hindquarters to the enemy and flattens its ears back to locate the opponent, ensuring that its intended kick won’t miss.

6. Of Attention.

6. On Attention.

The eye takes in at once but half our horizon, and that only in the day, and our smell informs us of no very distant objects, hence we confide principally in the organ of hearing to apprize us of danger: when we hear any the smallest sound, that we cannot immediately account for, our fears are alarmed, we suspend our steps, hold every muscle still, open our mouths a little, erect our ears, and listen to gain further information: and this by habit becomes the general language of attention to objects of sight, as well as of hearing; and even to the successive trains of our ideas.

The eye only captures half of our surroundings at once, and that’s only during the day. Our sense of smell doesn’t help us detect things that are far away, so we mainly rely on our hearing to alert us to danger. Whenever we hear any small sound that we can’t immediately identify, our fears kick in; we stop moving, hold our bodies still, slightly open our mouths, perk up our ears, and listen to gather more information. Over time, this response has become the standard way we focus on both what we see and hear, as well as the flow of our thoughts.

The natural language of violent pain, which is expressed by writhing the body, grinning, and screaming; and that of tumultuous pleasure, expressed in loud laughter; belong to Section XXXIV. on Diseases from Volition.

The natural language of violent pain, which is expressed by writhing the body, grinning, and screaming; and that of tumultuous pleasure, expressed in loud laughter; belong to Section XXXIV. on Diseases from Volition.

IX. It must have already appeared to the reader, that all other animals, as well as man, are possessed of this natural language of the passions, expressed in signs or tones; and we shall endeavour to evince, that those animals, which have preserved themselves from being enslaved by mankind, and are associated in flocks, are also possessed of some artificial language, and of some traditional knowledge.

IX. It should be clear to the reader that all animals, including humans, have a natural way of expressing emotions, shown through signs or sounds. We will try to demonstrate that animals that have managed to avoid being enslaved by humans and live in groups also have a form of communication and some shared knowledge passed down through generations.

The mother-turkey, when she eyes a kite hovering high in air, has either seen her own parents thrown into fear at his presence, or has by observation been acquainted with his dangerous designs upon her young. She becomes agitated with fear, and uses the natural language of that passion, her young ones catch the fear by imitation, and in an instant conceal themselves in the grass.

The mother turkey, when she spots a kite flying high in the sky, has either witnessed her own parents becoming scared at its presence or has learned from experience about its dangerous intentions towards her chicks. She gets anxious and expresses that fear, and her young ones pick up on her fear and quickly hide in the grass.

At the same time that she shews her fears by her gesture and deportment, she uses a certain exclamation, Koe-ut, Koe-ut, and the young ones afterwards know, when they hear this note, though they do not see their dam, that the presence of their adversary is denounced, and hide themselves as before.

At the same time she expresses her fears through her gestures and behavior, she makes a certain sound, Koe-ut, Koe-ut. The young ones then know, even when they can't see their mother, that their enemy is near, and they hide just like before.

The wild tribes of birds have very frequent opportunities of knowing their enemies, by observing the destruction they make among their progeny, of which every year but a small part escapes to maturity: but to our domestic birds these opportunities so rarely occur, that their knowledge of their distant enemies must frequently be delivered by tradition in the manner above explained, through many generations.

The wild tribes of birds often have plenty of chances to recognize their enemies by watching the damage done to their young, as only a small number make it to adulthood each year. However, for our domesticated birds, these opportunities are so rare that their awareness of distant threats has to be passed down through tradition, as described earlier, over many generations.

This note of danger, as well as the other notes of the mother-turkey, when she calls her flock to their food, or to sleep under her wings, appears to be an artificial language, both as expressed by the mother, and as understood by the progeny. For a hen teaches this language with equal ease to the ducklings, she has hatched from suppositious eggs, and educates as her own offspring: and the wagtails, or hedge-sparrows, learn it from the young cuckoo their softer nursling, and supply him with food long after he can fly about, whenever they hear his cuckooing, which Linnæus tells us, is his call of hunger, (Syst. Nat.) And all our domestic animals are readily taught to come to us for food, when we use one tone of voice, and to fly from our anger, when we use another.

This warning sound, along with the other calls of the mother turkey, when she gathers her flock for food or to sleep under her wings, seems to be a form of communication, both as conveyed by the mother and understood by the young. A hen teaches this communication just as easily to the ducklings that she has hatched from fictitious eggs and raises as her own. Additionally, the wagtails or hedge-sparrows learn it from the young cuckoo, their softer dependent, and continue to feed him long after he can fly, whenever they hear his cuckooing, which Linnæus tells us is his way of signaling hunger. All our domestic animals can easily be trained to come to us for food when we use one tone of voice, and to avoid us when we use another.

Rabbits, as they cannot easily articulate sounds, and are formed into societies, that live under ground, have a very different method of giving alarm. When danger is threatened, they thump on the ground with one of their hinder feet, and produce a sound, that can be heard a great way by animals near the surface of the earth, which would seem to be an artificial sign both from its singularity and its aptness to the situation of the animal.

Rabbits, since they can't easily make sounds and live in underground communities, have a unique way of signaling danger. When they sense a threat, they thump the ground with one of their back feet, creating a sound that can be heard from far away by animals on the surface. This serves as a clear alarm due to its distinctiveness and how well it fits the rabbit's situation.

The rabbits on the island of Sor, near Senegal, have white flesh, and are well tasted, but do not burrow in the earth, so that we may suspect their digging themselves houses in this cold climate is an acquired art, as well as their note of alarm, (Adanson's Voyage to Senegal).

The rabbits on the island of Sor, near Senegal, have white meat and taste great, but they don't dig burrows in the ground. This makes us think that their ability to make homes in this cold climate is a learned skill, just like their warning call, (Adanson's Voyage to Senegal).

The barking of dogs is another curious note of alarm, and would seem to be an acquired language, rather than a natural sign: for "in the island of Juan Fernandes, the dogs did not attempt to bark, till some European dogs were put among them, and then they gradually begun to imitate them, but in a strange manner at first, as if they were learning a thing that was not natural to them," (Voyage to South America by Don G. Juan, and Don Ant. de Ulloa. B. 2. c. 4).

The barking of dogs is another interesting sign of alarm, and it seems to be a learned behavior rather than a natural instinct. On the island of Juan Fernandes, the dogs didn't start barking until some European dogs were introduced among them. They gradually began to imitate the barking, but at first, it was in a strange way, as if they were trying to learn something that was unfamiliar to them. (Voyage to South America by Don G. Juan, and Don Ant. de Ulloa. B. 2. c. 4).

Linnæus also observes, that the dogs of South America do not bark at strangers, (Syst. Nat.) And the European dogs, that have been carried to Guinea, are said in three or four generations to cease to bark, and only howl, like the dogs that are natives of that coast, (World Displayed, Vol. XVII. p. 26.)

Linnæus also notes that the dogs of South America do not bark at strangers (Syst. Nat.). Additionally, European dogs that have been brought to Guinea are reported to stop barking in three or four generations and only howl, similar to the native dogs of that region (World Displayed, Vol. XVII. p. 26).

A circumstance not dissimilar to this, and equally curious, is mentioned by Kircherus, de Musurgia, in his Chapter de Lusciniis, "That the young nightingales, that are hatched under other birds, never sing till they are instructed by the company of other nightingales." And Jonston affirms, that the nightingales that visit Scotland, have not the same harmony as those of Italy, (Pennant's Zoology, octavo, p. 255); which would lead us to suspect that the singing of birds, like human music, is an artificial language rather than a natural expression of passion.

A situation somewhat like this, and just as intriguing, is noted by Kircherus in de Musurgia, in his chapter on nightingales: "Young nightingales that are raised by other birds never sing until they learn from the company of actual nightingales." Additionally, Jonston claims that the nightingales that come to Scotland don't have the same melody as those from Italy (Pennant's Zoology, octavo, p. 255); this suggests that bird song, like human music, is more of an acquired skill than a natural expression of emotion.

X. Our music like our language, is perhaps entirely constituted of artificial tones, which by habit suggest certain agreeable passions. For the same combination of notes and tones do not excite devotion, love, or poetic melancholy in a native of Indostan and of Europe. And "the Highlander has the same warlike ideas annexed to the sound of a bagpipe (an instrument which an Englishman derides), as the Englishman has to that of a trumpet or fife," (Dr. Brown's Union of Poetry and Music, p. 58.) So "the music of the Turks is very different from the Italian, and the people of Fez and Morocco have again a different kind, which to us appears very rough and horrid, but is highly pleasing to them," (L'Arte Armoniaca a Giorgio Antoniotto). Hence we see why the Italian opera does not delight an untutored Englishman; and why those, who are unaccustomed to music, are more pleased with a tune, the second or third time they hear it, than the first. For then the same melodious train of sounds excites the melancholy, they had learned from the song; or the same vivid combination of them recalls all the mirthful ideas of the dance and company.

X. Our music, like our language, is probably made up entirely of artificial sounds that through habit suggest certain pleasant emotions. The same set of notes and tones doesn't stir feelings of devotion, love, or poetic sadness in someone from India the way it does in someone from Europe. And "the Highlander has the same martial associations connected to the sound of a bagpipe (an instrument that an Englishman mocks), as the Englishman has to that of a trumpet or fife," (Dr. Brown's Union of Poetry and Music, p. 58.) Likewise, "the music of the Turks is very different from the Italian, and the people from Fez and Morocco have yet another style, which seems very rough and unpleasant to us, but is highly enjoyable for them," (L'Arte Armoniaca a Giorgio Antoniotto). This is why the Italian opera doesn’t appeal to an untrained Englishman; and why those who are not used to music tend to enjoy a tune more the second or third time they hear it than the first. Because then the same pleasant sequence of sounds evokes the sadness they picked up from the song; or the same vivid mixture of them brings back all the joyful memories of the dance and the company.

Even the sounds, that were once disagreeable to us, may by habit be associated with other ideas, so as to become agreeable. Father Lasitau, in his account of the Iroquois, says "the music and dance of those Americans, have something in them extremely barbarous, which at first disgusts. We grow reconciled to them by degrees, and in the end partake of them with pleasure, the savages themselves are fond of them to distraction," (Mœurs des Savages, Tom. ii.)

Even the sounds that used to annoy us can become pleasant by association through repetition. Father Lasitau, in his description of the Iroquois, notes that "the music and dance of those Americans have something quite barbaric in them that initially repulses us. However, we gradually get used to them and eventually enjoy them; the savages themselves are so fond of them that they can’t get enough," (Mœurs des Savages, Tom. ii.)

There are indeed a few sounds, that we very generally associate with agreeable ideas, as the whistling of birds, or purring of animals, that are delighted; and some others, that we as generally associate with disagreeable ideas, as the cries of animals in pain, the hiss of some of them in anger, and the midnight howl of beasts of prey. Yet we receive no terrible or sublime ideas from the lowing of a cow, or the braying of an ass. Which evinces, that these emotions are owing to previous associations. So if the rumbling of a carriage in the street be for a moment mistaken for thunder, we receive a sublime sensation, which ceases as soon as we know it is the noise of a coach and six.

There are indeed a few sounds that we generally connect with pleasant ideas, like the whistling of birds or the purring of happy animals. On the flip side, there are sounds we associate with unpleasant ideas, such as the cries of animals in distress, the hissing of some in anger, and the midnight howls of predators. However, we don’t get any strong or awe-inspiring feelings from the mooing of a cow or the braying of a donkey. This shows that these emotions are based on prior associations. For example, if we momentarily mistake the rumble of a carriage in the street for thunder, we experience a sense of awe that disappears as soon as we realize it's just the sound of a coach and horses.

There are other disagreeable sounds, that are said to set the teeth on edge; which, as they have always been thought a necessary effect of certain discordant notes, become a proper subject of our enquiry. Every one in his childhood has repeatedly bit a part of the glass or earthen vessel, in which his food has been given him, and has thence had a very disagreeable sensation in the teeth, which sensation was designed by nature to prevent us from exerting them on objects harder than themselves. The jarring sound produced between the cup and the teeth is always attendant on this disagreeable sensation: and ever after when such a sound is accidentally produced by the conflict of two hard bodies, we feel by association of ideas the concomitant disagreeable sensation in our teeth.

There are other unpleasant sounds that are said to make your teeth hurt, which have always been considered a natural result of certain discordant notes, making them a suitable topic for our discussion. Everyone, when they were a child, has often bitten down on a part of a glass or ceramic dish that held their food, resulting in a very unpleasant feeling in their teeth. This sensation was meant by nature to stop us from using our teeth on objects that are harder than they are. The jarring sound made when the cup hits the teeth always comes with this unpleasant feeling, and afterward, whenever such a sound happens accidentally between two hard objects, we feel that same unpleasant sensation in our teeth through the association of ideas.

Others have in their infancy frequently held the corner of a silk handkerchief in their mouth, or the end of the velvet cape of their coat, whilst their companions in play have plucked it from them, and have given another disagreeable sensation to their teeth, which has afterwards recurred on touching those materials. And the sight of a knife drawn along a china plate, though no sound is excited by it, and even the imagination of such a knife and plate so scraped together, I know by repeated experience will produce the same disagreeable sensation of the teeth.

Others have often held the corner of a silk handkerchief in their mouth or the end of the velvet cape of their coat during childhood, while their playmates have tugged it away, creating an uncomfortable sensation in their teeth that later resurfaces when they touch those materials again. Even the sight of a knife scraping against a china plate, without any sound, and just the thought of such a knife and plate grinding together, will provoke that same unpleasant feeling in the teeth, based on my repeated experience.

These circumstances indisputably prove, that this sensation of the tooth-edge is owing to associated ideas; as it is equally excitable by sight, touch, hearing, or imagination.

These circumstances clearly show that this feeling at the edge of the tooth is due to connected ideas, as it can be triggered by sight, touch, hearing, or imagination.

In respect to the artificial proportions of sound excited by musical instruments, those, who have early in life associated them with agreeable ideas, and have nicely attended to distinguish them from each other, are said to have a good ear, in that country where such proportions are in fashion: and not from any superior perfection in the organ of hearing, or any intuitive sympathy between certain sounds and passions.

In relation to the artificial sound qualities produced by musical instruments, people who have grown up connecting them with pleasant ideas and have carefully learned to tell them apart are said to have a good ear in places where such qualities are popular. This ability isn't due to any exceptional skill in hearing or an innate connection between specific sounds and emotions.

I have observed a child to be exquisitely delighted with music, and who could with great facility learn to sing any tune that he heard distinctly, and yet whole organ of hearing was so imperfect, that it was necessary to speak louder to him in common conversation than to others.

I have noticed a child who is incredibly excited by music and can easily learn to sing any tune he hears clearly, yet his hearing is so poor that it's necessary to speak louder to him in regular conversation than to others.

Our music, like our architecture, seems to have no foundation in nature, they are both arts purely of human creation, as they imitate nothing. And the professors of them have only classed those circumstances, that are most agreeable to the accidental taste of their age, or country; and have called it Proportion. But this proportion must always fluctuate, as it rests on the caprices, that are introduced into our minds by our various modes of education. And these fluctuations of taste must become more frequent in the present age, where mankind have enfranchised themselves from the blind obedience to the rules of antiquity in perhaps every science, but that of architecture. See Sect. XII. 7. 3.

Our music, like our architecture, seems to have no foundation in nature, they are both arts purely of human creation, as they imitate nothing. And the professors of them have only classed those circumstances, that are most agreeable to the accidental taste of their age, or country; and have called it Proportion. But this proportion must always fluctuate, as it rests on the caprices, that are introduced into our minds by our various modes of education. And these fluctuations of taste must become more frequent in the present age, where mankind have enfranchised themselves from the blind obedience to the rules of antiquity in perhaps every science, but that of architecture. See Sect. XII. 7. 3.

XI. There are many articles of knowledge, which the animals in cultivated countries seem to learn very early in their lives, either from each other, or from experience, or observation: one of the most general of these is to avoid mankind. There is so great a resemblance in the natural language of the passions of all animals, that we generally know, when they are in a pacific, or in a malevolent humour, they have the same knowledge of us; and hence we can scold them from us by some tones and gestures, and could possibly attract them to us by others, if they were not already apprized of our general malevolence towards them. Mr. Gmelin, Professor at Petersburg, assures us, that in his journey into Siberia, undertaken by order of the Empress of Russia, he saw foxes, that expressed no fear of himself or companions, but permitted him to come quite near them, having never seen the human creature before. And Mr. Bongainville relates, that at his arrival at the Malouine, or Falkland's Islands, which were not inhabited by men, all the animals came about himself and his people; the fowls settling upon their heads and shoulders, and the quadrupeds running about their feet. From the difficulty of acquiring the confidence of old animals, and the ease of taming young ones, it appears that the fear, they all conceive at the sight of mankind, is an acquired article of knowledge.

XI. There are many things that animals in cultivated areas seem to learn early in their lives, whether from each other, their experiences, or observation: one of the most common is to stay away from humans. The natural expression of emotions in all animals is so similar that we usually understand when they're feeling peaceful or aggressive, and they recognize the same in us; this allows us to chase them away with certain sounds and gestures, and we could likely attract them with others, if they weren't already aware of our general hostility towards them. Mr. Gmelin, a professor in Petersburg, tells us that during his journey to Siberia, ordered by the Empress of Russia, he encountered foxes that showed no fear of him or his companions, allowing him to approach them closely since they had never seen a human before. Mr. Bougainville also notes that when he arrived at the Malouines, or Falkland Islands, which were uninhabited by humans, all the animals gathered around him and his crew; birds would land on their heads and shoulders, and quadrupeds would run around their feet. The difficulty of gaining the trust of older animals versus the ease of taming younger ones suggests that the fear they have of humans is something they learn over time.

This knowledge is more nicely understood by rooks, who are formed into societies, and build, as it were, cities over our heads; they evidently distinguish, that the danger is greater when a man is armed with a gun. Every one has seen this, who in the spring of the year has walked under a rookery with a gun in his hand: the inhabitants of the trees rise on their wings, and scream to the unfledged young to shrink into their nests from the sight of the enemy. The vulgar observing this circumstance so uniformly to occur, assert that rooks can smell gun-powder.

This knowledge is better understood by rooks, who form communities and build, in a way, cities overhead; they clearly recognize that the danger is greater when a person is holding a gun. Anyone who has walked under a rookery with a gun in hand during the spring has seen this: the birds take to the sky and scream at their young to hide in their nests at the sight of the threat. People commonly believe that rooks can smell gunpowder because this happens so consistently.

The fieldfares, (turdus pilarus) which breed in Norway, and come hither in the cold season for our winter berries; as they are associated in flocks, and are in a foreign country, have evident marks of keeping a kind of watch, to remark and announce the appearance of danger. On approaching a tree, that is covered with them, they continue fearless till one at the extremity of the bush rising on his wings gives a loud and peculiar note of alarm, when they all immediately fly, except one other, who continues till you approach still nearer, to certify as it were the reality of the danger, and then he also flies off repeating the note of alarm.

The fieldfares (Turdus pilarus), which breed in Norway and migrate here during the cold season for our winter berries, gather in flocks and, being in a foreign land, clearly show signs of keeping watch for potential danger. When you approach a tree full of them, they stay fearless until one at the edge of the bush takes flight and emits a loud, distinctive alarm call. At that point, they all immediately take off, except for one other, who stays put until you get even closer, as if to confirm the presence of danger, and then he also flies away while repeating the alarm call.

And in the woods about Senegal there is a bird called uett-uett by the negroes, and squallers by the French, which, as soon as they see a man, set up a loud scream, and keep flying round him, as if their intent was to warn other birds, which upon hearing the cry immediately take wing. These birds are the bane of sportsmen, and frequently put me into a passion, and obliged me to shoot them, (Adanson's Voyage to Senegal, 78). For the same intent the lesser birds of our climate seem to fly after a hawk, cuckoo, or owl, and scream to prevent their companions from being surprised by the general enemies of themselves, or of their eggs and progeny.

And in the woods around Senegal, there’s a bird known as uett-uett by the locals and squallers by the French. As soon as they spot a person, they let out a loud scream and start flying around him, as if trying to alert other birds, which take off as soon as they hear the noise. These birds are a nightmare for hunters and often drive me crazy, forcing me to shoot them (Adanson's Voyage to Senegal, 78). Similarly, smaller birds in our area seem to chase after a hawk, cuckoo, or owl and squawk to warn their friends from being caught off guard by their common enemies, or by threats to their eggs and young ones.

But the lapwing, (charadrius pluvialis Lin.) when her unfledged offspring run about the marshes, where they were hatched, not only gives the note of alarm at the approach of men or dogs, that her young may conceal themselves; but flying and screaming near the adversary, she appears more felicitous and impatient, as he recedes from her family, and thus endeavours to mislead him, and frequently succeeds in her design. These last instances are so apposite to the situation, rather than to the natures of the creatures, that use them; and are so similar to the actions of men in the same circumstances, that we cannot but believe, that they proceed from a similar principle.

But the lapwing (Charadrius pluvialis Lin.) when her young chicks are running around the marshes where they were born, not only sounds an alarm when she sees people or dogs coming, so her chicks can hide, but also flies around and screams near the threat, appearing more agitated and eager as he backs away from her family. In doing this, she tries to mislead him and often succeeds. These actions are so relevant to the situation rather than the nature of the animals that display them, and are so similar to how humans act in the same situations, that we can't help but believe they arise from a similar instinct.

Miss M.E. Jacson acquainted me, that she witnessed this autumn an agreeable instance of sagacity in a little bird, which seemed to use the means to obtain an end; the bird repeatedly hopped upon a poppy-stem, and shook the head with its bill, till many seeds were scattered, then it settled on the ground, and eat the seeds, and again repeated the same management. Sept. 1, 1794.

Miss M.E. Jacson told me that she saw a clever little bird this autumn that seemed to understand how to get what it wanted. The bird kept hopping on a poppy stem and shaking its head with its beak until a bunch of seeds fell out. Then it landed on the ground, ate the seeds, and did the same thing again. Sept. 1, 1794.

On the northern coast of Ireland a friend of mine saw above a hundred crows at once preying upon muscles; each crow took a muscle up into the air twenty or forty yards high, and let it fall on the stones, and thus by breaking the shell, got possession of the animal.—A certain philosopher (I think it was Anaxagoras) walking along the sea-shore to gather shells, one of these unlucky birds mistaking his bald head for a stone, dropped a shell-fish upon it, and killed at once a philosopher and an oyster.

On the northern coast of Ireland, a friend of mine saw over a hundred crows at once feeding on mussels; each crow would take a mussel up into the air twenty or forty yards high and drop it on the stones, breaking the shell and claiming the creature inside. A certain philosopher (I think it was Anaxagoras), while strolling along the beach to collect shells, had one of these unfortunate birds mistake his bald head for a rock and drop a shellfish on it, instantly killing both a philosopher and an oyster.

Our domestic animals, that have some liberty, are also possessed of some peculiar traditional knowledge: dogs and cats have been forced into each other's society, though naturally animals of a very different kind, and have hence learned from each other to eat dog's grass (agrostis canina) when they are sick, to promote vomiting. I have seen a cat mistake the blade of barley for this grass, which evinces it is an acquired knowledge. They have also learnt of each other to cover their excrement and urine;—about a spoonful of water was spilt upon my hearth from the tea-kettle, and I observed a kitten cover it with ashes. Hence this must also be an acquired art, as the creature mistook the application of it.

Our pets, which have some freedom, also have some unique traditional knowledge: dogs and cats have been thrown together, even though they're very different animals, and have learned from each other to eat dog’s grass (agrostis canina) when they're not well to help induce vomiting. I’ve seen a cat confuse a blade of barley for this grass, showing that it’s something they learned. They’ve also picked up the habit of covering their waste; once, a bit of water spilled on my floor from the kettle, and I saw a kitten try to cover it with ashes. Clearly, this is also a learned behavior, as the kitten misunderstood how to apply it.

To preserve their fur clean, and especially their whiskers, cats wash their faces, and generally quite behind their ears, every time they eat. As they cannot lick those places with their tongues, they first wet the inside of the leg with saliva, and then repeatedly wash their faces with it, which must originally be an effect of reasoning, because a means is used to produce an effect; and seems afterwards to be taught or acquired by imitation, like the greatest part of human arts.

To keep their fur clean, especially their whiskers, cats wash their faces and often behind their ears every time they eat. Since they can’t reach those spots with their tongues, they first wet the inside of their leg with saliva and then repeatedly wash their faces with it. This behavior likely started as a result of reasoning because they use a method to achieve a result, and it seems to be learned or picked up through imitation, similar to most human skills.

These animals seem to possess something like an additional sense by means of their whiskers; which have perhaps some analogy to the antennæ of moths and butterflies. The whiskers of cats consist not only of the long hairs on their upper lips, but they have also four or five long hairs standing up from each eyebrow, and also two or three on each cheek; all which, when the animal erects them, make with their points so many parts of the periphery of a circle, of an extent at least equal to the circumference of any part of their own bodies. With this instrument, I conceive, by a little experience, they can at once determine, whether any aperture amongst hedges or shrubs, in which animals of this genus live in their wild state, is large enough to admit their bodies; which to them is a matter of the greatest consequence, whether pursuing or pursued. They have likewise a power of erecting and bringing forward the whiskers on their lips; which probably is for the purpose of feeling, whether a dark hole be further permeable.

These animals seem to have an extra sense thanks to their whiskers, which are somewhat similar to the antennae of moths and butterflies. The whiskers of cats include not just the long hairs on their upper lips but also four or five long hairs that stand up from each eyebrow, as well as two or three on each cheek. When the animal raises them, the tips form parts of the perimeter of a circle that's at least as large as the circumference of any part of their body. With this ability, I believe they can quickly determine, with a bit of experience, whether any opening among the hedges or bushes where these animals live in the wild is big enough for them to fit through. This is extremely important for them, whether they are chasing something or being chased. They also have the ability to raise and push forward the whiskers on their lips, probably to check if a dark hole is passable.

The antennæ, or horns, of butterflies and moths, who have awkward wings, the minute feathers of which are very liable to injury, serve, I suppose, a similar purpose of measuring, as they fly or creep amongst the leaves of plants and trees, whither their wings can pass without touching them.

The antennae, or horns, of butterflies and moths, which have clumsy wings that can easily get damaged because of their tiny scales, probably serve a similar purpose by helping them navigate as they fly or crawl among the leaves of plants and trees, where their wings can move without making contact.

Mr. Leonard, a very intelligent friend of mine, saw a cat catch a trout by darting upon it in a deep clear water at the mill at Weaford, near Lichfield. The cat belonged to Mr. Stanley, who had often seen her catch fish in the same manner in summer, when the mill-pool was drawn so low, that the fish could be seen. I have heard of other cats taking fish in shallow water, as they stood on the bank. This seems a natural art of taking their prey in cats, which their acquired delicacy by domestication has in general prevented them from using, though their desire of eating fish continues in its original strength.

Mr. Leonard, a really smart friend of mine, saw a cat catch a trout by pouncing on it in the deep, clear water at the mill in Weaford, near Lichfield. The cat belonged to Mr. Stanley, who had often seen her catch fish this way in the summer when the mill pool was drawn so low that the fish were visible. I've heard of other cats catching fish in shallow water while standing on the bank. This seems to be a natural skill for cats to catch their prey, which the finer tastes they develop through domestication generally prevent them from using, even though their desire for fish remains strong.

Mr. White, in his ingenious History of Selbourn, was witness to a cat's suckling a young hare, which followed her about the garden, and came jumping to her call of affection. At Elford, near Lichfield, the Rev. Mr. Sawley had taken the young ones out of a hare, which was shot; they were alive, and the cat, who had just lost her own kittens, carried them away, as it was supposed, to eat them; but it presently appeared, that it was affection not hunger which incited her, as she suckled them, and brought them up as their mother.

Mr. White, in his clever History of Selbourn, saw a cat nursing a young hare, which followed her around the garden and jumped to her when she called it affectionately. In Elford, near Lichfield, Rev. Mr. Sawley found some baby hares that had been taken from a hare that was shot; they were still alive, and the cat, who had just lost her own kittens, carried them away, presumably to eat them. However, it soon became clear that her motivation was affection, not hunger, as she nursed them and raised them like their mother.

Other instances of the mistaken application of what has been termed instinct may be observed in flies in the night, who mistaking a candle for day-light, approach and perish in the flame. So the putrid smell of the stapelia, or carrion-flower, allures the large flesh-fly to deposit its young worms on its beautiful petals, which perish there for want of nourishment. This therefore cannot be a necessary instinct, because the creature mistakes the application of it.

Other examples of the incorrect use of what people call instinct can be seen in flies at night, who mistake a candle for daylight, getting close and burning themselves in the flame. Similarly, the foul odor of the stapelia, or carrion flower, attracts the large flesh fly to lay its eggs on its pretty petals, where the larvae end up dying due to lack of food. Thus, this cannot be considered a necessary instinct, as the creature misapplies it.

Though in this country horses shew little vestiges of policy, yet in the deserts of Tartary, and Siberia, when hunted by the Tartars they are seen to form a kind of community, set watches to prevent their being surprised, and have commanders, who direct, and hasten their flight, Origin of Language, Vol. I. p. 212. In this country, where four or five horses travel in a line, the first always points his ears forward, and the last points his backward, while the intermediate ones seem quite careless in this respect; which seems a part of policy to prevent surprise. As all animals depend most on the ear to apprize them of the approach of danger, the eye taking in only half the horizon at once, and horses possess a great nicety of this sense; as appears from their mode of fighting mentioned No. 8. 5. of this Section, as well as by common observation.

Though in this country horses shew little vestiges of policy, yet in the deserts of Tartary, and Siberia, when hunted by the Tartars they are seen to form a kind of community, set watches to prevent their being surprised, and have commanders, who direct, and hasten their flight, Origin of Language, Vol. I. p. 212. In this country, where four or five horses travel in a line, the first always points his ears forward, and the last points his backward, while the intermediate ones seem quite careless in this respect; which seems a part of policy to prevent surprise. As all animals depend most on the ear to apprize them of the approach of danger, the eye taking in only half the horizon at once, and horses possess a great nicety of this sense; as appears from their mode of fighting mentioned No. 8. 5. of this Section, as well as by common observation.

There are some parts of a horse, which he cannot conveniently rub, when they itch, as about the shoulder, which he can neither bite with his teeth, nor scratch with his hind foot; when this part itches, he goes to another horse, and gently bites him in the part which he wishes to be bitten, which is immediately done by his intelligent friend. I once observed a young foal thus bite its large mother, who did not choose to drop the grass she had in her mouth, and rubbed her nose against the foal's neck instead of biting it; which evinces that she knew the design of her progeny, and was not governed by a necessary instinct to bite where she was bitten.

There are parts of a horse that it can't easily scratch when they itch, like around the shoulder, where it can’t bite with its teeth or use its hind foot. When that spot itches, the horse goes over to another horse and gently bites it in the area it wants scratched, and the other horse responds immediately. I once saw a young foal bite its big mother, who didn’t want to drop the grass she was eating, so instead, she rubbed her nose against the foal’s neck instead of biting it. This shows that she understood what her offspring wanted and wasn't just acting on instinct to bite where she was bitten.

Many of our shrubs, which would otherwise afford an agreeable food to horses, are armed with thorns or prickles, which secure them from those animals; as the holly, hawthorn, gooseberry, gorse. In the extensive moorlands of Staffordshire, the horses have learnt to stamp upon a gorse-bush with one of their fore-feet for a minute together, and when the points are broken, they eat it without injury. The horses in the new forest in Hampshire are affirmed to do the same by Mr. Gilpin. Forest Scenery, II. 251, and 112. Which is an art other horses in the fertile parts of the country do not possess, and prick their mouths till they bleed, if they are induced by hunger or caprice to attempt eating gorse.

Many of our shrubs, which would normally provide a nice food source for horses, are covered in thorns or prickles to protect them from those animals, like holly, hawthorn, gooseberry, and gorse. In the vast moorlands of Staffordshire, horses have learned to stomp on a gorse bush with one of their front feet for a minute, and once the spines are broken, they eat it without getting hurt. Horses in the New Forest in Hampshire are said to do the same, according to Mr. Gilpin. Forest Scenery, II. 251, and 112. This is a skill that other horses in more fertile areas don't have, and they end up injuring their mouths and bleeding if they try to eat gorse out of hunger or curiosity.

Swine have a sense of touch as well as of smell at the end of their nose, which they use as a hand, both to root up the soil, and to turn over and examine objects of food, somewhat like the proboscis of an elephant. As they require shelter from the cold in this climate, they have learnt to collect straw in their mouths to make their nest, when the wind blows cold; and to call their companions by repeated cries to assist in the work, and add to their warmth by their numerous bedfellows. Hence these animals, which are esteemed so unclean, have also learned never to befoul their dens, where they have liberty, with their own excrement; an art, which cows and horses, which have open hovels to run into, have never acquired. I have observed great sagacity in swine; but the short lives we allow them, and their general confinement, prevents their improvement, which might probably be otherwise greater than that of dogs.

Pigs have a sense of touch and smell at the tip of their nose, which they use like a hand to dig up the ground and examine food, similar to how an elephant uses its trunk. They need shelter from the cold in this climate, so they’ve figured out how to gather straw in their mouths to build nests when it’s windy and cold. They also call their friends with repeated sounds to help with the work and keep each other warm by snuggling together. Because of this, these animals, often seen as dirty, have learned not to dirty their living spaces with their own waste when they have the freedom to roam; a skill that cows and horses, which have open shelters, haven’t developed. I’ve noticed a lot of intelligence in pigs, but the short lives we give them and their usual confinement limit their potential, which could be greater than that of dogs.

Instances of the sagacity and knowledge of animals are very numerous to every observer, and their docility in learning various arts from mankind, evinces that they may learn similar arts from their own species, and thus be possessed of much acquired and traditional knowledge.

Instances of the wisdom and intelligence of animals are very common to any observer, and their ability to learn various skills from humans shows that they can also learn similar skills from their own kind, which means they can possess a lot of learned and traditional knowledge.

A dog whose natural prey is sheep, is taught by mankind, not only to leave them unmolested, but to guard them; and to hunt, to set, or to destroy other kinds of animals, as birds, or vermin; and in some countries to catch fish, in others to find truffles, and to practise a great variety of tricks; is it more surprising that the crows should teach each other, that the hawk can catch less birds, by the superior swiftness of his wing, and if two of them follow him, till he succeeds in his design, that they can by force share a part of the capture? This I have formerly observed with attention and astonishment.

A dog, which naturally preys on sheep, is trained by humans not only to leave them alone but also to protect them; and to hunt, chase, or kill other animals, like birds or pests; and in some places to catch fish, in others to find truffles, and to perform a variety of tricks. Is it really so surprising that crows teach each other that a hawk can catch fewer birds due to its greater speed, and that if two of them follow the hawk until it succeeds, they can forcefully share the catch? I have observed this with both attention and amazement in the past.

There is one kind of pelican mentioned by Mr. Osbeck, one of Linnæus's travelling pupils (the pelicanus aquilus), whose food is fish; and which it takes from other birds, because it is not formed to catch them itself; hence it is called by the English a Man-of-war-bird, Voyage to China, p. 88. There are many other interesting anecdotes of the pelican and cormorant, collected from authors of the best authority, in a well-managed Natural History for Children, published by Mr. Galton. Johnson. London.

There is a type of pelican mentioned by Mr. Osbeck, one of Linnaeus's traveling students (the pelicanus aquilus), whose diet consists of fish, which it takes from other birds because it isn’t built to catch them itself; that’s why it’s called a Man-of-war bird in English, Voyage to China, p. 88. There are many other intriguing stories about the pelican and cormorant gathered from reputable sources in a well-done Natural History for Children, published by Mr. Galton. Johnson. London.

And the following narration from the very accurate Mons. Adanson, in his Voyage to Senegal, may gain credit with the reader: as his employment in this country was solely to make observations in natural history. On the river Niger, in his road to the island Griel, he saw a great number of pelicans, or wide throats. "They moved with great state like swans upon the water, and are the largest bird next to the ostrich; the bill of the one I killed was upwards of a foot and half long, and the bag fastened underneath it held two and twenty pints of water. They swim in flocks, and form a large circle, which they contract afterwards, driving the fish before them with their legs: when they see the fish in sufficient number confined in this space, they plunge their bill wide open into the water, and shut it again with great quickness. They thus get fish into their throat-bag, which they eat afterwards on shore at their leisure." P. 247.

And the following account from the very accurate Mons. Adanson, in his Voyage to Senegal, may be believable to the reader, as his purpose in this country was solely to observe natural history. On the river Niger, on his way to the island Griel, he saw many pelicans, or "wide throats." "They moved gracefully like swans on the water and are the largest birds after the ostrich; the bill of the one I shot was over a foot and a half long, and the pouch underneath it could hold twenty-two pints of water. They swim in groups and form a large circle, which they then close in, driving the fish toward them with their legs: when they see enough fish gathered in this space, they plunge their bill wide open into the water and quickly shut it again. This way, they catch fish in their throat pouch, which they later eat on shore at their leisure." P. 247.

XII. The knowledge and language of those birds, that frequently change their climate with the seasons, is still more extensive: as they perform these migrations in large societies, and are less subject to the power of man, than the resident tribes of birds. They are said to follow a leader during the day, who is occasionally changed, and to keep a continual cry during the night to keep themselves together. It is probable that these emigrations were at first undertaken as accident directed, by the more adventurous of their species, and learned from one another like the discoveries of mankind in navigation. The following circumstances strongly support this opinion.

XII. The knowledge and communication of those birds, which often change their location with the seasons, is even more extensive: since they migrate in large groups and are less controlled by humans than the resident bird species. They are said to follow a leader during the day, who is occasionally switched, and they keep a continuous call at night to stay together. It’s likely that these migrations initially happened by chance, driven by the more daring members of their species, and they learned from each other just like humans did with navigation. The following factors strongly support this view.

1. Nature has provided these animals, in the climates where they are produced, with another resource: when the season becomes too cold for their constitutions, or the food they were supported with ceases to be supplied, I mean that of sleeping. Dormice, snakes, and bats, have not the means of changing their country; the two former from the want of wings, and the latter from his being not able to bear the light of the day. Hence these animals are obliged to make use of this resource, and sleep during the winter. And those swallows that have been hatched too late in the year to acquire their full strength of pinion, or that have been maimed by accident or disease, have been frequently found in the hollows of rocks on the sea coasts, and even under water in this torpid state, from which they have been revived by the warmth of a fire. This torpid state of swallows is testified by innumerable evidences both of antient and modern names. Aristotle speaking of the swallows says, "They pass into warmer climates in winter, if such places are at no great distance; if they are, they bury themselves in the climates where they dwell," (8. Hist. c. 16. See also Derham's Phys. Theol. v. ii. p. 177.)

1. Nature has provided these animals, in the climates where they live, with another resource: when the season gets too cold for their bodies, or the food they rely on runs out, they sleep. Dormice, snakes, and bats can't move to another location; the former because they don't have wings, and the latter because they can't handle bright daylight. As a result, these animals have to use this resource and sleep through the winter. Swallows that hatch too late in the year to gain enough strength to fly, or that have been injured or gotten sick, are often found in crevices of rocks along the coast and even submerged underwater in this inactive state, from which they've been revived by the warmth of a fire. This inactive state of swallows is supported by countless historical and contemporary accounts. Aristotle mentions the swallows, saying, "They move to warmer climates in winter if those places are not too far away; if they are, they bury themselves in their current habitats," (8. Hist. c. 16. See also Derham's Phys. Theol. v. ii. p. 177.)

Hence their emigrations cannot depend on a necessary instinct, as the emigrations themselves are not necessary.

Hence their emigrations cannot depend on a necessary instinct, as the emigrations themselves are not necessary.

2. When the weather becomes cold, the swallows in the neighbourhood assemble in large flocks; that is, the unexperienced attend those that have before experienced the journey they are about to undertake: they are then seen some time to hover on the coast, till there is calm whether, or a wind, that suits the direction of their flight. Other birds of passage have been drowned by thousands in the sea, or have settled on ships quite exhausted with fatigue. And others, either by mistaking their course, or by distress of weather, have arrived in countries where they were never seen before: and thus are evidently subject to the same hazards that the human species undergo, in the execution of their artificial purposes.

2. When the weather gets cold, the swallows in the area gather in large groups; the inexperienced ones follow those who have previously made the journey they are about to take. They can be seen lingering on the coast for a while until the weather is calm or the wind is right for their flight. Other migratory birds have drowned by the thousands at sea or have landed on ships, completely worn out. Some, either by losing their way or due to bad weather, have ended up in places where they’ve never been seen before. They clearly face the same risks that humans do when pursuing their planned goals.

3. The same birds are emigrant from some countries and not so from others: the swallows were seen at Goree in January by an ingenious philosopher of my acquaintance, and he was told that they continued there all the year; as the warmth of the climate was at all seasons sufficient for their own constitutions, and for the production of the flies that supply them with nourishment. Herodotus says, that in Libya, about the springs of the Nile, the swallows continue all the year. (L. 2.)

3. The same birds migrate from some countries and not from others: my clever friend spotted swallows at Goree in January, and he was informed that they stay there year-round since the warm climate is suitable for them and for the flies they feed on. Herodotus mentions that in Libya, near the Nile's springs, swallows remain all year long. (L. 2.)

Quails (tetrao corturnix, Lin.) are birds of passage from the coast of Barbary to Italy, and have frequently settled in large shoals on ships fatigued with their flight. (Ray, Wisdom of God, p. 129. Derham. Physic. Theol. v. ii. p. 178,) Dr. Ruffel, in his History of Aleppo, observes that the swallows visit that country about the end of February, and having hatched their young disappear about the end of July; and returning again about the beginning of October, continue about a fortnight, and then again disappear. (P. 70.)

Quails (tetraco corturnix, Lin.) are migrating birds that travel from the coast of Barbary to Italy and often rest in large groups on ships exhausted from their journey. (Ray, Wisdom of God, p. 129. Derham. Physic. Theol. v. ii. p. 178.) Dr. Ruffel, in his History of Aleppo, notes that swallows arrive in that region around the end of February, nest their young, and then leave by the end of July. They return in early October, stay for about two weeks, and then vanish again. (P. 70.)

When my late friend Dr. Chambres, of Derby, was on the island of Caprea in the bay of Naples, he was informed that great flights of quails annually settle on that island about the beginning of May, in their passage from Africa to Europe. And that they always come when the south-east wind blows, are fatigued when they rest on this island, and are taken in such amazing quantities and sold to the Continent, that the inhabitants pay the bishop his stipend out of the profits arising from the sale of them.

When my late friend Dr. Chambres from Derby was on the island of Caprea in the Bay of Naples, he learned that large flocks of quails arrive on the island each year around early May during their journey from Africa to Europe. They always come when the southeast wind blows, are tired when they land on this island, and are caught in such large numbers that they’re sold to the mainland, allowing the locals to pay the bishop his salary from the profits made from the sales.

The flights of these birds across the Mediterranean are recorded near three thousand years ago. "There went forth a wind from the Lord and brought quails from the sea, and let them fall upon the camp, a day's journey round about it, and they were two cubits above the earth," (Numbers, chap. ii. ver. 31.)

The migrations of these birds over the Mediterranean were documented almost three thousand years ago. "A wind from the Lord blew and brought quails from the sea, and they fell around the camp, about a day's journey away, and they were two cubits above the ground," (Numbers, chap. ii. ver. 31.)

In our country, Mr. Pennant informs us, that some quails migrate, and others only remove from the internal parts of the island to the coasts, (Zoology, octavo, 210.) Some of the ringdoves and stares breed here, others migrate, (ibid. 510, ii.) And the slender billed small birds do not all quit these kingdoms in the winter, though the difficulty of procuring the worms and insects, that they feed on, supplies the same reason for migration to them all, (ibid. 511.)

In our country, Mr. Pennant tells us that some quails migrate, while others only move from the interior of the island to the coast, (Zoology, octavo, 210.) Some of the ringdoves and starlings breed here, while others migrate, (ibid. 510, ii.) And not all of the slender-billed small birds leave these regions in the winter, although the challenge of finding the worms and insects they eat is the same reason for migration for all of them, (ibid. 511.)

Linnæus has observed, that in Sweden the female chaffinches quit that country in September, migrating into Holland, and leave their mates behind till their return in spring. Hence he has called them Fringilla cælebs, (Amæn. Acad. ii. 42. iv. 595.) Now in our climate both sexes of them are perennial birds. And Mr. Pennant observes that the hoopoe, chatterer, hawfinch, and crossbill, migrate into England so rarely, and at such uncertain times, as not to deserve to be ranked among our birds of passage, (ibid. 511.)

Linnaeus noted that in Sweden, female chaffinches leave the country in September, migrating to Holland, while their mates stay behind until they return in the spring. That's why he named them Fringilla cælebs, (Amæn. Acad. ii. 42. iv. 595.) In our climate, both males and females are year-round residents. Mr. Pennant also mentions that the hoopoe, chatterer, hawfinch, and crossbill migrate to England so infrequently and at such unpredictable times that they shouldn’t be considered among our migratory birds, (ibid. 511.)

The water fowl, as geese and ducks, are better adapted for long migrations, than the other tribes of birds, as, when the weather is calm, they can not only rest themselves, or sleep upon the ocean, but possibly procure some kind of food from it.

The waterfowl, like geese and ducks, are better suited for long migrations than other types of birds because, when the weather is calm, they can not only rest or sleep on the ocean but also potentially find some food from it.

Hence in Siberia, as soon as the lakes are frozen, the water fowl, which are very numerous, all disappear, and are supposed to fly to warmer climates, except the rail, which, from its inability for long flights, probably sleeps, like our bat, in their winter. The following account from the Journey of Professor Gmelin, may entertain the reader. "In the neighbourhood of Krasnoiark, amongst many other emigrant water fowls, we observed a great number of rails, which when pursued never took flight, but endeavoured to escape by running. We enquired how these birds, that could not fly, could retire into other countries in the winter, and were told, both by the Tartars and Assanians, that they well knew those birds could not alone pass into other countries: but when the cranes (les grues) retire in autumn, each one takes a rail (un rale) upon his back, and carries him to a warmer climate."

So in Siberia, as soon as the lakes freeze over, the waterfowl, which are very numerous, all vanish and are thought to migrate to warmer climates, except for the rail, which, unable to fly far, probably hibernates like our bats do in winter. The following account from Professor Gmelin's Journey might interest the reader: "In the vicinity of Krasnoiarsk, among many other migratory waterfowl, we noticed a large number of rails that, when chased, never took to the air but tried to escape by running. We asked how these birds, which couldn’t fly, could migrate to other countries in the winter, and were told by both the Tartars and Assanians that they knew well that these birds couldn’t migrate alone: when the cranes migrate in autumn, each one takes a rail on its back and carries it to a warmer climate."

Recapitulation.

Summary.

1. All birds of passage can exist in the climates, where they are produced.

1. All migratory birds can live in the climates where they were born.

2. They are subject in their migrations to the same accidents and difficulties, that mankind are subject to in navigation.

2. They face the same challenges and difficulties in their migrations that humans encounter in navigation.

3. The same species of birds migrate from some countries, and are resident in others.

3. The same types of birds migrate from some countries and stay in others.

From all these circumstances it appears that the migrations of birds are not produced by a necessary instinct, but are accidental improvements, like the arts among mankind, taught by their cotemporaries, or delivered by tradition from one generation of them to another.

From all these circumstances, it seems that bird migrations are not driven by a basic instinct, but rather are random enhancements, similar to the arts among humans, learned from their peers or passed down through tradition from one generation to the next.

XIII. In that season of the year which supplies the nourishment proper for the expected brood, the birds enter into a contract of marriage, and with joint labour construct a bed for the reception of their offspring. Their choice of the proper season, their contracts of marriage, and the regularity with which they construct their nests, have in all ages excited the admiration of naturalists; and have always been attributed to the power of instinct, which, like the occult qualities of the antient philosophers, prevented all further enquiry. We shall consider them in their order.

XIII. During the time of year that provides the right nourishment for the upcoming young, birds pair up and work together to build a nest for their offspring. Their timing, the way they form pairs, and their consistent efforts in nest-building have fascinated naturalists throughout history, and these behaviors have often been attributed to instinct, which, like the mysterious properties claimed by ancient philosophers, stopped any further investigation. We'll look at these aspects in the order they arise.

Their Choice of the Season.

Their Pick for the Season.

Our domestic birds, that are plentifully supplied throughout the year with their adapted food, and are covered with houses from the inclemency of the weather, lay their eggs at any season: which evinces that the spring of the year is not pointed out to them by a necessary instinct.

Our domestic birds, which have a constant supply of suitable food all year round and are protected from harsh weather, lay their eggs at any time of the year. This shows that they don't rely on an instinct to indicate the arrival of spring.

Whilst the wild tribes of birds choose this time of the year from their acquired knowledge, that the mild temperature of the air is more convenient for hatching their eggs, and is soon likely to supply that kind of nourishment, that is wanted for their young.

While the wild bird tribes choose this time of year based on their experience, knowing that the mild air temperature is better for hatching their eggs and will soon provide the food their chicks need.

If the genial warmth of the spring produced the passion of love, as it expands the foliage of trees, all other animals should feel its influence as well as birds: but, the viviparous creatures, as they suckle their young, that is, as they previously digest the natural food, that it may better suit the tender stomachs of their offspring, experience the influence of this passion at all seasons of the year, as cats and bitches. The graminivorous animals indeed generally produce their young about the time when grass is supplied in the greatest plenty, but this is without any degree of exactness, as appears from our cows, sheep, and hares, and may be a part of the traditional knowledge, which they learn from the example of their parents.

If the pleasant warmth of spring sparks the feeling of love, just like it brings out the leaves on trees, then all animals should experience its effects, not just birds. However, the mammals that nurse their young—meaning they digest food first so it’s easier for their babies to handle—feel this emotion throughout the year, just like cats and dogs do. Grazing animals usually give birth around the same time that grass is most abundant, but this isn't exactly consistent, as seen with our cows, sheep, and rabbits. It could be part of what they learn from their parents' behavior.

Their Contracts of Marriage.

Their Marriage Contracts.

Their mutual passion, and the acquired knowledge, that their joint labour is necessary to procure sustenance for their numerous family, induces the wild birds to enter into a contract of marriage, which does not however take place among the ducks, geese, and fowls, that are provided with their daily food from our barns.

Their shared passion and the knowledge that their teamwork is essential to provide for their large family lead the wild birds to enter into a marriage contract. However, this doesn't happen among the ducks, geese, and chickens that receive their daily food from our barns.

An ingenious philosopher has lately denied, that animals can enter into contracts, and thinks this an essential difference between them and the human creature:—but does not daily observation convince us, that they form contracts of friendship with each other, and with mankind? When puppies and kittens play together, is there not a tacit contract, that they will not hurt each other? And does not your favorite dog expect you should give him his daily food, for his services and attention to you? And thus barters his love for your protection? In the same manner that all contracts are made amongst men, that do not understand each others arbitrary language.

A clever philosopher has recently argued that animals can’t enter into contracts, claiming this as a key difference between them and humans. But don’t everyday observations show us that they form bonds of friendship with each other and with us? When puppies and kittens play together, isn’t there an unspoken agreement that they won’t hurt each other? And doesn’t your favorite dog expect you to provide his daily food in exchange for his loyalty and attention? He essentially trades his affection for your care, much like the way all contracts are made among humans who don’t understand each other’s arbitrary languages.

Construction of their Nests.

Building Their Nests.

1. They seem to be instructed how to build their nests from their observation of that, in which they were educated, and from their knowledge of those things, that are most agreeable to their touch in respect: to warmth, cleanliness, and stability. They choose their situations from their ideas of safety from their enemies, and of shelter from the weather. Nor is the colour of their nests a circumstance unthought of; the finches, that build in green hedges, cover their habitations with green moss; the swallow or martin, that builds against rocks and houses, covers her's with clay, whilst the lark chooses vegetable straw nearly of the colour of the ground she inhabits: by this contrivance, they are all less liable to be discovered by their adversaries.

1. They seem to learn how to build their nests from observing their surroundings and from what they've been taught. They understand which materials feel nice regarding warmth, cleanliness, and stability. They select their locations based on how safe they are from predators and how well they are sheltered from the weather. The color of their nests is also something they consider; finches that nest in green hedges cover their homes with green moss, while swallows or martins that build on cliffs and houses use clay, and larks opt for plant straw that matches the color of the ground they live on. This way, they are all less likely to be spotted by their enemies.

2. Nor are the nests of the same species of birds constructed always of the same materials, nor in the same form; which is another circumstance that ascertains, that they are led by observation.

2. The nests of the same species of birds are not always made from the same materials or built in the same shape; this is another factor that confirms they learn through observation.

In the trees before Mr. Levet's house in Lichfield, there are annually nests built by sparrows, a bird which usually builds under the tiles of houses, or the thatch of barns. Not finding such convenient situations for their nests, they build a covered nest bigger than a man's head, with an opening like a mouth at the side, resembling that of a magpie, except that it is built with straw and hay, and lined with feathers, and so nicely managed as to be a defence against both wind and rain.

In the trees in front of Mr. Levet's house in Lichfield, sparrows make nests every year. These birds typically build nests under roof tiles or in the thatch of barns. When those spots aren't available, they create a covered nest that's bigger than a person's head, featuring an opening on the side that looks like a mouth, similar to a magpie's nest, but made with straw and hay and lined with feathers. It's cleverly designed to protect against both wind and rain.

The following extract from a Letter of the Rev. Mr. J. Darwin, of Carleton Scroop in Lincolnshire, authenticates a curious fact of this kind. "When I mentioned to you the circumstance of crows or rooks building in the spire of Welbourn church, you expressed a desire of being well informed of the certainty of the fact. Welbourn is situated in the road from Grantham to Lincoln on the Cliff row; I yesterday took a ride thither, and enquired of the rector, Mr. Ridgehill, whether the report was true, that rooks built in the spire of his church. He assured me it was true, and that they had done so time immemorial, as his parishioners affirmed. There was a common tradition, he said, that formerly a rookery in some high trees adjoined the church yard, which being cut down (probably in the spring, the building season), the rooks removed to the church, and built their nests on the outside of the spire on the tops of windows, which by their projection a little from the spire made them convenient room, but that they built also on the inside. I saw two nests made with sticks on the outside, and in the spires, and Mr. Ridgehill said there were always a great many.

The following extract from a letter by Rev. Mr. J. Darwin, from Carleton Scroop in Lincolnshire, confirms a curious fact. "When I mentioned the situation of crows or rooks nesting in the spire of Welbourn church, you expressed a desire to be sure of the truth of the matter. Welbourn is located along the road from Grantham to Lincoln on the Cliff row; I rode there yesterday and asked the rector, Mr. Ridgehill, if it was true that rooks built in the spire of his church. He assured me it was true and that they had been doing so for as long as anyone could remember, according to his parishioners. There was a common belief, he said, that there used to be a rookery in some tall trees near the churchyard, which were cut down (probably in the spring, during nesting season), causing the rooks to move to the church and build their nests on the outside of the spire on the tops of the windows, which provided a good spot because they jutted out a bit from the spire, but they also built on the inside. I saw two nests made of sticks on the outside and inside the spires, and Mr. Ridgehill stated that there were always many."

"I spent the day with Mr. Wright, a clergyman, at Fulbeck, near Welbourn, and in the afternoon Dr. Ellis of Headenham, about two miles from Welbourn, drank tea at Mr. Wright's, who said he remembered, when Mr. Welby lived at Welbourn, that he received a letter from an acquaintance in the west of England, desiring an answer, whether the report of rooks building in Welbourn church was true, as a wager was depending on that subject; to which he returned an answer ascertaining the fact, and decided the wager." Aug. 30, 1794.

"I spent the day with Mr. Wright, a clergyman, in Fulbeck, near Welbourn, and in the afternoon Dr. Ellis from Headenham, about two miles from Welbourn, came over for tea at Mr. Wright's place. Mr. Wright mentioned that when Mr. Welby lived in Welbourn, he received a letter from someone in the west of England asking whether it was true that rooks were nesting in Welbourn church, as there was a bet riding on it. He replied, confirming the fact, and settled the bet." Aug. 30, 1794.

So the jackdaw (corvus monedula) generally builds in church-steeples, or under the roofs of high houses; but at Selbourn, in Southamptonshire, where towers and steeples are not sufficiently numerous, these birds build in forsaken rabbit burrows. See a curious account of these subterranean nests in White's History of Selbourn, p. 59. Can the skilful change of architecture in these birds and the sparrows above mentioned be governed by instinct? Then they must have two instincts, one for common, and the other for extraordinary occasions.

So the jackdaw (corvus monedula) usually builds its nests in church steeples or under the roofs of tall buildings; however, in Selbourn, Southamptonshire, where there aren't enough towers and steeples, these birds build in abandoned rabbit burrows. Check out a fascinating description of these underground nests in White's History of Selbourn, p. 59. Can the expert adjustments in architecture made by these birds and the sparrows mentioned earlier be driven by instinct? If that's the case, they must have two instincts—one for regular situations and another for exceptional ones.

I have seen green worsted in a nest, which no where exists in nature: and the down of thistles in those nests, that were by some accident constructed later in the summer, which material could not be procured for the earlier nests: in many different climates they cannot procure the same materials, that they use in ours. And it is well known, that the canary birds, that are propagated in this country, and the finches, that are kept tame, will build their nests of any flexile materials, that are given them. Plutarch, in his Book on Rivers, speaking of the Nile, says, "that the swallows collect a material, when the waters recede, with which they form nests, that are impervious to water." And in India there is a swallow that collects a glutinous substance for this purpose, whose nest is esculent, and esteemed a principal rarity amongst epicures, (Lin. Syst. Nat.) Both these must be constructed of very different materials from those used by the swallows of our country.

I have seen green worsted in a nest, which doesn't exist anywhere in nature: and the down of thistles in nests that were accidentally built later in the summer, as that material couldn't be found for the earlier nests. In many different climates, they can't find the same materials that we use here. It’s well known that the canary birds bred in this country and the finches that are kept as pets will build their nests from any flexible materials provided to them. Plutarch, in his Book on Rivers, talking about the Nile, says, "the swallows gather materials when the waters recede, with which they create nests that are waterproof." In India, there's a swallow that collects a sticky substance for this purpose, and its nest is edible, considered a delicacy among gourmets (Lin. Syst. Nat.). Both of these must be made from very different materials than those used by the swallows in our country.

In India the birds exert more artifice in building their nests on account of the monkeys and snakes: some form their pensile nests in the shape of a purse, deep and open at top; others with a hole in the side; and others, still more cautious, with an entrance at the very bottom, forming their lodge near the summit. But the taylor-bird will not ever trust its nest to the extremity of a tender twig, but makes one more advance to safety by fixing it to the leaf itself. It picks up a dead leaf, and sews it to the side of a living one, its slender bill being its needle, and its thread some fine fibres; the lining consists of feathers, gossamer, and down; its eggs are white, the colour of the bird light yellow, its length three inches, its weight three sixteenths of an ounce; so that the materials of the nest, and the weight of the bird, are not likely to draw down an habitation so slightly suspended. A nest of this bird is preserved in the British Museum, (Pennant's Indian Zoology). This calls to one's mind the Mosaic account of the origin of mankind, the first dawning of art there ascribed to them, is that of sewing leaves together. For many other curious kinds of nests see Natural History for Children, by Mr. Galton. Johnson. London. Part I. p. 47. Gen. Oriolus.

In India, birds are more creative in building their nests due to the presence of monkeys and snakes. Some create hanging nests shaped like a purse, which are deep and open at the top; others have a hole in the side; and some, even more cautious, have an entrance at the bottom, building their nest near the top of a tree. However, the tailorbird never trusts its nest to the end of a fragile twig, instead securing it to the leaf itself. It takes a dead leaf and stitches it to the side of a living one, using its slender beak as a needle and fine fibers as thread. The lining consists of feathers, gossamer, and down. Its eggs are white, the bird is a light yellow color, measuring about three inches in length and weighing approximately three sixteenths of an ounce, ensuring that the materials of the nest and the bird's weight aren’t likely to pull down such a delicately suspended home. A nest of this bird is kept in the British Museum (Pennant's Indian Zoology). This brings to mind the Biblical account of the origin of humanity, where the first hint of art is described as sewing leaves together. For many other fascinating types of nests, see "Natural History for Children" by Mr. Galton. Johnson. London. Part I. p. 47. Gen. Oriolus.

3. Those birds that are brought up by our care, and have had little communication with others of their own species, are very defective in this acquired knowledge; they are not only very awkward in the construction of their nests, but generally scatter their eggs in various parts of the room or cage, where they are confined, and seldom produce young ones, till, by failing in their first attempt, they have learnt something from their own observation.

3. Birds that are raised under our care and have limited interaction with their own kind often lack essential skills. They struggle with building their nests and usually spread their eggs all over the room or cage where they live. They rarely have chicks until they fail in their first attempt and learn something from their observations.

4. During the time of incubation birds are said in general to turn their eggs every day; some cover them, when they leave the nest, as ducks and geese; in some the male is said to bring food to the female, that she may have less occasion of absence, in others he is said to take her place, when she goes in quest of food; and all of them are said to leave their eggs a shorter time in cold weather than in warm. In Senegal the ostrich sits on her eggs only during the night, leaving them in the day to the heat of the sun; but at the Cape of Good Hope, where the heat is less, she sits on them day and night.

4. While incubating, birds are generally known to turn their eggs daily; some cover them when they leave the nest, like ducks and geese. In some species, the male brings food to the female so she doesn’t have to leave the nest as much, while in others, he takes her place when she goes to find food. All of them tend to leave their eggs for shorter periods in colder weather than in warmer weather. In Senegal, the ostrich only sits on her eggs at night, leaving them during the day to the sun's heat; but at the Cape of Good Hope, where it’s cooler, she sits on them both day and night.

If it should be asked, what induces a bird to sit weeks on its first eggs unconscious that a brood of young ones will be the product? The answer must be, that it is the same passion that induces the human mother to hold her offspring whole nights and days in her fond arms, and press it to her bosom, unconscious of its future growth to sense and manhood, till observation or tradition have informed her.

If someone were to ask why a bird sits on its first eggs for weeks without realizing that they will hatch into chicks, the answer would be that it's the same instinct that drives a human mother to hold her baby in her arms for entire nights and days, cradling it close to her heart, unaware of its future development into a person, until experience or knowledge teaches her otherwise.

5. And as many ladies are too refined to nurse their own children, and deliver them to the care and provision of others; so is there one instance of this vice in the feathered world. The cuckoo in some parts of England, as I am well informed by a very distinct and ingenious gentleman, hatches and educates her own young; whilst in other parts she builds no nest, but uses that of some lesser bird, generally either of the wagtail, or hedge sparrow, and depositing one egg in it, takes no further care of her progeny.

5. Just like many ladies are too sophisticated to nurse their own children and instead leave them in the care of others, there's a similar behavior in the bird world. The cuckoo, in certain areas of England, as I’ve learned from a very knowledgeable and insightful gentleman, hatches and raises her own chicks; however, in other areas, she doesn’t build a nest at all but uses the nest of a smaller bird, usually either a wagtail or a hedge sparrow, laying one egg in it and showing no further concern for her offspring.

As the Rev. Mr. Stafford was walking in Glosop Dale, in the Peak of Derbyshire, he saw a cuckoo rise from its nest. The nest was on the stump of a tree, that had been some time felled, among some chips that were in part turned grey, so as much to resemble the colour of the bird, in this nest were two young cuckoos: tying a string about the leg of one of them, he pegged the other end of it to the ground, and very frequently for many days beheld the old cuckoo feed these her young, as he stood very near them.

As Rev. Mr. Stafford was walking in Glossop Dale, in the Peak District of Derbyshire, he saw a cuckoo fly out of its nest. The nest was on the stump of a tree that had been cut down some time ago, among some chips that had partly turned gray, making them look similar to the color of the bird. In this nest were two young cuckoos. Tying a string around one of their legs, he pegged the other end to the ground and often stood nearby for many days, watching as the mother cuckoo fed her young.

The following extract of a Letter from the Rev. Mr. Wilmot, of Morley, near Derby, strengthens the truth of the fact above mentioned, of the cuckoo sometimes making a nest, and hatching her own young.

The following excerpt from a letter by Rev. Mr. Wilmot, from Morley near Derby, supports the truth of the earlier mentioned fact that sometimes a cuckoo makes a nest and hatches her own young.

"In the beginning of July 1792, I was attending some labourers on my farm, when one of them said to me, "There is a bird's nest upon one of the Coal-slack Hills; the bird is now sitting, and is exactly like a cuckoo. They say that cuckoo's never hatch their own eggs, otherwise I should have sworn it was one." He took me to the spot, it was in an open fallow ground; the bird was upon the nest, I stood and observed her some time, and was perfectly satisfied it was a cuckoo; I then put my hand towards her, and she almost let me touch her before she rose from the nest, which she appeared to quit with great uneasiness, skimming over the ground in the manner that a hen partridge does when disturbed from a new hatched brood, and went only to a thicket about forty or fifty yards from the nest; and continued there as long as I staid to observe her, which was not many minutes. In the nest, which was barely a hole scratched out of the coal-slack in the manner of a plover's nest, I observed three eggs, but did not touch them. As I had labourers constantly at work in that field, I went thither every day, and always looked to see if the bird was there, but did not disturb her for seven or eight days, when I was tempted to drive her from the nest, and found two young ones, that appeared to have been hatched some days, but there was no appearance of the third egg. I then mentioned this extraordinary circumstance (for such I thought it) to Mr. and Mrs. Holyoak of Bidford Grange, Warwickshire, and to Miss M. Willes, who were on a visit at my house, and who all went to see it. Very lately I reminded Mr. Holyoak of it, who told me he had a perfect recollection of the whole, and that, considering it a curiosity, he walked to look at it several times, was perfectly satisfied as to its being a cuckoo, and thought her more attentive to her young, than any other bird he ever observed, having always found her brooding her young. In about a week after I first saw the young ones, one of them was missing, and I rather suspected my plough-boys having taken it; though it might possibly have been taken by a hawk, some time when the old one was seeking food. I never found her off her nest but once, and that was the last time I saw the remaining young one, when it was almost full feathered. I then went from home for two or three days, and, when I returned, the young one was gone, which I take for granted had flown. Though during this time I frequently saw cuckoos in the thicket I mention, I never observed any one, that I supposed to be the cock-bird, paired with this hen."

"In early July 1792, I was checking on some workers at my farm when one of them said to me, 'There’s a bird’s nest on one of the Coal-slack Hills; the bird is sitting on it and looks just like a cuckoo. They say cuckoos never hatch their own eggs, otherwise I would have sworn it was one.' He took me to the spot, which was in an open fallow field; the bird was on the nest, and I watched her for a while, convinced she was a cuckoo. I then reached my hand toward her, and she almost allowed me to touch her before she flew off the nest, leaving it reluctantly, gliding over the ground like a hen partridge does when it’s disturbed from a new brood, and she went to a thicket about forty or fifty yards away from the nest. She stayed there as long as I watched her, which wasn’t many minutes. In the nest, which was just a small hole scraped out of the coal-slack like a plover’s nest, I noticed three eggs, but I didn’t touch them. Since I had workers regularly in that field, I went there every day, always checking to see if the bird was around, but I didn’t disturb her for seven or eight days. Eventually, I was tempted to drive her off the nest and found two young ones that looked like they had hatched a few days earlier, but the third egg was missing. I then shared this unusual discovery (as I thought it was) with Mr. and Mrs. Holyoak of Bidford Grange, Warwickshire, and Miss M. Willes, who were visiting my house, and they all went to see it. Recently, I reminded Mr. Holyoak about it, and he told me he clearly remembered the entire situation and, considering it a curiosity, visited it several times. He was completely convinced it was a cuckoo and thought she was more attentive to her young than any other bird he’d observed, as he always found her brooding her chicks. About a week after I first saw the young ones, one was missing, and I suspected my plough boys might have taken it, although it could have been a hawk during the time the mother was searching for food. I never saw her off the nest except once, which was the last time I saw the remaining young one, when it was almost fully feathered. I then left home for a couple of days, and when I came back, the young one was gone, which I assumed had flown away. Even though I often saw cuckoos in the thicket I mentioned, I never noticed a male cuckoo paired with this female."

Nor is this a new observation, though it is entirely overlooked by the modern naturalists, for Aristotle speaking of the cuckoo, asserts that she sometimes builds her nest among broken rocks, and on high mountains, (L. 6. H. c. 1.) but adds in another place that she generally possesses the nest of another bird, (L. 6. H. c. 7.) And Niphus says that cuckoos rarely build for themselves, most frequently laying their eggs in the nests of other birds, (Gesner, L. 3. de Cuculo.)

This isn't a new observation, though modern naturalists completely overlook it. Aristotle, when talking about the cuckoo, notes that it sometimes builds its nest among broken rocks and on high mountains (L. 6. H. c. 1.). However, he also mentions elsewhere that it usually takes over the nest of another bird (L. 6. H. c. 7.). Additionally, Niphus states that cuckoos rarely build their own nests, often laying their eggs in the nests of other birds (Gesner, L. 3. de Cuculo.).

The Philosopher who is acquainted with these facts concerning the cuckoo, would seem to have very little reason himself, if he could imagine this neglect of her young to be a necessary instinct!

The philosopher who knows these facts about the cuckoo would seem to have very little reason if he could think that this neglect of her young is a necessary instinct!

XIV. The deep recesses of the ocean are inaccessible to mankind, which prevents us from having much knowledge of the arts and government of its inhabitants.

XIV. The ocean's deep areas are unreachable for humans, which limits our understanding of the skills and governance of the creatures that live there.

1. One of the baits used by the fisherman is an animal called an Old Soldier, his size and form are somewhat like the craw-fish, with this difference, that his tail is covered with a tough membrane instead of a shell; and to obviate this defect, he seeks out the uninhabited shell of some dead fish, that is large enough to receive his tail, and carries it about with him as part of his clothing or armour.

1. One of the baits used by the fisherman is an animal called an Old Soldier. Its size and shape are somewhat similar to a crawfish, but unlike a crawfish, its tail is covered with a tough membrane instead of a shell. To deal with this issue, it searches for the empty shell of a dead fish that’s large enough to fit its tail and carries it around as part of its outfit or armor.

2. On the coasts about Scarborough, where the haddocks, cods, and dog-fish, are in great abundance, the fishermen universally believe that the dog-fish make a line, or semicircle, to encompass a shoal of haddocks and cod, confining them within certain limits near the shore, and eating them as occasion requires. For the haddocks and cod are always found near the shore without any dog-fish among them, and the dog-fish further off without any haddocks or cod; and yet the former are known to prey upon the latter, and in some years devour such immense quantities as to render this fishery more expensive than profitable.

2. Along the coasts near Scarborough, where haddocks, cod, and dogfish are plentiful, fishermen commonly believe that dogfish form a line or semicircle to trap a school of haddocks and cod, keeping them confined near the shore and eating them when they need to. Haddocks and cod are always found near the shore without any dogfish among them, while dogfish are further out without any haddocks or cod. Still, it’s known that haddocks and cod are preyed upon by dogfish, and in some years, they consume such large quantities that this fishery becomes more costly than beneficial.

3. The remora, when he wishes to remove his situation, as he is a very slow swimmer, is content to take an outside place on whatever conveyance is going his way; nor can the cunning animal be tempted to quit his hold of a ship when she is sailing, not even for the lucre of a piece of pork, lest it should endanger the loss of his passage: at other times he is easily caught with the hook.

3. The remora, when he wants to change his situation, since he swims very slowly, is satisfied to hitch a ride on whatever vessel is going in his direction; nor can the clever creature be persuaded to let go of a ship while it is sailing, not even for the tempting offer of a piece of pork, as it might risk losing his chance for a ride: at other times he can be easily caught with a hook.

4. The crab-fish, like many other testaceous animals, annually changes its shell; it is then in a soft state, covered only with a mucous membrane, and conceals itself in holes in the sand or under weeds; at this place a hard shelled crab always stands centinel, to prevent the sea insects from injuring the other in its defenceless state; and the fishermen from his appearance know where to find the soft ones, which they use for baits in catching other fish.

4. The crab, like many other shellfish, changes its shell every year. During this time, it's soft and only covered by a slimy membrane, so it hides in sand holes or under weeds. A hard-shelled crab usually stands guard there, preventing sea creatures from attacking the vulnerable one. Fishermen can spot this setup and know where to find the soft crabs, which they use as bait to catch other fish.

And though the hard shelled crab, when he is on this duty, advances boldly to meet the foe, and will with difficulty quit the field; yet at other times he shews great timidity, and has a wonderful speed in attempting his escape; and, if often interrupted, will pretend death like the spider, and watch an opportunity to sink himself into the sand, keeping only his eyes above. My ingenious friend Mr. Burdett, who favoured me with these accounts at the time he was surveying the coasts, thinks the commerce between the sexes takes place at this time, and inspires the courage of the creature.

And even though the hard-shelled crab approaches its opponent bravely during this duty and finds it hard to leave the battlefield, at other times it shows significant fear and is incredibly fast when trying to escape. If it's frequently disturbed, it will pretend to be dead, like a spider, waiting for a chance to bury itself in the sand while keeping just its eyes visible. My clever friend Mr. Burdett, who shared these observations with me while he was surveying the coasts, believes that this is when the mating ritual occurs, which boosts the creature's courage.

5. The shoals of herrings, cods, haddocks, and other fish, which approach our shores at certain seasons, and quit them at other seasons without leaving one behind; and the salmon, that periodically frequent our rivers, evince, that there are vagrant tribes of fish, that perform as regular migrations as the birds of passage already mentioned.

5. The schools of herring, cod, haddock, and other fish that come to our shores at certain times of the year and leave without a single one remaining; and the salmon, which regularly visit our rivers, show that there are wandering groups of fish that migrate just as consistently as the migratory birds we talked about earlier.

6. There is a cataract on the river Liffey in Ireland about nineteen feet high: here in the salmon season many of the inhabitants amuse themselves in observing these fish leap up the torrent. They dart themselves quite out of the water as they ascend, and frequently fall back many times before they surmount it, and baskets made of twigs are placed near the edge of the stream to catch them in their fall.

6. There's a waterfall on the river Liffey in Ireland that's about nineteen feet high. During salmon season, many locals enjoy watching these fish leap up the rapids. They jump completely out of the water as they try to climb up, often falling back several times before they finally make it. Baskets made of twigs are set up near the edge of the stream to catch them when they fall.

I have observed, as I have sat by a spout of water, which descends from a stone trough about two feet into a stream below, at particular seasons of the year, a great number of little fish called minums, or pinks, throw themselves about twenty times their own length out of the water, expecting to get into the trough above.

I’ve noticed that while sitting by a water spout, which comes down from a stone trough and falls about two feet into the stream below, during certain times of the year, a lot of little fish called minums, or pinks, jump about twenty times their own length out of the water, trying to get into the trough above.

This evinces that the storgee, or attention of the dam to provide for the offspring, is strongly exerted amongst the nations of fish, where it would seem to be the most neglected; as these salmon cannot be supposed to attempt so difficult and dangerous a task without being conscious of the purpose or end of their endeavours.

This shows that the storgee, or the mother’s care to provide for her young, is really strong among fish, even in species where it seems to be most overlooked. These salmon couldn’t be attempting such a challenging and risky task if they weren’t aware of the reason or goal behind their efforts.

It is further remarkable, that most of the old salmon return to the sea before it is proper for the young shoals to attend them, yet that a few old ones continue in the rivers so late, that they become perfectly emaciated by the inconvenience of their situation, and this apparently to guide or to protect the unexperienced brood.

It’s also noteworthy that most of the older salmon head back to the sea before it’s time for the young schools to follow them. However, a few older ones stay in the rivers so long that they become completely thin and weak from their situation, seemingly to guide or protect the inexperienced young fish.

Of the smaller water animals we have still less knowledge, who nevertheless probably possess many superior arts; some of these are mentioned in Botanic Garden, P. I. Add. Note XXVII. and XXVIII. The nympha of the water-moths of our rivers, which cover themselves with cases of straw, gravel, and shell, contrive to make their habitations, nearly in equilibrium with the water; when too heavy, they add a bit of wood or straw; when too light, a bit of gravel. Edinb. Trans.

Of the smaller aquatic creatures, we know even less, yet they likely have many remarkable abilities. Some of these are mentioned in the Botanic Garden, P. I. Add. Note XXVII. and XXVIII. The nymphs of the water-moths in our rivers, which create homes from straw, gravel, and shells, manage to build their habitats almost perfectly balanced with the water; if they become too heavy, they add a piece of wood or straw; if they’re too light, they add some gravel. Edinb. Trans.

All these circumstances bear a near resemblance to the deliberate actions of human reason.

All these circumstances closely resemble the intentional actions of human reasoning.

XV. We have a very imperfect acquaintance with the various tribes of insects: their occupations, manner of life, and even the number of their senses, differ from our own, and from each other; but there is reason to imagine, that those which possess the sense of touch in the most exquisite degree, and whole occupations require the most constant exertion of their powers, are induced with a greater proportion or knowledge and ingenuity.

XV. Our understanding of the many insect species is still quite limited: their roles, lifestyles, and even the number of senses they have vary from ours and from each other; however, it's reasonable to believe that those insects with a highly developed sense of touch and whose activities demand continuous use of that ability may possess a greater level of knowledge and creativity.

The spiders of this country manufacture nets of various forms, adapted to various situations, to arrest the flies that are their food; and some of them have a house or lodging-place in the middle of the net, well contrived for warmth, security, or concealment. There is a large spider in South America, who constructs nets of so strong a texture as to entangle small birds, particularly the humming bird. And in Jamaica there is another spider, who digs a hole in the earth obliquely downwards, about three inches in length, and one inch in diameter, this cavity she lines with a tough thick web, which when taken out resembles a leathern purse: but what is most curious, this house has a door with hinges, like the operculum of some sea shells; and herself and family, who tenant this nest, open and shut the door, whenever they pass or repass. This history was told me, and the nest with its operculum shewn me by the late Dr. Butt of Bath, who was some years physician in Jamaica.

The spiders in this country make webs in different shapes, designed for various situations, to catch the flies that they eat; and some of them have a shelter or home in the middle of the web, cleverly built for warmth, safety, or hiding. There’s a large spider in South America that builds webs so strong they can trap small birds, especially hummingbirds. In Jamaica, there’s another spider that digs a hole in the ground slantwise, about three inches deep and one inch wide, which it lines with a tough, thick web that looks like a leather purse when pulled out. But the most interesting part is that this home has a door with hinges, similar to the lid of some sea shells; and the spider and its family, who live in this nest, open and close the door whenever they come and go. I heard this story and saw the nest with its lid from the late Dr. Butt of Bath, who was a physician in Jamaica for several years.

The production of these nets is indeed a part of the nature or conformation of the animal, and their natural use is to supply the place of wings, when she wishes to remove to another situation. But when she employs them to entangle her prey, there are marks of evident design, for she adapts the form of each net to its situation, and strengthens those lines, that require it, by joining others to the middle of them, and attaching those others to distant objects, with the same individual art, that is used by mankind in supporting the masts and extending the sails of ships. This work is executed with more mathematical exactness and ingenuity by the field spiders, than by those in our houses, as their constructions are more subjected to the injuries of dews and tempests.

The way these nets are made is really part of the animal's nature, and they naturally serve as a replacement for wings when it wants to move to a different spot. However, when it uses them to capture its prey, there's a clear purpose behind it. The animal shapes each net according to its surroundings and reinforces the strands that need it by adding extra lines in the middle and connecting those to faraway objects, using the same skill that humans use to support masts and spread sails on ships. Field spiders create these structures with more precise mathematical planning and cleverness than the ones found in our homes, as their designs are more exposed to the damage caused by dew and storms.

Besides the ingenuity shewn by these little creatures in taking their prey, the circumstance of their counterfeiting death, when they are put into terror, is truly wonderful; and as soon as the object of terror is removed, they recover and run away. Some beetles are also said to possess this piece of hypocrisy.

Besides the cleverness shown by these little creatures in catching their prey, their ability to fake death when they're scared is truly amazing; as soon as the threat is gone, they recover and scurry away. Some beetles are also said to have this skill of deception.

The curious webs, or chords, constructed by some young caterpillars to defend themselves from cold, or from insects of prey; and by silk-worms and some other caterpillars, when they transmigrate into aureliæ or larvæ, have deservedly excited the admiration of the inquisitive. But our ignorance of their manner of life, and even of the number of their senses, totally precludes us from understanding the means by which they acquire this knowledge.

The fascinating webs, or threads, built by some young caterpillars to protect themselves from the cold or from predatory insects, as well as by silkworms and other caterpillars during their transformation into pupae or larvae, have rightly captured the interest of the curious. However, our lack of knowledge about their way of life, and even how many senses they have, completely prevents us from grasping how they gain this understanding.

The care of the salmon in choosing a proper situation for her spawn, the structure of the nests of birds, their patient incubation, and the art of the cuckoo in depositing her egg in her neighbour's nursery, are instances of great sagacity in those creatures: and yet they are much inferior to the arts exerted by many of the insect tribes on similar occasions. The hairy excrescences on briars, the oak apples, the blasted leaves of trees, and the lumps on the backs of cows, are situations that are rather produced than chosen by the mother insect for the convenience of her offspring. The cells of bees, wasps, spiders, and of the various coralline insects, equally astonish us, whether we attend to the materials or to the architecture.

The way salmon choose the right spot for their eggs, the way birds build their nests and patiently incubate them, and the way a cuckoo lays its egg in another bird's nest are examples of impressive intelligence in these creatures. However, they pale in comparison to the strategies used by many insect species in similar situations. The fuzzy growths on briars, oak galls, the damaged leaves of trees, and the lumps on the backs of cows are places that mother insects create for the benefit of their young rather than just selecting. The structures built by bees, wasps, spiders, and various coral insects are equally astonishing, whether we look at the materials they use or their design.

But the conduct of the ant, and of some species of the ichneumon fly in the incubation of their eggs, is equal to any exertion of human science. The ants many times in a day move their eggs nearer the surface of their habitation, or deeper below it, as the heat of the weather varies; and in colder days lie upon them in heaps for the purpose of incubation: if their mansion is too dry, they carry them to places where there is moisture, and you may distinctly see the little worms move and suck up the water. When too much moisture approaches their nest, they convey their eggs deeper in the earth, or to some other place of safety. (Swammerd. Epil. ad Hist. Insects, p. 153. Phil. Trans. No. 23. Lowthrop. V. 2. p. 7.)

But the behavior of ants and certain types of ichneumon flies when incubating their eggs is comparable to human scientific efforts. Ants frequently move their eggs closer to the surface or deeper into their homes throughout the day, depending on the weather. On colder days, they pile on top of the eggs for warmth. If their home gets too dry, they take the eggs to moister areas, and you can actually see the little larvae moving and absorbing water. When there's too much moisture near their nest, they move their eggs deeper into the ground or to another safe spot. (Swammerd. Epil. ad Hist. Insects, p. 153. Phil. Trans. No. 23. Lowthrop. V. 2. p. 7.)

There is one species of ichneumon-fly, that digs a hole in the earth, and carrying into it two or three living caterpillars, deposits her eggs, and nicely closing up the nest leaves them there; partly doubtless to assist the incubation, and partly to supply food to her future young, (Derham. B. 4, c. 13. Aristotle Hist. Animal, L. 5. c. 20.)

There’s one type of ichneumon fly that digs a hole in the ground, carries in two or three live caterpillars, lays her eggs, and neatly closes up the nest, leaving them there; partly to help with the incubation and partly to provide food for her future young. (Derham. B. 4, c. 13. Aristotle Hist. Animal, L. 5. c. 20.)

A friend of mine put about fifty large caterpillars collected from cabbages on some bran and a few leaves into a box, and covered it with gauze to prevent their escape. After a few days we saw, from more than three fourths of them, about eight or ten little caterpillars of the ichneumon-fly come out of their backs, and spin each a small cocoon of silk, and in a few days the large caterpillars died. This small fly it seems lays its egg in the back of the cabbage caterpillar, which when hatched preys upon the material, which is produced there for the purpose of making silk for the future nest of the cabbage caterpillar; of which being deprived, the creature wanders about till it dies, and thus our gardens are preserved by the ingenuity of this cruel fly. This curious property of producing a silk thread, which is common to some sea animals, see Botanic Garden, Part I. Note XXVII. and is designed for the purpose of their transformation as in the silk-worm, is used for conveying themselves from higher branches to lower ones of trees by some caterpillars, and to make themselves temporary nests or tents, and by the spider for entangling his prey. Nor is it strange that so much knowledge should be acquired by such small animals; since there is reason to imagine, that these insects have the sense of touch, either in their proboscis, or their antennæ, to a great degree of perfection; and thence may possess, as far as their sphere extends, as accurate knowledge, and as subtle invention, as the discoverers of human arts.

A friend of mine collected about fifty large caterpillars from cabbages and put them on some bran and a few leaves in a box, covering it with gauze to keep them from escaping. After a few days, we noticed that from more than three-quarters of them, about eight or ten small caterpillars of the ichneumon fly emerged from their backs and spun small silk cocoons. A few days later, the large caterpillars died. It seems this small fly lays its eggs in the back of the cabbage caterpillar, which, when hatched, feeds on the material meant to create silk for the future nest of the cabbage caterpillar. Deprived of this material, the caterpillar wanders around until it dies, which helps protect our gardens thanks to the cunning of this merciless fly. This interesting ability to produce a silk thread, common in some sea creatures, as noted in the Botanic Garden, Part I, Note XXVII, is used by some caterpillars to move from higher branches to lower ones, to create temporary nests or tents, and by spiders to trap their prey. It’s not surprising that such small animals can gain so much knowledge; there’s reason to believe these insects have a highly developed sense of touch, either in their proboscis or their antennas, allowing them to acquire the same level of knowledge and subtle creativity within their realm as the inventors of human arts.

XVI. 1. If we were better acquainted with the histories of those insects that are formed into societies, as the bees, wasps, and ants, I make no doubt but we should find, that their arts and improvements are not so similar and uniform as they now appear to us, but that they arose in the same manner from experience and tradition, as the arts of our own species; though their reasoning is from fewer ideas, is busied about fewer objects, and is exerted with less energy.

XVI. 1. If we knew more about the histories of social insects like bees, wasps, and ants, I’m sure we would see that their skills and advancements are not as similar and uniform as they seem. Instead, they likely developed in the same way as our own—through experience and tradition. However, their reasoning is based on fewer ideas, focuses on fewer things, and is less vigorous.

There are some kinds of insects that migrate like the birds before mentioned. The locust of warmer climates has sometimes come over to England; it is shaped like a grasshopper, with very large wings, and a body above an inch in length. It is mentioned as coming into Egypt with an east wind, "The lord brought an east wind upon the land all that day and night, and in the morning the east wind brought the locusts, and covered the face of the earth, so that the land was dark," Exod. x. 13. The migrations of these insects are mentioned in another part of the scripture, "The locusts have no king, yet go they forth all of them in bands," Prov. xxx. 27.

There are certain types of insects that migrate like the birds mentioned earlier. The locusts from warmer regions have occasionally made their way to England; they resemble grasshoppers, have very large wings, and their bodies are over an inch long. They are noted for arriving in Egypt with an east wind: "The Lord brought an east wind upon the land all that day and night, and in the morning the east wind brought the locusts, and covered the face of the earth, so that the land was dark," Exod. x. 13. The migrations of these insects are referred to in another part of the scripture: "The locusts have no king, yet they all go forth in bands," Prov. xxx. 27.

The accurate Mr. Adanson, near the river Gambia in Africa, was witness to the migration of these insects. "About eight in the morning, in the month of February, there suddenly arose over our heads a thick cloud, which darkened the air, and deprived us of the rays of the sun. We found it was a cloud of locusts raised about twenty or thirty fathoms from the ground, and covering an extent of several leagues; at length a shower of these insects descended, and after devouring every green herb, while they rested, again resumed their flight. This cloud was brought by a strong east-wind, and was all the morning in passing over the adjacent country." (Voyage to Senegal, 158.)

The precise Mr. Adanson, near the Gambia River in Africa, witnessed the migration of these insects. "Around eight in the morning in February, a thick cloud suddenly appeared above us, blocking the sunlight and darkening the air. We realized it was a cloud of locusts, about twenty or thirty fathoms off the ground, covering several leagues. Eventually, a swarm of these insects descended, devouring every green plant as they settled, and then took flight again. This cloud was carried by a strong east wind and spent the entire morning moving across the nearby land." (Voyage to Senegal, 158.)

In this country the gnats are sometimes seen to migrate in clouds, like the musketoes of warmer climates, and our swarms of bees frequently travel many miles, and are said in North America always to fly towards the south. The prophet Isaiah has a beautiful allusion to these migrations, "The Lord shall call the fly from the rivers of Egypt, and shall hiss for the bee that is in the land of Assyria," Isa. vii. 18. which has been lately explained by Mr. Bruce, in his travels to discover the source of the Nile.

In this country, gnats can sometimes be seen migrating in swarms, similar to the mosquitoes found in warmer climates, and our honeybees often fly many miles. It's said that in North America, they always head south. The prophet Isaiah makes a lovely reference to these migrations: "The Lord shall call the fly from the rivers of Egypt, and shall hiss for the bee that is in the land of Assyria," Isaiah 7:18. This was recently explained by Mr. Bruce during his travels to find the source of the Nile.

2. I am well informed that the bees that were carried into Barbadoes, and other western islands, ceased to lay up any honey after the first year, as they found it not useful to them: and are now become very troublesome to the inhabitants of those islands by infesting their sugar houses; but those in Jamaica continue to make honey, as the cold north winds, or rainy seasons of that island, confine them at home for several weeks together. And the bees of Senegal, which differ from those of Europe only in size, make their honey not only superior to ours in delicacy of flavour, but it has this singularity, that it never concretes, but remains liquid as syrup, (Adanson). From some observations of Mr. Wildman, and of other people of veracity, it appears, that during the severe part of the winter season for weeks together the bees are quite benumbed and torpid from the cold, and do not consume any of their provision. This state of sleep, like that of swallows and bats, seems to be the natural resource of those creatures in cold climates, and the making of honey to be an artificial improvement.

2. I’ve heard that the bees brought to Barbados and other western islands stopped making honey after the first year because they found it unnecessary. Now, they have become quite a nuisance for the locals by invading their sugar houses. However, the bees in Jamaica still produce honey since the cold northern winds or rainy seasons confine them to their hives for several weeks. The bees in Senegal, which are only different from the European ones in size, produce honey that is not only more delicately flavored than ours but also has the unique characteristic of never solidifying, remaining liquid like syrup (Adanson). According to observations by Mr. Wildman and other trustworthy individuals, it seems that during the harsh part of winter, bees become numb and inactive due to the cold for several weeks and don’t consume any of their stored food. This state of dormancy, similar to that of swallows and bats, appears to be a natural survival strategy for these creatures in cold climates, with honey production being more of an artificial enhancement.

As the death of our hives of bees appears to be owning to their being kept so warm, as to require food when their stock is exhausted; a very observing gentleman at my request put two hives for many weeks into a dry cellar, and observed, during all that time, they did not consume any of their provision, for their weight did not decrease as it had done when they were kept in the open air. The same observation is made in the Annual Register for 1768, p. 113. And the Rev. Mr. White, in his Method of preserving Bees, adds, that those on the north side of his house consumed less honey in the winter than those on the south side.

As it seems the decline of our bee colonies is due to them being kept too warm, which makes them need food when their supplies run out, a very observant gentleman, at my request, placed two hives in a dry cellar for several weeks. During that time, he noticed they didn’t eat any of their food, as their weight didn’t decrease like it did when they were outside. The same observation is noted in the Annual Register for 1768, p. 113. Additionally, Rev. Mr. White, in his Method of Preserving Bees, mentions that the bees on the north side of his house consumed less honey in winter compared to those on the south side.

There is another observation on bees well ascertained, that they at various times, when the season begins to be cold, by a general motion of their legs as they hang in clusters produce a degree of warmth, which is easily perceptible by the hand. Hence by this ingenious exertion, they for a long time prevent the torpid state they would naturally fall into.

There’s another well-known observation about bees: at different times when the weather starts getting colder, they create warmth by moving their legs while hanging in clusters, and it’s easy to feel this warmth by hand. As a result of this clever effort, they can delay the sluggish state they would normally enter.

According to the late observations of Mr. Hunter, it appears that the bee's-wax is not made from the dust of the anthers of flowers, which they bring home on their thighs, but that this makes what is termed bee-bread, and is used for the purpose of feeding the bee-maggots; in the same manner butterflies live on honey, but the previous caterpillar lives on vegetable leaves, while the maggots of large flies require flesh for their food, and those of the ichneumon fly require insects for their food. What induces the bee who lives on honey to lay up vegetable powder for its young? What induces the butterfly to lay its eggs on leaves, when itself feeds on honey? What induces the other flies to seek a food for their progeny different from what they consume themselves? If these are not deductions from their own previous experience or observation, all the actions of mankind must be resolved into instinct.

According to Mr. Hunter's recent observations, it seems that beeswax isn't made from the pollen collected on their legs; instead, that pollen creates what we call bee-bread, which is used to feed the bee larvae. Similarly, butterflies feed on nectar, but caterpillars thrive on plant leaves. In contrast, the larvae of large flies require meat, and those of the ichneumon fly eat other insects. What drives the honeybee, which feeds on nectar, to collect plant material for its young? What makes the butterfly lay its eggs on leaves when it itself feeds on nectar? What leads other flies to find food for their offspring that’s different from what they eat? If these behaviors aren’t based on their past experiences or observations, then all human actions must just be instinct.

3. The dormouse consumes but little of its food during the rigour of the season, for they roll themselves up, or sleep, or lie torpid the greatest part of the time; but on warm sunny days experience a short revival, and take a little food, and then relapse into their former state." (Pennant Zoolog. p. 67.) Other animals, that sleep in winter without laying up any provender, are observed to go into their winter beds fat and strong, but return to day-light in the spring season very lean and feeble. The common flies sleep during the winter without any provision for their nourishment, and are daily revived by the warmth of the sun, or of our fires. These whenever they see light endeavour to approach it, having observed, that by its greater vicinity they get free from the degree of torpor, that the cold produces; and are hence induced perpetually to burn themselves in our candles: deceived, like mankind, by the misapplication of their knowledge. Whilst many of the subterraneous insects, as the common worms, seem to retreat so deep into the earth as not to be enlivened or awakened by the difference of our winter days; and stop up their holes with leaves or straws, to prevent the frosts from injuring them, or the centipes from devouring them. The habits of peace, or the stratagems of war, of these subterranean nations are covered from our view; but a friend of mine prevailed on a distressed worm to enter the hole of another worm on a bowling-green, and he presently returned much wounded about his head. And I once saw a worm rise hastily out of the earth into the sunshine, and observed a centipes hanging at its tail: the centipes nimbly quitted the tail, and seizing the worm about its middle cut it in half with its forceps, and preyed upon one part, while the other escaped. Which evinces they have design in stopping the mouths of their habitations.

3. The dormouse barely eats during the harsh season, as it tends to curl up, sleep, or stay inactive most of the time. However, on warm sunny days, it wakes up briefly, eats a little, and then goes back to its previous state. (Pennant Zoolog. p. 67.) Other animals that hibernate without storing food start winter fat and healthy, but emerge in spring very thin and weak. Common flies hibernate in winter without any food reserves and are revived daily by the warmth of the sun or our fires. Whenever they see light, they try to get closer to it, having learned that being near light helps them shake off the cold's torpor; this often leads them to burn themselves in our candles, misled like humans by their misunderstanding. Meanwhile, many underground insects, like common worms, seem to burrow so deep into the earth that they're unaffected by the winter days, blocking their holes with leaves or straws to protect themselves from frost or predators like centipedes. The peaceful behaviors and survival tactics of these underground creatures remain hidden from us, though a friend of mine once convinced a distressed worm to enter the hole of another worm on a bowling green, after which it returned wounded. I once saw a worm quickly emerge from the ground into the sunlight, with a centipede hanging onto its tail. The centipede quickly let go and seized the worm around the middle, cutting it in half with its pincers, eating one half while the other escaped. This suggests that they have a strategy for sealing their homes.

4. The wasp of this country fixes his habitation under ground, that he may not be affected with the various changes of our climate; but in Jamaica he hangs it on the bough of a tree, where the seasons are less severe. He weaves a very curious paper of vegetable fibres to cover his nest, which is constructed on the same principle with that of the bee, but with a different material; but as his prey consists of flesh, fruits, and insects, which are perishable commodities, he can lay up no provender for the winter.

4. The wasp in this country builds its home underground to avoid the various changes in our climate; however, in Jamaica, it hangs its nest in the branches of a tree, where the seasons are milder. It creates a very interesting paper from plant fibers to cover its nest, which is built on the same principle as that of the bee, but with different materials. Since its food consists of meat, fruits, and insects—items that spoil easily—it can't store food for the winter.

M. de la Loubiere, in his relation of Siam, says, "That in a part of that kingdom, which lies open to great inundations, all the ants make their settlements upon trees; no ants' nests are to be seen any where else." Whereas in our country the ground is their only situation. From the scriptual account of these insects, one might be led to suspect, that in some climates they lay up a provision for the winter. Origen affirms the same, (Cont. Cels. L. 4.) But it is generally believed that in this country they do not, (Prov. vi. 6. xxx. 25.) The white ants of the coast of Africa make themselves pyramids eight or ten feet high, on a base of about the same width, with a smooth surface of rich clay, excessively hard and well built, which appear at a distance like an assemblage of the huts of the negroes, (Adanson). The history of these has been lately well described in the Philosoph. Transactions, under the name of termes, or termites. These differ very much from the nest of our large ant; but the real history of this creature, as well as of the wasp, is yet very imperfectly known.

M. de la Loubiere, in his account of Siam, says, "In a part of that kingdom that is prone to severe flooding, all the ants build their homes in trees; you won’t find any ant nests anywhere else." In our country, they only use the ground for their homes. From the biblical description of these insects, one might think that in some climates they store food for winter. Origen agrees with this (Cont. Cels. L. 4.) But it’s commonly believed that in this country they don’t (Prov. vi. 6. xxx. 25.) The white ants along the coast of Africa build pyramids that are eight to ten feet tall, with a base about the same width, made of a smooth, rich clay that is very hard and well-constructed, resembling a collection of huts made by black Africans from a distance (Adanson). Their history has recently been thoroughly documented in the Philosoph. Transactions, under the term termes, or termites. These are very different from the nests of our large ant; however, the true history of this creature, along with that of the wasp, is still not very well understood.

Wasps are said to catch large spiders, and to cut off their legs, and carry their mutilated bodies to their young, Dict. Raison. Tom. I. p. 152.

Wasps are known to catch large spiders, cut off their legs, and bring their mutilated bodies to their young, Dict. Raison. Tom. I. p. 152.

One circumstance I shall relate which fell under my own eye, and shewed the power or reason in a wasp, as it is exercised among men. A wasp, on a gravel walk, had caught a fly nearly as large as himself; kneeling on the ground I observed him separate the tail and the head from the body part, to which the wings were attached. He then took the body part in his paws, and rose about two feet from the ground with it; but a gentle breeze wafting the wings of the fly turned him round in the air, and he settled again with his prey upon the gravel. I then distinctly observed him cut off with his mouth, first one of the wings, and then the other, after which he flew away with it unmolested by the wind.

One situation I witnessed that demonstrated the reasoning power in a wasp, similar to that of humans, was quite interesting. A wasp on a gravel path had caught a fly that was almost as big as itself. Kneeling on the ground, I saw him separate the tail and the head from the body part to which the wings were attached. He then grabbed the body part with his legs and lifted it about two feet off the ground. However, a gentle breeze moved the wings of the fly, causing him to turn mid-air, and he landed back on the gravel with his catch. I then clearly saw him use his mouth to cut off one of the wings, and then the other, after which he flew away without being disturbed by the wind.

Go, thou sluggard, learn arts and industry from the bee, and from the ant!

Go, you slacker, learn work and skill from the bee and the ant!

Go, proud reasoner, and call the worm thy sister!

Go ahead, proud thinker, and call the worm your sister!

XVII. Conclusion.

XVII. Conclusion.

It was before observed how much the superior accuracy of our sense of touch contributes to increase our knowledge; but it is the greater energy and activity of the power of volition (as explained in the former Sections of this work) that marks mankind, and has given him the empire of the world.

It has been noted before how much our sense of touch enhances our understanding; however, it is the stronger energy and activity of our willpower (as discussed in previous sections of this work) that distinguishes humans and has granted us dominion over the world.

There is a criterion by which we may distinguish our voluntary acts or thoughts from those that are excited by our sensations: "The former are always employed about the means to acquire pleasureable objects, or to avoid painful ones: while the latter are employed about the possession of those that are already in our power."

There’s a way to tell the difference between our voluntary actions or thoughts and those triggered by our senses: “The former are always focused on the means to get pleasurable things or to steer clear of painful ones, while the latter are focused on the possession of things we already have.”

If we turn our eyes upon the fabric of our fellow animals, we find they are supported with bones, covered with skins, moved by muscles; that they possess the same senses, acknowledge the same appetites, and are nourished by the same aliment with ourselves; and we should hence conclude from the strongest analogy, that their internal faculties were also in some measure similar to our own.

If we look at the bodies of other animals, we see that they have bones, are covered in skin, and are moved by muscles; they have the same senses, share the same desires, and are fed by the same food as we are; from this, we should reasonably conclude that their internal abilities are also somewhat similar to ours.

Mr. Locke indeed published an opinion, that other animals possessed no abstract or general ideas, and thought this circumstance was the barrier between the brute and the human world. But these abstracted ideas have been since demonstrated by Bishop Berkley, and allowed by Mr. Hume, to have no existence in nature, not even in the mind of their inventor, and we are hence necessitated to look for some other mark of distinction.

Mr. Locke did share the view that other animals didn’t have abstract or general ideas, and he believed this was what separated the animal world from the human world. However, Bishop Berkeley later showed that these abstract ideas don’t actually exist in nature, not even in the mind of the person who created them, so we must seek another way to distinguish between the two.

The ideas and actions of brutes, like those of children, are almost perpetually produced by their present pleasures, or their present pains; and, except in the few instances that have been mentioned in this Section, they seldom busy themselves about the means of procuring future bliss, or of avoiding future misery.

The thoughts and behaviors of animals, like those of kids, are mostly driven by their current pleasures or pains. Other than the few examples already mentioned in this section, they rarely focus on the ways to achieve future happiness or avoid future suffering.

Whilst the acquiring of languages, the making of tools, and the labouring for money; which are all only the means of procuring pleasure; and the praying to the Deity, as another means to procure happiness, are characteristic of human nature.

While learning languages, creating tools, and working for money— which are all just the ways to gain pleasure; and praying to God, as another way to gain happiness, are typical of human nature.



SECT. XVII.

THE CATENATION OF MOTIONS.

THE CHAINING OF MOVEMENTS.

I. 1. Catenations of animal motion. 2. Are produced by irritations, by sensations, by volitions. 3. They continue some time after they have been excited. Cause of catenation. 4. We can then exert our attention on other objects. 5. Many catenations of motions go on together. 6. Some links of the catenations of motions may be left out without disuniting the chain. 7. Interrupted circles of motion continue confusedly till they come to the part of the circle, where they were disturbed. 8. Weaker catenations are dissevered by stronger. 9. Then new catenations take place. 10. Much effort prevents their reuniting. Impediment of speech. 11. Trains more easily dissevered than circles. 12. Sleep destroys volition and external stimulus. II. Instances of various catenations in a young lady playing on the harpsichord. III. 1. What catenations are the strongest. 2. Irritations joined with associations from strongest connexions. Vital motions. 3. New links with increased force, cold fits of fever produced. 4. New links with decreased force. Cold bath. 5. Irritation joined with sensation. Inflammatory fever. Why children cannot tickle themselves. 6. Volition joined with sensation. Irritative ideas of sound become sensible. 7. Ideas of imagination, dissevered by irritations, by volition, production of surprise.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Connections of animal movement. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Are caused by irritations, sensations, and intentions. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. They persist for some time after being triggered. The cause of these connections. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. We can then focus our attention on other things. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Many movements happen simultaneously. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Some links in the movement connections can be missing without breaking the chain. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Interrupted movement cycles continue chaotically until they reach the point in the cycle where they were disrupted. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Weaker connections are broken by stronger ones. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Then new connections occur. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Great effort prevents them from rejoining. Speech impediment. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. Chains are more easily broken than circles. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. Sleep inhibits intention and external stimuli. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. Examples of various connections in a young woman playing the harpsichord. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. What connections are the strongest. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. Irritations linked with associations create the strongest connections. Vital movements. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. New links formed with increased strength, cold fits of fever are produced. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_18__. New links formed with decreased strength. Cold bath. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_19__. Irritation combined with sensation. Inflammatory fever. This is why children can't tickle themselves. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_20__. Intention combined with sensation. Irritating ideas of sound become perceptible. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_21__. Imagined ideas, severed by irritations and intentions, produce surprise.

I. 1. To investigate with precision the catenations of animal motions, it would be well to attend to the manner of their production; but we cannot begin this disquisition early enough for this purpose, as the catenations of motion seem to begin with life, and are only extinguishable with it; We have spoken of the power of irritation, of sensation, of volition, and of association, as preceding the fibrous motions; we now step forwards, and consider, that conversely they are in their turn preceded by those motions; and that all the successive trains or circles of our actions are composed of this twofold concatenation. Those we shall call trains of action, which continue to proceed without any stated repetitions; and those circles of action, when the parts of them return at certain periods, though the trains, of which they consist, are not exactly similar. The reading an epic poem is a train of actions; the reading a song with a chorus at equal distances in the measure constitutes so many circles of action.

I. 1. To analyze the connections of animal movements with accuracy, we should focus on how they are generated; however, we can’t start this discussion too early, as these connections of motion seem to begin with life and only cease when it does. We have already talked about the influence of irritation, sensation, volition, and association as precursors to fibrous motions; now we move forward to recognize that they are also preceded by those motions. All the various sequences or cycles of our actions consist of this twofold connection. We will refer to sequences of action as those that continue without any set repetitions, and cycles of action occur when parts of them recur at specific intervals, even if the sequences they contain aren't exactly the same. Reading an epic poem is a sequence of actions; singing a song with a chorus at regular intervals creates multiple cycles of action.

2. Some catenations of animal motion are produced by reiterated successive irritations, as when we learn to repeat the alphabet in its order by frequently reading the letters of it. Thus the vermicular motions of the bowels were originally produced by the successive irritations of the passing aliment; and the succession of actions of the auricles and ventricles of the heart was originally formed by successive stimulus of the blood, these afterwards become part of the diurnal circles of animal actions, as appears by the periodical returns of hunger, and the quickened pulse of weak people in the evening.

2. Some patterns of animal movement come from repeated stimuli, like when we learn to recite the alphabet in order by constantly reading the letters. For example, the wave-like movements of the intestines were originally caused by the repeated stimulation from food traveling through them; similarly, the sequence of contractions in the heart's atria and ventricles was initially triggered by the flow of blood. Over time, these actions became part of the daily rhythms of an animal's behavior, as seen in the regular cycles of hunger and the increased heartbeat of weak individuals in the evening.

Other catenations of animal motion are gradually acquired by successive agreeable sensations, as in learning a favourite song or dance; others by disagreeable sensations, as in coughing or nictitation; these become associated by frequent repetition, and afterwards compose parts of greater circles of action like those above mentioned.

Other connections of animal movement are slowly developed through repeated pleasant experiences, like learning a favorite song or dance; others come from unpleasant experiences, such as coughing or blinking; these get linked together through frequent repetition and later form parts of larger patterns of behavior like those mentioned above.

Other catenations of motions are gradually acquired by frequent voluntary repetitions; as when we deliberately learn to march, read, fence, or any mechanic art, the motions of many of our muscles become gradually linked together in trains, tribes, or circles of action. Thus when any one at first begins to use the tools in turning wood or metals in a lathe, he wills the motions of his hand or fingers, till at length these actions become so connected with the effect, that he seems only to will the point of the chisel. These are caused by volition, connected by association like those above described, and afterwards become parts of our diurnal trains or circles of action.

Other sets of motions are gradually learned through frequent voluntary practice; for example, when we intentionally learn to march, read, fence, or any skill, the movements of many of our muscles become linked together in sequences, groups, or cycles of action. So, when someone first starts using tools to turn wood or metal on a lathe, they consciously control the movements of their hand or fingers until eventually, those actions are so connected to the outcome that it feels like they are only willing the tip of the chisel. These are driven by intention, linked by association like those mentioned earlier, and later become parts of our daily routines or cycles of action.

3. All these catenations of animal motions, are liable to proceed some time after they are excited, unless they are disturbed or impeded by other irritations, sensations, or volitions; and in many instances in spite of our endeavours to stop them; and this property of animal motions is probably the cause of their catenation. Thus when a child revolves some minute on one foot, the spectra of the ambient objects appear to circulate round him some time after he falls upon the ground. Thus the palpitation of the heart continues some time after the object of fear, which occasioned it, is removed. The blush of shame, which is an excess of sensation, and the glow of anger, which is an excess of volition, continue some time, though the affected person finds, that those emotions were caused by mistaken facts, and endeavours to extinguish their appearance. See Sect. XII. 1. 5.

3. All these catenations of animal motions, are liable to proceed some time after they are excited, unless they are disturbed or impeded by other irritations, sensations, or volitions; and in many instances in spite of our endeavours to stop them; and this property of animal motions is probably the cause of their catenation. Thus when a child revolves some minute on one foot, the spectra of the ambient objects appear to circulate round him some time after he falls upon the ground. Thus the palpitation of the heart continues some time after the object of fear, which occasioned it, is removed. The blush of shame, which is an excess of sensation, and the glow of anger, which is an excess of volition, continue some time, though the affected person finds, that those emotions were caused by mistaken facts, and endeavours to extinguish their appearance. See Sect. XII. 1. 5.

4. When a circle of motions becomes connected, by frequent repetitions as above, we can exert our attention strongly on other objects, and the concatenated circle of motions will nevertheless proceed in due order; as whilst you are thinking on this subject, you use variety of muscles in walking about your parlour, or in sitting at your writing-table.

4. When a series of movements becomes linked through frequent repetition as described, we can focus our attention on other things, and the connected series of movements will still continue in the right order; just as when you’re thinking about this topic, you engage different muscles while walking around your room or sitting at your desk.

5. Innumerable catenations of motions may proceed at the same time, without incommoding each other. Of these are the motions of the heart and arteries; those of digestion and glandular secretion; of the ideas, or sensual motions; those of progression, and of speaking; the great annual circle of actions so apparent in birds in their times of breeding and moulting; the monthly circles of many female animals; and the diurnal circles of sleeping and waking, of fulness and inanition.

5. Countless chains of movement can happen at the same time without disturbing each other. These include the movements of the heart and arteries, those involved in digestion and gland secretion, the flow of thoughts or sensory movements, movements for progressing and speaking, the significant annual patterns seen in birds during their breeding and molting seasons, the monthly cycles of many female animals, and the daily cycles of sleeping and waking, of being full and empty.

6. Some links of successive trains or of synchronous tribes of action may be left out without disjoining the whole. Such are our usual trains of recollection; after having travelled through an entertaining country, and viewed many delightful lawns, rolling rivers, and echoing rocks; in the recollection of our journey we leave out the many districts, that we crossed, which were marked with no peculiar pleasure. Such also are our complex ideas, they are catenated tribes of ideas, which do not perfectly resemble their correspondent perceptions, because some of the parts are omitted.

6. Some links between successive trains or groups of actions can be left out without disrupting the whole. This is similar to our usual memories; after exploring a fascinating place and seeing many beautiful lawns, flowing rivers, and echoing rocks, we often forget the many areas we passed through that didn’t stand out in any special way. Our complex ideas work in the same way—they are interconnected groups of thoughts that don’t fully reflect their related perceptions because some parts are missing.

7. If an interrupted circle of actions is not entirely dissevered, it will continue to proceed confusedly, till it comes to the part of the circle, where it was interrupted.

7. If an interrupted circle of actions isn't completely broken, it will keep going chaotically until it reaches the point in the circle where it was interrupted.

The vital motions in a fever from drunkenness, and in other periodical diseases, are instances of this circumstance. The accidental inebriate does not recover himself perfectly till about the same hour on the succeeding day. The accustomed drunkard is disordered, if he has not his usual potation of fermented liquor. So if a considerable part of a connected tribe of action be disturbed, that whole tribe goes on with confusion, till the part of the tribe affected regains its accustomed catenations. So vertigo produces vomiting, and a great secretion of bile, as in sea-sickness, all these being parts of the tribe of irritative catenations.

The important reactions during a fever from drinking too much, and in other recurring illnesses, are examples of this situation. Someone who has accidentally gotten drunk doesn’t fully recover until around the same time the next day. A regular drinker feels off if they don’t have their usual drink of alcohol. Similarly, if a significant part of a connected chain of actions is disrupted, that entire chain continues in chaos until the affected part gets back to its normal connections. For instance, vertigo causes vomiting and a lot of bile production, similar to seasickness, all of which are parts of the chain of irritative reactions.

8. Weaker catenated trains may be dissevered by the sudden exertion of the stronger. When a child first attempts to walk across a room, call to him, and he instantly falls upon the ground. So while I am thinking over the virtues of my friends, if the tea-kettle spurt out some hot water on my stocking; the sudden pain breaks the weaker chain of ideas, and introduces a new group of figures of its own. This circumstance is extended to some unnatural trains of action, which have not been confirmed by long habit; as the hiccough, or an ague-fit, which are frequently curable by surprise. A young lady about eleven years old had for five days had a contraction of one muscle in her fore arm, and another in her arm, which occurred four or five times every minute; the muscles were seen to leap, but without bending the arm. To counteract this new morbid habit, an issue was placed over the convulsed muscle of her arm, and an adhesive plaster wrapped tight like a bandage over the whole fore arm, by which the new motions were immediately destroyed, but the means were continued some weeks to prevent a return.

8. Weaker connected trains can be separated by the sudden force of the stronger ones. When a child first tries to walk across a room, if you call to him, he instantly falls to the ground. Similarly, while I’m pondering the qualities of my friends, if the tea kettle splashes some hot water on my sock, the sudden pain disrupts the weaker chain of thoughts and brings in a new set of ideas. This situation also applies to some unnatural behaviors that haven’t been reinforced by long-standing habits, like hiccups or a fit of shivering, which can often be cured by a surprise. There was a young girl, around eleven years old, who for five days experienced a contraction of one muscle in her forearm and another in her arm, which happened four or five times every minute; her muscles would twitch but without bending her arm. To counter this new troubling habit, a treatment was applied over the affected muscle in her arm, and an adhesive bandage was tightly wrapped around her entire forearm, which immediately stopped the new movements, but the treatment continued for several weeks to prevent a relapse.

9. If any circle of actions is dissevered, either by omission of some of the links, as in sleep, or by insertion of other links, as in surprise, new catenations take place in a greater or less degree. The last link of the broken chain of actions becomes connected with the new motion which has broken it, or with that which was nearest the link omitted; and these new catenations proceed instead of the old ones. Hence the periodic returns of ague-fits, and the chimeras of our dreams.

9. If any chain of actions is interrupted, either by leaving out some links, like in sleep, or by adding new ones, like in surprise, new connections form to varying degrees. The last link of the disrupted chain becomes connected with the new action that caused the break or with the link that was closest to the one that was omitted; and these new connections take place instead of the old ones. This is why we have periodic episodes of chills and the strange images in our dreams.

10. If a train of actions is dissevered, much effort of volition or sensation will prevent its being restored. Thus in the common impediment of speech, when the association of the motions of the muscles of enunciation with the idea of the word to be spoken is disordered, the great voluntary efforts, which distort the countenance, prevent the rejoining of the broken associations. See No. II. 10. of this Section. It is thus likewise observable in some inflammations of the bowels, the too strong efforts made by the muscles to carry forwards the offending material fixes it more firmly in its place, and prevents the cure. So in endeavouring to recal to our memory some particular word of a sentence, if we exert ourselves too strongly about it, we are less likely to regain it.

10. If a train of actions is dissevered, much effort of volition or sensation will prevent its being restored. Thus in the common impediment of speech, when the association of the motions of the muscles of enunciation with the idea of the word to be spoken is disordered, the great voluntary efforts, which distort the countenance, prevent the rejoining of the broken associations. See No. II. 10. of this Section. It is thus likewise observable in some inflammations of the bowels, the too strong efforts made by the muscles to carry forwards the offending material fixes it more firmly in its place, and prevents the cure. So in endeavouring to recal to our memory some particular word of a sentence, if we exert ourselves too strongly about it, we are less likely to regain it.

11. Catenated trains or tribes of action are easier dissevered than catenated circles of action. Hence in epileptic fits the synchronous connected tribes of action, which keep the body erect, are dissevered, but the circle of vital motions continues undisturbed.

11. Linked groups of actions are easier to separate than linked cycles of action. Thus, during epileptic seizures, the synchronized connected groups of actions that maintain the body's posture are divided, but the cycle of vital movements remains unaffected.

12. Sleep destroys the power of volition, and precludes the stimuli of external objects, and thence dissevers the trains, of which these are a part; which confirms the other catenations, as those of the vital motions, secretions, and absorptions; and produces the new trains of ideas, which constitute our dreams.

12. Sleep takes away our ability to make choices and blocks the influence of outside factors, which interrupts the connections they belong to. This reinforces other processes, like vital movements, secretions, and absorptions, and creates new streams of thoughts that make up our dreams.

II. 1. All the preceding circumstances of the catenations of animal motions will be more clearly understood by the following example of a person learning music; and when we recollect the variety of mechanic arts, which are performed by associated trains of muscular actions catenated with the effects they produce, as in knitting, netting, weaving; and the greater variety of associated trains of ideas caused or catenated by volitions or sensations, as in our hourly modes of reasoning, or imagining, or recollecting, we shall gain some idea of the innumerable catenated trains and circles of action, which form the tenor of our lives, and which began, and will only cease entirely with them.

II. 1. All the previous aspects of how animal movements are connected will be clearer with the following example of someone learning music; and when we think about the various mechanical skills, which are carried out through linked sequences of muscle actions connected with the results they create, as in knitting, netting, and weaving; and the even broader range of connected sequences of thoughts triggered by our actions or sensations, like in our daily reasoning, imagining, or remembering, we will get a sense of the countless linked sequences and cycles of activity that shape our lives, which began, and will only completely end with them.

2. When a young lady begins to learn music, she voluntarily applies herself to the characters of her music-book, and by many repetitions endeavours to catenate them with the proportions of sound, of which they are symbols. The ideas excited by the musical characters are slowly connected with the keys of the harpsichord, and much effort is necessary to produce every note with the proper finger, and in its due place and time; till at length a train of voluntary exertions becomes catenated with certain irritations. As the various notes by frequent repetitions become connected in the order, in which they are produced, a new catenation of sensitive exertions becomes mixed with the voluntary ones above described; and not only the musical symbols of crotchets and quavers, but the auditory notes and tones at the same time, become so many successive or synchronous links in this circle of catenated actions.

2. When a young woman starts to learn music, she actively engages with the symbols in her music book and, through repeated practice, tries to link them to the sounds they represent. The concepts triggered by the musical symbols are gradually associated with the keys of the harpsichord, and significant effort is needed to play each note accurately, using the correct fingers, and at the right time; eventually, a series of intentional actions becomes connected to specific responses. As the various notes become linked through constant repetition in the order they are played, a new connection of sensitive responses merges with the intentional actions described above; and not only the musical symbols of quarter notes and eighth notes, but also the auditory notes and tones simultaneously, become a series of successive or simultaneous links in this cycle of connected actions.

At length the motions of her fingers become catenated with the musical characters; and these no sooner strike the eye, than the finger presses down the key without any voluntary attention between them; the activity of the hand being connected with the irritation of the figure or place of the musical symbol on the retina; till at length by frequent repetitions of the same tune the movements of her fingers in playing, and the muscles of the larynx in singing, become associated with each other, and form part of those intricate trains and circles of catenated motions, according with the second article of the preceding propositions in No. 1. of this Section.

At length the motions of her fingers become catenated with the musical characters; and these no sooner strike the eye, than the finger presses down the key without any voluntary attention between them; the activity of the hand being connected with the irritation of the figure or place of the musical symbol on the retina; till at length by frequent repetitions of the same tune the movements of her fingers in playing, and the muscles of the larynx in singing, become associated with each other, and form part of those intricate trains and circles of catenated motions, according with the second article of the preceding propositions in No. 1. of this Section.

3. Besides the facility, which by habit attends the execution of this musical performance, a curious circumstance occurs, which is, that when our young musician has began a tune, she finds herself inclined to continue it; and that even when she is carelessly singing alone without attending to her own song; according with the third preceding article.

3. In addition to the venue that typically hosts this musical performance, an interesting situation arises: when our young musician starts a tune, she feels compelled to keep going, even if she's just singing to herself without focusing on her own song, in line with the third article mentioned earlier.

4. At the same time that our young performer continues to play with great exactness this accustomed tune, she can bend her mind, and that intensely, on some other object, according with the fourth article of the preceding proportions.

4. While our young performer keeps playing this familiar tune with great precision, she can also focus her mind intensely on something else, in line with the fourth article of the previous guidelines.

The manuscript copy of this work was lent to many of my friends at different times for the purpose of gaining their opinions and criticisms on many parts of it, and I found the following anecdote written with a pencil opposite to this page, but am not certain by whom. "I remember seeing the pretty young actress, who succeeded Mrs. Arne in the performance of the celebrated Padlock, rehearse the musical parts at her harpsichord under the eye of her master with great taste and accuracy; though I observed her countenance full of emotion, which I could not account for; at last she suddenly burst into tears; for she had all this time been eyeing a beloved canary bird, suffering great agonies, which at that instant fell dead from its perch."

The manuscript of this work was shared with many of my friends at different times to get their opinions and feedback on various parts of it, and I found the following anecdote written in pencil next to this page, although I'm not sure who wrote it. "I remember seeing the beautiful young actress who took over for Mrs. Arne in the performance of the famous Padlock, rehearsing the musical parts at her harpsichord under her teacher's watchful eye with great skill and accuracy; however, I noticed her face was full of emotion, which I couldn't explain. Suddenly, she burst into tears because she had been watching her beloved canary bird, which was in great distress and just fell dead from its perch."

5. At the same time many other catenated circles of action are going on in the person of our fair musician, as well as the motions of her fingers, such as the vital motions, respiration, the movements of her eyes and eyelids, and of the intricate muscles of vocality, according with the fifth preceding article.

5. Meanwhile, many other connected actions are happening within our talented musician, alongside the movements of her fingers, including essential functions like breathing, the movements of her eyes and eyelids, and the complex muscles involved in vocalization, as described in the fifth previous article.

6. If by any strong impression on the mind of our fair musician she should be interrupted for a very inconsiderable time, she can still continue her performance, according to the sixth article.

6. If our talented musician is briefly interrupted by any strong thought, she can still carry on with her performance, as stated in the sixth article.

7. If however this interruption be greater, though the chain of actions be not dissevered, it proceeds confusedly, and our young performer continues indeed to play, but in a hurry without accuracy and elegance, till she begins the tune again, according to the seventh of the preceding articles.

7. However, if this interruption is more significant, the flow of actions may not be completely broken, but it becomes disorganized. Our young performer may still play, but she does so in a rush, lacking precision and grace, until she restarts the tune, based on the seventh point of the previous articles.

8. But if this interruption be still greater, the circle of actions becomes entirely dissevered, and she finds herself immediately under the necessity to begin over again to recover the lost catenation, according to the eighth preceding article.

8. But if this interruption is even more significant, the sequence of actions breaks apart completely, and she must start over to regain the lost connections, as stated in the eighth previous article.

9. Or in trying to recover it she will sing some dissonant notes, or strike some improper keys, according to the ninth preceding article.

9. Or while trying to get it back, she will sing some off-key notes or hit some wrong keys, as stated in the ninth preceding article.

10. A very remarkable thing attends this breach of catenation, if the performer has forgotten some word of her song, the more energy of mind she uses about it, the more distant is she from regaining it; and artfully employs her mind in part on some other object, or endeavours to dull its perceptions, continuing to repeat, as it were inconsciously, the former part of the song, that she remembers, in hopes to regain the lost connexion.

10. A really interesting thing happens when this chain of thoughts is broken: if the performer forgets a word from her song, the harder she tries to remember it, the further away she seems from actually getting it back. Instead, she skillfully shifts her focus to something else or tries to dull her thoughts, continuing to repeat, almost unconsciously, the part of the song she does remember, hoping to reconnect the lost part.

For if the activity of the mind itself be more energetic, or takes its attention more, than the connecting word, which is wanted; it will not perceive the slighter link of this lost word; as who listens to a feeble sound, must be very silent and motionless; so that in this case the very vigour of the mind itself seems to prevent it from regaining the lost catenation, as well as the too great exertion in endeavouring to regain it, according to the tenth preceding article.

For if the mind's activity is stronger or demands more focus than the connecting word that's needed, it won't notice the weaker connection of that missing word. Just like someone who listens for a faint sound has to be completely quiet and still, in this situation, the very intensity of the mind seems to hinder it from recovering the lost connection, as well as the excessive effort put into trying to get it back, as stated in the previous tenth article.

We frequently experience, when we are doubtful about the spelling of a word, that the greater voluntary exertion we use, that is the more intensely we think about it, the further are we from regaining the lost association between the letters of it, but which readily recurs when we have become careless about it. In the same manner, after having for an hour laboured to recollect the name of some absent person, it shall seem, particularly after sleep, to come into the mind as it were spontaneously; that is the word we are in search of, was joined to the preceding one by association; this association being dissevered, we endeavour to recover it by volition; this very action of the mind strikes our attention more, than the faint link of association, and we find it impossible by this means to retrieve the lost word. After sleep, when volition is entirely suspended, the mind becomes capable of perceiving the fainter link of association, and the word is regained.

We often find that when we're unsure about how to spell a word, the more we try to remember it and think hard about it, the harder it is to get back to the connection between the letters. Ironically, it often comes back to us when we stop stressing over it. Similarly, after spending an hour trying to remember the name of someone who isn't there, it can suddenly pop into our minds, especially after we've slept. The word we were searching for was linked to a previous one by association; when that connection is broken, we try to recall it deliberately. This mental effort draws our attention away from the faint association, making it tough to recover the word. However, after sleep, when we stop forcing it, our mind can pick up on that weaker link of association, allowing us to find the word again.

On this circumstance depends the impediment of speech before mentioned; the first syllable of a word is causable by volition, but the remainder of it is in common conversation introduced by its associations with this first syllable acquired by long habit. Hence when the mind of the stammerer is vehemently employed on some idea of ambition of shining, or fear of not succeeding, the associations of the motions of the muscles of articulation with each other become dissevered by this greater exertion, and he endeavours in vain by voluntary efforts to rejoin the broken association. For this purpose he continues to repeat the first syllable, which is causable by volition, and strives in vain, by various distortions of countenance, to produce the next links, which are subject to association. See Class IV. 3. 1. 1.

The difficulty in speaking mentioned earlier depends on this situation; the first syllable of a word can be controlled by willpower, but the rest of it comes out in regular conversation because of its connections with that first syllable formed through long-term habits. Therefore, when a stammerer’s mind is intensely focused on some ambition to impress or on the fear of failing, the connections between the movements of the speech muscles get disrupted by this added stress, and they struggle in vain to reconnect those broken associations through sheer will. To try to fix this, they keep repeating the first syllable, which they can control, and they attempt various facial contortions to produce the next parts, which rely on those associations. See Class IV. 3. 1. 1.

11. After our accomplished musician has acquired great variety of tunes and songs, so that some of them begin to cease to be easily recollected, she finds progressive trains of musical notes more frequently forgotten, than those which are composed of reiterated circles, according with the eleventh preceding article.

11. After our skilled musician has gathered a wide range of tunes and songs, causing some to become harder to remember, she finds that more complex musical sequences are often forgotten compared to those that consist of repeated patterns, as stated in the eleventh preceding article.

12. To finish our example with the preceding articles we must at length suppose, that our fair performer falls asleep over her harpsichord; and thus by the suspension of volition, and the exclusion of external stimuli, she dissevers the trains and circles of her musical exertions.

12. To wrap up our example with the previous articles, we must finally imagine that our talented performer dozes off at her harpsichord. In doing so, by stopping her decision-making and blocking out outside distractions, she disconnects from the flow and patterns of her musical efforts.

III. 1. Many of these circumstances of catenations of motions receive an easy explanation from the four following consequences to the seventh law of animal causation in Sect. IV. These are, first, that those successions or combinations of animal motions, whether they were united by causation, association, or catenation, which have been most frequently repeated, acquire the strongest connection. Secondly, that of these, those, which have been less frequently mixed with other trains or tribes of motion, have the strongest connection. Thirdly, that of these, those, which were first formed, have the strongest connection. Fourthly, that if an animal motion be excited by more than one causation, association, or catenation, at the same time, it will be performed with greater energy.

III. 1. Many of these circumstances of catenations of motions receive an easy explanation from the four following consequences to the seventh law of animal causation in Sect. IV. These are, first, that those successions or combinations of animal motions, whether they were united by causation, association, or catenation, which have been most frequently repeated, acquire the strongest connection. Secondly, that of these, those, which have been less frequently mixed with other trains or tribes of motion, have the strongest connection. Thirdly, that of these, those, which were first formed, have the strongest connection. Fourthly, that if an animal motion be excited by more than one causation, association, or catenation, at the same time, it will be performed with greater energy.

2. Hence also we understand, why the catenations of irritative motions are more strongly connected than those of the other classes, where the quantity of unmixed repetition has been equal; because they were first formed. Such are those of the secerning and absorbent systems of vessels, where the action of the gland produces a fluid, which stimulates the mouths of its correspondent absorbents. The associated motions seem to be the next most strongly united, from their frequent repetition; and where both these circumstances unite, as in the vital motions, their catenations are indissoluble but by the destruction of the animal.

2. This is why we understand that the connections of irritative motions are stronger than those of other types, when the amount of pure repetition is the same; because they were formed first. This includes the systems of vessels that secrete and absorb, where the action of the gland creates a fluid that stimulates the openings of its related absorbents. The associated motions seem to be the next most tightly connected, due to their frequent repetition; and where both of these factors come together, as seen in vital motions, their connections can only be broken by the destruction of the organism.

3. Where a new link has been introduced into a circle of actions by some accidental defect of stimulus; if that defect of stimulus be repeated at the same part of the circle a second or a third time, the defective motions thus produced, both by the repeated defect of stimulus and by their catenation with the parts of the circle of actions, will be performed with less and less energy. Thus if any person is exposed to cold at a certain hour to-day, so long as to render some part of the system for a time torpid; and is again exposed to it at the same hour to-morrow, and the next day; he will be more and more affected by it, till at length a cold fit of fever is completely formed, as happens at the beginning of many of those fevers, which are called nervous or low fevers. Where the patient has slight periodical shiverings and paleness for many days before the febrile paroxysm is completely formed.

3. When a new link is created in a series of actions due to an accidental defect in stimulus; if that defect is repeated in the same part of the series a second or third time, the resulting defective motions, caused by the repeated defect in stimulus and their connection with the parts of the action series, will be executed with less and less energy. For example, if someone is exposed to cold at a certain time today, making part of their system temporarily numb; and then they face the same cold exposure at the same time tomorrow and the next day, they will become increasingly affected by it, until eventually, a cold fit of fever fully develops, which often occurs at the start of many fevers known as nervous or low fevers. In these cases, the patient experiences mild periodic shivering and paleness for several days before the fever episode is completely established.

4. On the contrary, if the exposure to cold be for so short a time, as not to induce any considerable degree of torpor or quiescence, and is repeated daily as above mentioned, it loses its effect more and more at every repetition, till the constitution can bear it without inconvenience, or indeed without being conscious of it. As in walking into the cold air in frosty weather. The same rule is applicable to increased stimulus, as of heat, or of vinous spirit, within certain limits, as is applied in the two last paragraphs to Deficient Stimulus; as is further explained in Sect. XXXVI. on the Periods of Diseases.

4. On the contrary, if the exposure to cold be for so short a time, as not to induce any considerable degree of torpor or quiescence, and is repeated daily as above mentioned, it loses its effect more and more at every repetition, till the constitution can bear it without inconvenience, or indeed without being conscious of it. As in walking into the cold air in frosty weather. The same rule is applicable to increased stimulus, as of heat, or of vinous spirit, within certain limits, as is applied in the two last paragraphs to Deficient Stimulus; as is further explained in Sect. XXXVI. on the Periods of Diseases.

5. Where irritation coincides with sensation to produce the same catenations of motion, as in inflammatory fevers, they are excited with still greater energy than by the irritation alone. So when children expect to be tickled in play, by a feather lightly passed over the lips, or by gently vellicating the soles of their feet, laughter is most vehemently excited; though they can stimulate these parts with their own fingers unmoved. Here the pleasureable idea of playfulness coincides with the vellication; and there is no voluntary exertion used to diminish the sensation, as there would be, if a child should endeavour to tickle himself. See Sect. XXXIV. 1. 4.

5. Where irritation coincides with sensation to produce the same catenations of motion, as in inflammatory fevers, they are excited with still greater energy than by the irritation alone. So when children expect to be tickled in play, by a feather lightly passed over the lips, or by gently vellicating the soles of their feet, laughter is most vehemently excited; though they can stimulate these parts with their own fingers unmoved. Here the pleasureable idea of playfulness coincides with the vellication; and there is no voluntary exertion used to diminish the sensation, as there would be, if a child should endeavour to tickle himself. See Sect. XXXIV. 1. 4.

6. And lastly, the motions excited by the junction of voluntary exertion with irritation are performed with more energy, than those by irritation singly; as when we listen to small noises, as to the ticking of a watch in the night, we perceive the most weak sounds, that are at other times unheeded. So when we attend to the irritative ideas of sound in our ears, which are generally not attended to, we can hear them; and can see the spectra of objects, which remain in the eye, whenever we please to exert our voluntary power in aid of those weak actions of the retina, or of the auditory nerve.

6. Finally, when we combine our efforts with irritation, we experience more robust reactions than we do with irritation alone. For example, when we listen to soft sounds, like a watch ticking at night, we become aware of faint noises that usually go unnoticed. Similarly, when we focus on the irritating sounds in our ears that we typically ignore, we can actually hear them, and we can also visualize the afterimages of objects that linger in our sight whenever we choose to consciously engage our will to support those subtle responses of the retina or the auditory nerve.

7. The temporary catenations of ideas, which are caused by the sensations of pleasure or pain, are easily dissevered either by irritations, as when a sudden noise disturbs a day-dream; or by the power of volition, as when we awake from sleep. Hence in our waking hours, whenever an idea occurs, which is incongruous to our former experience, we instantly dissever the train of imagination by the power of volition, and compare the incongruous idea with our previous knowledge of nature, and reject it. This operation of the mind has not yet acquired a specific name, though it is exerted every minute of our waking hours; unless it may be termed INTUITIVE ANALOGY. It is an act of reasoning of which we are unconscious except from its effects in preserving the congruity of our ideas, and bears the same relation to the sensorial power of volition, that irritative ideas, of which we are inconscious except by their effects, do to the sensorial power of irritation; as the former is produced by volition without our attention to it, and the latter by irritation without our attention to them.

7. The temporary connections of ideas, which are triggered by feelings of pleasure or pain, can easily be broken by distractions, like when a sudden noise interrupts a daydream, or by our willpower, as when we wake up from sleep. So, in our waking life, whenever we come across an idea that doesn’t fit with our previous experiences, we quickly break the train of thought using our will, comparing the out-of-place idea with what we already know about the world, and dismiss it. This mental process hasn’t been given a specific name yet, even though it happens every minute while we’re awake; unless we call it INTUITIVE ANALOGY. It’s a reasoning process that we aren’t aware of unless we notice its impact on keeping our ideas consistent, and it relates to the sensory power of will in the same way that distracting ideas, which we are unaware of except by their effects, relate to the sensory power of distraction; the former is triggered by our will without us focusing on it, and the latter by distraction without us being aware of them.

If on the other hand a train of imagination or of voluntary ideas are excited with great energy, and passing on with great vivacity, and become dissevered by some violent stimulus, as the discharge of a pistol near one's ear, another circumstance takes place, which is termed SURPRISE; which by exciting violent irritation, and violent sensation, employs for a time the whole sensorial energy, and thus dissevers the passing trains of ideas, before the power of volition has time to compare them with the usual phenomena of nature. In this case fear is generally the companion of surprise, and adds to our embarrassment, as every one experiences in some degree when he hears a noise in the dark, which he cannot instantly account for. This catenation of fear with surprise is owing to our perpetual experience of injuries from external bodies in motion, unless we are upon our guard against them. See Sect. XVIII. 17. XIX. 2.

If on the other hand a train of imagination or of voluntary ideas are excited with great energy, and passing on with great vivacity, and become dissevered by some violent stimulus, as the discharge of a pistol near one's ear, another circumstance takes place, which is termed Surprise!; which by exciting violent irritation, and violent sensation, employs for a time the whole sensorial energy, and thus dissevers the passing trains of ideas, before the power of volition has time to compare them with the usual phenomena of nature. In this case fear is generally the companion of surprise, and adds to our embarrassment, as every one experiences in some degree when he hears a noise in the dark, which he cannot instantly account for. This catenation of fear with surprise is owing to our perpetual experience of injuries from external bodies in motion, unless we are upon our guard against them. See Sect. XVIII. 17. XIX. 2.

Many other examples of the catenations of animal motions are explained in Sect. XXXVI. on the Periods of Diseases.

Many other examples of the catenations of animal motions are explained in Sect. XXXVI. on the Periods of Diseases.



SECT. XVIII.

OF SLEEP.

SLEEP.

1. Volition is suspended in sleep. 2. Sensation continues. Dreams prevent delirium and inflammation. 3. Nightmare. 4. Ceaseless flow of ideas in dreams. 5. We seem to receive them by the senses. Optic nerve perfectly sensible in sleep. Eyes less dazzled after dreaming of visible objects. 6. Reverie, belief. 7. How we distinguish ideas from perceptions. 8. Variety of scenery in dreams, excellence of the sense of vision. 9. Novelty of combination in dreams. 10. Distinctness of imagery in dreams. 11. Rapidity of transaction in dreams. 12. Of measuring time. Of dramatic time and place. Why a dull play induces sleep, and an interesting one reverie. 13. Consciousness of our existence and identity in dreams. 14. How we awake sometimes suddenly, sometimes frequently. 15. Irritative motions continue in sleep, internal irritations are succeeded by sensation. Sensibility increases during sleep, and irritability. Morning dreams. Why epilepsies occur in sleep. Ecstacy of children. Case of convulsions in sleep. Cramp, why painful. Asthma. Morning sweats. Increase of heat. Increase of urine in sleep. Why more liable to take cold in sleep. Catarrh from thin night-caps. Why we feel chilly at the approach of sleep, and at waking in the open air. 16. Why the gout commences in sleep. Secretions are more copious in sleep, young animals and plants grow more in sleep. 17. Inconsistency of dreams. Absence of surprise in dreams. 18. Why we forget some dreams and not others. 19. Sleep-talkers awake with surprise. 20. Remote causes of sleep. Atmosphere with less oxygene. Compression of the brain in spina bifida. By whirling on an horizontal wheel. By cold. 21. Definition of sleep.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Willpower is paused during sleep. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Sensations persist. Dreams stop confusion and irritation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Nightmare. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Endless stream of thoughts in dreams. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. It feels like we receive them through our senses. The optic nerve is fully responsive in sleep. Our eyes are less overwhelmed after dreaming of visible things. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Daydreaming, belief. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. How we differentiate ideas from perceptions. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Diverse landscapes in dreams, clarity of the sense of sight. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Original combinations in dreams. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Clarity of imagery in dreams. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Speed of events in dreams. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. On measuring time. On dramatic timing and setting. Why a boring play makes us sleepy, while an engaging one induces daydreaming. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. Awareness of our existence and identity in dreams. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. Why we sometimes wake up suddenly, and other times frequently. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. Reflex movements continue in sleep, internal stimuli lead to sensations. Sensitivity rises during sleep, as does irritability. Morning dreams. Why epileptic episodes happen during sleep. Children's ecstasy. Instances of convulsions in sleep. Why cramps are painful. Asthma. Morning sweating. Increased body temperature. Increased urination during sleep. Why we’re more prone to catch a cold while sleeping. Nasal congestion from thin nightcaps. Why we feel cold as we drift into sleep, and when waking outdoors. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. Why gout begins in sleep. Bodily secretions are more abundant during sleep; young animals and plants grow more in their sleep. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. Inconsistencies in dreams. Lack of surprise in dreams. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. Why we remember some dreams but not others. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_18__. People who talk in their sleep wake up surprised. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_19__. Underlying causes of sleep. An atmosphere with less oxygen. Pressure on the brain from spina bifida. Spinning on a horizontal wheel. Exposure to cold. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_20__. Definition of sleep.

1. There are four situations of our system, which in their moderate degrees are not usually termed diseases, and yet abound with many very curious and instructive phenomena; these are sleep, reverie, vertigo, drunkenness. These we shall previously consider, before we step forwards to develop the causes and cures of diseases with the modes of the operation of medicines.

1. There are four conditions in our system that, while not typically classified as diseases in their milder forms, still present many fascinating and educational phenomena. These conditions are sleep, daydreaming, dizziness, and intoxication. We will examine these first before moving on to discuss the causes and treatments of diseases and how medicines work.

As all those trains and tribes of animal motion, which are subjected to volition, were the last that were caused, their connection is weaker than that of the other classes; and there is a peculiar circumstance attending this causation, which is, that it is entirely suspended during sleep; whilst the other classes of motion, which are more immediately necessary to life, as those caused by internal stimuli, for instance the pulsations of the heart and arteries, or those catenated with pleasurable sensation, as the powers of digestion, continue to strengthen their habits without interruption. Thus though man in his sleeping state is a much less perfect animal, than in his waking hours; and though he consumes more than one third of his life in this his irrational situation; yet is the wisdom of the Author of nature manifest even in this seeming imperfection of his work!

As all those trains and groups of animal movement, which are controlled by will, were the last to be created, their connection is weaker than that of the other classes; and there is a unique circumstance regarding this causation, which is that it is completely paused during sleep; while the other classes of motion, which are more directly essential to life, like those driven by internal stimuli, for instance, the beating of the heart and arteries, or those linked to pleasurable sensations, like the digestive processes, keep strengthening their patterns without interruption. Thus, although a person in their sleeping state is a far less developed being than during their waking hours; and though they spend over a third of their life in this irrational state; the wisdom of the Creator of nature is evident even in this apparent flaw in His design!

The truth of this assertion with respect to the large muscles of the body, which are concerned in locomotion, is evident; as no one in perfect sanity walks about in his sleep, or performs any domestic offices: and in respect to the mind, we never exercise our reason or recollection in dreams; we may sometimes seem distracted between contending passions, but we never compare their objects, or deliberate about the acquisition of those objects, if our sleep is perfect. And though many synchronous tribes or successive trains of ideas may represent the houses or walks, which have real existence, yet are they here introduced by their connection with our sensations, and are in truth ideas of imagination, not of recollection.

The truth of this statement regarding the large muscles of the body that are involved in movement is clear; no one in their right mind walks around in their sleep or does any household chores. As for the mind, we don’t use our reasoning or memory while dreaming; we might sometimes feel torn between conflicting emotions, but we never analyze their subjects or think about acquiring those subjects if our sleep is deep. Even though many related thoughts or sequences of ideas may reflect places or activities that really exist, they are connected to our sensations and are actually products of imagination, not memory.

2. For our sensations of pleasure and pain are experienced with great vivacity in our dreams; and hence all that motley group of ideas, which are caused by them, called the ideas of imagination, with their various associated trains, are in a very vivid manner acted over in the sensorium; and these sometimes call into action the larger muscles, which have been much associated with them; as appears from the muttering sentences, which some people utter in their dreams, and from the obscure barking of sleeping dogs, and the motions of their feet and nostrils.

2. Our feelings of pleasure and pain are intensely felt in our dreams; therefore, all those mixed ideas that arise from them, referred to as the ideas of imagination, along with their various related thoughts, are vividly replayed in the mind; these can sometimes trigger movement in the larger muscles that are closely linked to them, as shown by the murmured sentences some people say in their sleep, the faint barking of dogs, and the movements of their feet and noses.

This perpetual flow of the trains of ideas, which constitute our dreams, and which are caused by painful or pleasurable sensation, might at first view be conceived to be an useless expenditure of sensorial power. But it has been shewn, that those motions, which are perpetually excited, as those of the arterial system by the stimulus of the blood, are attended by a great accumulation of sensorial power, after they have been for a time suspended; as the hot-fit of fever is the consequence of the cold one. Now as these trains of ideas caused by sensation are perpetually excited during our waking hours, if they were to be suspended in sleep like the voluntary motions, (which are exerted only by intervals during our waking hours,) an accumulation of sensorial power would follow; and on our awaking a delirium would supervene, since these ideas caused by sensation would be produced with such energy, that we should mistake the trains of imagination for ideas excited by irritation; as perpetually happens to people debilitated by fevers on their first awaking; for in these fevers with debility the general quantity of irritation being diminished, that of sensation is increased. In like manner if the actions of the stomach, intestines, and various glands, which are perhaps in part at least caused by or catenated with agreeable sensation, and which perpetually exist during our waking hours, were like the voluntary motions suspended in our sleep; the great accumulation of sensorial power, which would necessarily follow, would be liable to excite inflammation in them.

This constant flow of thoughts that make up our dreams, driven by both painful and pleasurable sensations, might initially seem like a waste of mental energy. However, it's been shown that those actions that are consistently triggered, like the movements of the arterial system due to blood flow, accumulate significant mental energy after being paused for a while, much like a fever's hot phase follows its cold phase. Since these thought patterns, prompted by sensations, are continuously active while we’re awake, if they were to be halted in sleep like voluntary movements, which only occur occasionally when awake, a buildup of mental energy would occur. Upon waking, this could lead to delirium because these sensation-driven thoughts would emerge with such force that we might confuse our imaginative thoughts with those triggered by irritation, as often happens to people weakened by fevers when they first wake up. In such fevers, the general level of irritation is reduced while sensation intensifies. Similarly, if the actions of the stomach, intestines, and various glands—which are possibly linked to pleasurable sensations—were to be paused during sleep like voluntary movements, the significant accumulation of mental energy could lead to inflammation in these areas.

3. When by our continued posture in sleep, some uneasy sensations are produced, we either gradually awake by the exertion of volition, or the muscles connected by habit with such sensations alter the position of the body; but where the sleep is uncommonly profound, and those uneasy sensations great, the disease called the incubus, or nightmare, is produced. Here the desire of moving the body is painfully exerted, by the power of moving it, or volition, is incapable of action, till we awake. Many less disagreeable struggles in our dreams, as when we wish in vain to fly from terrifying objects, constitute a slighter degree of this disease. In awaking from the nightmare I have more than once observed, that there was no disorder in my pulse; nor do I believe the respiration is laborious, as some have affirmed. It occurs to people whose sleep is too profound, and some disagreeable sensation exists, which at other times would have awakened them, and have thence prevented the disease of nightmare; as after great fatigue or hunger with too large a supper and wine, which occasion our sleep to be uncommonly profound. See No. 14, of this Section.

3. When by our continued posture in sleep, some uneasy sensations are produced, we either gradually awake by the exertion of volition, or the muscles connected by habit with such sensations alter the position of the body; but where the sleep is uncommonly profound, and those uneasy sensations great, the disease called the incubus, or nightmare, is produced. Here the desire of moving the body is painfully exerted, by the power of moving it, or volition, is incapable of action, till we awake. Many less disagreeable struggles in our dreams, as when we wish in vain to fly from terrifying objects, constitute a slighter degree of this disease. In awaking from the nightmare I have more than once observed, that there was no disorder in my pulse; nor do I believe the respiration is laborious, as some have affirmed. It occurs to people whose sleep is too profound, and some disagreeable sensation exists, which at other times would have awakened them, and have thence prevented the disease of nightmare; as after great fatigue or hunger with too large a supper and wine, which occasion our sleep to be uncommonly profound. See No. 14, of this Section.

4. As the larger muscles of the body are much more frequently excited by volition than by sensation, they are but seldom brought into action in our sleep: but the ideas of the mind are by habit much more frequently connected with sensation than with volition; and hence the ceaseless flow of our ideas in dreams. Every one's experience will teach him this truth, for we all daily exert much voluntary muscular motion: but few of mankind can bear the fatigue of much voluntary thinking.

4. Since the larger muscles in our bodies are usually activated by conscious effort rather than by feeling, they rarely get used while we’re asleep. On the other hand, our thoughts are often more linked to sensations than to conscious choices, which explains the constant stream of ideas we experience in dreams. Everyone knows this from personal experience; we all engage in a lot of voluntary physical movement every day, but only a few people can handle the strain of heavy, voluntary thinking.

5. A very curious circumstance attending these our sleeping imaginations is, that we seem to receive them by the senses. The muscles, which are subservient to the external organs of sense, are connected with volition, and cease to act in sleep; hence the eyelids are closed, and the tympanum of the ear relaxed; and it is probable a similarity of voluntary exertion may be necessary for the perceptions of the other nerves of sense; for it is observed that the papillæ of the tongue can be seen to become erected, when we attempt to taste any thing extremely grateful. Hewson Exper. Enquir. V. 2. 186. Albini Annot. Acad. L. i. c. 15. Add to this, that the immediate organs of sense have no objects to excite them in the darkness and silence of the night, but their nerves of sense nevertheless continue to possess their perfect activity subservient to all their numerous sensitive connections. This vivacity of our nerves of sense during the time of sleep is evinced by a circumstance, which almost every one must at some time or other have experienced; that is, if we sleep in the daylight, and endeavour to see some object in our dream, the light is exceedingly painful to our eyes; and after repeated struggles we lament in our sleep, that we cannot see it. In this case I apprehend the eyelid is in some degree opened by the vehemence of our sensations; and, the iris being dilated, the optic nerve shews as great or greater sensibility than in our waking hours. See No. 15. of this Section.

5. A very curious circumstance attending these our sleeping imaginations is, that we seem to receive them by the senses. The muscles, which are subservient to the external organs of sense, are connected with volition, and cease to act in sleep; hence the eyelids are closed, and the tympanum of the ear relaxed; and it is probable a similarity of voluntary exertion may be necessary for the perceptions of the other nerves of sense; for it is observed that the papillæ of the tongue can be seen to become erected, when we attempt to taste any thing extremely grateful. Hewson Exper. Enquir. V. 2. 186. Albini Annot. Acad. L. i. c. 15. Add to this, that the immediate organs of sense have no objects to excite them in the darkness and silence of the night, but their nerves of sense nevertheless continue to possess their perfect activity subservient to all their numerous sensitive connections. This vivacity of our nerves of sense during the time of sleep is evinced by a circumstance, which almost every one must at some time or other have experienced; that is, if we sleep in the daylight, and endeavour to see some object in our dream, the light is exceedingly painful to our eyes; and after repeated struggles we lament in our sleep, that we cannot see it. In this case I apprehend the eyelid is in some degree opened by the vehemence of our sensations; and, the iris being dilated, the optic nerve shews as great or greater sensibility than in our waking hours. See No. 15. of this Section.

When we are forcibly waked at midnight from profound sleep, our eyes are much dazzled with the light of the candle for a minute or two, after there has been sufficient time allowed for the contraction of the iris; which is owing to the accumulation of sensorial power in the organ of vision during its state of less activity. But when we have dreamt much of visible objects, this accumulation of sensorial power in the organ of vision is lessened or prevented, and we awake in the morning without being dazzled with the light, after the iris has had time to contract itself. This is a matter of great curiosity, and may be thus tried by any one in the day-light. Close your eyes, and cover them with your hat; think for a minute on a tune, which you are accustomed to, and endeavour to sing it with as little activity of mind as possible. Suddenly uncover and open your eyes, and in one second of time the iris will contract itself, but you will perceive the day more luminous for several seconds, owing to the accumulation of sensorial power in the optic nerve.

When we’re suddenly woken up at midnight from deep sleep, our eyes are really dazzled by the candlelight for a minute or two, even after our irises have contracted. This happens because the visual system has built up sensory power during its more inactive state. But if we’ve spent a lot of time dreaming about things we can see, that sensory buildup is reduced or avoided, so when we wake up in the morning, we’re not blinded by the light after our irises have had a chance to adjust. This is pretty interesting, and anyone can test it during the day. Close your eyes and cover them with your hat; think for a minute about a tune you know well and try to sing it without overthinking. Then suddenly take off the hat and open your eyes, and in just a second, your iris will contract, but you’ll notice that the daylight feels much brighter for several seconds, due to the buildup of sensory power in the optic nerve.

Then again close and cover your eyes, and think intensely on a cube of ivory two inches diameter, attending first to the north and south sides of it, and then to the other four sides of it; then get a clear image in your mind's eye of all the sides of the same cube coloured red; and then of it coloured green; and then of it coloured blue; lastly, open your eyes as in the former experiment, and after the first second of time allowed for the contraction of the iris, you will not perceive any increase of the light of the day, or dazzling; because now there is no accumulation of sensorial power in the optic nerve; that having been expended by its action in thinking over visible objects.

Then close your eyes again and focus intensely on a cube of ivory that's two inches in diameter. Start by concentrating on the north and south sides, then move on to the other four sides. Next, create a clear image in your mind of all the sides of the cube colored red, then green, and finally blue. Afterward, open your eyes just like in the previous experiment. After a moment to let your pupils adjust, you won’t notice any increase in daylight or glare because there's no buildup of sensory power in your optic nerve; that’s been used up by considering visible objects.

This experiment is not easy to be made at first, but by a few patient trials the fact appears very certain; and shews clearly, that our ideas of imagination are repetitions of the motions of the nerve, which were originally occasioned by the stimulus of external bodies; because they equally expend the sensorial power in the organ of sense. See Sect. III. 4. which is analogous to our being as much fatigued by thinking as by labour.

This experiment is not easy to be made at first, but by a few patient trials the fact appears very certain; and shews clearly, that our ideas of imagination are repetitions of the motions of the nerve, which were originally occasioned by the stimulus of external bodies; because they equally expend the sensorial power in the organ of sense. See Sect. III. 4. which is analogous to our being as much fatigued by thinking as by labour.

6. Nor is it in our dreams alone, but even in our waking reveries, and in great efforts of invention, so great is the vivacity of our ideas, that we do not for a time distinguish them from the real presence of substantial objects; though the external organs of sense are open, and surrounded with their usual stimuli. Thus whilst I am thinking over the beautiful valley, through which I yesterday travelled, I do not perceive the furniture of my room: and there are some, whose waking imaginations are so apt to run into perfect reverie, that in their common attention to a favourite idea they do not hear the voice of the companion, who accosts them, unless it is repeated with unusual energy.

6. It's not just in our dreams, but also in our waking thoughts and moments of great creativity that our ideas are so vivid that we often can't tell them apart from the actual presence of real things, even though our senses are wide awake and surrounded by their usual stimuli. For example, while I'm reflecting on the beautiful valley I traveled through yesterday, I don't notice the furniture in my room. Some people get so caught up in their thoughts that they completely zone out, and they won't even hear a friend trying to get their attention unless it's said with a lot of energy.

This perpetual mistake in dreams and reveries, where our ideas of imagination are attended with a belief of the presence of external objects, evinces beyond a doubt, that all our ideas are repetitions of the motions of the nerves of sense, by which they were acquired; and that this belief is not, as some late philosophers contend, an instinct necessarily connected only with our perceptions.

This constant error in dreams and daydreams, where our imaginative thoughts come with a belief that external objects are present, clearly shows that all our ideas are just repetitions of the sensory nerve movements through which we got them; and that this belief is not, as some recent philosophers argue, an instinct that’s only tied to our perceptions.

7. A curious question demands our attention in this place; as we do not distinguish in our dreams and reveries between our perceptions of external objects, and our ideas of them in their absence, how do we distinguish them at any time? In a dream, if the sweetness of sugar occurs to my imagination, the whiteness and hardness of it, which were ideas usually connected with the sweetness, immediately follow in the train; and I believe a material lump of sugar present before my senses: but in my waking hours, if the sweetness occurs to my imagination, the stimulus of the table to my hand, or of the window to my eye, prevents the other ideas of the hardness and whiteness of the sugar from succeeding; and hence I perceive the fallacy, and disbelieve the existence of objects correspondent to those ideas, whose tribes or trains are broken by the stimulus of other objects. And further in our waking hours, we frequently exert our volition in comparing present appearances with such, as we have usually observed; and thus correct the errors of one sense by our general knowledge of nature by intuitive analogy. See Sect. XVII. 3. 7. Whereas in dreams the power of volition is suspended, we can recollect and compare our present ideas with none of our acquired knowledge, and are hence incapable of observing any absurdities in them.

7. A curious question demands our attention in this place; as we do not distinguish in our dreams and reveries between our perceptions of external objects, and our ideas of them in their absence, how do we distinguish them at any time? In a dream, if the sweetness of sugar occurs to my imagination, the whiteness and hardness of it, which were ideas usually connected with the sweetness, immediately follow in the train; and I believe a material lump of sugar present before my senses: but in my waking hours, if the sweetness occurs to my imagination, the stimulus of the table to my hand, or of the window to my eye, prevents the other ideas of the hardness and whiteness of the sugar from succeeding; and hence I perceive the fallacy, and disbelieve the existence of objects correspondent to those ideas, whose tribes or trains are broken by the stimulus of other objects. And further in our waking hours, we frequently exert our volition in comparing present appearances with such, as we have usually observed; and thus correct the errors of one sense by our general knowledge of nature by intuitive analogy. See Sect. XVII. 3. 7. Whereas in dreams the power of volition is suspended, we can recollect and compare our present ideas with none of our acquired knowledge, and are hence incapable of observing any absurdities in them.

By this criterion we distinguish our waking from our sleeping hours, we can voluntarily recollect our sleeping ideas, when we are awake, and compare them with our waking ones; but we cannot in our sleep voluntarily recollect our waking ideas at all.

By this standard, we can tell our waking hours apart from our sleeping hours. We can intentionally remember our sleeping thoughts when we are awake and compare them with our waking thoughts; however, we cannot intentionally recall our waking thoughts while we are asleep at all.

8. The vast variety of scenery, novelty of combination, and distinctness of imagery, are other curious circumstances of our sleeping imaginations. The variety of scenery seems to arise from the superior activity and excellence of our sense of vision; which in an instant unfolds to the mind extensive fields of pleasurable ideas; while the other senses collect their objects slowly, and with little combination; add to this, that the ideas, which this organ presents us with, are more frequently connected with our sensation than those of any other.

8. The wide range of scenery, unique combinations, and clarity of images are interesting features of our dreaming minds. The variety of scenery appears to come from the heightened activity and quality of our sense of sight, which can instantly unfold vast landscapes of enjoyable thoughts. In contrast, our other senses gather information slowly and with less variety. Additionally, the ideas that our eyes provide are more often linked to our feelings than those from any other sense.

9. The great novelty of combination is owing to another circumstance; the trains of ideas, which are carried on in our waking thoughts, are in our dreams dissevered in a thousand places by the suspension of volition, and the absence of irritative ideas, and are hence perpetually falling into new catenations. As explained in Sect. XVII. 1. 9. For the power of volition is perpetually exerted during our waking hours in comparing our passing trains of ideas with our acquired knowledge of nature, and thus forms many intermediate links in their catenation. And the irritative ideas excited by the stimulus of the objects, with which we are surrounded, are every moment intruded upon us, and form other links of our unceasing catenations of ideas.

9. The great novelty of combination is owing to another circumstance; the trains of ideas, which are carried on in our waking thoughts, are in our dreams dissevered in a thousand places by the suspension of volition, and the absence of irritative ideas, and are hence perpetually falling into new catenations. As explained in Sect. XVII. 1. 9. For the power of volition is perpetually exerted during our waking hours in comparing our passing trains of ideas with our acquired knowledge of nature, and thus forms many intermediate links in their catenation. And the irritative ideas excited by the stimulus of the objects, with which we are surrounded, are every moment intruded upon us, and form other links of our unceasing catenations of ideas.

10. The absence of the stimuli of external bodies, and of volition, in our dreams renders the organs of sense liable to be more strongly affected by the powers of sensation, and of association. For our desires or aversions, or the obtrusions of surrounding bodies, dissever the sensitive and associate tribes of ideas in our waking hours by introducing those of irritation and volition amongst them. Hence proceeds the superior distinctness of pleasurable or painful imagery in our sleep; for we recal the figure and the features of a long lost friend, whom we loved, in our dreams with much more accuracy and vivacity than in our waking thoughts. This circumstance contributes to prove, that our ideas of imagination are reiterations of those motions of our organs of sense, which were excited by external objects; because while we are exposed to the stimuli of present objects, our ideas of absent objects cannot be so distinctly formed.

10. The lack of external stimuli and conscious choice in our dreams makes our senses more intensely influenced by feelings and associations. In our waking hours, our wants, dislikes, and the presence of nearby objects disrupt the sensitive and associative connections of ideas by mixing in irritation and willpower. This is why we experience clearer pleasurable or painful images in our sleep; we can recall the appearance and traits of a long-lost friend we loved in our dreams with much more detail and intensity than when we are awake. This situation supports the idea that our imaginary thoughts are repetitions of the sensory experiences triggered by external objects; because when we are affected by present stimuli, our thoughts about absent objects can't be formed as clearly.

11. The rapidity of the succession of transactions in our dreams is almost inconceivable; insomuch that, when we are accidentally awakened by the jarring of a door, which is opened into our bed-chamber, we sometimes dream a whole history of thieves or fire in the very instant of awaking.

11. The speed at which events happen in our dreams is nearly unbelievable; so much so that when we're suddenly woken by the noise of a door opening into our bedroom, we can sometimes have an entire story about thieves or a fire in that very moment of waking up.

During the suspension of volition we cannot compare our other ideas with those of the parts of time in which they exist; that is, we cannot compare the imaginary scene, which is before us, with those changes of it, which precede or follow it: because this act of comparing requires recollection or voluntary exertion. Whereas in our waking hours, we are perpetually making this comparison, and by that means our waking ideas are kept confident with each other by intuitive analogy; but this companion retards the succession of them, by occasioning their repetition. Add to this, that the transactions of our dreams consist chiefly of visible ideas, and that a whole history of thieves and fire may be beheld in an instant of time like the figures in a picture.

During the suspension of choice, we can't compare our other thoughts with the moments in which they exist; that is, we can't compare the imaginary scene in front of us with the changes that came before or after it: because this act of comparison requires memory or conscious effort. However, in our waking hours, we are constantly making this comparison, which keeps our waking thoughts aligned with each other through intuitive connections. But this comparison slows down the flow of our thoughts because it leads to their repetition. Furthermore, the events in our dreams mainly consist of visual ideas, and an entire story of thieves and fire can be seen in an instant, like the images in a painting.

12. From this incapacity of attending to the parts of time in our dreams, arises our ignorance of the length of the night; which, but from our constant experience to the contrary, we should conclude was but a few minutes, when our sleep is perfect. The same happens in our reveries; thus when we are possessed with vehement joy, grief, or anger, time appears short, for we exert no volition to compare the present scenery with the past or future; but when we are compelled to perform those exercises of mind or body, which, are unmixed with passion, as in travelling over a dreary country, time appears long; for our desire to finish our journey occasions us more frequently to compare our present situation with the parts of time or place, which are before and behind us.

12. Because we can't focus on the passing time in our dreams, we don't really know how long the night lasts; otherwise, based on our usual experience, we might think it only lasts a few minutes when we're sleeping soundly. The same goes for our daydreams; when we're consumed by strong emotions like joy, sadness, or anger, time seems to fly because we don't actively compare the present moment with the past or future. However, when we're forced to engage in tasks that don't involve strong feelings, like traveling through a dull landscape, time feels much longer because our eagerness to reach our destination makes us frequently compare where we are with where we've been and where we're going.

So when we are enveloped in deep contemplation of any kind, or in reverie, as in reading a very interesting play or romance, we measure time very inaccurately; and hence, if a play greatly affects our passions, the absurdities of passing over many days or years, and or perpetual changes of place, are not perceived by the audience; as is experienced by every one, who reads or sees some plays of the immortal Shakespear; but it is necessary for inferior authors to observe those rules of the πιθανον and πρεπον inculcated by Aristotle, because their works do not interest the passions sufficiently to produce complete reverie.

So when we get really lost in thought, or daydreaming, like when we're reading a captivating play or romance, we don't keep track of time very well. Because of this, if a play really stirs our emotions, we don’t notice the ridiculousness of skipping over many days or years, or the constant changes in location; anyone who reads or watches some of Shakespeare's timeless plays can relate to this. However, lesser authors need to follow the rules of the likely and πρεπον taught by Aristotle, because their work doesn't engage the emotions enough to create a full daydream.

Those works, however, whether a romance or a sermon, which do not interest us so much as to induce reverie, may nevertheless incline us to sleep. For those pleasurable ideas, which are presented to us, and are too gentle to excite laughter, (which is attended with interrupted voluntary exertions, as explained Sect. XXXIV. 1. 4.) and which are not accompanied with any other emotion, which usually excites some voluntary exertion, as anger, or fear, are liable to produce sleep; which consists in a suspension of all voluntary power. But if the ideas thus presented to us, and interest our attention, are accompanied with so much pleasurable or painful sensation as to excite our voluntary exertion at the same time, reverie is the consequence. Hence an interesting play produces reverie, a tedious one produces sleep: in the latter we become exhausted by attention, and are not excited to any voluntary exertion, and therefore sleep; in the former we are excited by some emotion, which prevents by its pain the suspension of volition, and in as much as it interests us, induces reverie, as explained in the next Section.

Those works, however, whether a romance or a sermon, which do not interest us so much as to induce reverie, may nevertheless incline us to sleep. For those pleasurable ideas, which are presented to us, and are too gentle to excite laughter, (which is attended with interrupted voluntary exertions, as explained Sect. XXXIV. 1. 4.) and which are not accompanied with any other emotion, which usually excites some voluntary exertion, as anger, or fear, are liable to produce sleep; which consists in a suspension of all voluntary power. But if the ideas thus presented to us, and interest our attention, are accompanied with so much pleasurable or painful sensation as to excite our voluntary exertion at the same time, reverie is the consequence. Hence an interesting play produces reverie, a tedious one produces sleep: in the latter we become exhausted by attention, and are not excited to any voluntary exertion, and therefore sleep; in the former we are excited by some emotion, which prevents by its pain the suspension of volition, and in as much as it interests us, induces reverie, as explained in the next Section.

But when our sleep is imperfect, as when we have determined to rise in half an hour, time appears longer to us than in most other situations. Here our solicitude not to oversleep the determined time induces us in this imperfect sleep to compare the quick changes of imagined scenery with the parts of time or place, they would have taken up, had they real exigence; and that more frequently than in our waking hours; and hence the time appears longer to us: and I make no doubt, but the permitted time appears long to a man going to the gallows, as the fear of its quick lapse will make him think frequently about it.

But when our sleep isn't great, like when we've decided to wake up in half an hour, time feels longer to us than in most other situations. Our worry about not oversleeping makes us mentally compare the fast changes of imagined scenes with how much time or space they would actually take up if they were real, and we do this more often than when we're awake. Because of this, time seems to stretch for us. I have no doubt that the time feels long for a man walking to the gallows, as the fear of it passing quickly makes him think about it constantly.

13. As we gain our knowledge of time by comparing the present scenery with the past and future, and of place by comparing the situations of objects with each other; so we gain our idea of consciousness by comparing ourselves with the scenery around us; and of identity by comparing our present consciousness with our past consciousness: as we never think of time or place, but when we make the companions above mentioned, so we never think of consciousness, but when we compare our own existence with that of other objects; nor of identity, but when we compare our present and our past consciousness. Hence the consciousness of our own existence, and of our identity, is owing to a voluntary exertion of our minds: and on that account in our complete dreams we neither measure time, are surprised at the sudden changes of place, nor attend to our own existence, or identity; because our power of volition is suspended. But all these circumstances are more or less observable in our incomplete ones; for then we attend a little to the lapse of time, and the changes of place, and to our own existence; and even to our identity of person; for a lady seldom dreams, that she is a soldier; nor a man, that he is brought to bed.

13. As we understand time by comparing our current surroundings with the past and the future, and space by looking at how objects relate to each other, we also form our idea of consciousness by comparing ourselves to the environment around us. Similarly, we grasp our sense of identity by reflecting on our current consciousness alongside our past consciousness. We only think about time or space when we make those comparisons; likewise, we only consider consciousness when we contrast our own existence with that of other objects, and we only think about identity when we compare our present consciousness with our past. Therefore, our awareness of our own existence and identity depends on a conscious effort of our minds. That’s why in vivid dreams, we don’t measure time, feel surprised by sudden changes in our surroundings, or pay attention to our existence or identity, because our ability to make choices is temporarily paused. However, in less vivid dreams, we do notice the passage of time, changes in location, our own existence, and even aspects of our identity; for instance, a woman rarely dreams that she is a soldier, nor does a man dream he is giving birth.

14. As long as our sensations only excite their sensual motions, or ideas, our sleep continues sound; but as soon as they excite desires or aversions, our sleep becomes imperfect; and when that desire or aversion is so strong, as to produce voluntary motions, we begin to awake; the larger muscles of the body are brought into action to remove that irritation or sensation, which a continued posture has caused; we stretch our limbs, and yawn, and our sleep is thus broken by the accumulation of voluntary power.

14. As long as our sensations only trigger their physical responses or thoughts, our sleep remains deep; but once they spark desires or dislikes, our sleep starts to falter; and when that desire or dislike becomes strong enough to cause us to move consciously, we begin to wake up; the larger muscles of our body start to activate to relieve that irritation or sensation caused by staying in one position for too long; we stretch our limbs, yawn, and our sleep is disrupted by the build-up of conscious effort.

Sometimes it happens, that the act of waking is suddenly produced, and this soon after the commencement of sleep; which is occasioned by some sensation so disagreeable, as instantaneously to excite the power of volition; and a temporary action of all the voluntary motions suddenly succeeds, and we start awake. This is sometimes accompanied with loud noise in the ears, and with some degree of fear; and when it is in great excess, so as to produce continued convulsive motions of those muscles, which are generally subservient to volition, it becomes epilepsy: the fits of which in some patients generally commence during sleep. This differs from the night-mare described in No. 3. of this Section, because in that the disagreeable sensation is not so great as to excite the power of volition into action; for as soon as that happens, the disease ceases.

Sometimes it happens, that the act of waking is suddenly produced, and this soon after the commencement of sleep; which is occasioned by some sensation so disagreeable, as instantaneously to excite the power of volition; and a temporary action of all the voluntary motions suddenly succeeds, and we start awake. This is sometimes accompanied with loud noise in the ears, and with some degree of fear; and when it is in great excess, so as to produce continued convulsive motions of those muscles, which are generally subservient to volition, it becomes epilepsy: the fits of which in some patients generally commence during sleep. This differs from the night-mare described in No. 3. of this Section, because in that the disagreeable sensation is not so great as to excite the power of volition into action; for as soon as that happens, the disease ceases.

Another circumstance, which sometimes awakes people soon after the commencement of their sleep, is where the voluntary power is already so great in quantity as almost to prevent them from falling asleep, and then a little accumulation of it soon again awakens them; this happens in cases of insanity, or where the mind has been lately much agitated by fear or anger. There is another circumstance in which sleep is likewise of short duration, which arises from great debility, as after great over-fatigue, and in some fevers, where the strength of the patient is greatly diminished, as in these cases the pulse intermits or flutters, and the respiration is previously affected, it seems to originate from the want of some voluntary efforts to facilitate respiration, as when we are awake. And is further treated of in Vol. II. Class I. 2. 1. 2. on the Diseases of the Voluntary Power. Art. Somnus interruptus.

Another situation that can wake people shortly after they fall asleep is when their ability to control their thoughts is so high that it makes it hard for them to actually fall asleep, and a small amount of this control can wake them up again; this often occurs in cases of mental illness or when the mind has recently been disturbed by fear or anger. There’s also another situation where sleep is short-lived, which happens due to extreme weakness, such as after intense exhaustion or during certain fevers, where the patient’s strength is significantly reduced. In these cases, the pulse may skip or be irregular, and breathing can be affected, likely because of a lack of voluntary efforts to help with breathing, like we do when we are awake. This is further discussed in Vol. II. Class I. 2. 1. 2. on the Diseases of the Voluntary Power. Art. Somnus interruptus.

15. We come now to those motions which depend on irritation. The motions of the arterial and glandular systems continue in our sleep, proceeding slower indeed, but stronger and more uniformly, than in our waking hours, when they are incommoded by external stimuli, or by the movements of volition; the motions of the muscles subservient to respiration continue to be stimulated into action, and the other internal senses of hunger, thirst, and lust, are not only occasionally excited in our sleep, but their irritative motions are succeeded by their usual sensations, and make a part of the farrago of our dreams. These sensations of the want of air, of hunger, thirst, and lust, in our dreams, contribute to prove, that the nerves of the external senses are also alive and excitable in our sleep; but as the stimuli of external objects are either excluded from them by the darkness and silence of the night, or their access to them is prevented by the suspension of volition, these nerves of sense fall more readily into their connexions with sensation and with association; because much sensorial power, which during the day was expended in moving the external organs of sense in consequence of irritation from external stimuli, or in consequence of volition, becomes now in some degree accumulated, and renders the internal or immediate organs of sense more easily excitable by the other sensorial powers. Thus in respect to the eye, the irritation from external stimuli, and the power of volition during our waking hours, elevate the eye-lids, adapt the aperture of the iris to the quantity of light, the focus of the crystalline humour, and the angle of the optic axises to the distance of the object, all which perpetual activity during the day expends much sensorial power, which is saved during our sleep.

15. Now, let's talk about the movements linked to irritation. The motions of the arterial and glandular systems keep going while we sleep, slower but stronger and more consistent than when we’re awake, since they’re interrupted by outside stimuli or our conscious control. The muscles that help us breathe remain active, and other internal feelings like hunger, thirst, and desire not only occasionally arise in our dreams, but their irritating motions also lead to familiar sensations, becoming part of the mix in our dreams. These sensations of needing air, hunger, thirst, and desire in our dreams suggest that the nerves of our external senses are still responsive and active while we sleep. However, since the stimuli from outside are blocked by the darkness and quiet of night, or access to them is cut off by our lack of conscious will, these sensory nerves are more easily connected to sensations and associations. This is because a lot of the sensory energy, which was used during the day to move our external sense organs due to irritation from outside stimuli or from our conscious choices, gets somewhat built up and makes the internal or immediate sense organs more easily stimulated by other sensory powers. For example, regarding our eyes, the irritation from external stimuli and our conscious control while awake raises our eyelids, adjusts the shape of the iris based on light, focuses the lens, and aligns the optic axes according to the distance of the object. All this constant activity throughout the day uses up a lot of sensory energy, which gets conserved during our sleep.

Hence it appears, that not only those parts of the system, which are always excited by internal stimuli, as the stomach, intestinal canal, bile-ducts, and the various glands, but the organs of sense also may be more violently excited into action by the irritation from internal stimuli, or by sensation, during our sleep than in our waking hours; because during the suspension of volition, there is a greater quantity of the spirit of animation to be expended by the other sensorial powers. On this account our irritability to internal stimuli, and our sensibility to pain or pleasure, is not only greater in sleep, but increases as our sleep is prolonged. Whence digestion and secretion are performed better in sleep, than in our waking hours, and our dreams in the morning have greater variety and vivacity, as our sensibility increases, than at night when we first lie down. And hence epileptic fits, which are always occasioned by some disagreeable sensation, so frequently attack those, who are subject to them, in their sleep; because at this time the system is more excitable by painful sensation in consequence of internal stimuli; and the power of volition is then suddenly exerted to relieve this pain, as explained Sect. XXXIV. 1. 4.

Hence it appears, that not only those parts of the system, which are always excited by internal stimuli, as the stomach, intestinal canal, bile-ducts, and the various glands, but the organs of sense also may be more violently excited into action by the irritation from internal stimuli, or by sensation, during our sleep than in our waking hours; because during the suspension of volition, there is a greater quantity of the spirit of animation to be expended by the other sensorial powers. On this account our irritability to internal stimuli, and our sensibility to pain or pleasure, is not only greater in sleep, but increases as our sleep is prolonged. Whence digestion and secretion are performed better in sleep, than in our waking hours, and our dreams in the morning have greater variety and vivacity, as our sensibility increases, than at night when we first lie down. And hence epileptic fits, which are always occasioned by some disagreeable sensation, so frequently attack those, who are subject to them, in their sleep; because at this time the system is more excitable by painful sensation in consequence of internal stimuli; and the power of volition is then suddenly exerted to relieve this pain, as explained Sect. XXXIV. 1. 4.

There is a disease, which frequently affects children in the cradle, which is termed ecstasy, and seems to consist in certain exertions to relieve painful sensation, in which the voluntary power is not so far excited as totally to awaken them, and yet is sufficient to remove the disagreeable sensation, which excites it; in this case changing the posture of the child frequently relieves it.

There’s a disease that often affects babies, called ecstasy, which seems to involve certain efforts to ease pain. In this condition, the baby's voluntary control isn’t fully engaged to completely wake them up, but it’s enough to alleviate the discomfort that’s causing the reaction. In such cases, frequently changing the baby’s position helps provide relief.

I have at this time under my care an elegant young man about twenty-two years of age, who seldom sleeps more than an hour without experiencing a convulsion fit; which ceases in about half a minute without any subsequent stupor. Large doses of opium only prevented the paroxysms, so long as they prevented him from sleeping by the intoxication, which they induced. Other medicines had no effect on him. He was gently awakened every half hour for one night, but without good effect, as he soon slept again, and the fit returned at about the same periods of time, for the accumulated sensorial power, which occasioned the increased sensibility to pain, was not thus exhausted. This case evinces, that the sensibility of the system to internal excitation increases, as our sleep is prolonged; till the pain thus occasioned produces voluntary exertion; which, when it is in its usual degree, only awakens us; but when it is more violent, it occasions convulsions.

I currently have in my care a sophisticated young man around twenty-two years old, who rarely sleeps more than an hour without having a convulsion that lasts about half a minute and doesn't leave him in a stupor afterward. High doses of opium only stopped the fits as long as they kept him awake due to the intoxication they caused. Other medications had no effect on him. One night, he was gently woken up every half hour, but it didn’t help, as he quickly fell asleep again, and the fits came back at roughly the same intervals. This happened because the buildup of sensory power, which led to heightened sensitivity to pain, wasn’t depleted. This case shows that the body’s sensitivity to internal stimulation increases as our sleep extends, until the pain produced triggers voluntary movement; usually, this just wakes us up, but if it’s more intense, it leads to convulsions.

The cramp in the calf of the leg is another kind of convulsion, which generally commences in sleep, occasioned by the continual increase of irritability from internal stimuli, or of sensibility, during that state of our existence. The cramp is a violent exertion to relieve pain, generally either of the skin from cold, or of the bowels, as in some diarrhœas, or from the muscles having been previously overstretched, as in walking up or down steep hills. But in these convulsions of the muscles, which form the calf of the leg, the contraction is so violent as to occasion another pain in consequence of their own too violent contraction; as soon as the original pain, which caused the contraction, is removed. And hence the cramp, or spasm, of these muscles is continued without intermission by this new pain, unlike the alternate convulsions and remissions in epileptic fits. The reason, that the contraction of these muscles of the calf of the leg is more violent during their convulsion than that of others, depends on the weakness of their antagonist muscles; for after these have been contracted in their usual action, as at every step in walking, they are again extended, not, as most other muscles are, by their antagonists, but by the weight of the whole body on the balls of the toes; and that weight applied to great mechanical advantage on the heel, that is, on the other end of the bone of the foot, which thus acts as a lever.

The cramp in the calf of the leg is another type of spasm that usually starts during sleep, caused by the ongoing increase of irritation from internal triggers or sensitivity during that state of being. The cramp is a strong effort to relieve pain, typically either from cold skin, issues in the bowels, like some cases of diarrhea, or due to the muscles being overstretched, as when walking up or down steep hills. However, in these muscle spasms in the calf, the contraction is so intense that it creates additional pain due to their own excessive tightening, once the original pain causing the contraction is gone. This means that the cramp or spasm in these muscles continues without interruption because of this new pain, unlike the alternating spasms and relief seen in epileptic seizures. The reason the contraction in the calf muscles is more intense during a spasm than in other muscles is due to the weakness of their opposing muscles; after these opposing muscles have contracted during their usual activity, like every step in walking, they are then extended not by their antagonists, as most other muscles are, but by the weight of the entire body pressing down on the balls of the toes, which applies significant mechanical advantage on the heel, effectively turning the foot into a lever.

Another disease, the periods of which generally commence during our sleep, is the asthma. Whatever may be the remote cause of paroxysms of asthma, the immediate cause of the convulsive respiration, whether in the common asthma, or in what is termed the convulsive asthma, which are perhaps only different degrees of the same disease, must be owing to violent voluntary exertions to relieve pain, as in other convulsions; and the increase of irritability to internal stimuli, or of sensibility, during sleep must occasion them to commence at this time.

Another condition that usually starts while we’re sleeping is asthma. No matter what triggers an asthma attack, the immediate cause of the labored breathing, whether in typical asthma or what is called convulsive asthma—which are likely just different levels of the same condition—seems to be linked to intense voluntary efforts to relieve discomfort, similar to other types of convulsions. The heightened sensitivity to internal triggers, or sensitivity in general, during sleep likely causes these attacks to happen at that time.

Debilitated people, who have been unfortunately accustomed to great ingurgitation of spirituous potation, frequently part with a great quantity of water during the night, but with not more than usual in the day-time. This is owing to a beginning torpor of the absorbent system, and precedes anasarca, which commences in the day, but is cured in the night by the increase of the irritability of the absorbent system during sleep, which thus imbibes from the cellular membrane the fluids, which had been accumulated there during the day; though it is possible the horizontal position of the body may contribute something to this purpose, and also the greater irritability of some branches of the absorbent vessels, which open their mouths in the cells of the cellular membrane, than that of other branches.

People who are unfortunately used to drinking a lot of alcohol often lose a significant amount of water at night, but the amount during the day is pretty normal. This happens because of an early sluggishness in their absorbent system, which leads to swelling (anasarca) that starts during the day but is treated at night. While they sleep, the absorbent system becomes more active, absorbing the fluids that have built up in the cellular membranes during the day. It’s also possible that lying down helps with this, along with the increased sensitivity of certain parts of the absorbent vessels that open in the cellular membrane compared to others.

As soon as a person begins to sleep, the irritability and sensibility of the system begins to increase, owing to the suspension of volition and the exclusion of external stimuli. Hence the actions of the vessels in obedience to internal stimulation become stronger and more energetic, though less frequent in respect to number. And as many of the secretions are increased, so the heat of the system is gradually increased, and the extremities of feeble people, which had been cold during the day, become warm. Till towards morning many people become so warm, as to find it necessary to throw off some of their bed-clothes, as soon as they awake; and in others sweats are so liable to occur towards morning during their sleep.

As soon as someone starts to sleep, their body's irritability and sensitivity increase due to the halt of conscious control and removal of external stimuli. As a result, the actions of the blood vessels respond more vigorously to internal signals, becoming stronger and more intense, even if they occur less frequently. With many bodily secretions on the rise, the body's temperature gradually increases, and the extremities of people who are weak, which were cold during the day, start to warm up. By morning, many people feel so warm that they need to kick off some of their blankets as soon as they wake up; for others, sweating becomes common toward morning during their sleep.

Thus those, who are not accustomed to sleep in the open air, are very liable to take cold, if they happen to fall asleep on a garden bench, or in a carriage with the window open. For as the system is warmer during sleep, as above explained, if a current of cold air affects any part of the body, a torpor of that part is more effectually produced, as when a cold blast of air through a key-hole or casement falls upon a person in a warm room. In those cases the affected part possesses less irritability in respect to heat, from its having previously been exposed to a greater stimulus of heat, as in the warm room, or during sleep; and hence, when the stimulus of heat is diminished, a torpor is liable to ensue; that is, we take cold. Hence people who sleep in the open air, generally feel chilly both at the approach of sleep, and on their awaking; and hence many people are perpetually subject to catarrhs if they sleep in a less warm head-dress, than that which they wear in the day.

So, for those who aren’t used to sleeping outdoors, they can easily catch a cold if they happen to doze off on a garden bench or in a car with the window open. Since the body is warmer during sleep, as mentioned earlier, if a draft of cold air hits any part of the body, it causes that area to become numb more effectively, just like when a cold breeze from a crack in the door or window hits someone in a warm room. In those situations, the affected area is less responsive to heat because it was previously exposed to a higher temperature, like in the warm room or while sleeping; therefore, when the heat is reduced, numbness can follow, which means we can catch a cold. People who sleep outdoors often feel cold when they start to drift off and when they wake up, which is why many consistently suffer from colds if they wear a lighter head covering at night compared to what they wear during the day.

16. Not only the sensorial powers of irritation and of sensation, but that of association also appear to act with greater vigour during the suspension of volition in sleep. It will be shewn in another place, that the gout generally first attacks the liver, and that afterwards an inflammation of the ball of the great toe commences by association, and that of the liver ceases. Now as this change or metastasis of the activity of the system generally commences in sleep, it follows, that these associations of motion exist with greater energy at that time; that is, that the sensorial faculty of association, like those of irritation and of sensation, becomes in some measure accumulated during the suspension of volition.

16. Not only do the sensory responses of irritation and sensation become stronger, but the ability to associate also seems to be more active during the lack of willpower in sleep. It will be shown elsewhere that gout typically first targets the liver, and later an inflammation of the big toe begins through association, while the liver's issue subsides. Since this shift or relocation of the body's activity usually starts in sleep, it follows that these movements of association are more powerful at that time; in other words, the ability to associate, like the senses of irritation and sensation, builds up somewhat during the suspension of willpower.

Other associate tribes and trains of motions, as well as the irritative and sensitive ones, appear to be increased in their activity during the suspension of volition in sleep. As those which contribute to circulate the blood, and to perform the various secretions; as well as the associate tribes and trains of ideas, which contribute to furnish the perpetual dreams of our dreaming imaginations.

Other related tribes and movements, along with the irritative and sensitive ones, seem to become more active during the lack of will during sleep. This includes those that help circulate the blood and carry out various secretions, as well as the connected groups and series of thoughts that provide the endless dreams of our dreaming minds.

In sleep the secretions have generally been supposed to be diminished, as the expectorated mucus in coughs, the fluids discharged in diarrhœas, and in salivation, except indeed the secretion of sweat, which is often visibly increased. This error seems to have arisen from attention to the excretions rather than to the secretions. For the secretions, except that of sweat, are generally received into reservoirs, as the urine into the bladder, and the mucus of the intestines and lungs into their respective cavities; but these reservoirs do not exclude these fluids immediately by their stimulus, but require at the same time some voluntary efforts, and therefore permit them to remain during sleep. And as they thus continue longer in those receptacles in our sleeping hours, a greater part is absorbed from them, and the remainder becomes thicker, and sometimes in less quantity, though at the time it was secreted the fluid was in greater quantity than in our waking hours. Thus the urine is higher coloured after long sleep; which shews that a greater quantity has been secreted, and that more of the aqueous and saline part has been reabsorbed, and the earthy part left in the bladder; hence thick urine in fevers shews only a greater action of the vessels which secrete it in the kidneys, and of those which absorb it from the bladder.

During sleep, it's generally believed that secretions are reduced, like the mucus expelled during coughs, the fluids released during diarrhea, and saliva, except for sweat, which often increases significantly. This misconception seems to come from focusing on excretions rather than secretions. Most secretions, apart from sweat, are stored in reservoirs, like urine in the bladder and mucus in the intestines and lungs. However, these reservoirs don’t expel fluids immediately due to their stimulation; they require some voluntary effort, which allows these fluids to remain during sleep. As a result, they stay in these reservoirs longer while we sleep, leading to greater absorption and causing the remaining fluids to thicken, sometimes even decreasing in quantity, although more fluid is secreted than when we’re awake. For example, urine appears darker after a long sleep, indicating that more has been secreted, and that a larger portion of the water and salt has been reabsorbed, leaving the solid parts in the bladder. Therefore, thick urine during fevers indicates greater activity in the vessels that secrete it in the kidneys and those that absorb it from the bladder.

The same happens to the mucus expectorated in coughs, which is thus thickened by absorption of its aqueous and saline parts; and the same of the feces of the intestines. From hence it appears, and from what has been said in No. 15. of this Section concerning the increase of irritability and of sensibility during sleep, that the secretions are in general rather increased than diminished during these hours of our existence; and it is probable that nutrition is almost entirely performed in sleep; and that young animals grow more at this time than in their waking hours, as young plants have long since been observed to grow more in the night, which is their time of sleep.

The same happens to the mucus expectorated in coughs, which is thus thickened by absorption of its aqueous and saline parts; and the same of the feces of the intestines. From hence it appears, and from what has been said in No. 15. of this Section concerning the increase of irritability and of sensibility during sleep, that the secretions are in general rather increased than diminished during these hours of our existence; and it is probable that nutrition is almost entirely performed in sleep; and that young animals grow more at this time than in their waking hours, as young plants have long since been observed to grow more in the night, which is their time of sleep.

17. Two other remarkable circumstances of our dreaming ideas are their inconsistency, and the total absence of surprise. Thus we seem to be present at more extraordinary metamorphoses of animals or trees, than are to be met with in the fables of antiquity; and appear to be transported from place to place, which seas divide, as quickly as the changes of scenery are performed in a play-house; and yet are not sensible of their inconsistency, nor in the least degree affected with surprise.

17. Two other notable aspects of our dreams are their lack of consistency and the complete absence of surprise. We seem to witness more incredible transformations of animals or trees than those found in ancient fables; we appear to be moved from one place to another, divided by seas, as quickly as scenery changes in a theater; and yet, we don’t recognize the inconsistencies and are not at all surprised.

We must consider this circumstance more minutely. In our waking trains of ideas, those that are inconsistent with the usual order of nature, so rarely have occurred to us, that their connexion is the slightest of all others: hence, when a consistent train of ideas is exhausted, we attend to the external stimuli, that usually surround us, rather than to any inconsistent idea, which might otherwise present itself; and if an inconsistent idea should intrude itself, we immediately compare it with the preceding one, and voluntarily reject the train it would introduce; this appears further in the Section on Reverie, in which state of the mind external stimuli are not attended to, and yet the streams of ideas are kept consistent by the efforts of volition. But as our faculty of volition is suspended, and all external stimuli are excluded in sleep, this slighter connexion of ideas takes place; and the train is said to be inconsistent; that is, dissimilar to the usual order of nature.

We need to look at this situation more closely. In our waking thoughts, ideas that don’t fit the normal way things work rarely come to mind, so their connection is the weakest among all the others. Therefore, when a consistent flow of thoughts runs dry, we focus on the external stimuli around us instead of any inconsistent ideas that might surface; and if an inconsistent idea does creep in, we immediately compare it to the previous one and choose to dismiss the line of thought it would bring in. This is also seen in the section on Daydreaming, where in that state of mind we don't pay attention to external stimuli, yet the flow of ideas remains consistent thanks to our willpower. However, when our willpower is inactive and all external stimuli are cut off during sleep, this weaker connection of ideas occurs, making the thoughts seem inconsistent, which means they are different from the typical order of nature.

But, when any consistent train of sensitive or voluntary ideas is flowing along, if any external stimulus affects us so violently, as to intrude irritative ideas forcibly into the mind, it disunites the former train of ideas, and we are affected with surprise. These stimuli of unusual energy or novelty not only disunite our common trains of ideas, but the trains of muscular motions also, which have not been long established by habit, and disturb those that have. Some people become motionless by great surprise, the fits of hiccup and or ague have been often removed by it, and it even affects the movements of the heart, and arteries; but in our sleep, all external stimuli are excluded, and in consequence no surprise can exist. See Section XVII. 3. 7.

But, when any consistent train of sensitive or voluntary ideas is flowing along, if any external stimulus affects us so violently, as to intrude irritative ideas forcibly into the mind, it disunites the former train of ideas, and we are affected with surprise. These stimuli of unusual energy or novelty not only disunite our common trains of ideas, but the trains of muscular motions also, which have not been long established by habit, and disturb those that have. Some people become motionless by great surprise, the fits of hiccup and or ague have been often removed by it, and it even affects the movements of the heart, and arteries; but in our sleep, all external stimuli are excluded, and in consequence no surprise can exist. See Section XVII. 3. 7.

18. We frequently awake with pleasure from a dream, which has delighted us, without being able to recollect the transactions of it; unless perhaps at a distance of time, some analogous idea may introduce afresh this forgotten train: and in our waking reveries we sometimes in a moment lose the train of thought, but continue to feel the glow of pleasure, or the depression of spirits, it occasioned: whilst at other times we can retrace with ease these histories of our reveries and dreams.

18. We often wake up feeling happy from a dream that has pleased us, even though we can’t remember what happened in it; unless maybe after some time, a similar thought brings back that forgotten sequence. In our waking thoughts, we sometimes suddenly lose our train of thought but still feel the warmth of joy or the heaviness of sadness it caused. Other times, we can easily go back and recall the stories from our daydreams and dreams.

The above explanation of surprise throws light upon this subject. When we are suddenly awaked by any violent stimulus, the surprise totally disunites the trains of our sleeping ideas from these of our waking ones; but if we gradually awake, this does not happen; and we readily unravel the preceding trains of imagination.

The explanation above about surprise sheds light on this topic. When we're suddenly jolted awake by a strong stimulus, the shock completely disconnects our sleeping thoughts from our waking ones; however, if we wake up gradually, this doesn’t occur, and we easily untangle the previous streams of imagination.

19. There are various degrees of surprise; the more intent we are upon the train of ideas, which we are employed about, the more violent must be the stimulus that interrupts them, and the greater is the degree of surprise. I have observed dogs, who have slept by the fire, and by their obscure barking and struggling have appeared very intent on their prey, that shewed great surprise for a few seconds after their awaking by looking eagerly around them; which they did not do at other times of waking. And an intelligent friend of mine has remarked, that his lady, who frequently speaks much and articulately in her sleep, could never recollect her dreams in the morning, when this happened to her: but that when she did not speak in her sleep, she could always recollect them.

19. There are different levels of surprise; the more focused we are on the thoughts we’re engaged in, the stronger the interruption needs to be to disrupt them, leading to a greater level of surprise. I’ve noticed dogs that sleep by the fire, and who, in their obscure barking and struggles, seem very focused on their prey, showing a lot of surprise for a few seconds after waking up by looking around eagerly; they don’t usually do this upon waking at other times. An insightful friend of mine has pointed out that his wife, who often talks a lot and clearly in her sleep, can never remember her dreams in the morning when that happens. However, when she doesn’t talk in her sleep, she can always remember them.

Hence, when our sensations act so strongly in sleep as to influence the larger muscles, as in those, who talk or struggle in their dreams; or in those, who are affected with complete reverie (as described in the next Section), great surprise is produced, when they awake; and these as well as those, who are completely drunk or delirious, totally forget afterwards their imaginations at those times.

Hence, when our sensations are so intense during sleep that they affect our larger muscles, like in those who talk or fight in their dreams, or in those who experience complete reverie (as described in the next section), it can lead to great surprise upon waking. Both these people and those who are completely drunk or delirious completely forget their fantasies from those times afterwards.

20. As the immediate cause of sleep consists in the suspension of volition, it follows, that whatever diminishes the general quantity of sensorial power, or derives it from the faculty of volition, will constitute a remote cause of sleep; such as fatigue from muscular or mental exertion, which diminishes the general quantity of sensorial power; or an increase of the sensitive motions, as by attending to soft music, which diverts the sensorial power from the faculty of volition; or lastly, by increase of the irritative motions, as by wine, or food; or warmth; which not only by their expenditure of sensorial power diminish the quantity of volition; but also by their producing pleasureable sensations (which occasion other muscular or sensual motions in consequence), doubly decrease the voluntary power, and thus more forcibly produce sleep. See Sect. XXXIV. 1. 4.

20. As the immediate cause of sleep consists in the suspension of volition, it follows, that whatever diminishes the general quantity of sensorial power, or derives it from the faculty of volition, will constitute a remote cause of sleep; such as fatigue from muscular or mental exertion, which diminishes the general quantity of sensorial power; or an increase of the sensitive motions, as by attending to soft music, which diverts the sensorial power from the faculty of volition; or lastly, by increase of the irritative motions, as by wine, or food; or warmth; which not only by their expenditure of sensorial power diminish the quantity of volition; but also by their producing pleasureable sensations (which occasion other muscular or sensual motions in consequence), doubly decrease the voluntary power, and thus more forcibly produce sleep. See Sect. XXXIV. 1. 4.

Another method of inducing sleep is delivered in a very ingenious work lately published by Dr. Beddoes. Who, after lamenting that opium frequently occasions restlessness, thinks, "that in most cases it would be better to induce sleep by the abstraction of stimuli, than by exhausting the excitability;" and adds, "upon this principle we could not have a better soporific than an atmosphere with a diminished proportion of oxygene air, and that common air might be admitted after the patient was asleep." (Observ. on Calculus, &c. by Dr. Beddoes, Murray.) If it should be found to be true, that the excitability of the system depends on the quantity of oxygene absorbed by the lungs in respiration according to the theory of Dr. Beddoes, and of M. Girtanner, this idea of sleeping in an atmosphere with less oxygene in its composition might be of great service in epileptic cases, and in cramp, and even in fits of the asthma, where their periods commence from the increase of irritability during sleep.

Another way to help people fall asleep is presented in a clever recent publication by Dr. Beddoes. He points out that opium often causes restlessness and suggests, "In most cases, it would be better to induce sleep by reducing stimuli rather than exhausting the body's responsiveness." He adds, "Based on this principle, we couldn't have a better sedative than an environment with a lower level of oxygen, and normal air could be introduced after the patient has fallen asleep." (Observ. on Calculus, &c. by Dr. Beddoes, Murray.) If it's proven that the body's responsiveness relies on the amount of oxygen absorbed by the lungs during breathing, as theorized by Dr. Beddoes and M. Girtanner, this concept of sleeping in an air with less oxygen could be very helpful in cases of epilepsy, cramps, and even asthma attacks, which often start due to increased irritability during sleep.

Sleep is likewise said to be induced by mechanic pressure on the brain in the cases of spina bifida. Where there has been a defect of one of the vertebræ of the back, a tumour is protruded in consequence; and, whenever this tumour has been compressed by the hand, sleep is said to be induced, because the whole of the brain both within the head and spine becomes compressed by the retrocession of the fluid within the tumour. But by what means a compression of the brain induces sleep has not been explained, but probably by diminishing the secretion of sensorial power, and then the voluntary motions become suspended previously to the irritative ones, as occurs in most dying persons.

Sleep is also said to be triggered by pressure on the brain in cases of spina bifida. When there’s a defect in one of the vertebrae of the back, a tumor pushes out as a result. Whenever this tumor is pressed by hand, it’s said to induce sleep because the entire brain, both in the head and spine, gets compressed due to the fluid receding within the tumor. However, the exact way that brain compression leads to sleep hasn’t been explained, but it’s likely due to a reduction in the secretion of sensory power, causing voluntary movements to stop before the involuntary ones, similar to what happens in most dying individuals.

Another way of procuring sleep mechanically was related to me by Mr. Brindley, the famous canal engineer, who was brought up to the business of a mill-wright; he told me, that he had more than once seen the experiment of a man extending himself across the large stone of a corn-mill, and that by gradually letting the stone whirl, the man fell asleep, before the stone had gained its full velocity, and he supposed would have died without pain by the continuance or increase of the motion. In this case the centrifugal motion of the head and feet must accumulate the blood in both those extremities of the body, and thus compress the brain.

Another way to mechanically induce sleep was shared with me by Mr. Brindley, the well-known canal engineer, who was trained as a millwright. He told me that he had seen more than one experiment where a man stretched himself across the large stone of a corn mill. By gradually letting the stone spin, the man would fall asleep before the stone reached its full speed, and he believed he would have died painlessly if the motion continued or increased. In this case, the centrifugal motion of the head and feet would collect blood in those extremities of the body, thereby putting pressure on the brain.

Lastly, we should mention the application of cold; which, when in a less degree, produces watchfulness by the pain it occasions, and the tremulous convulsions of the subcutaneous muscles; but when it is applied in great degree, is said to produce sleep. To explain this effect it has been said, that as the vessels of the skin and extremities become first torpid by the want of the stimulus of heat, and as thence less blood is circulated through them, as appears from their paleness, a greater quantity of blood poured upon the brain produces sleep by its compression of that organ. But I should rather imagine, that the sensorial power becomes exhausted by the convulsive actions in consequence of the pain of cold, and of the voluntary exercise previously used to prevent it, and that the sleep is only the beginning to die, as the suspension of voluntary power in lingering deaths precedes for many hours the extinction of the irritative motions.

Lastly, we should mention the use of cold, which, when applied in a mild way, causes alertness due to the pain it brings and the shaking of the muscles just under the skin; but when applied heavily, it is said to induce sleep. To explain this effect, it has been suggested that when the blood vessels in the skin and extremities become sluggish due to the lack of heat, less blood circulates through them, which can be seen from their paleness, and a larger volume of blood flowing into the brain causes sleep by pressing on that organ. However, I believe that the sensorial power is more likely exhausted by the convulsive movements caused by the pain of the cold, along with the physical effort made previously to avoid it, and that sleep is simply the initial stage of dying, as the loss of voluntary control in prolonged deaths often occurs many hours before the end of involuntary movements.

21. The following are the characteristic circumstances attending perfect sleep.

21. Here are the key situations that come with perfect sleep.

1. The power of volition is totally suspended.

1. The power of choice is completely paused.

2. The trains of ideas caused by sensation proceed with greater facility and vivacity; but become inconsistent with the usual order of nature. The muscular motions caused by sensation continue; as those concerned in our evacuations during infancy, and afterwards in digestion, and in priapismus.

2. The trains of thought triggered by sensation move along more easily and vividly, but they become inconsistent with the normal order of things. The muscle movements caused by sensation persist, such as those involved in our bodily functions during infancy, and later in digestion, and in priapism.

3. The irritative muscular motions continue, as those concerned in the circulation, in secretion, in respiration. But the irritative sensual motions, or ideas, are not excited; as the immediate organs of sense are not stimulated into action by external objects, which are excluded by the external organs of sense; which are not in sleep adapted to their reception by the power of volition, as in our waking hours.

3. The irritating muscle movements keep going, just like those involved in circulation, secretion, and breathing. However, the irritating sensory movements or ideas aren’t triggered because the immediate sensory organs aren't activated by outside objects, which are blocked by the external sensory organs. These aren’t suited for receiving them in sleep through the power of will, like they are when we’re awake.

4. The associate motions continue; but their first link is not excited into action by volition, or by external stimuli. In all respects, except those above mentioned, the three last sensorial powers are somewhat increased in energy during the suspension of volition, owing to the consequent accumulation of the spirit of animation.

4. The associate motions keep going; however, their initial connection isn't triggered by willpower or outside stimuli. In every way, except for the ones mentioned above, the last three sensory powers are somewhat heightened in energy during the suspension of willpower, due to the resulting buildup of the spirit of animation.



SECT. XIX.

OF REVERIE.

Daydreaming.

1. Various degrees of reverie. 2. Sleep-walkers. Case of a young lady. Great surprise at awaking. And total forgetfulness of what passed in reverie. 3. No suspension of volition in reverie. 4. Sensitive motions continue, and are consistent. 5. Irritative motions continue, but are not succeeded by sensation. 6. Volition necessary for the perception of feeble impressions. 7. Associated motions continue. 8. Nerves of sense are irritable in sleep, but not in reverie. 9. Somnambuli are not asleep. Contagion received but once. 10. Definition of reverie.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Different levels of daydreaming. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Sleepwalkers. A case involving a young woman. Major surprise upon waking. And complete forgetfulness of what happened during the daydream. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. No loss of will in daydreaming. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Sensitive movements continue and are consistent. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Reflexive movements carry on but are not followed by sensation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Willpower is needed to perceive weak impressions. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Related movements persist. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Sensory nerves are reactive in sleep but not in daydreaming. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Sleepwalkers are not truly asleep. They respond to suggestions only once. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Definition of daydreaming.

1. When we are employed with great sensation of pleasure, or with great efforts of volition, in the pursuit of some interesting train of ideas, we cease to be conscious of our existence, are inattentive to time and place, and do not distinguish this train of sensitive and voluntary ideas from the irritative ones excited by the presence of external objects, though our organs of sense are furnished with their accustomed stimuli, till at length this interesting train of ideas becomes exhausted, or the appulses of external objects are applied with unusual violence, and we return with surprise, or with regret, into the common track of life. This is termed reverie or studium.

1. When we're deeply engaged in something that brings us joy or when we're putting in a lot of effort to follow a captivating line of thought, we lose awareness of ourselves, ignore the passage of time and our surroundings, and don’t see the difference between this captivating flow of thoughts and the distracting ones triggered by what’s happening around us, even though our senses are still receiving their usual stimuli, until eventually this engaging thought process runs out, or the impact of external stimuli hits us harder than usual, pulling us back with surprise or regret into the everyday routine. This is called reverie or studium.

In some constitutions these reveries continue a considerable time, and are not to be removed without greater difficulty, but are experienced in a less degree by us all; when we attend earnestly to the ideas excited by volition or sensation, with their associated connexions, but are at the same time conscious at intervals of the stimuli of surrounding bodies. Thus in being present at a play, or in reading a romance, some persons are so totally absorbed as to forget their usual time of sleep, and to neglect their meals; while others are said to have been so involved in voluntary study as not to have heard the discharge of artillery; and there is a story of an Italian politician, who could think so intensely on other subjects, as to be insensible to the torture of the rack.

In some people, these daydreams can last a long time and are hard to shake off, but we all experience them to some extent. When we focus deeply on the thoughts triggered by our will or senses, we also intermittently notice the influences from the world around us. For instance, while watching a play or reading a novel, some individuals can become so completely engrossed that they lose track of time and skip meals. Others reportedly get so caught up in their studies that they don’t even hear gunfire. There’s even a story about an Italian politician who could think so intensely about other topics that he was oblivious to the pain of being tortured.

From hence it appears, that these catenations of ideas and muscular motions, which form the trains of reverie, are composed both of voluntary and sensitive associations of them; and that these ideas differ from those of delirium or of sleep, as they are kept consistent by the power of volition; and they differ also from the trains of ideas belonging to insanity, as they are as frequently excited by sensation as by volition. But lastly, that the whole sensorial power is so employed on these trains of complete reverie, that like the violent efforts of volition, as in convulsions or insanity; or like the great activity of the irritative motions in drunkenness; or of the sensitive motions in delirium; they preclude all sensation consequent to external stimulus.

From this, it’s clear that these connections of thoughts and muscle movements, which create streams of daydreaming, consist of both intentional and sensory associations. These thoughts are different from those in delirium or sleep, as they are maintained by the power of will. They also differ from the thought patterns seen in insanity, as they are triggered just as often by sensation as by will. Lastly, the entire sensory ability is so focused on these complete streams of daydreaming that, like the intense efforts of will seen in convulsions or insanity, or the heightened activity of muscle movements in drunkenness, or the sensory activity in delirium, they block out all sensations resulting from external stimuli.

2. Those persons, who are said to walk in their sleep, are affected with reverie to so great a degree, that it becomes a formidable disease; the essence of which consists in the inaptitude of the mind to attend to external stimuli. Many histories of this disease have been published by medical writers; of which there is a very curious one in the Lausanne Transactions. I shall here subjoin an account of such a case, with its cure, for the better illustration of this subject.

2. People who are said to sleepwalk are so deeply caught up in their thoughts that it turns into a serious condition; the core issue is that their minds struggle to respond to outside stimuli. Many medical writers have published accounts of this condition, including a particularly interesting one in the Lausanne Transactions. Here, I will include a case study of such an occurrence along with its treatment to better illustrate this topic.

A very ingenious and elegant young lady, with light eyes and hair, about the age of seventeen, in other respects well, was suddenly seized soon after her usual menstruation with this very wonderful malady. The disease began with vehement convulsions of almost every muscle of her body, with great but vain efforts to vomit, and the most violent hiccoughs, that can be conceived: these were succeeded in about an hour with a fixed spasm; in which one hand was applied to her head, and the other to support it: in about half an hour these ceased, and the reverie began suddenly, and was at first manifest by the look of her eyes and countenance, which seemed to express attention. Then she conversed aloud with imaginary persons with her eyes open, and could not for about an hour be brought to attend to the stimulus of external objects by any kind of violence, which it was proper to use; these symptoms returned in this order every day for five or six weeks.

A very clever and elegant young woman, with light eyes and hair, around seventeen years old, was suddenly struck by this very unusual illness shortly after her regular menstruation. The condition started with intense convulsions affecting almost every muscle in her body, accompanied by strong but unsuccessful attempts to vomit, and the most violent hiccups imaginable. About an hour later, this was followed by a fixed spasm, in which one hand was positioned on her head while the other supported it. After roughly half an hour, these symptoms stopped, and a trance began abruptly, first evident in the expression of her eyes and face, which seemed to show focus. Then she started talking aloud to imaginary people with her eyes open and couldn’t be made to respond to external stimuli through any sort of force, which was deemed appropriate; these symptoms recurred in this sequence every day for five to six weeks.

These conversations were quite consistent, and we could understand, what she supposed her imaginary companions to answer, by the continuation of her part of the discourse. Sometimes she was angry, at other times shewed much wit and vivacity, but was most frequently inclined to melancholy. In these reveries she sometimes sung over some music with accuracy, and repeated whole pages from the English poets. In repeating some lines from Mr. Pope's works she had forgot one word, and began again, endeavouring to recollect it; when she came to the forgotten word, it was shouted aloud in her ear, and this repeatedly, to no purpose; but by many trials she at length regained it herself.

These conversations were pretty consistent, and we could understand what she thought her imaginary friends would say based on how she continued her side of the chat. Sometimes she was angry, other times she showed a lot of wit and energy, but most often she leaned towards feeling sad. In these daydreams, she sometimes sang some music accurately and recited entire pages from English poets. While reciting some lines from Mr. Pope’s works, she forgot one word and started over, trying to remember it; when she got to the missing word, it was shouted in her ear repeatedly, but it didn’t help. After many attempts, she finally remembered it herself.

These paroxysms were terminated with the appearance of inexpressible surprise, and great fear, from which she was some minutes in recovering herself, calling on her sister with great agitation, and very frequently underwent a repetition of convulsions, apparently from the pain of fear. See Sect. XVII. 3. 7.

These paroxysms were terminated with the appearance of inexpressible surprise, and great fear, from which she was some minutes in recovering herself, calling on her sister with great agitation, and very frequently underwent a repetition of convulsions, apparently from the pain of fear. See Sect. XVII. 3. 7.

After having thus returned for about an hour every day for two or three weeks, the reveries seemed to become less complete, and some of their circumstances varied; so that she could walk about the room in them without running against any of the furniture; though these motions were at first very unsteady and tottering. And afterwards she once drank a dish of tea, when the whole apparatus of the tea-table was set before her; and expressed some suspicion, that a medicine was put into it, and once seemed to smell of a tuberose, which was in flower in her chamber, and deliberated aloud about breaking it from the stem, saying, "it would make her sister so charmingly angry." At another time in her melancholy moments she heard the sound of a passing bell, "I wish I was dead," she cried, listening to the bell, and then taking off one of her shoes, as she sat upon the bed, "I love the colour black," says she, "a little wider, and a little longer, even this might make me a coffin!"—Yet it is evident, she was not sensible at this time, any more than formerly, of seeing or hearing any person about her; indeed when great light was thrown upon her by opening the shutters of the window, her trains of ideas seemed less melancholy; and when I have forcibly held her hands, or covered her eyes, she appeared to grow impatient, and would say, she could not tell what to do, for she could neither see nor move. In all these circumstances her pulse continued unaffected as in health. And when the paroxysm was over, she could never recollect a single idea of what had passed in it.

After returning for about an hour every day for two or three weeks, her daydreams started to become less vivid, and some details changed; she could walk around the room without bumping into any furniture, though at first her movements were pretty shaky. Then, one time, she drank a cup of tea with all the tea set in front of her and showed some doubt that it was just tea, expressing concern that something might have been added to it. She even caught a whiff of a tuberose that was blooming in her room and thought aloud about picking it, saying, "it would make my sister so adorably angry." Another time, feeling down, she heard the sound of a passing bell and exclaimed, "I wish I was dead," while listening to it and then took off one of her shoes while sitting on the bed, saying, "I love the color black; a little wider and a little longer, even this could be my coffin!" Yet, it was clear she wasn’t aware, just like before, of anyone around her. In fact, when a bright light came in from opening the window shutters, her thoughts seemed less gloomy. When I held her hands tightly or covered her eyes, she got restless and would say she didn’t know what to do since she could neither see nor move. Despite all this, her pulse remained steady as if she were healthy. And when the episode was over, she never remembered a single thought from it.

This astonishing disease, after the use of many other medicines and applications in vain, was cured by very large doses of opium given about an hour before the expected returns of the paroxysms; and after a few relapses, at the intervals of three or four months, entirely disappeared. But she continued at times to have other symptoms of epilepsy.

This remarkable disease, after the use of many other medicines and treatments without success, was cured by very large doses of opium taken about an hour before the anticipated episodes. After a few relapses, spaced three to four months apart, it completely went away. However, she occasionally experienced other symptoms of epilepsy.

3. We shall only here consider, what happened during the time of her reveries, as that is our present subject; the fits of convulsion belong to another part of this treatise. Sect. XXXIV. 1. 4.

3. We shall only here consider, what happened during the time of her reveries, as that is our present subject; the fits of convulsion belong to another part of this treatise. Sect. XXXIV. 1. 4.

There seems to have been no suspension of volition during the fits of reverie, because she endeavoured to regain the lost idea in repeating the lines of poetry, and deliberated about breaking the tuberose, and suspected the tea to have been medicated.

There doesn't seem to have been any loss of will during the daydreams, because she tried to recover the forgotten thought by repeating the lines of poetry, thought about picking the tuberose, and suspected that the tea had been laced with something.

4. The ideas and muscular movements depending on sensation were exerted with their usual vivacity, and were kept from being inconsistent by the power of volition, as appeared from her whole conversation, and was explained in Sect. XVII. 3. 7. and XVIII. 16.

4. The ideas and muscular movements depending on sensation were exerted with their usual vivacity, and were kept from being inconsistent by the power of volition, as appeared from her whole conversation, and was explained in Sect. XVII. 3. 7. and XVIII. 16.

5. The ideas and motions dependant on irritation during the first weeks of her disease, whilst the reverie was complete, were never succeeded by the sensation of pleasure or pain; as she neither saw, heard, nor felt any of the surrounding objects. Nor was it certain that any irritative motions succeeded the stimulus of external objects, till the reverie became less complete, and then she could walk about the room without running against the furniture of it. Afterwards, when the reverie became still less complete from the use of opium, some few irritations were at times succeeded by her attention to them. As when she smelt at a tuberose, and drank a dish of tea, but this only when she seemed voluntarily to attend to them.

5. The thoughts and movements triggered by irritation during the first weeks of her illness, while she was in a deep reverie, were never followed by feelings of pleasure or pain; she was completely unaware of the objects around her—she neither saw, heard, nor felt anything. It was also unclear if any reactive movements occurred in response to external stimuli until her reverie started to fade, allowing her to walk around the room without bumping into the furniture. Later, as her reverie diminished further due to the use of opium, she occasionally became aware of some irritations. For instance, when she inhaled the scent of a tuberose or drank a cup of tea, this awareness only happened when she seemed to consciously focus on them.

6. In common life when we listen to distant sounds, or wish to distinguish objects in the night, we are obliged strongly to exert our volition to dispose the organs of sense to perceive them, and to suppress the other trains of ideas, which might interrupt these feeble sensations. Hence in the present history the strongest stimuli were not perceived, except when the faculty of volition was exerted on the organ of sense; and then even common stimuli were sometimes perceived: for her mind was so strenuously employed in pursuing its own trains of voluntary or sensitive ideas, that no common stimuli could so far excite her attention as to disunite them; that is, the quantity of volition or of sensation already existing was greater than any, which could be produced in consequence of common degrees of stimulation. But the few stimuli of the tuberose, and of the tea, which she did perceive, were such, as accidentally coincided with the trains of thought, which were passing in her mind; and hence did not disunite those trains, and create surprise. And their being perceived at all was owing to the power of volition preceding or coinciding with that of irritation.

6. In everyday life, when we hear distant sounds or try to see things in the dark, we need to really focus our will to make our senses pick them up and block out other thoughts that could interfere with these faint signals. So in this situation, the strongest stimuli were only noticed when she actively focused on her sense organs; even then, only ordinary stimuli sometimes caught her attention. Her mind was so intensely focused on its own flow of thoughts, whether voluntary or based on sensations, that no ordinary stimuli could grab her attention enough to break that focus. In other words, the amount of willpower or sensation she was experiencing was greater than what could be triggered by typical levels of stimulation. However, the few scents of the tuberose and the tea that she did notice happened to align with the thoughts running through her mind, so they didn’t interrupt those thoughts or surprise her. The fact that she noticed them at all was due to her willpower coinciding with the sensory input.

This explication is countenanced by a fact mentioned concerning a somnambulist in the Lausanne Transactions, who sometimes opened his eyes for a short time to examine, where he was, or where his ink-pot stood, and then shut them again, dipping his pen into the pot every now and then, and writing on, but never opening his eyes afterwards, although he wrote on from line to line regularly, and corrected some errors of the pen, or in spelling: so much easier was it to him to refer to his ideas of the positions of things, than to his perceptions of them.

This explanation is supported by a fact mentioned about a sleepwalker in the Lausanne Transactions, who sometimes opened his eyes briefly to see where he was or where his inkpot was, and then closed them again, occasionally dipping his pen into the pot and continuing to write, but never opening his eyes again. He wrote line by line consistently and fixed some mistakes in his writing or spelling: it was much easier for him to rely on his mental image of where things were than on his actual perception of them.

7. The associated motions persisted in their usual channel, as appeared by the combinations of her ideas, and the use of her muscles, and the equality of her pulse; for the natural motions of the arterial system, though originally excited like other motions by stimulus, seem in part to continue by their association with each other. As the heart of a viper pulsates long after it is cut out of the body, and removed from the stimulus of the blood.

7. The related movements continued in their typical way, as shown by her thoughts, the use of her muscles, and the consistency of her heartbeat; because the natural movements of the arterial system, though initially triggered by stimuli like other movements, seem to keep going partly due to their connections with one another. Just as a viper's heart beats long after it has been removed from the body and is no longer exposed to the blood's stimulation.

8. In the section on sleep, it was observed that the nerves of sense are equally alive and susceptible to irritation in that state, as when we are awake; but that they are secluded from stimulating objects, or rendered unfit to receive them: but in complete reverie the reverse happens, the immediate organs of sense are exposed to their usual stimuli; but are either not excited into action at all, or not into so great action, as to produce attention or sensation.

8. In the section about sleep, it was noted that our senses are just as alert and responsive to irritation during this state as they are when we're awake; however, they are cut off from stimulating stimuli or unable to process them. In deep reverie, the opposite occurs: the sensory organs are exposed to their usual stimuli but are either not activated at all or not activated enough to cause attention or sensation.

The total forgetfulness of what passes in reveries; and the surprise on recovering from them, are explained in Section XVIII. 19. and in Section XVII. 3. 7.

The total forgetfulness of what passes in reveries; and the surprise on recovering from them, are explained in Section XVIII. 19. and in Section XVII. 3. 7.

9. It appears from hence, that reverie is a disease of the epileptic or cataleptic kind, since the paroxysms of this young lady always began and frequently terminated with convulsions; and though in its greatest degree it has been called somnambulation, or sleep-walking, it is totally different from sleep; because the essential character of sleep consists in the total suspension of volition, which in reverie is not affected; and the essential character of reverie consists not in the absence of those irritative motions of our senses, which are occasioned by the stimulus of external objects, but in their never being productive of sensation. So that during a fit of reverie that strange event happens to the whole system of nerves, which occurs only to some particular branches of them in those, who are a second time exposed to the action of contagious matter. If the matter of the small-pox be inserted into the arm of one, who has previously had that disease, it will stimulate the wound, but the general sensation or inflammation of the system does not follow, which constitutes the disease. See Sect. XII. 3. 6. XXXIII. 2. 8.

9. It appears from hence, that reverie is a disease of the epileptic or cataleptic kind, since the paroxysms of this young lady always began and frequently terminated with convulsions; and though in its greatest degree it has been called somnambulation, or sleep-walking, it is totally different from sleep; because the essential character of sleep consists in the total suspension of volition, which in reverie is not affected; and the essential character of reverie consists not in the absence of those irritative motions of our senses, which are occasioned by the stimulus of external objects, but in their never being productive of sensation. So that during a fit of reverie that strange event happens to the whole system of nerves, which occurs only to some particular branches of them in those, who are a second time exposed to the action of contagious matter. If the matter of the small-pox be inserted into the arm of one, who has previously had that disease, it will stimulate the wound, but the general sensation or inflammation of the system does not follow, which constitutes the disease. See Sect. XII. 3. 6. XXXIII. 2. 8.

10. The following is the definition or character of complete reverie. 1. The irritative motions occasioned by internal stimuli continue, those from the stimuli of external objects are either not produced at all, or are never succeeded by sensation or attention, unless they are at the same time excited by volition. 2. The sensitive motions continue, and are kept consistent by the power of volition. 3. The voluntary motions continue undisturbed. 4. The associate motions continue undisturbed.

10. Here’s the definition or description of complete reverie. 1. The irritating movements caused by internal stimuli persist, while those triggered by external objects are either completely absent or do not lead to any sensation or focus, unless they are also stimulated by will. 2. The responsive movements continue and remain consistent due to the power of will. 3. The voluntary movements continue without interruption. 4. The associated movements continue without interruption.

Two other cases of reverie are related in Section XXXIV. 3. which further evince, that reverie is an effort of the mind to relieve some painful sensation, and is hence allied to convulsion, and to insanity. Another case is related in Class III. 1. 2. 2.

Two other cases of reverie are related in Section XXXIV. 3. which further evince, that reverie is an effort of the mind to relieve some painful sensation, and is hence allied to convulsion, and to insanity. Another case is related in Class III. 1. 2. 2.



SECT. XX.

OF VERTIGO.

VERTIGO.

1. We determine our perpendicularity by the apparent motions of objects. A person hood-winked cannot walk in a straight line. Dizziness in looking from a tower, in a room stained with uniform lozenges, on riding over snow. 2. Dizziness from moving objects. A whirling-wheel. Fluctuations of a river. Experiment with a child. 3. Dizziness from our own motions and those of other objects. 4. Riding over a broad stream. Sea-sickness. 5. Of turning round on one foot. Dervises in Turkey. Attention of the mind prevents slight sea-sickness. After a voyage ideas of vibratory motions are still perceived on shore. 6. Ideas continue some time after they are excited. Circumstances of turning on one foot, standing on a tower, and walking in the dark, explained. 7. Irritative ideas of apparent motions. Irritative ideas of sounds. Battèment of the sound of bells and organ-pipes. Vertiginous noise in the head. Irritative motions of the stomach, intestines, and glands. 8. Symptoms that accompany vertigo. Why vomiting comes on in strokes of the palsy. By the motion of a ship. By injuries on the head. Why motion makes sick people vomit. 9. Why drunken people are vertiginous. Why a stone in the ureter, or bile-duct, produces vomiting. 10. Why after a voyage ideas of vibratory motions are perceived on shore. 11. Kinds of vertigo and their cure. 12. Definition of vertigo.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. We determine how upright we are based on the apparent movements of objects. A blindfolded person can't walk in a straight line. Feeling dizzy while looking from a tower, in a room with patterned walls, or when riding on snow. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Dizziness caused by moving objects. A spinning wheel. Waves in a river. An experiment with a child. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Dizziness from our own movements and the movements of other objects. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Riding across a wide river. Motion sickness. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Spinning around on one foot. Whirling dervishes in Turkey. Focus of the mind can lessen mild motion sickness. After a journey, sensations of movement can still be felt on land. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Thoughts linger for a while after being triggered. Explained are the effects of turning on one foot, standing in a tall place, and walking in the dark. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Stimulated ideas from apparent movements. Stimulated ideas from sounds. The rhythm of bell sounds and organ pipes. Dizziness in the head. Stimulated movements in the stomach, intestines, and glands. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Symptoms that come with dizziness. Why vomiting occurs in strokes from paralysis. Caused by the motion of a ship. Due to head injuries. Why motion causes vomiting in sick individuals. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Why drunk people experience dizziness. Why a stone in the ureter or bile duct causes vomiting. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Why after a journey, sensations of movement can be felt on land. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Different types of dizziness and their treatments. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. Definition of dizziness.

1. In learning to walk we judge of the distances of the objects, which we approach, by the eye; and by observing their perpendicularity determine our own. This circumstance not having been attended to by the writers on vision, the disease called vertigo or dizziness has been little understood.

1. When we learn to walk, we assess the distances to the objects we approach based on what we see; and by noticing their uprightness, we figure out our own balance. This aspect has been overlooked by authors discussing vision, leading to a lack of understanding about the condition known as vertigo or dizziness.

When any person loses the power of muscular action, whether he is erect or in a sitting posture, he sinks down upon the ground; as is seen in fainting fits, and other instances of great debility. Hence it follows, that some exertion of muscular power is necessary to preserve our perpendicular attitude. This is performed by proportionally exerting the antagonist muscles of the trunk, neck, and limbs; and if at any time in our locomotions we find ourselves inclining to one side, we either restore our equilibrium by the efforts of the muscles on the other side, or by moving one of our feet extend the base, which we rest upon, to the new center of gravity.

When a person loses control of their muscles, whether standing or sitting, they fall to the ground, as seen in fainting spells and other cases of extreme weakness. Therefore, it follows that some muscle activity is necessary to maintain our upright position. This is done by using opposing muscles in the trunk, neck, and limbs, and if we ever start leaning to one side while moving, we either correct our balance by engaging the muscles on the opposite side or by moving one of our feet to widen our base and align it with the new center of gravity.

But the most easy and habitual manner of determining our want of perpendicularity, is by attending to the apparent motion of the objects within the sphere of distinct vision; for this apparent motion of objects, when we incline from our perpendicularity, or begin to fall, is as much greater than the real motion of the eye, as the diameter of the sphere of distinct vision is to our perpendicular height.

But the easiest and most common way to notice if we’re not standing straight is by observing the apparent movement of objects within our clear view. This apparent motion of objects, when we tilt away from being upright or start to fall, is much greater than the actual movement of our eyes, just like the diameter of the clear view is to our height when standing straight.

Hence no one, who is hood-winked, can walk in a straight line for a hundred steps together; for he inclines so greatly, before he is warned of his want of perpendicularity by the sense of touch, not having the apparent motions of ambient objects to measure this inclination by, that he is necessitated to move one of his feet outwards, to the right or to the left, to support the new centre of gravity, and thus errs from the line he endeavours to proceed in.

Hence, no one who is blinded can walk in a straight line for a hundred steps at a time; they lean so much before they realize their lack of balance through touch, not having the visible movement of surrounding objects to judge their tilt, that they are forced to move one foot outward, either to the right or to the left, to support their new center of gravity, and in doing so, they stray from the path they are trying to follow.

For the same reason many people become dizzy, when they look from the summit of a tower, which is raised much above all other objects, as these objects are out of the sphere of distinct vision, and they are obliged to balance their bodies by the less accurate feelings of their muscles.

For the same reason many people feel dizzy when they look from the top of a tower that’s much higher than everything else, those objects fall outside their clear line of sight, and they have to stabilize their bodies using the less precise sensations from their muscles.

There is another curious phenomenon belonging to this place, if the circumjacent visible objects are so small, that we do not distinguish their minute parts; or so similar, that we do not know them from each other; we cannot determine our perpendicularity by them. Thus in a room hung with a paper, which is coloured over with similar small black lozenges or rhomboids, many people become dizzy; for when they begin to fall, the next and the next lozenge succeeds upon the eye; which they mistake for the first, and are not aware, that they have any apparent motion. But if you fix a sheet of paper, or draw any other figure, in the midst of these lozenges, the charm ceases, and no dizziness is perceptible.—The same occurs, when we ride over a plain covered with snow without trees or other eminent objects.

There’s another interesting thing about this place: if the surrounding visible objects are so small that we can’t distinguish their tiny parts, or so similar that we can’t tell them apart, we can’t figure out if we’re upright. For example, in a room decorated with wallpaper that has small black lozenges or diamonds all over it, many people feel dizzy. When they start to fall, their eyes keep seeing the next lozenge, which they mistake for the first one, and they won’t realize they’re moving at all. But if you place a sheet of paper or draw any other shape in the middle of those lozenges, the effect disappears and no dizziness is felt. The same thing happens when we ride across a flat area covered in snow without any trees or prominent objects.

2. But after having compared visible objects at rest with the sense of touch, and learnt to distinguish their shapes and shades, and to measure our want of perpendicularity by their apparent motions, we come to consider them in real motion. Here a new difficulty occurs, and we require some experience to learn the peculiar mode of motion of any moving objects, before we can make use of them for the purposes of determining our perpendicularity. Thus some people become dizzy at the sight of a whirling wheel, or by gazing on the fluctuations of a river, if no steady objects are at the same time within the sphere of their distinct vision; and when a child first can stand erect upon his legs, if you gain his attention to a white handkerchief steadily extended like a sail, and afterwards make it undulate, he instantly loses his perpendicularity, and tumbles on the ground.

2. But after comparing stationary objects with our sense of touch, and learning to identify their shapes and shades, as well as to gauge our lack of uprightness by their apparent movements, we start to think about them in actual motion. Here, a new challenge arises, and we need some experience to understand the specific way in which any moving object behaves before we can use them to determine our vertical alignment. For example, some people feel dizzy when they see a spinning wheel or watch the flow of a river, especially if there are no stable objects within their clear view at the same time; and when a child first stands upright, if you capture their attention with a white handkerchief held out like a sail and then make it wave, they quickly lose their balance and fall to the ground.

3. A second difficulty we have to encounter is to distinguish our own real movements from the apparent motions of objects. Our daily practice of walking and riding on horseback soon instructs us with accuracy to discern these modes of motion, and to ascribe the apparent motions of the ambient objects to ourselves; but those, which we have not acquired by repeated habit, continue to confound us. So as we ride on horseback the trees and cottages, which occur to us, appear at rest; we can measure their distances with our eye, and regulate our attitude by them; yet if we carelessly attend to distant hills or woods through a thin hedge, which is near us, we observe the jumping and progressive motions of them; as this is increased by the paralax of these objects; which we have not habituated ourselves to attend to. When first an European mounts an elephant sixteen feet high, and whose mode of motion he is not accustomed to, the objects seem to undulate, as he passes, and he frequently becomes sick and vertiginous, as I am well informed. Any other unusual movement of our bodies has the same effect, as riding backwards in a coach, swinging on a rope, turning round swiftly on one leg, scating on the ice, and a thousand others. So after a patient has been long confined to his bed, when he first attempts to walk, he finds himself vertiginous, and is obliged by practice to learn again the particular modes of the apparent motions of objects, as he walks by them.

3. A second challenge we face is distinguishing our actual movements from the perceived motions of surrounding objects. Our everyday experiences of walking and riding a horse quickly teach us to accurately identify these types of motion and attribute the apparent movements of nearby objects to ourselves; however, those movements we haven't learned through repeated experience continue to confuse us. For example, when we ride a horse, the trees and cottages we pass by seem stationary; we can judge their distances visually and adjust our position accordingly. But if we carelessly focus on distant hills or forests beyond a nearby hedge, we notice their jumping and shifting motions, which is amplified by the parallax of these objects that we haven't trained ourselves to recognize. When a European first climbs onto a sixteen-foot-high elephant, whose way of moving is unfamiliar, the objects around them seem to sway as they pass, and they often feel dizzy and nauseous, as I’ve been told. Any other unusual movement of our bodies can have the same effect, like riding backward in a coach, swinging on a rope, spinning quickly on one leg, skating on ice, and many others. Likewise, after a patient has been bedridden for a long time, when they first try to walk, they feel dizzy and must relearn the specific ways the motions of objects appear as they move past them.

4. A third difficulty, which occurs to us in learning to balance ourselves by the eye, is, when both ourselves and the circumjacent objects are in real motion. Here it is necessary, that we should be habituated to both these modes of motion in order to preserve our perpendicularity. Thus on horseback we accurately observe another person, whom we meet, trotting towards us, without confounding his jumping and progressive motion with our own, because we have been accustomed to them both; that is, to undergo the one, and to see the other at the same time. But in riding over a broad and fluctuating stream, though we are well experienced in the motions of our horse, we are liable to become dizzy from our inexperience in that of the water. And when first we go on ship-board, where the movements of ourselves, and the movements of the large waves are both new to us, the vertigo is almost unavoidable with the terrible sickness, which attends it. And this I have been assured has happened to several from being removed from a large ship into a small one; and again from a small one into a man of war.

4. A third difficulty we face in learning to balance ourselves by sight is when both we and the objects around us are actually moving. In this case, we need to be accustomed to both types of motion in order to stay upright. For example, when we're on horseback, we can clearly see another person approaching us on foot without mixing up their jumping and forward motion with our own, because we're used to experiencing both at the same time. However, when we ride over a wide and choppy stream, even though we’re experienced with our horse's movements, we can get dizzy from not being used to the motion of the water. And when we first go on a ship, where both our movements and the movements of the huge waves are unfamiliar, getting dizzy is almost inevitable, along with the terrible seasickness that comes with it. I've been told this has happened to several people when moving from a large ship to a smaller one, and then again from a small one to a warship.

5. From the foregoing examples it is evident, that, when we are surrounded with unusual motions, we lose our perpendicularity: but there are some peculiar circumstances attending this effect of moving objects, which we come now to mention, and shall hope from the recital of them to gain some insight into the manner of their production.

5. From the examples above, it's clear that when we're surrounded by unusual movements, we lose our sense of balance. However, there are specific factors related to this effect of moving objects that we now need to discuss, and we hope that by detailing them, we'll gain some understanding of how they occur.

When a child moves round quick upon one foot, the circumjacent objects become quite indistinct, as their distance increases their apparent motions; and this great velocity confounds both their forms, and their colours, as is seen in whirling round a many coloured wheel; he then loses his usual method of balancing himself by vision, and begins to stagger, and attempts to recover himself by his muscular feelings. This staggering adds to the instability of the visible objects by giving a vibratory motion besides their rotatory one. The child then drops upon the ground, and the neighbouring objects seem to continue for some seconds of time to circulate around him, and the earth under him appears to librate like a balance. In some seconds of time these sensations of a continuation of the motion of objects vanish; but if he continues turning round somewhat longer, before he falls, sickness and vomiting are very liable to succeed. But none of these circumstances affect those who have habituated themselves to this kind of motion, as the dervises in Turkey, amongst whom these swift gyrations are a ceremony of religion.

When a child spins quickly on one foot, the things around them become blurry because their distance makes their movements look amplified; this fast motion blurs both their shapes and colors, much like when you spin a colorful wheel. The child then loses their usual way of balancing by looking and starts to wobble, trying to regain stability using their muscles. This wobbling makes the visible objects seem even less stable, adding a shaking motion to their spinning. The child then falls to the ground, and nearby objects appear to keep spinning around them for a few seconds, while the ground beneath feels like it’s swaying like a scale. After a little while, these sensations of continuing motion fade away; however, if they keep spinning for a bit longer before falling, they are likely to feel nauseous and may vomit. But none of this bothers those who are used to this kind of motion, like the dervishes in Turkey, for whom these fast spins are part of a religious ceremony.

In an open boat passing from Leith to Kinghorn in Scotland, a sudden change of the wind shook the undistended sail, and stopt our boat; from this unusual movement the passengers all vomited except myself. I observed, that the undulation of the ship, and the instability of all visible objects, inclined me strongly to be sick; and this continued or increased, when I closed my eyes, but as often as I bent my attention with energy on the management and mechanism of the ropes and sails, the sickness ceased; and recurred again, as often as I relaxed this attention; and I am assured by a gentleman of observation and veracity, that he has more than once observed, when the vessel has been in immediate danger, that the sea-sickness of the passengers has instantaneously ceased, and recurred again, when the danger was over.

In an open boat traveling from Leith to Kinghorn in Scotland, a sudden shift in the wind jolted the sail and stopped our boat. This unexpected movement made all the passengers except me feel nauseous. I noticed that the rocking of the boat and the instability of everything around me made me feel really sick, and it got worse when I closed my eyes. However, whenever I focused intently on managing the ropes and sails, the nausea went away. Sadly, it would come back whenever I relaxed my focus. A reliable gentleman I know has told me that he has seen several times, when the boat was in immediate danger, that the passengers’ sea sickness abruptly vanished, only to return once the danger passed.

Those, who have been upon the water in a boat or ship so long, that they have acquired the necessary habits of motion upon that unstable element, at their return on land frequently think in their reveries, or between sleeping and waking, that they observe the room, they sit in, or some of its furniture, to librate like the motion of the vessel. This I have experienced myself, and have been told, that after long voyages, it is some time before these ideas entirely vanish. The same is observable in a less degree after having travelled some days in a stage coach, and particularly when we lie down in bed, and compose ourselves to sleep; in this case it is observable, that the rattling noise of the coach, as well as the undulatory motion, haunts us. The drunken vertigo, and the vulgar custom of rocking children, will be considered in the next Section.

Those who have spent a long time on the water in a boat or ship tend to develop the necessary habits to navigate that unstable surface. When they return to land, they often find themselves thinking—either in daydreams or in that space between sleeping and waking—that the room they're in or some of its furniture is swaying like the motion of the vessel. I've experienced this myself, and I've heard that after long voyages, it can take a while for these feelings to completely fade away. A similar effect, though to a lesser degree, can be observed after traveling for a few days in a stagecoach, especially when we lie down in bed and settle in to sleep; in this case, the rattling noise of the coach and the rocking motion seem to linger. The effects of drunkenness and the common practice of rocking children will be discussed in the next section.

6. The motions, which are produced by the power of volition, may be immediately stopped by the exertion of the same power on the antagonist muscles; otherwise these with all the other classes of motion continue to go on, some time after they are excited, as the palpitation of the heart continues after the object of fear, which occasioned it, is removed. But this circumstance is in no class of motions more remarkable than in those dependent on irritation; thus if any one looks at the sun, and then covers his eyes with his hand, he will for many seconds of time, perceive the image of the sun marked on his retina: a similar image of all other visible objects would remain some time formed on the retina, but is extinguished by the perpetual change of the motions of this nerve in our attention to other objects. To this must be added, that the longer time any movements have continued to be excited without fatigue to the organ, the longer will they continue spontaneously, after the excitement is withdrawn: as the taste of tobacco in the mouth after a person has been smoaking it.

6. The movements caused by our will can be instantly stopped by using that same will to engage the opposing muscles; if not, these and other types of movements continue for a while after being triggered, much like how the heart keeps racing even after the fear that caused it disappears. This is particularly true for movements caused by irritation; for instance, if someone looks at the sun and then covers their eyes with their hand, they will still see the sun's image on their retina for several seconds. A similar image of other visible objects lingers for a bit on the retina but fades due to the constant changes in the nerve activity as we focus on other things. Additionally, the longer a movement has been active without tiring out the organ, the longer it will continue on its own after the stimulation ends, like the lingering taste of tobacco after someone has smoked it.

This taste remains so strong, that if a person continues to draw air through a tobacco pipe in the dark, after having been smoking some time, he cannot distinguish whether his pipe be lighted or not.

This taste is so intense that if someone keeps inhaling through a tobacco pipe in the dark after smoking for a while, they can't tell if their pipe is lit or not.

From these two considerations it appears, that the dizziness felt in the head, after seeing objects in unusual motion, is no other than a continuation of the motions of the optic nerve excited by those objects and which engage our attention. Thus on turning round on one foot, the vertigo continues for some seconds of time after the person is fallen on the ground; and the longer he has continued to revolve, the longer will continue these successive motions of the parts of the optic nerve.

From these two points, it seems that the dizziness experienced in the head after observing moving objects is simply a continuation of the activity in the optic nerve triggered by those objects, which capture our focus. For example, when someone spins around on one foot, the spinning sensation persists for a few seconds after they fall to the ground; and the longer they keep spinning, the longer these ongoing movements in the optic nerve will last.

Additional Observations on VERTIGO.

Additional Observations on VERTIGO.

After revolving with your eyes open till you become vertiginous, as soon as you cease to revolve, not only the circum-ambient objects appear to circulate round you in a direction contrary to that, in which you have been turning, but you are liable to roll your eyes forwards and backwards; as is well observed, and ingeniously demonstrated by Dr. Wells in a late publication on vision. The same occurs, if you revolve with your eyes closed, and open them immediately at the time of your ceasing to turn; and even during the whole time of revolving, as may be felt by your hand pressed lightly on your closed eyelids. To these movements of the eyes, of which he supposes the observer to be inconscious, Dr. Wells ascribes the apparent circumgyration of objects on ceasing to revolve.

After spinning with your eyes open until you feel dizzy, when you stop, not only do the objects around you seem to swirl in the opposite direction of your spin, but your eyes can also roll back and forth. This effect is well documented and nicely illustrated by Dr. Wells in a recent publication about vision. The same effect occurs if you spin with your eyes closed and then open them immediately after stopping; you can even feel this if you gently press your hand against your closed eyelids while spinning. Dr. Wells believes that these eye movements, which the observer may not be aware of, are responsible for the appearance of objects rotating when you stop spinning.

The cause of thus turning our eyes forwards, and then back again, after our body is at rest, depends, I imagine, on the same circumstance, which induces us to follow the indistinct spectra, which are formed on one side of the center of the retina, when we observe them apparently on clouds, as described in Sect. XL. 2. 2.; and then not being able to gain a more distinct vision of them, we turn our eyes back, and again and again pursue the flying shade.

The reason our eyes move forward and then back again after we stop spinning likely relates to the same principle that causes us to follow the blurry images created on one side of the retina when we observe them seemingly on clouds, as mentioned in Sect. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Frustratingly, if we can't see them clearly, we turn our eyes back and repeatedly chase the moving shadow.

But this rolling of the eyes, after revolving till we become vertiginous, cannot cause the apparent circumgyration of objects, in a direction contrary to that in which we have been revolving, for the following reasons. 1. Because in pursuing a spectrum in the sky, or on the ground, as above mentioned, we perceive no retrograde motions of objects. 2. Because the apparent retrograde motions of objects, when we have revolved till we are vertiginous, continues much longer than the rolling of the eyes above described.

However, the rolling of the eyes after spinning can't account for the perception that objects are moving in the opposite direction for several reasons. 1. When we observe a spectrum in the sky or on the ground, as noted earlier, we don’t see any backward motion of objects. 2. The apparent backward motion of objects after spinning lasts much longer than the described eye-rolling.

3. When we have revolved from right to left, the apparent motion of objects, when we stop, is from left to right; and when we have revolved from left to right, the apparent circulation of objects is from right to left; yet in both these cases the eyes of the revolver are seen equally to roll forwards and backwards.

3. When we spin from right to left, the visible movement of objects seems to go from left to right when we stop, and when we spin from left to right, it appears as if objects are moving from right to left; yet, in both cases, the person's eyes are seen to roll back and forth.

4. Because this rolling of the eyes backwards and forwards takes place during our revolving, as may be perceived by the hand lightly pressed on the closed eyelids, and therefore exists before the effect ascribed to it.

4. Since this rolling of the eyes occurs while we are spinning, which can be detected by gently resting a hand on the closed eyelids, it takes place before the effects it’s blamed for.

And fifthly, I now come to relate an experiment, in which the rolling of the eyes does not take place at all after revolving, and yet the vertigo is more distressing than in the situations above mentioned. If any one looks steadily at a spot in the ceiling over his head, or indeed at his own finger held up high over his head, and in that situation turns round till he becomes giddy; and then stops, and looks horizontally; he now finds, that the apparent rotation of objects is from above downwards, or from below upwards; that is, that the apparent circulation of objects is now vertical instead of horizontal, making part of a circle round the axis of his eye; and this without any rolling of his eyeballs. The reason of there being no rolling of the eyeballs, perceived after this experiment, is, because the images of objects are formed in rotation round the axis of the eye, and not from one side to the other of the axis of it; so that, as the eyeball has not power to turn in its socket round its own axis, it cannot follow the apparent motions of these evanescent spectra, either before or after the body is at rest. From all which arguments it is manifest, that these apparent retrograde gyrations of objects are not caused by the rolling of the eyeballs; first, because no apparent retrogression of objects is observed in other rollings of the eyes: secondly, because the apparent retrogression of objects continues many seconds after the rolling of the eyeballs ceases. Thirdly, because the apparent retrogression of objects is sometimes one way, and sometimes another, yet the rolling of the eyeballs is the same. Fourthly, because the rolling of the eyeballs exists before the apparent retrograde motions of objects is observed; that is, before the revolving person stops. And fifthly, because the apparent retrograde gyration of objects is produced, when there is no rolling of the eyeballs at all.

Lastly, I will present an experiment where the eyes don’t roll at all after spinning, and yet the dizziness is more intense than previously mentioned. If someone focuses on a spot on the ceiling or their finger held above them while spinning until they feel dizzy, then stops and looks horizontally, they will notice that surrounding objects seem to move from above downwards or from below upwards; in other words, the movement appears vertical rather than horizontal, forming part of a circle around the axis of their eye. This occurs without any rolling of the eyeballs. The absence of rolling is because the images of objects rotate around the axis of the eye instead of shifting from one side to the other; since the eyeball cannot rotate in its socket around its own axis, it cannot keep up with the illusory movements of these fleeting images, whether before or after the body is at rest. All these points suggest that the perceived backward rotations of objects are not caused by the rolling of the eyeballs; first, because no backward motion of objects is observed with other eye movements; second, because the apparent backward motion of objects persists for several seconds after the eyeball movement stops. Third, because the perceived backward motion can alternate directions, yet the rolling of the eyeballs does not change. Fourth, because the rolling of the eyeballs happens prior to the perceived backward movements of objects being observed; that is, before the person spinning comes to a stop. And fifth, because the illusion of backward rotation of objects can happen without any rolling of the eyeballs at all.

Doctor Wells imagines, that no spectra can be gained in the eye, if a person revolves with his eyelids closed, and thinks this a sufficient argument against the opinion, that the apparent progression of the spectra of light or colours in the eye can cause the apparent retrogression of objects in the vertigo above described; but it is certain, when any person revolves in a light room with his eyes closed, that he nevertheless perceives differences of light both in quantity and colour through his eyelids, as he turns round; and readily gains spectra of those differences. And these spectra are not very different except in vivacity from those, which he acquires, when he revolves with unclosed eyes, since if he then revolves very rapidly the colours and forms of surrounding objects are as it were mixed together in his eye;. as when, the prismatic colours are painted on a wheel, they appear white as they revolve. The truth of this is evinced by the staggering or vertigo of men perfectly blind, when they turn round; which is not attended with apparent circulation of objects, but is a vertiginous disorder of the sense of touch. Blind men balance themselves by their sense of touch; which, being less adapted for perceiving small deviations from their perpendicular, occasions them to carry themselves more erect in walking. This method of balancing themselves by the direction of their pressure against the floor, becomes disordered by the unusual mode of action in turning round, and they begin to lose their perpendicularity, that is, they become vertiginous; but without any apparent circular motions of visible objects.

Dr. Wells argues that no visual effects can be seen with closed eyelids while a person spins, considering this a strong argument against the notion that visible progression of light or colors in the eye can lead to the perceived backward motion of objects during the vertigo he described. However, it's clear that when someone spins in a well-lit room with their eyes closed, they still notice changes in light brightness and color through their eyelids as they turn, and they easily create visual effects from those differences. These effects are similar, except in intensity, to what they experience when spinning with their eyes open; when spinning quickly, the colors and shapes of nearby objects seem to blend together in their vision, much like how the colors of a prism appear white on a spinning wheel. This is further illustrated by the dizziness experienced by completely blind individuals when they spin; their experience does not include the apparent motion of objects but instead involves a disorienting sensation related to touch. Blind individuals depend on their sense of touch for balance, which is less effective for detecting minor shifts from their upright position, causing them to stand straighter while walking. This method of balancing through the pressure they apply against the ground becomes disrupted during spinning, making it harder for them to maintain balance, resulting in vertigo even without visible objects moving around them.

It will appear from the following experiments, that the apparent progression of the ocular spectra of light or colours is the cause of the apparent retrogression of objects, after a person has revolved, till he is vertiginous.

It will become evident from the following experiments that the visible shift in light spectrums or colors leads to the perceived backward motion of objects after someone has spun until they feel dizzy.

First, when a person turns round in a light room with his eyes open, but closes them before he stops, he will seem to be carried forwards in the direction he was turning for a short time after he stops. But if he opens his eyes again, the objects before him instantly appear to move in a retrograde direction, and he loses the sensation of being carried forwards. The same occurs if a person revolves in a light room with his eyes closed; when he stops, he seems to be for a time carried forwards, if his eyes are still closed; but the instant he opens them, the surrounding objects appear to move in retrograde gyration. From hence it may be concluded, that it is the sensation or imagination of our continuing to go forwards in the direction in which we were turning, that causes the apparent retrograde circulation of objects.

First, when someone spins in a well-lit room with their eyes open but closes them before they stop, they will feel as if they’re being pushed forward in the direction they were turning for a brief time after stopping. However, as soon as they open their eyes, the objects in front of them appear to move backward, and the sensation of being pushed forward disappears. The same effect occurs if someone spins in a light room with closed eyes; when they stop, they feel like they're still moving forward with their eyes closed, but the moment they open them, the surrounding objects seem to swirl backward. From this, we can conclude that the sensation or idea of moving forward in the direction they were turning creates the illusion of objects moving backward.

Secondly, though there is an audible vertigo, as is known by the battement, or undulations of sound in the ears, which many vertiginous people experience; and though there is also a tangible vertigo, as when a blind person turns round, as mentioned above; yet as this circumgyration of objects is an hallucination or deception of the sense of sight, we are to look for the cause of our appearing to move forward, when we stop with our eyes closed after gyration, to some affection of this sense. Now, thirdly, if the spectra formed in the eye during our rotation, continue to change, when we stand still, like the spectra described in Sect. III. 3. 6. such changes must suggest to us the idea or sensation of our still continuing to turn round; as is the case, when we revolve in a light room, and close our eyes before we stop. And lastly, on opening our eyes in the situation above described, the objects we chance to view amid these changing spectra in the eye, must seem to move in a contrary direction; as the moon sometimes appears to move retrograde, when swift-gliding clouds are passing forwards so much nearer the eye of the beholder.

Secondly, even though there's an audible vertigo, as known from the battement or undulations of sound in the ears, which many people experience during vertigo; and although there's also a tangible vertigo, like when a blind person spins, as mentioned earlier; since this apparent rotation of objects is a hallucination or visual trick, we should seek the cause of the sensation of continuing to move forward after stopping with our eyes closed due to some effect on our vision. Now, thirdly, if the spectrums formed in the eye during our rotation continue to change when we stand still, as described in Sect. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__, such changes must suggest to us the idea or sensation of still rotating; as is the case when we rotate in a well-lit room and close our eyes before stopping. And finally, upon opening our eyes in the situation above, the objects we happen to view among these changing spectrums in the eye must appear to move in the opposite direction; much like how the moon sometimes seems to move backward when fast-moving clouds pass forward, much closer to the observer.

To make observations on faint ocular spectra requires some degree of habit, and composure of mind, and even patience; some of those described in Sect. XL. were found difficult to see, by many, who tried them; now it happens, that the mind, during the confusion of vertigo, when all the other irritative tribes of motion, as well as those of vision, are in some degree disturbed, together with the fear of falling, is in a very unfit state for the contemplation of such weak sensations, as are occasioned by faint ocular spectra. Yet after frequently revolving, both with my eyes closed, and with them open, and attending to the spectra remaining in them, by shading the light from my eyelids more or less with my hand, I at length ceased to have the idea of going forward, after I stopped with my eyes closed; and saw changing spectra in my eyes, which seemed to move, as it were, over the field of vision; till at length, by repeated trials on sunny days, I persuaded myself, on opening my eyes, after revolving some time, on a shelf of gilded books in my library, that I could perceive the spectra in my eyes move forwards over one or two of the books, like the vapours in the air of a summer's day; and could so far undeceive myself, as to perceive the books to stand still. After more trials I sometimes brought myself to believe, that I saw changing spectra of lights and shades moving in my eyes, after turning round for some time, but did not imagine either the spectra or the objects to be in a state of gyration. I speak, however, with diffidence of these facts, as I could not always make the experiments succeed, when there was not a strong light in my room, or when my eyes were not in the most proper state for such observations.

Making observations on faint ocular spectrums requires some level of practice, mental focus, and even patience; some of those described in Sect. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__ were difficult for many who attempted to see them. However, it seems that the mind, during the confusion of vertigo, when all other types of motion sensations, including those of vision, are somewhat disturbed, alongside the fear of falling, is in a very poor state for contemplating such weak sensations caused by faint ocular spectrums. Yet after frequently spinning, both with my eyes closed and open, and paying attention to the spectra remaining in my eyes by adjusting the light shade with my hand, I eventually stopped perceiving the sensation of moving forward after halting with my eyes closed, and began seeing changing spectrums in my eyes that seemed to move across my field of vision; until ultimately, through repeated trials on sunny days, I convinced myself that by opening my eyes after spinning for a while and focusing on a shelf of gilded books in my library, I could observe the spectra in my eyes moving over one or two of the books, like the vapors in the air on a summer's day; and I could even deceive myself into perceiving the books as standing still. With more practice, I sometimes convinced myself that I saw changing spectra of light and shade moving in my eyes after spinning for some time, but I did not imagine that either the spectra or the objects were in a state of rotation. However, I discuss these observations with caution, as I could not always make the experiments succeed when there was not strong light in my room or when my eyes were not in the right state for such observations.

The ingenious and learned M. Sauvage has mentioned other theories to account for the apparent circumgyration of objects in vertiginous people. As the retrograde motions of the particles of blood in the optic arteries, by spasm, or by fear, as is seen in the tails of tadpoles, and membranes between the fingers of frogs. Another cause he thinks may be from the librations to one side, and to the other, of the crystalline lens in the eye, by means of involuntary actions of the muscles, which constitute the ciliary process. Both these theories lie under the same objection as that of Dr. Wells before mentioned; namely, that the apparent motions of objects, after the observer has revolved for some time, should appear to vibrate this way and that; and not to circulate uniformly in a direction contrary to that, in which the observer had revolved.

The astute and knowledgeable M. Sauvage has put forward other theories to explain the perceived circling of objects for dizzy individuals. One theory suggests it's due to the backward movement of blood particles in the optic arteries, triggered by spasms or fear, similar to phenomena observed in tadpole tails and the membranes between frog fingers. Another idea he proposes is that it might stem from the side-to-side movements of the crystalline lens in the eye, resulting from involuntary muscle actions involved in the ciliary process. Both of these theories encounter the same criticism as Dr. Wells' earlier idea; namely, that the perceived movements of objects after the observer has been spinning should appear to shake back and forth rather than move uniformly in the opposite direction of the observer's rotation.

M. Sauvage has, lastly, mentioned the theory of colours left in the eye, which he has termed impressions on the retina. He says, "Experience teaches us, that impressions made on the retina by a visible object remain some seconds after the object is removed; as appears from the circle of fire which we see, when a fire-stick is whirled round in the dark; therefore when we are carried round our own axis in a circle, we undergo a temporary vertigo, when we stop; because the impressions of the circumjacent objects remain for a time afterwards on the retina." Nosolog. Method. Clas. VIII. I. 1. We have before observed, that the changes of these colours remaining in the eye, evinces them to be motions of the fine terminations of the retina, and not impressions on it; as impressions on a passive substance must either remain, or cease intirely. See an additional note at the end of the second volume.

M. Sauvage finally discusses the theory concerning colors that linger in the eye, which he refers to as impressions on the retina. He states, "Experience shows us that impressions left on the retina by a visible object persist for a few seconds after the object is gone; this can be observed in the circle of fire we notice when a fire-stick is spun around in the dark. Thus, when we spin around our own axis in a circle, we experience a temporary dizziness upon stopping, because the impressions of surrounding objects linger on the retina for a while." Nosolog. Method. Clas. VIII. I. 1. We have previously noted that changes in these lingering colors in the eye indicate movements of the delicate ends of the retina, not mere impressions; as impressions on a passive surface must either remain or disappear entirely. See an additional note at the end of the second volume.

Any one, who stands alone on the top of a high tower, if he has not been accustomed to balance himself by objects placed at such distances and with such inclinations, begins to stagger, and endeavours to recover himself by his muscular feelings. During this time the apparent motion of objects at a distance below him is very great, and the spectra of these apparent motions continue a little time after he has experienced them; and he is persuaded to incline the contrary way to counteract their effects; and either immediately falls, or applying his hands to the building, uses his muscular feelings to preserve his perpendicular attitude, contrary to the erroneous persuasions of his eyes. Whilst the person, who walks in the dark, staggers, but without dizziness; for he neither has the sensation of moving objects to take off his attention from his muscular feelings, nor has he the spectra of those motions continued on his retina to add to his confusion. It happens indeed sometimes to one landing on a tower, that the idea of his not having room to extend his base by moving one of his feet outwards, when he begins to incline, superadds fears to his other inconveniences; which like surprise, joy, or any great degree of sensation, enervates him in a moment, by employing the whole sensorial power, and by thus breaking all the associated trains and tribes of motion.

Anyone who stands alone on top of a tall tower, if they're not used to balancing themselves by focusing on objects that are far away and at various angles, starts to feel unsteady and tries to regain their balance using their muscles. During this time, the apparent movement of things below looks intense, and the afterimages of these motions linger for a bit after they've experienced them; this makes them want to lean the opposite way to counteract those effects. They either immediately fall or, by putting their hands on the building, rely on their muscle feelings to keep upright, going against the misleading signals from their eyes. Meanwhile, a person walking in the dark may stumble but without feeling dizzy; they don't have the sensation of moving objects to distract them from their muscle feelings, nor do they have the afterimages of those motions lingering in their vision to add to their confusion. Sometimes, when someone is stepping onto a tower, the thought that they can't move one of their feet outward to establish a wider base when they start to lean adds to their anxiety on top of their other troubles; this surprise, joy, or any intense feeling can weaken them instantly by using up all their mental energy and thus breaking all the connected thoughts and movements.

7. The irritative ideas of objects, whilst we are awake, are perpetually present to our sense of sight; as we view the furniture of our rooms, or the ground, we tread upon, throughout the whole day without attending to it. And as our bodies are never at perfect rest during our waking hours, these irritative ideas of objects are attended perpetually with irritative ideas of their apparent motions. The ideas of apparent motions are always irritative ideas, because we never attend to them, whether we attend to the objects themselves, or to their real motions, or to neither. Hence the ideas of the apparent motions of objects are a complete circle of irritative ideas, which continue throughout the day.

7. The annoying thoughts of objects are constantly in front of our eyes while we’re awake; as we look at the furniture in our rooms or the ground we walk on throughout the day, we hardly notice them. Since our bodies are never completely still during our waking hours, these annoying thoughts about objects are always accompanied by annoying thoughts about their perceived movements. The thoughts of these perceived movements are always bothersome because we seldom pay attention to them, whether we focus on the objects themselves, their actual movements, or neither. Therefore, the thoughts about the perceived movements of objects create a continuous cycle of irritating ideas that lasts the entire day.

Also during all our waking hours, there is a perpetual confused sound of various bodies, as of the wind in our rooms, the fire, distant conversations, mechanic business; this continued buzz, as we are seldom quite motionless, changes its loudness perpetually, like the sound of a bell; which rises and falls as long as it continues, and seems to pulsate on the ear. This any one may experience by turning himself round near a waterfall; or by striking a glass bell, and then moving the direction of its mouth towards the ears, or from them, as long as its vibrations continue. Hence this undulation of indistinct sound makes another concomitant circle of irritative ideas, which continues throughout the day.

Also, throughout all our waking hours, there's a constant, confused sound from various sources, like the wind in our rooms, the fire, distant conversations, and everyday activities. This ongoing buzz, since we're rarely completely still, changes in loudness all the time, similar to the sound of a bell that rises and falls as it rings, creating a pulsing effect on the ear. Anyone can notice this by standing near a waterfall or by striking a glass bell and then moving the opening toward or away from their ears while the vibrations last. So, this wave of vague sound creates another ongoing circle of stimulating thoughts that lasts throughout the day.

We hear this undulating sound, when we are perfectly at rest ourselves, from other sonorous bodies besides bells; as from two organ-pipes, which are nearly but not quite in unison, when they are sounded together. When a bell is struck, the circular form is changed into an eliptic one; the longest axis of which, as the vibrations continue, moves round the periphery of the bell; and when either axis of this elipse is pointed towards our ears, the sound is louder; and less when the intermediate parts of the elipse are opposite to us. The vibrations of the two organ-pipes may be compared to Nonius's rule; the sound is louder, when they coincide, and less at the intermediate times. But, as the sound of bells is the most familiar of those sounds, which have a considerable battement, the vertiginous patients, who attend to the irritative circles of sounds above described, generally compare it to the noise of bells.

We hear this wave-like sound when we’re completely still ourselves, not just from bells but also from other resonant objects, like two organ pipes that are almost but not quite in tune when played together. When a bell rings, its circular shape shifts to an elliptical one; the longest part of this ellipse moves around the edge of the bell as the vibrations continue. When either end of this ellipse faces our ears, the sound is louder; it’s quieter when the middle parts of the ellipse are pointed at us. The vibrations of the two organ pipes can be compared to Nonius’s rule: the sound is louder when they align, and softer at the times in between. However, since the sound of bells is the most familiar among those sounds that have a noticeable beating, people with vertigo who pay attention to the irritating circles of sound often liken it to the noise of bells.

The peristaltic motions of our stomach and intestines, and the secretions of the various glands, are other circles of irritative motions, some of them more or less complete, according to our abstinence or satiety.

The rhythmic movements of our stomach and intestines, along with the secretions from different glands, are other cycles of stimulating motions, some of which are more or less complete depending on whether we're hungry or full.

So that the irritative ideas of the apparent motions of objects, the irritative battements of sounds, and the movements of our bowels and glands compose a great circle of irritative tribes of motion: and when one considerable part of this circle of motions becomes interrupted, the whole proceeds in confusion, as described in Section XVII. 1. 7. on Catenation of Motions.

So that the irritative ideas of the apparent motions of objects, the irritative battements of sounds, and the movements of our bowels and glands compose a great circle of irritative tribes of motion: and when one considerable part of this circle of motions becomes interrupted, the whole proceeds in confusion, as described in Section XVII. 1. 7. on Catenation of Motions.

8. Hence a violent vertigo, from whatever cause it happens, is generally attended with undulating noise in the head, perversions of the motions of the stomach and duodenum, unusual excretion of bile and gastric juice, with much pale urine, sometimes with yellowness of the skin, and a disordered secretion of almost every gland of the body, till at length the arterial system is affected, and fever succeeds.

8. So, a severe dizziness, no matter what causes it, usually comes with a swaying noise in the head, disruptions in the movement of the stomach and small intestine, unusual bile and stomach acid production, lots of pale urine, occasionally with yellowing of the skin, and an unregulated secretion from almost every gland in the body, until eventually the arterial system is impacted, leading to a fever.

Thus bilious vomitings accompany the vertigo occasioned by the motion of a ship; and when the brain is rendered vertiginous by a paralytic affection of any part of the body, a vomiting generally ensues, and a great discharge of bile: and hence great injuries of the head from external violence are succeeded with bilious vomitings, and sometimes with abscesses of the liver. And hence, when a patient is inclined to vomit from other causes, as in some fevers, any motions of the attendants in his room, or of himself when he is raised or turned in his bed, presently induces the vomiting by superadding a degree of vertigo.

So, nausea and vomiting often come with the dizziness caused by the movement of a ship; and when the brain feels dizzy due to a paralysis affecting any part of the body, vomiting usually follows along with a significant release of bile. As a result, serious head injuries from external impacts are often accompanied by bile-related vomiting and sometimes liver abscesses. Therefore, when a patient feels like vomiting for other reasons, such as during certain fevers, any movement from the people in the room or themselves when they are lifted or turned in bed can quickly trigger vomiting by adding to the dizziness.

9. And conversely it is very usual with those, whose stomachs are affected from internal causes, to be afflicted with vertigo, and noise in the head; such is the vertigo of drunken people, which continues, when their eyes are closed, and themselves in a recumbent posture, as well as when they are in an erect posture, and have their eyes open. And thus the irritation of a stone in the bile-duct, or in the ureter, or an inflammation of any of the intestines, are accompanied with vomitings and vertigo.

9. Similarly, it's quite common for people whose stomachs are affected by internal issues to experience dizziness and ringing in the ears; this is similar to the dizziness experienced by intoxicated individuals, which persists whether their eyes are closed or they are lying down, as well as when they are standing up with their eyes open. Therefore, irritation from a stone in the bile duct or ureter, or inflammation in any part of the intestines, is often accompanied by vomiting and dizziness.

In these cases the irritative motions of the stomach, which are in general not attended to, become so changed by some unnatural stimulus, as to become uneasy, and excite our sensation or attention. And thus the other irritative trains of motions, which are associated with it, become disordered by their sympathy. The same happens, when a piece of gravel sticks in the ureter, or when some part of the intestinal canal becomes inflamed. In these cases the irritative muscular motions are first disturbed by unusual stimulus, and a disordered action of the sensual motions, or dizziness ensues. While in sea-sickness the irritative sensual motions, as vertigo, precedes; and the disordered irritative muscular motions, as those of the stomach in vomiting, follow.

In these situations, the irritating movements of the stomach, which are usually overlooked, get altered by some unnatural stimulus, making them uncomfortable and capturing our attention. As a result, the other corresponding movements that are connected to it become disorganized due to their relationship. The same thing occurs when a small stone gets stuck in the ureter or when part of the digestive tract becomes inflamed. In these instances, the irritating muscular movements are initially disturbed by an unusual stimulus, leading to a disordered response in our sensory movements or dizziness. However, in the case of seasickness, the irritating sensory movements, like vertigo, occur first, followed by the disordered muscular movements in the stomach that cause vomiting.

10. When these irritative motions are disturbed, if the degree be not very great, the exertion of voluntary attention to any other object, or any sudden sensation, will disjoin these new habits of motion. Thus some drunken people have become sober immediately, when any accident has strongly excited their attention; and sea-sickness has vanished, when the ship has been in danger. Hence when our attention to other objects is most relaxed, as just before we fall asleep, or between our reveries when awake, these irritative ideas of motion and sound are most liable to be perceived; as those, who have been at sea, or have travelled long in a coach, seem to perceive the vibrations of the ship, or the rattling of the wheels, at these intervals; which cease again, as soon as they exert their attention. That is, at those intervals they attend to the apparent motions, and to the battement of sounds of the bodies around them, and for a moment mistake them for those real motions of the ship, and noise of wheels, which they had lately been accustomed to: or at these intervals of reverie, or on the approach of sleep, these supposed motions or sounds may be produced entirely by imagination.

10. When these irritating movements are disrupted, if it's not too severe, focusing our attention on something else or experiencing a sudden sensation can break these new habits of movement. For example, some drunken individuals have become sober instantly when something has caught their attention strongly; similarly, seasickness has disappeared when the ship was in danger. Therefore, when our attention to other things is at its lowest, like just before we fall asleep or during daydreams while awake, we are more likely to notice these irritating ideas of motion and sound. People who have been at sea or traveled long distances in a coach tend to notice the vibrations of the ship or the rattling wheels during these moments; however, this sensation goes away as soon as they focus their attention. In those intervals, they pay attention to the perceived movements and sounds around them, momentarily mistaking them for the actual movements of the ship and the noise of the wheels they were used to. Alternatively, during these moments of daydreaming or as they approach sleep, these perceived movements or sounds might be entirely products of their imagination.

We may conclude from this account of vertigo, that sea-sickness is not an effort of nature to relieve herself, but a necessary consequence of the associations or catenations of animal motions. And may thence infer, that the vomiting, which attends the gravel in the ureter, inflammations of the bowels, and the commencement of some fevers, has a similar origin, and is not always an effort of the vis medicatrix naturæ. But where the action of the organ is the immediate consequence of the stimulating cause, it is frequently exerted to dislodge that stimulus, as in vomiting up an emetic drug; at other times, the action of an organ is a general effort to relieve pain, as in convulsions of the locomotive muscles; other actions drink up and carry on the fluids, as in absorption and secretion; all which may be termed efforts of nature to relieve, or to preserve herself.

We can conclude from this description of vertigo that sea sickness isn't just nature's way of coping but rather a necessary result of how our bodies react to movement. From this, we can also infer that vomiting, which happens with kidney stones, inflammation of the intestines, and the onset of some fevers, has a similar cause and isn't always just a natural healing process. However, when the organ's action directly results from a stimulating cause, it often works to remove that stimulus, like when someone vomits after taking an emetic. At other times, the organ's action is a general response to relieve pain, like in muscle spasms; other mechanisms help move and process fluids, like absorption and secretion; all of which can be seen as nature's efforts to heal or protect itself.

11. The cure of vertigo will frequently depend on our previously investigating the cause of it, which from what has been delivered above may originate from the disorder of any part of the great tribes of irritative motions, and of the associate motions catenated with them.

11. Treating vertigo often relies on our prior investigation into its cause, which, as mentioned earlier, may stem from issues in any part of the major systems of irritative movements and the associated motions connected to them.

Many people, when they arrive at fifty or sixty years of age, are affected with slight vertigo; which is generally but wrongly ascribed to indigestion, but in reality arises from a beginning defect of their sight; as about this time they also find it necessary to begin to use spectacles, when they read small prints, especially in winter, or by candle light, but are yet able to read without them during the summer days, when the light is stronger. These people do not see objects so distinctly as formerly, and by exerting their eyes more than usual, they perceive the apparent motions of objects, and confound them with the real motions of them; and therefore cannot accurately balance themselves so as easily to preserve their perpendicularity by them.

Many people, when they reach their fifties or sixties, experience mild dizziness, which is often mistakenly attributed to indigestion. In reality, it stems from early issues with their eyesight. Around this time, they notice they need to start wearing glasses for reading small print, especially in winter or by candlelight. However, they can still read without them during the longer summer days when the light is better. These individuals don't see things as clearly as they used to, and by straining their eyes more than normal, they start to notice objects moving in a way that confuses them with actual motion. As a result, they struggle to balance themselves properly and maintain their upright position.

That is, the apparent motions of objects, which are at rest, as we move by them, should only excite irritative ideas: but as these are now become less distinct, owing to the beginning imperfection of our sight, we are induced voluntarily to attend to them; and then these apparent motions become succeeded by sensation; and thus the other parts of the trains of irritative ideas, or irritative muscular motions, become disordered, as explained above. In these cases of slight vertigo I have always promised my patients, that they would get free from it in two or three months, as they should acquire the habit of balancing their bodies by less distinct objects, and have seldom been mistaken in my prognostic.

That is, the way we see objects that are stationary as we pass by them should only trigger minor reactions in our minds. However, since these reactions are becoming less clear due to the gradual decline in our vision, we tend to pay more attention to them voluntarily. This attention leads to actual sensations following those apparent motions, causing other related thoughts or muscle movements to become disordered, as mentioned earlier. In these cases of mild dizziness, I've always assured my patients that they would overcome it in two to three months as they learn to balance their bodies using less clear objects, and I’ve rarely been wrong in that prediction.

There is an auditory vertigo, which is called a noise in the head, explained in No. 7. of this section, which also is very liable to affect people in the advance of life, and is owing to their hearing less perfectly than before. This is sometimes called a ringing, and sometimes a singing, or buzzing, in the ears, and is occasioned by our first experiencing a disagreeable sensation from our not being able distinctly to hear the sounds, we used formerly to hear distinctly. And this disagreeable sensation excites desire and consequent volition; and when we voluntarily attend to small indistinct sounds, even the whispering of the air in a room, and the pulsations of the arteries of the ear are succeeded by sensation; which minute sounds ought only to have produced irritative sensual motions, or unperceived ideas. See Section XVII. 3. 6. These patients after a while lose this auditory vertigo, by acquiring a new habit of not attending voluntarily to these indistinct sounds, but contenting themselves with the less accuracy of their sense of hearing.

There is an auditory vertigo, which is called a noise in the head, explained in No. 7. of this section, which also is very liable to affect people in the advance of life, and is owing to their hearing less perfectly than before. This is sometimes called a ringing, and sometimes a singing, or buzzing, in the ears, and is occasioned by our first experiencing a disagreeable sensation from our not being able distinctly to hear the sounds, we used formerly to hear distinctly. And this disagreeable sensation excites desire and consequent volition; and when we voluntarily attend to small indistinct sounds, even the whispering of the air in a room, and the pulsations of the arteries of the ear are succeeded by sensation; which minute sounds ought only to have produced irritative sensual motions, or unperceived ideas. See Section XVII. 3. 6. These patients after a while lose this auditory vertigo, by acquiring a new habit of not attending voluntarily to these indistinct sounds, but contenting themselves with the less accuracy of their sense of hearing.

Another kind of vertigo begins with the disordered action of some irritative muscular motions, as those of the stomach from intoxication, or from emetics; or those of the ureter, from the stimulus of a stone lodged in it; and it is probable, that the disordered motions of some of the great congeries of glands, as of those which form the liver, or of the intestinal canal, may occasion vertigo in consequence of their motions being associated or catenated with the great circles of irritative motions; and from hence it appears, that the means of cure must be adapted to the cause.

Another type of vertigo starts with the irregular actions of some irritating muscle movements, like those in the stomach caused by intoxication or by vomiting; or those in the ureter due to the pressure of a stone lodged there. It’s likely that the irregular movements of some large groups of glands, such as those in the liver or the digestive tract, can trigger vertigo because their motions are linked to the major circles of irritating movements. Thus, it seems that the treatment must be tailored to the underlying cause.

To prevent sea-sickness it is probable, that the habit of swinging for a week or two before going on shipboard might be of service. For the vertigo from failure of sight, spectacles may be used. For the auditory vertigo, æther may be dropt into the ear to stimulate the part, or to dissolve ear-wax, if such be a part of the cause. For the vertigo arising from indigestion, the peruvian bark and a blister are recommended. And for that owing to a stone in the ureter, venesection, cathartics, opiates, sal soda aerated.

To avoid sea sickness, it might help to swing for a week or two before getting on a ship. For dizziness caused by vision issues, you can use glasses. For dizziness related to hearing, you can drop ether in the ear to stimulate the area or to clear out earwax if that's part of the problem. For dizziness from indigestion, Peruvian bark and a blister are suggested. And for dizziness due to a stone in the ureter, treatments include bloodletting, laxatives, opiates, and carbonated soda.

12. Definition of vertigo. 1. Some of the irritative sensual, or muscular motions, which were usually not succeeded by sensation, are in this disease succeeded by sensation; and the trains or circles of motions, which were usually catenated with them, are interrupted, or inverted, or proceed in confusion. 2. The sensitive and voluntary motions continue undisturbed. 3. The associate trains or circles of motions continue; but their catenations with some of the irritative motions are disordered, or inverted, or dissevered.

12. Definition of vertigo. 1. In this condition, some of the irritating sensory or muscular movements that normally don't result in any sensation are followed by sensation; and the usual sequences or patterns of these movements are interrupted, reversed, or confused. 2. The sensory and voluntary movements remain unaffected. 3. The associated sequences or patterns of movements continue, but their connections with some of the irritating movements are disrupted, reversed, or separated.



SECT. XXI.

OF DRUNKENNESS.

OF DRUNKENNESS.

1. Sleep from satiety of hunger. From rocking children. From uniform sounds. 2. Intoxication from common food after fatigue and inanition. 3. From wine or of opium. Chilness after meals. Vertigo. Why pleasure is produced by intoxication, and by swinging and rocking children. And why pain is relieved by it. 4. Why drunkards stagger and stammer, and are liable to weep. 5. And become delirious, sleepy, and stupid. 6. Or make pale urine and vomit. 7. Objects are seen double. 8. Attention of the mind diminishes drunkenness. 9. Disordered irritative motions of all the senses. 10. Diseases from drunkenness. 11. Definition of drunkenness.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Sleep from being full. From rocking children. From steady sounds. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Intoxication from regular food after being tired and weak. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. From wine or opium. Coldness after meals. Dizziness. Why pleasure comes from intoxication, and from swinging and rocking children. And why it alleviates pain. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Why drunk people stumble and slur their words, and are prone to crying. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. And become delirious, sleepy, and dull. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Or produce pale urine and vomit. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Seeing objects as double. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Mental focus reduces the effects of drunkenness. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Disordered, irritating sensations of all the senses. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Health issues caused by drunkenness. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Definition of drunkenness.

1. In the state of nature when the sense of hunger is appeased by the stimulus of agreeable food, the business of the day is over, and the human savage is at peace with the world, he then exerts little attention to external objects, pleasing reveries of imagination succeed, and at length sleep is the result: till the nourishment which he has procured, is carried over every part of the system to repair the injuries of action, and he awakens with fresh vigour, and feels a renewal of his sense of hunger.

1. In a natural state, when hunger is satisfied by tasty food, the day's activities come to a close, and the human primitive is at peace with the world. At this point, he pays little attention to the outside world; pleasant daydreams take over, and eventually, sleep follows. This sleep allows the nourishment he's gathered to spread throughout his body, repairing the wear and tear from the day’s actions, and he wakes up feeling refreshed, ready to face hunger once more.

The juices of some bitter vegetables, as of the poppy and the laurocerasus, and the ardent spirit produced in the fermentation of the sugar found in vegetable juices, are so agreeable to the nerves of the stomach, that, taken in a small quantity, they instantly pacify the sense of hunger; and the inattention to external stimuli with the reveries of imagination, and sleep, succeeds, in the same manner as when the stomach is filled with other less intoxicating food.

The juices from some bitter vegetables, like poppy and laurocerasus, and the strong alcohol made from fermenting the sugar in these vegetable juices are so pleasant to the stomach's nerves that just a small amount can quickly satisfy hunger. This leads to a disregard for outside distractions, along with daydreaming and sleep, similar to what happens when the stomach is filled with other, less intoxicating foods.

This inattention to the irritative motions occasioned by external stimuli is a very important circumstance in the approach of sleep, and is produced in young children by rocking their cradles: during which all visible objects become indistinct to them. An uniform soft repeated sound, as the murmurs of a gentle current, or of bees, are said to produce the same effect, by presenting indistinct ideas of inconsequential sounds, and by thus stealing our attention from other objects, whilst by their continued reiterations they become familiar themselves, and we cease gradually to attend to any thing, and sleep ensues.

This lack of focus on annoying movements caused by outside stimuli is a key factor in falling asleep, and it can be triggered in young children by rocking their cradles. During this time, everything they see becomes fuzzy to them. A steady, soft, repeated sound, like the murmur of a gentle stream or the buzzing of bees, is known to have the same effect by presenting vague sounds that don’t matter, which distracts us from other things. As these sounds continue, they become familiar, and we gradually stop paying attention to anything else, leading to sleep.

2. After great fatigue or inanition, when the stomach is suddenly filled with flesh and vegetable food, the inattention to external stimuli, and the reveries of imagination, become so conspicuous as to amount to a degree of intoxication. The same is at any time produced by superadding a little wine or opium to our common meals; or by taking these separately in considerable quantity; and this more efficaciously after fatigue or inanition; because a less quantity of any stimulating material will excite an organ into energetic action, after it has lately been torpid from defect of stimulus; as objects appear more luminous, after we have been in the dark; and because the suspension of volition, which is the immediate cause of sleep, is sooner induced, after a continued voluntary exertion has in part exhausted the sensorial power of volition; in the same manner as we cannot contract a single muscle long together without intervals of inaction.

2. After a lot of exhaustion or hunger, when the stomach is suddenly filled with meat and vegetables, the lack of attention to outside stimuli and the daydreaming become so noticeable that it feels a bit like being intoxicated. The same happens when we add a bit of wine or opium to our regular meals, or when we take them separately in larger amounts; this effect is stronger after fatigue or hunger because a smaller amount of any stimulating substance will kickstart an organ into active work after it’s been sluggish due to lack of stimulus. It’s similar to how things seem brighter after being in the dark, and how the pause of willpower, which directly leads to sleep, happens more quickly after extended periods of voluntary effort that have partly drained our ability to use willpower, just as we can't flex a single muscle for too long without taking breaks.

3. In the beginning of intoxication we are inclined to sleep, as mentioned above, but by the excitement of external circumstances, as of noise, light, business, or by the exertion of volition, we prevent the approaches of it, and continue to take into our stomach greater quantities of the inebriating materials. By these means the irritative movements of the stomach are excited into greater action than is natural; and in consequence all the irritative tribes and trains of motion, which are catenated with them, become susceptible of stronger action from their accustomed stimuli; because these motions are excited both by their usual irritation, and by their association with the increased actions of the stomach and lacteals. Hence the skin glows, and the heat of the body is increased, by the more energetic action of the whole glandular system; and pleasure is introduced in consequence of these increased motions from internal stimulus. According to Law 5. Sect. IV. on Animal Causation.

3. In the beginning of intoxication we are inclined to sleep, as mentioned above, but by the excitement of external circumstances, as of noise, light, business, or by the exertion of volition, we prevent the approaches of it, and continue to take into our stomach greater quantities of the inebriating materials. By these means the irritative movements of the stomach are excited into greater action than is natural; and in consequence all the irritative tribes and trains of motion, which are catenated with them, become susceptible of stronger action from their accustomed stimuli; because these motions are excited both by their usual irritation, and by their association with the increased actions of the stomach and lacteals. Hence the skin glows, and the heat of the body is increased, by the more energetic action of the whole glandular system; and pleasure is introduced in consequence of these increased motions from internal stimulus. According to Law 5. Sect. IV. on Animal Causation.

From this great increase of irritative motions from internal stimulus, and the increased sensation introduced into the system in consequence; and secondly, from the increased sensitive motions in consequence of this additional quantity of sensation, so much sensorial power is expended, that the voluntary power becomes feebly exerted, and the irritation from the stimulus of external objects is less forcible; the external parts of the eye are not therefore voluntarily adapted to the distances of objects, whence the apparent motions of those objects either are seen double, or become too indistinct for the purpose of balancing the body, and vertigo is induced.

From the significant increase in irritative motions caused by internal stimulation, along with the heightened sensation introduced into the system as a result, and, additionally, from the increased sensitive motions due to this extra sensation, so much sensory power is used up that voluntary control becomes weak, making irritation from external stimuli less intense. Therefore, the outer parts of the eye are not voluntarily adjusted to the distances of objects, causing the apparent motion of those objects to either appear double or become too blurry, which disrupts balance and leads to dizziness.

Hence we become acquainted with that very curious circumstance, why the drunken vertigo is attended with an increase of pleasure; for the irritative ideas and motions occasioned by internal stimulus, that were not attended to in our sober hours, are now just so much increased as to be succeeded by pleasurable sensation, in the same manner as the more violent motions of our organs are succeeded by painful sensation. And hence a greater quantity of pleasurable sensation is introduced into the constitution; which is attended in some people with an increase of benevolence and good humour.

So, we come to understand that interesting fact about why being drunk can actually feel more pleasurable; the stirrings and movements caused by internal impulses, which we ignored while sober, now become heightened enough to bring about pleasurable feelings, just like how more intense movements in our bodies can lead to discomfort. As a result, a larger amount of pleasurable feelings enters our experience, which sometimes leads to more kindness and cheerfulness in certain people.

If the apparent motions of objects is much increased, as when we revolve on one foot, or are swung on a rope, the ideas of these apparent motions are also attended to, and are succeeded with pleasureable sensation, till they become familiar to us by frequent use. Hence children are at first delighted with these kinds of exercise, and with riding, and failing, and hence rocking young children inclines them to sleep. For though in the vertigo from intoxication the irritative ideas of the apparent motions of objects are indistinct from their decrease of energy: yet in the vertigo occasioned by rocking or swinging the irritative ideas of the apparent motions of objects are increased in energy, and hence they induce pleasure into the system, but are equally indistinct, and in consequence equally unfit to balance ourselves by. This addition of pleasure precludes desire or aversion, and in consequence the voluntary power is feebly exerted, and on this account rocking young children inclines them to sleep.

If the visible movements of objects are significantly heightened, like when we spin on one foot or are swung on a rope, we pay attention to these apparent motions, and they are accompanied by pleasurable feelings until they become familiar to us through repeated experience. That's why children are initially thrilled by these activities, as well as by riding and falling; that's also why rocking young children tends to make them sleepy. Although the dizziness from intoxication makes the stimulating ideas of the apparent movements of objects less clear due to reduced energy, the dizziness from rocking or swinging increases the energy of those stimulating ideas, bringing pleasure to the system, yet they are still unclear and just as ineffective for maintaining balance. This added pleasure dampens desire or dislike, resulting in weak voluntary control, which is why rocking young children often puts them to sleep.

In what manner opium and wine act in relieving pain is another article, that well deserves our attention. There are many pains that originate from defect as well as from excess of stimulus; of these are those of the six appetites of hunger, thirst, lust, the want of heat, of distention, and of fresh air. Thus if our cutaneous capillaries cease to act from the diminished stimulus of heat, when we are exposed to cold weather, or our stomach is uneasy for want of food; these are both pains from defect of stimulus, and in consequence opium, which stimulates all the moving system into increased action, must relieve them. But this is not the case in those pains, which arise from excess of stimulus, as in violent inflammations: in these the exhibition of opium is frequently injurious by increasing the action of the system already too great, as in inflammation of the bowels mortification is often produced by the stimulus of opium. Where, however, no such bad consequences follow; the stimulus of opium, by increasing all the motions of the system, expends so much of the sensorial power, that the actions of the whole system soon become feebler, and in consequence those which produced the pain and inflammation.

The way opium and wine help relieve pain is another topic that really deserves our attention. Many pains come from both a lack and an excess of stimulation; these include the pains related to the six appetites: hunger, thirst, lust, the need for warmth, the sensation of fullness, and the need for fresh air. For example, if our skin's capillaries stop functioning due to reduced warmth when we are in cold weather, or if our stomach hurts because we haven't eaten, these pains are due to a lack of stimulation, and as a result, opium, which boosts the whole system’s activity, can alleviate them. However, this logic doesn't apply to pains caused by too much stimulation, like severe inflammation. In such cases, using opium can actually make things worse by further increasing the already excessive activity of the system; for instance, in bowel inflammation, opium can lead to tissue death due to its stimulating effects. Yet, when no harmful effects occur, the stimulation from opium increases all the system's functions, temporarily using up a lot of the sensory power, ultimately making the actions of the entire system weaker, including those that cause pain and inflammation.

4. When intoxication proceeds a little further, the quantity of pleasurable sensation is so far increased, that all desire ceases, for there is no pain in the system to excite it. Hence the voluntary exertions are diminished, staggering and stammering succeed; and the trains of ideas become more and more inconsistent from this defect of voluntary exertion, as explained in the sections on sleep and reverie, whilst those passions which are unmixed with volition are more vividly felt, and shewn with less reserve; hence pining love, or superstitious fear, and the maudling tear dropped on the remembrance of the most trifling distress.

4. As intoxication progresses, the amount of pleasure increases to the point where all desire fades away, since there’s no pain in the system to stir it up. Consequently, voluntary actions decrease, leading to staggering and slurring; thoughts become more and more scattered because of this lack of voluntary control, as discussed in the sections on sleep and daydreaming. Meanwhile, emotions that aren’t mixed with willpower are felt more intensely and expressed more openly, resulting in lovesickness, irrational fears, and tears shed over even the smallest troubles.

5. At length all these circumstances are increased; the quantity of pleasure introduced into the system by the increased irritative muscular motions of the whole sanguiferous, and glandular, and absorbent systems, becomes so great, that the organs of sense are more forcibly excited into action by this internal pleasurable sensation, than by the irritation from the stimulus of external objects. Hence the drunkard ceases to attend to external stimuli, and as volition is now also suspended, the trains of his ideas become totally inconsistent as in dreams, or delirium: and at length a stupor succeeds from the great exhaustion of sensorial power, which probably does not even admit of dreams, and in which, as in apoplexy, no motions continue but those from internal stimuli, from sensation, and from association.

5. Eventually, all these factors increase; the amount of pleasure flowing into the system from the heightened muscular activity across the circulatory, glandular, and absorbent systems becomes so intense that the sensory organs are more strongly stimulated by this internal pleasurable feeling than by any external stimuli. As a result, the drunkard stops paying attention to outside triggers, and since willpower is also dulled, their thoughts become completely erratic, similar to those experienced in dreams or delirium. Ultimately, a stupor sets in due to the significant depletion of mental energy, which likely prevents even dreams, and during this state, similar to apoplexy, the only movements that occur are those driven by internal stimuli, sensations, and associations.

6. In other people a paroxysm of drunkenness has another termination; the inebriate, as soon as he begins to be vertiginous, makes pale urine in great quantities and very frequently, and at length becomes sick, vomits repeatedly, or purges, or has profuse sweats, and a temporary fever ensues with a quick strong pulse. This in some hours is succeeded by sleep; but the unfortunate bacchanalian does not perfectly recover himself till about the same time of the succeeding day, when his course of inebriation began. As shewn in Sect. XVII. 1. 7. on Catenation. The temporary fever with strong pulse is owing to the same cause as the glow on the skin mentioned in the third paragraph of this Section: the flow of urine and sickness arises from the whole system of irritative motions being thrown into confusion by their associations with each other; as in sea-sickness, mentioned in Sect. XX. 4. on Vertigo; and which is more fully explained in Section XXIX. on Diabetes.

6. In other people a paroxysm of drunkenness has another termination; the inebriate, as soon as he begins to be vertiginous, makes pale urine in great quantities and very frequently, and at length becomes sick, vomits repeatedly, or purges, or has profuse sweats, and a temporary fever ensues with a quick strong pulse. This in some hours is succeeded by sleep; but the unfortunate bacchanalian does not perfectly recover himself till about the same time of the succeeding day, when his course of inebriation began. As shewn in Sect. XVII. 1. 7. on Catenation. The temporary fever with strong pulse is owing to the same cause as the glow on the skin mentioned in the third paragraph of this Section: the flow of urine and sickness arises from the whole system of irritative motions being thrown into confusion by their associations with each other; as in sea-sickness, mentioned in Sect. XX. 4. on Vertigo; and which is more fully explained in Section XXIX. on Diabetes.

7. In this vertigo from internal causes we see objects double, as two candles instead of one, which is thus explained. Two lines drawn through the axes of our two eyes meet at the object we attend to: this angle of the optic axes increases or diminishes with the less or greater distances of objects. All objects before or behind the place where this angle is formed, appear double; as any one may observe by holding up a pen between his eyes and the candle; when he looks attentively at a spot on the pen, and carelessly at the candle, it will appear double; and the reverse when he looks attentively at the candle and carelessly at the pen; so that in this case the muscles of the eye, like those of the limbs, stagger and are disobedient to the expiring efforts of volition. Numerous objects are indeed sometimes seen by the inebriate, occasioned by the refractions made by the tears, which stand upon his eye-lids.

7. In this dizziness from within, we see objects as if they are doubled, like seeing two candles instead of one. This happens because two lines drawn from the center of our eyes meet at the object we’re focusing on. The angle between our eye axes changes based on how far away the objects are. Any objects in front of or behind where this angle is formed appear double; for example, if someone holds a pen between their eyes and a candle, when they focus on a spot on the pen while glancing at the candle, the candle appears double. The opposite occurs if they focus on the candle and glance at the pen. In this situation, the eye muscles, much like limb muscles, become uncoordinated and unable to follow our commands. People under the influence of alcohol sometimes see multiple objects due to the way light bends through the tears resting on their eyelids.

8. This vertigo also continues, when the inebriate lies in his bed, in the dark, or with his eyes closed; and this more powerfully than when he is erect, and in the light. For the irritative ideas of the apparent motions of objects are now excited by irritation from internal stimulus, or by association with other irritative motions; and the inebriate, like one in a dream, believes the objects of these irritative motions to be present, and feels himself vertiginous. I have observed in this situation, so long as my eyes and mind were intent upon a book, the sickness and vertigo ceased, and were renewed again the moment I discontinued this attention; as was explained in the preceding account of sea-sickness. Some drunken people have been known to become sober instantly from some accident, that has strongly excited their attention, as the pain of a broken bone, or the news of their house being on fire.

8. This dizziness also persists when the drunk person is in bed, in the dark, or with their eyes shut; and it’s even stronger than when they’re standing up and in the light. The irritating feelings of the seeming movements of objects are now triggered by internal stimuli or by association with other irritating motions; and the drunk person, like someone dreaming, thinks these objects are actually there and feels dizzy. I’ve noticed that when I focused my eyes and mind on a book in this state, the nausea and dizziness went away, and they came back as soon as I lost that focus; as explained in the previous description of sea sickness. Some drunks have been known to become sober instantly due to a sudden incident that grabbed their attention, like the pain from a broken bone or hearing that their house is on fire.

9. Sometimes the vertigo from internal causes, as from intoxication, or at the beginning of some fevers, becomes so universal, that the irritative motions which belong to other organs of sense are succeeded by sensation or attention, as well as those of the eye. The vertiginous noise in the ears has been explained in Section XX. on Vertigo. The taste of the saliva, which in general is not attended to, becomes perceptible, and the patients complain of a bad taste in their mouth.

9. Sometimes the vertigo from internal causes, as from intoxication, or at the beginning of some fevers, becomes so universal, that the irritative motions which belong to other organs of sense are succeeded by sensation or attention, as well as those of the eye. The vertiginous noise in the ears has been explained in Section XX. on Vertigo. The taste of the saliva, which in general is not attended to, becomes perceptible, and the patients complain of a bad taste in their mouth.

The common smells of the surrounding air sometimes excite the attention of these patients, and bad smells are complained of, which to other people are imperceptible. The irritative motions that belong to the sense of pressure, or of touch, are attended to, and the patient conceives the bed to librate, and is fearful of falling out of it. The irritative motions belonging to the senses of distention, and of heat, like those above mentioned, become attended to at this time: hence we feel the pulsation of our arteries all over us, and complain of heat, or of cold, in parts of the body where there is no accumulation or diminution of actual heat. All which are to be explained, as in the last paragraph, by the irritative ideas belonging to the various senses being now excited by internal stimuli, or by their associations with other irritative motions. And that the inebriate, like one in a dream, believes the external objects, which usually caused these irritative ideas, to be now present.

The familiar smells in the air sometimes grab the attention of these patients, and they complain about bad odors that others can't even notice. They become aware of sensations related to pressure or touch, leading them to feel like the bed is shifting and making them afraid of falling out. During this time, they also notice sensations related to swelling and heat, much like those mentioned before: they feel their heartbeats throughout their bodies and complain of feeling hot or cold in areas where there's actually no change in temperature. All this can be explained, as mentioned in the last paragraph, by the heightened sensory perceptions triggered by internal stimuli or their connections with other disruptive sensations. Like someone in a dream, the intoxicated person believes the external objects that usually trigger these sensations are now present.

10. The diseases in consequence of frequent inebriety, or of daily taking much vinous spirit without inebriety, consist in the paralysis, which is liable to succeed violent stimulation. Organs, whose actions are associated with others, are frequently more affected than the organ, which is stimulated into too violent action. See Sect. XXIV. 2. 8. Hence in drunken people it generally happens, that the secretory vessels of the liver become first paralytic, and a torpor with consequent gall-stones or schirrus of this viscus is induced with concomitant jaundice; otherwise it becomes inflamed in consequence of previous torpor, and this inflammation is frequently transferred to a more sensible part, which is associated with it, and produces the gout, or the rosy eruption of the face, or some other leprous eruption on the head, or arms, or legs. Sometimes the stomach is first affected, and paralysis of the lacteal system is induced: whence a total abhorrence from flesh-food, and general emaciation. In others the lymphatic system is affected with paralysis, and dropsy is the consequence. In some inebriates the torpor of the liver produces pain without apparent schirrus, or gall stones, or inflammation, or consequent gout, and in these epilepsy or insanity are often the consequence. All which will be more fully treated of in the course of the work.

10. The diseases in consequence of frequent inebriety, or of daily taking much vinous spirit without inebriety, consist in the paralysis, which is liable to succeed violent stimulation. Organs, whose actions are associated with others, are frequently more affected than the organ, which is stimulated into too violent action. See Sect. XXIV. 2. 8. Hence in drunken people it generally happens, that the secretory vessels of the liver become first paralytic, and a torpor with consequent gall-stones or schirrus of this viscus is induced with concomitant jaundice; otherwise it becomes inflamed in consequence of previous torpor, and this inflammation is frequently transferred to a more sensible part, which is associated with it, and produces the gout, or the rosy eruption of the face, or some other leprous eruption on the head, or arms, or legs. Sometimes the stomach is first affected, and paralysis of the lacteal system is induced: whence a total abhorrence from flesh-food, and general emaciation. In others the lymphatic system is affected with paralysis, and dropsy is the consequence. In some inebriates the torpor of the liver produces pain without apparent schirrus, or gall stones, or inflammation, or consequent gout, and in these epilepsy or insanity are often the consequence. All which will be more fully treated of in the course of the work.

I am well aware, that it is a common opinion, that the gout is as frequently owing to gluttony in eating, as to intemperance in drinking fermented or spirituous liquors. To this I answer, that I have seen no person afflicted with the gout, who has not drank freely of fermented liquor, as wine and water, or small beer; though as the disposition to all the diseases, which have originated from intoxication, is in some degree hereditary, a less quantity of spirituous potation will induce the gout in those, who inherit the disposition from their parents. To which I must add, that in young people the rheumatism is frequently mistaken for the gout.

I know that many people believe gout is often caused by overeating just as much as by drinking too much alcohol. In response, I can say that I haven't seen anyone with gout who hasn't consumed plenty of fermented drinks like wine and water or light beer. However, since the tendency to develop diseases linked to excessive drinking can be hereditary, even a smaller amount of strong alcohol can trigger gout in those who have inherited this tendency from their parents. Additionally, I should point out that young people often confuse rheumatism with gout.

Spice is seldom taken in such quantity as to do any material injury to the system, flesh-meats as well as vegetables are the natural diet of mankind; with these a glutton may be crammed up to the throat, and fed fat like a stalled ox; but he will not be diseased, unless he adds spirituous or fermented liquor to his food. This is well known in the distilleries, where the swine, which are fattened by the spirituous sediments of barrels, acquire diseased livers. But mark what happens to a man, who drinks a quart of wine or of ale, if he has not been habituated to it. He loses the use both of his limbs and of his understanding! He becomes a temporary idiot, and has a temporary stroke of the palsy! And though he slowly recovers after some hours, is it not reasonable to conclude, that a perpetual repetition of so powerful a poison must at length permanently affect him?—If a person accidentally becomes intoxicated by eating a few mushrooms of a peculiar kind, a general alarm is excited, and he is said to be poisoned, and emetics are exhibited; but so familiarised are we to the intoxication from vinous spirit, that it occasions laughter rather than alarm.

Spices are rarely consumed in amounts that would seriously harm the body; both meats and vegetables are the natural diet for humans. A glutton can eat until he's stuffed and gain weight like a fattened animal, but he won't get sick unless he adds alcoholic or fermented drinks to his meals. This is well understood in distilleries, where pigs fed on the leftover alcohol from barrels develop sick livers. But just look at what happens to a person who drinks a quart of wine or ale if they're not used to it. They lose the ability to move their limbs and think clearly! They become momentarily confused and may even suffer a temporary stroke! And while they slowly recover after a few hours, isn't it reasonable to think that continually introducing such a potent poison will eventually have lasting effects? If someone accidentally gets drunk from eating a specific type of mushroom, it causes widespread panic, and they're said to be poisoned, leading to treatments like inducing vomiting; yet, intoxication from alcoholic beverages has become so common that it often leads to laughter instead of concern.

There is however considerable danger in too hastily discontinuing the use of so strong a stimulus, lest the torpor of the system, or paralysis, should sooner be induced by the omission than by the continuance of this habit, when unfortunately acquired. A golden rule for determining the quantity, which may with safety be discontinued, is delivered in Sect. XII. 7. 8.

There is however considerable danger in too hastily discontinuing the use of so strong a stimulus, lest the torpor of the system, or paralysis, should sooner be induced by the omission than by the continuance of this habit, when unfortunately acquired. A golden rule for determining the quantity, which may with safety be discontinued, is delivered in Sect. XII. 7. 8.

11. Definition of drunkenness. Many of the irritative motions are much increased in energy by internal stimulation.

11. Definition of drunkenness. Many of the irritative movements are significantly amplified by internal stimulation.

2. A great additional quantity of pleasurable sensation is occasioned by this increased exertion of the irritative motions. And many sensitive motions are produced in consequence of this increased sensation.

2. A significant increase in enjoyable feelings is caused by this heightened activity of irritative movements. Many sensitive movements arise as a result of this increased sensation.

3. The associated trains and tribes of motions, catenated with the increased irritative and sensitive motions, are disturbed, and proceed in confusion.

3. The connected trains and groups of movements, linked with the heightened irritative and sensitive motions, are disrupted and move in chaos.

4. The faculty of volition is gradually impaired, whence proceeds the instability of locomotion, inaccuracy of perception, and inconsistency of ideas; and is at length totally suspended, and a temporary apoplexy succeeds.

4. The ability to make decisions is slowly weakened, which causes unsteady movement, distorted perception, and erratic thoughts; eventually, it completely stops, and a temporary loss of consciousness follows.



SECT. XXII.

OF PROPENSITY TO MOTION, REPETITION AND IMITATION.

OF PROPENSITY TO MOTION, REPETITION AND IMITATION.

I. Accumulation of sensorial power in hemiplagia, in sleep, in cold fit of fever, in the locomotive muscles, in the organs of sense. Produces propensity to action. II. Repetition by three sensorial powers. In rhimes and alliterations, in music, dancing, architecture, landscape-painting, beauty. III. 1. Perception consists in imitation. Four kinds of imitation. 2. Voluntary. Dogs taught to dance. 3. Sensitive. Hence sympathy, and all our virtues. Contagious matter of venereal ulcers, of hydrophobia, of jail-fever, of small-pox, produced by imitation, and the sex of the embryon. 4. Irritative imitation. 5. Imitations resolvable into associations.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. The buildup of sensory power in partial paralysis, during sleep, during chills from fever, in our movement muscles, and in our sensory organs creates a tendency to act. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. This happens through the repetition of three sensory powers, seen in rhymes and alliterations, in music, dancing, architecture, landscape painting, and notions of beauty. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Perception is based on imitation, with four types of imitation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Voluntary imitation, like dogs being trained to dance. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Sensitive imitation, which leads to empathy and all our virtues. Contagious agents of venereal diseases, rabies, jail fever, and smallpox arise from imitation, as well as the sex of the embryo. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Irritative imitation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Imitations can be broken down into associations.

I. 1. In the hemiplagia, when the limbs on one side have lost their power of voluntary motion, the patient is for many days perpetually employed in moving those of the other. 2. When the voluntary power is suspended during sleep, there commences a ceaseless flow of sensitive motions, or ideas of imagination, which compose our dreams. 3. When in the cold fit of an intermittent fever some parts of the system have for a time continued torpid, and have thus expended less than their usual expenditure of sensorial power; a hot fit succeeds, with violent action of those vessels, which had previously been quiescent. All these are explained from an accumulation of sensorial power during the inactivity of some part of the system.

I. 1. In hemiplegia, when the limbs on one side lose their ability to move voluntarily, the patient spends many days constantly moving the limbs on the other side. 2. When the ability to move voluntarily is paused during sleep, a continuous flow of sensations or imaginative ideas occurs, which make up our dreams. 3. When a part of the body remains inactive during the cold phase of an intermittent fever and uses less than its usual amount of sensory energy, a hot phase follows, bringing with it intense activity in those vessels that had been dormant. All these phenomena are explained by an accumulation of sensory energy during the inactivity of certain parts of the system.

Besides the very great quantity of sensorial power perpetually produced and expended in moving the arterial, venous, and glandular systems, with the various organs or digestion, as described in Section XXXII. 3. 2. there is also a constant expenditure of it by the action of our locomotive muscles and organs of sense. Thus the thickness of the optic nerves, where they enter the eye, and the great expansion of the nerves of touch beneath the whole of the cuticle, evince the great consumption of sensorial power by these senses. And our perpetual muscular actions in the common offices of life, and in constantly preserving the perpendicularity of our bodies during the day, evince a considerable expenditure of the spirit of animation by our locomotive muscles. It follows, that if the exertion of these organs of sense and muscles be for a while intermitted, that some quantity of sensorial power must be accumulated, and a propensity to activity of some kind ensue from the increased excitability of the system. Whence proceeds the irksomeness of a continued attitude, and of an indolent life.

Besides the very great quantity of sensorial power perpetually produced and expended in moving the arterial, venous, and glandular systems, with the various organs or digestion, as described in Section XXXII. 3. 2. there is also a constant expenditure of it by the action of our locomotive muscles and organs of sense. Thus the thickness of the optic nerves, where they enter the eye, and the great expansion of the nerves of touch beneath the whole of the cuticle, evince the great consumption of sensorial power by these senses. And our perpetual muscular actions in the common offices of life, and in constantly preserving the perpendicularity of our bodies during the day, evince a considerable expenditure of the spirit of animation by our locomotive muscles. It follows, that if the exertion of these organs of sense and muscles be for a while intermitted, that some quantity of sensorial power must be accumulated, and a propensity to activity of some kind ensue from the increased excitability of the system. Whence proceeds the irksomeness of a continued attitude, and of an indolent life.

However small this hourly accumulation of the spirit of animation may be, it produces a propensity to some kind of action; but it nevertheless requires either desire or aversion, either pleasure or pain, or some external stimulus, or a previous link of association, to excite the system into activity; thus it frequently happens, when the mind and body are so unemployed as not to possess any of the three first kinds of stimuli, that the last takes place, and consumes the small but perpetual accumulation of sensorial power. Whence some indolent people repeat the same verse for hours together, or hum the same tune. Thus the poet:

However small this hourly buildup of lively energy may be, it creates a tendency toward some form of action; yet it still needs either desire or aversion, pleasure or pain, an external trigger, or a prior association to get the system moving. As a result, it often happens that when the mind and body are so idle that they lack any of the first three types of triggers, the last one occurs and drains the small but constant buildup of sensory power. That's why some lazy people repeat the same line for hours or hum the same melody. Thus the poet:

Onward he trudged, not knowing what he sought,

Onward he walked, not knowing what he was looking for,

And whistled, as he went, for want of thought.

And whistled as he walked, not really thinking about anything.

II. The repetitions of motions may be at first produced either by volition, or by sensation, or by irritation, but they soon become easier to perform than any other kinds of action, because they soon become associated together, according to Law the seventh, Section IV. on Animal Causation. And because their frequency of repetition, if as much sensorial power be produced during every reiteration as is expended, adds to the facility of their production.

II. The repetitions of motions may be at first produced either by volition, or by sensation, or by irritation, but they soon become easier to perform than any other kinds of action, because they soon become associated together, according to Law the seventh, Section IV. on Animal Causation. And because their frequency of repetition, if as much sensorial power be produced during every reiteration as is expended, adds to the facility of their production.

If a stimulus be repeated at uniform intervals of time, as described in Sect. XII. 3. 3. the action, whether of our muscles or organs of sense, is produced with still greater facility or energy; because the sensorial power of association, mentioned above, is combined with the sensorial power of irritation; that is, in common language, the acquired habit assists the power of the stimulus.

If a stimulus be repeated at uniform intervals of time, as described in Sect. XII. 3. 3. the action, whether of our muscles or organs of sense, is produced with still greater facility or energy; because the sensorial power of association, mentioned above, is combined with the sensorial power of irritation; that is, in common language, the acquired habit assists the power of the stimulus.

This not only obtains in the annual, lunar, and diurnal catenations of animal motions, as explained in Sect. XXXVI. which are thus performed with great facility and energy; but in every less circle of actions or ideas, as in the burthen of a song, or the reiterations of a dance. To the facility and distinctness, with which we hear sounds at repeated intervals, we owe the pleasure, which we receive from musical time, and from poetic time; as described in Botanic Garden, P. 2. Interlude 3. And to this the pleasure we receive from the rhimes and alliterations of modern verification; the source of which without this key would be difficult to discover. And to this likewise should be ascribed the beauty of the duplicature in the perfect tense of the Greek verbs, and of some Latin ones, as tango tetegi, mordeo momordi.

This not only obtains in the annual, lunar, and diurnal catenations of animal motions, as explained in Sect. XXXVI. which are thus performed with great facility and energy; but in every less circle of actions or ideas, as in the burthen of a song, or the reiterations of a dance. To the facility and distinctness, with which we hear sounds at repeated intervals, we owe the pleasure, which we receive from musical time, and from poetic time; as described in Botanic Garden, P. 2. Interlude 3. And to this the pleasure we receive from the rhimes and alliterations of modern verification; the source of which without this key would be difficult to discover. And to this likewise should be ascribed the beauty of the duplicature in the perfect tense of the Greek verbs, and of some Latin ones, as tango tetegi, mordeo momordi.

There is no variety of notes referable to the gamut in the beating of the drum, yet if it be performed in musical time, it is agreeable to our ears; and therefore this pleasurable sensation must be owing to the repetition of the divisions of the sounds at certain intervals of time, or musical bars. Whether these times or bars are distinguished by a pause, or by an emphasis, or accent, certain it is, that this distinction is perpetually repeated; otherwise the ear could not determine instantly, whether the successions of sound were in common or in triple time. In common time there is a division between every two crotchets, or other notes of equivalent time; though the bar in written music is put after every fourth crotchet, or notes equivalent in time; in triple time the division or bar is after every three crotchets, or notes equivalent; so that in common time the repetition recurs more frequently than in triple time. The grave or heroic verses of the Greek and Latin poets are written in common time; the French heroic verses, and Mr. Anstie's humorous verses in his Bath Guide, are written in the same time as the Greek and Latin verses, but are one bar shorter. The English grave or heroic verses are measured by triple time, as Mr. Pope's translation of Homer.

There aren't different notes linked to the drumbeat scale, but when it's played in time with music, it sounds nice to us. So, this enjoyable feeling must come from the repetition of sound divisions at specific time intervals, or musical bars. Whether these bars are marked by a pause, emphasis, or accent, it’s clear that this distinction happens over and over; otherwise, our ears couldn't immediately tell if the sounds were in common or triple time. In common time, there’s a division between every two quarter notes or other notes with the same duration; even though the bar in written music appears after every fourth quarter note or notes of equivalent duration. In triple time, the division or bar happens after every three quarter notes or notes of equivalent duration, meaning that in common time, the repetition occurs more often than in triple time. The serious or heroic verses of Greek and Latin poets are written in common time; French heroic verses and Mr. Anstie's funny verses in his Bath Guide are also in the same time as Greek and Latin verses but are one bar shorter. English serious or heroic verses are measured by triple time, like Mr. Pope's translation of Homer.

But besides these little circles of musical time, there are the greater returning periods, and the still more distant choruses, which, like the rhimes at the ends of verses, owe their beauty to repetition; that is, to the facility and distinctness with which we perceive sounds, which we expect to perceive, or have perceived before; or in the language of this work, to the greater ease and energy with which our organ is excited by the combined sensorial powers of association and irritation, than by the latter singly.

But aside from these small moments of music, there are larger cycles that repeat over time, and even more distant harmonies that, like the rhymes at the end of lines, gain their beauty from repetition. This means we find it easier and clearer to recognize sounds that we expect to hear or have heard before. In the terms of this work, it's about how our senses respond more easily and strongly to the combined effects of memory and stimulation than to each one alone.

A certain uniformity or repetition of parts enters the very composition of harmony. Thus two octaves nearest to each other in the scale commence their vibrations together after every second vibration of the higher one. And where the first, third, and fifth compose a chord the vibrations concur or coincide frequently, though less to than in the two octaves. It is probable that these chords bear some analogy to a mixture of three alternate colours in the sun's spectrum separated by a prism.

A certain uniformity or repetition of parts is part of what makes harmony. For example, two octaves that are close together in pitch start vibrating together after every second pulse of the higher octave. When the first, third, and fifth notes create a chord, their vibrations often align or coincide, though not as frequently as in the two octaves. It's likely that these chords are similar to a mix of three alternating colors in the sun's spectrum that a prism separates.

The pleasure we receive from a melodious succession of notes referable to the gamut is derived from another source, viz. to the pandiculation or counteraction of antagonist fibres. See Botanic Garden, P. 2. Interlude 3. If to these be added our early associations of agreeable ideas with certain proportions of sound, I suppose, from these three sources springs all the delight of music, so celebrated by ancient authors, and so enthusiastically cultivated at present. See Sect. XVI. No. 10. on Instinct.

The pleasure we receive from a melodious succession of notes referable to the gamut is derived from another source, viz. to the pandiculation or counteraction of antagonist fibres. See Botanic Garden, P. 2. Interlude 3. If to these be added our early associations of agreeable ideas with certain proportions of sound, I suppose, from these three sources springs all the delight of music, so celebrated by ancient authors, and so enthusiastically cultivated at present. See Sect. XVI. No. 10. on Instinct.

This kind of pleasure arising from repetition, that is from the facility and distinctness, with which we perceive and understand repeated sensations, enters into all the agreeable arts; and when it is carried to excess is termed formality. The art of dancing like that of music depends for a great part of the pleasure, it affords, on repetition; architecture, especially the Grecian, consists of one part being a repetition of another; and hence the beauty of the pyramidal outline in landscape-painting; where one side of the picture may be said in some measure to balance the other. So universally does repetition contribute to our pleasure in the fine arts, that beauty itself has been defined by some writers to consist in a due combination of uniformity and variety. See Sect. XVI. 6.

This kind of pleasure arising from repetition, that is from the facility and distinctness, with which we perceive and understand repeated sensations, enters into all the agreeable arts; and when it is carried to excess is termed formality. The art of dancing like that of music depends for a great part of the pleasure, it affords, on repetition; architecture, especially the Grecian, consists of one part being a repetition of another; and hence the beauty of the pyramidal outline in landscape-painting; where one side of the picture may be said in some measure to balance the other. So universally does repetition contribute to our pleasure in the fine arts, that beauty itself has been defined by some writers to consist in a due combination of uniformity and variety. See Sect. XVI. 6.

III. 1. Man is termed by Aristotle an imitative animal; this propensity to imitation not only appears in the actions of children, but in all the customs and fashions of the world: many thousands tread in the beaten paths of others, for one who traverses regions of his own discovery. The origin of this propensity of imitation has not, that I recollect, been deduced from any known principle; when any action presents itself to the view of a child, as of whetting a knife, or threading a needle, the parts of this action in respect of time, motion, figure, is imitated by a part of the retina of his eye; to perform this action therefore with his hands is easier to him than to invent any new action, because it consists in repeating with another set of fibres, viz. with the moving muscles, what he had just performed by some parts of the retina; just as in dancing we transfer the times of motion from the actions of the auditory nerves to the muscles of the limbs. Imitation therefore consists of repetition, which we have shewn above to be the easiest kind of animal action, and which we perpetually fall into, when we possess an accumulation of sensorial power, which is not otherwise called into exertion.

III. 1. Aristotle refers to humans as imitative beings; this tendency to imitate shows up not only in children's behavior but also in all the customs and trends around the world: thousands follow the familiar paths laid out by others, while only a few venture into uncharted territory. I can't recall any established principle explaining the origin of this imitative tendency; when a child sees an action, like sharpening a knife or threading a needle, the components of this action, in terms of timing, movement, and shape, are mirrored by part of the retina in their eye. So, it's easier for them to perform this action with their hands than to come up with a new one, since it involves repeating what they just observed through different muscles, much like in dancing where we translate movements from our auditory senses to our limbs. Thus, imitation boils down to repetition, which, as we've discussed, is the simplest form of animal behavior and something we naturally do when we have a surplus of sensory energy that isn't being used elsewhere.

It has been shewn, that our ideas are configurations of the organs of sense, produced originally in consequence of the stimulus of external bodies. And that these ideas, or configurations of the organs of sense, referable in some property a correspondent property of external matter; as the parts of the senses of light and of touch, which are excited into action, resemble in figure the figure of the stimulating body; and probably also the colour, and the quantity of density, which they perceive. As explained in Sect. XIV. 2. 2. Hence it appears, that our perceptions themselves are copies, that is, imitations of some properties of external matter; and the propensity to imitation is thus interwoven with our existence, as it is produced by the stimuli of external bodies, and is afterwards repeated by our volitions and sensations, and thus constitutes all the operations of our minds.

It has been shewn, that our ideas are configurations of the organs of sense, produced originally in consequence of the stimulus of external bodies. And that these ideas, or configurations of the organs of sense, referable in some property a correspondent property of external matter; as the parts of the senses of light and of touch, which are excited into action, resemble in figure the figure of the stimulating body; and probably also the colour, and the quantity of density, which they perceive. As explained in Sect. XIV. 2. 2. Hence it appears, that our perceptions themselves are copies, that is, imitations of some properties of external matter; and the propensity to imitation is thus interwoven with our existence, as it is produced by the stimuli of external bodies, and is afterwards repeated by our volitions and sensations, and thus constitutes all the operations of our minds.

2. Imitations resolve themselves into four kinds, voluntary, sensitive, irritative, and associate. The voluntary imitations are, when we imitate deliberately the actions of others, either by mimicry, as in acting a play, or in delineating a flower; or in the common actions of our lives, as in our dress, cookery, language, manners, and even in our habits of thinking.

2. Imitations break down into four types: voluntary, sensitive, irritative, and associative. Voluntary imitations occur when we intentionally mimic the actions of others, whether through mimicry, like acting in a play, or by depicting a flower; or in the everyday activities of our lives, such as our clothing, cooking, language, behavior, and even our ways of thinking.

Not only the greatest part of mankind learn all the common arts of life by imitating others, but brute animals seem capable of acquiring knowledge with greater facility by imitating each other, than by any methods by which we can teach them; as dogs and cats, when they are sick, learn of each other to eat grass; and I suppose, that by making an artificial dog perform certain tricks, as in dancing on his hinder legs, a living dog might be easily induced to imitate them; and that the readiest way of instructing dumb animals is by practising them with others of the same species, which have already learned the arts we wish to teach them. The important use of imitation in acquiring natural language is mentioned in Section XVI. 7. and 8. on Instinct.

Not only the greatest part of mankind learn all the common arts of life by imitating others, but brute animals seem capable of acquiring knowledge with greater facility by imitating each other, than by any methods by which we can teach them; as dogs and cats, when they are sick, learn of each other to eat grass; and I suppose, that by making an artificial dog perform certain tricks, as in dancing on his hinder legs, a living dog might be easily induced to imitate them; and that the readiest way of instructing dumb animals is by practising them with others of the same species, which have already learned the arts we wish to teach them. The important use of imitation in acquiring natural language is mentioned in Section XVI. 7. and 8. on Instinct.

3. The sensitive imitations are the immediate consequences of pleasure or pain, and these are often produced even contrary to the efforts of the will. Thus many young men on seeing cruel surgical operations become sick, and some even feel pain in the parts of their own bodies, which they see tortured or wounded in others; that is, they in some measure imitate by the exertions of their own fibres the violent actions, which they witnessed in those of others. In this case a double imitation takes place, first the observer imitates with the extremities of the optic nerve the mangled limbs, which are present before his eyes; then by a second imitation he excites to violent action of the fibres of his own limbs as to produce pain in those parts of his own body, which he saw wounded in another. In these pains produced by imitation the effect has some similarity to the cause, which distinguishes them from those produced by association; as the pains of the teeth, called tooth-edge, which are produced by association with disagreeable sounds, as explained in Sect. XVI. 10.

3. The sensitive imitations are the immediate consequences of pleasure or pain, and these are often produced even contrary to the efforts of the will. Thus many young men on seeing cruel surgical operations become sick, and some even feel pain in the parts of their own bodies, which they see tortured or wounded in others; that is, they in some measure imitate by the exertions of their own fibres the violent actions, which they witnessed in those of others. In this case a double imitation takes place, first the observer imitates with the extremities of the optic nerve the mangled limbs, which are present before his eyes; then by a second imitation he excites to violent action of the fibres of his own limbs as to produce pain in those parts of his own body, which he saw wounded in another. In these pains produced by imitation the effect has some similarity to the cause, which distinguishes them from those produced by association; as the pains of the teeth, called tooth-edge, which are produced by association with disagreeable sounds, as explained in Sect. XVI. 10.

The effect of this powerful agent, imitation, in the moral world, is mentioned in Sect. XVI. 7. as it is the foundation of all our intellectual sympathies with the pains and pleasures of others, and is in consequence the source of all our virtues. For in what consists our sympathy with the miseries, or with the joys, of our fellow creatures, but in an involuntary excitation of ideas in some measure similar or imitative of those, which we believe to exist in the minds of the persons, whom we commiserate or congratulate?

The effect of this powerful agent, imitation, in the moral world, is mentioned in Sect. XVI. 7. as it is the foundation of all our intellectual sympathies with the pains and pleasures of others, and is in consequence the source of all our virtues. For in what consists our sympathy with the miseries, or with the joys, of our fellow creatures, but in an involuntary excitation of ideas in some measure similar or imitative of those, which we believe to exist in the minds of the persons, whom we commiserate or congratulate?

There are certain concurrent or successive actions of some of the glands, or other parts of the body, which are possessed of sensation, which become intelligible from this propensity to imitation. Of these are the production of matter by the membranes of the fauces, or by the skin, in consequence of the venereal disease previously affecting the parts of generation. Since as no fever is excited, and as neither the blood of such patients, nor even the matter from ulcers of the throat, or from cutaneous ulcers, will by inoculation produce the venereal disease in others, as observed by Mr. Hunter, there is reason to conclude, that no contagious matter is conveyed thither by the blood-vessels, but that a milder matter is formed by the actions of the fine vessels in those membranes imitating each other. See Section XXXIII. 2. 9. In this disease the actions of these vessels producing ulcers on the throat and skin are imperfect imitations of those producing chanker, or gonorrhœa; since the matter produced by them is not infectious, while the imitative actions in the hydrophobia appear to be perfect resemblances, as they produce a material equally infectious with the original one, which induced them.

There are certain concurrent or successive actions of some of the glands, or other parts of the body, which are possessed of sensation, which become intelligible from this propensity to imitation. Of these are the production of matter by the membranes of the fauces, or by the skin, in consequence of the venereal disease previously affecting the parts of generation. Since as no fever is excited, and as neither the blood of such patients, nor even the matter from ulcers of the throat, or from cutaneous ulcers, will by inoculation produce the venereal disease in others, as observed by Mr. Hunter, there is reason to conclude, that no contagious matter is conveyed thither by the blood-vessels, but that a milder matter is formed by the actions of the fine vessels in those membranes imitating each other. See Section XXXIII. 2. 9. In this disease the actions of these vessels producing ulcers on the throat and skin are imperfect imitations of those producing chanker, or gonorrhœa; since the matter produced by them is not infectious, while the imitative actions in the hydrophobia appear to be perfect resemblances, as they produce a material equally infectious with the original one, which induced them.

The contagion from the bite of a mad dog differs from other contagious materials, from its being communicable from other animals to mankind, and from many animals to each other; the phenomena attending the hydrophobia are in some degree explicable on the foregoing theory. The infectious matter does not appear to enter the circulation, as it cannot be traced along the course of the lymphatics from the wound, nor is there any swelling of the lymphatic glands, nor does any fever attend, as occurs in the small-pox, and in many other contagious diseases; yet by some unknown process the disease is communicated from the wound to the throat, and that many months after the injury, so as to produce pain and hydrophobia, with a secretion of infectious saliva of the same kind, as that of the mad dog, which inflicted the wound.

The infection from a rabid dog's bite is different from other contagious materials because it can be transmitted from other animals to humans and among various animals. The symptoms of rabies can be somewhat explained by this theory. The infectious substance doesn’t seem to enter the bloodstream, as it can’t be traced through the lymphatic system from the wound; there’s also no swelling of the lymph nodes, nor does it cause fever like smallpox and many other contagious diseases do. Yet, through some unknown process, the disease spreads from the wound to the throat, even many months after the injury, leading to pain and hydrophobia, along with the production of infectious saliva similar to that of the rabid dog that caused the wound.

This subject is very intricate.—It would appear, that by certain morbid actions of the salivary glands of the mad dog, a peculiar kind of saliva is produced; which being instilled into a wound of another animal stimulates the cutaneous or mucous glands into morbid actions, but which are ineffectual in respect to the production of a similar contagious material; but the salivary glands by irritative sympathy are thrown into similar action, and produce an infectious saliva similar to that instilled into the wound.

This topic is quite complex. It seems that certain abnormal functions of the salivary glands in a rabid dog lead to the production of a unique type of saliva. When this saliva enters a wound of another animal, it triggers the skin or mucous glands to act abnormally, but these responses do not create a similar contagious substance. However, through a process of irritative sympathy, the salivary glands also become active and produce an infectious saliva that resembles the one introduced into the wound.

Though in many contagious fevers a material similar to that which produced the disease, is thus generated by imitation; yet there are other infectious materials, which do not thus propagate themselves, but which seem to act like slow poisons. Of this kind was the contagious matter, which produced the jail-fever at the assizes at Oxford about a century ago. Which, though fatal to so many, was not communicated to their nurses or attendants. In these cases, the imitations of the fine vessels, as above described, appear to be imperfect, and do not therefore produce a matter similar to that, which stimulates them; in this circumstance resembling the venereal matter in ulcers of the throat or skin, according to the curious discovery of Mr. Hunter above related, who found, by repeated inoculations, that it would not infect. Hunter on Venereal Disease, Part vi. ch. 1.

Though in many contagious fevers a material similar to the one that caused the disease is generated through imitation, there are other infectious substances that don’t spread this way and seem to act like slow poisons. One example is the contagious material that caused jail fever during the assizes in Oxford about a hundred years ago. Even though it was deadly for many, it didn’t spread to their nurses or caregivers. In these cases, the imitations of the fine vessels, as described above, seem to be incomplete and therefore do not produce a material similar to that which stimulates them; this situation is similar to the venereal matter found in throat or skin ulcers, according to the fascinating discovery by Mr. Hunter mentioned earlier, who found through repeated inoculations that it wouldn’t cause infection. Hunter on Venereal Disease, Part vi. ch. 1.

Another example of morbid imitation is in the production of a great quantity of contagious matter, as in the inoculated small-pox, from a small quantity of it inserted into the arm, and probably diffused in the blood. These particles of contagious matter stimulate the extremities of the fine arteries of the skin, and cause them to imitate some properties of those particles of contagious matter, so as to produce a thousandfold of a similar material. See Sect. XXXIII. 2. 6. Other instances are mentioned in the Section on Generation, which shew the probability that the extremities of the seminal glands may imitate certain ideas of the mind, or actions of the organs of sense, and thus occasion the male or female sex of the embryon. See Sect. XXXIX. 6.

Another example of morbid imitation is in the production of a great quantity of contagious matter, as in the inoculated small-pox, from a small quantity of it inserted into the arm, and probably diffused in the blood. These particles of contagious matter stimulate the extremities of the fine arteries of the skin, and cause them to imitate some properties of those particles of contagious matter, so as to produce a thousandfold of a similar material. See Sect. XXXIII. 2. 6. Other instances are mentioned in the Section on Generation, which shew the probability that the extremities of the seminal glands may imitate certain ideas of the mind, or actions of the organs of sense, and thus occasion the male or female sex of the embryon. See Sect. XXXIX. 6.

4. We come now to those imitations, which are not attended with sensation. Of these are all the irritative ideas already explained, as when the retina of the eye imitates by its action or configuration the tree or the bench, which I shun in walking past without attending to them. Other examples of these irritative imitations are daily observable in common life; thus one yawning person shall set a whole company a yawning; and some have acquired winking of the eyes or impediments of speech by imitating their companions without being conscious of it.

4. Now let's talk about those imitations that don't involve any sensation. This includes all the irritative ideas we've already discussed, like when the retina of the eye mimics the shape or movement of a tree or a bench while I walk by them without really noticing. We see more examples of these irritative imitations in everyday life; for instance, when one person yawns, it can make everyone else yawn too. Some people have even developed habits like blinking or speech difficulties by unconsciously mimicking their friends.

5. Besides the three species of imitations above described there may be some associate motions, which may imitate each other in the kind as well as in the quantity of their action; but it is difficult to distinguish them from the associations of motions treated of in Section XXXV. Where the actions of other persons are imitated there can be no doubt, or where we imitate a preconceived idea by exertion of our locomotive muscles, as in painting a dragon; all these imitations may aptly be referred to the sources above described of the propensity to activity, and the facility of repetition; at the same time I do not affirm, that all those other apparent sensitive and irritative imitations may not be resolvable into associations of a peculiar kind, in which certain distant parts of similar irritability or sensibility, and which have habitually acted together, may affect each other exactly with the same kinds of motion; as many parts are known to sympathise in the quantity of their motions. And that therefore they may be ultimately resolvable into associations of action, as described in Sect. XXXV.

5. Besides the three species of imitations above described there may be some associate motions, which may imitate each other in the kind as well as in the quantity of their action; but it is difficult to distinguish them from the associations of motions treated of in Section XXXV. Where the actions of other persons are imitated there can be no doubt, or where we imitate a preconceived idea by exertion of our locomotive muscles, as in painting a dragon; all these imitations may aptly be referred to the sources above described of the propensity to activity, and the facility of repetition; at the same time I do not affirm, that all those other apparent sensitive and irritative imitations may not be resolvable into associations of a peculiar kind, in which certain distant parts of similar irritability or sensibility, and which have habitually acted together, may affect each other exactly with the same kinds of motion; as many parts are known to sympathise in the quantity of their motions. And that therefore they may be ultimately resolvable into associations of action, as described in Sect. XXXV.



SECT. XXIII.

OF THE CIRCULATORY SYSTEM.

CIRCULATORY SYSTEM.

I. The heart and arteries have no antagonist muscles. Veins absorb the blood, propel it forwards, and distend the heart; contraction of the heart distends the arteries. Vena portarum. II. Glands which take their fluids from the blood. With long necks, with short necks. III. Absorbent system. IV. Heat given out from glandular secretions. Blood changes colour in the lungs and in the glands and capillaries. V. Blood is absorbed by veins, as chyle by lacteal vessels, otherwise they could not join their streams. VI. Two kinds of stimulus, agreeable and disagreeable. Glandular appetency. Glands originally possessed sensation.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. The heart and arteries don’t have opposing muscles. Veins collect blood, push it forward, and stretch the heart; when the heart contracts, it stretches the arteries. Vena portarum. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Glands that extract fluids from the blood. Some have long necks, others have short necks. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Absorbent system. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Heat released from glandular secretions. Blood changes color in the lungs, glands, and capillaries. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Blood is absorbed by veins, just as chyle is by lacteal vessels; otherwise, they wouldn’t be able to merge their streams. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. There are two types of stimuli, pleasant and unpleasant. Glandular appetency. Glands originally had sensation.

I. We now step forwards to illustrate some of the phenomena of diseases, and to trace out their most efficacious methods of cure; and shall commence this subject with a short description of the circulatory system.

I. We now move ahead to show some of the effects of diseases and to outline their most effective treatments; we'll start this topic with a brief overview of the circulatory system.

As the nerves, whose extremities form our various organs of sense and muscles, are all joined, or communicate, by means of the brain, for the convenience perhaps of the distribution of a subtile ethereal fluid for the purpose of motion; so all those vessels of the body, which carry the grosser fluids for the purposes of nutrition, communicate with each other by the heart.

As the nerves that connect to our different senses and muscles are all linked through the brain, possibly to distribute a fine, ethereal fluid for movement; in a similar way, all the vessels in the body that transport denser fluids for nutrition connect with each other through the heart.

The heart and arteries are hollow muscles, and are therefore indued with power of contraction in consequence of stimulus, like all other muscular fibres; but, as they have no antagonist muscles, the cavities of the vessels, which they form, would remain for ever closed, after they have contracted themselves, unless some extraneous power be applied to again distend them. This extraneous power in respect to the heart is the current of blood, which is perpetually absorbed by the veins from the various glands and capillaries, and pushed into the heart by a power probably very similar to that, which raises the sap in vegetables in the spring, which, according to Dr. Hale's experiment on the stump of a vine, exerted a force equal to a column of water above twenty feet high. This force of the current of blood in the veins is partly produced by their absorbent power, exerted at the beginning of every fine ramification; which may be conceived to be a mouth absorbing blood, as the mouths of the lacteals and lymphatics absorb chyle and lymph. And partly by their intermitted compression by the pulsations of their generally concomitant arteries; by which the blood is perpetually propelled towards the heart, as the valves in many veins, and the absorbent mouths in them all, will not suffer it to return.

The heart and arteries are hollow muscles, which means they can contract in response to stimulation, just like other muscle fibers. However, since they don’t have opposing muscles, the channels they create would stay closed forever after they contract unless some external force is applied to open them again. For the heart, this external force is the flow of blood, which is constantly drawn in by the veins from various glands and capillaries, and pushed into the heart by a force similar to what raises sap in plants during spring. According to Dr. Hale's experiment with a vine stump, this force can equal the pressure of a water column over twenty feet high. The flow of blood in the veins is partly generated by their ability to absorb, acting at the start of every fine branch, like a mouth absorbing blood, similar to how the mouth of the lacteals and lymphatics absorb chyle and lymph. It’s also driven by the intermittent compression from the pulsations of the arteries that usually run alongside them, pushing blood towards the heart, while the valves in many veins and the absorbing mouths prevent it from flowing back.

The blood, thus forcibly injected into the chambers of the heart, distends this combination of hollow muscles; till by the stimulus of distention they contract themselves; and, pushing forwards the blood into the arteries, exert sufficient force to overcome in less than a second of time the vis inertiæ, and perhaps some elasticity, of the very extensive ramifications of the two great systems of the aortal and pulmonary arteries. The power necessary to do this in so short a time must be considerable, and has been variously estimated by different physiologists.

The blood, forcefully pumped into the heart's chambers, stretches this collection of hollow muscles. The pressure from this stretching causes them to contract, pushing the blood into the arteries with enough force to overcome inertia and possibly some elasticity in the wide branching networks of the aortic and pulmonary arteries in less than a second. The power needed to achieve this so quickly must be significant, and various physiologists have estimated it differently.

The muscular coats of the arterial system are then brought into action by the stimulus of distention, and propel the blood to the mouths, or through the convolutions, which precede the secretory apertures of the various glands and capillaries.

The strong walls of the arteries are activated by the force of expansion, pushing the blood toward the openings or through the twists that lead to the secretory openings of the different glands and capillaries.

In the vessels of the liver there is no intervention of the heart; but the vena portarum, which does the office of an artery, is distended by the blood poured into it from the mesenteric veins, and is by this distention stimulated to contract itself, and propel the blood to the mouths of the numerous glands, which compose that viscus.

In the vessels of the liver, the heart doesn't play a role; instead, the portal vein, which works like an artery, expands as blood flows into it from the mesenteric veins. This expansion triggers it to contract and push the blood to the many glands that make up the liver.

II. The glandular system of vessels may be divided into those, which take some fluid from the circulation; and those, which give something to it. Those, which take their fluid from the circulation are the various glands, by which the tears, bile, urine, perspiration, and many other secretions are produced; these glands probably consist of a mouth to select, a belly to digest, and an excretory aperture to emit their appropriated fluids; the blood is conveyed by the power of the heart and arteries to the mouths of these glands, it is there taken up by the living power of the gland, and carried forwards to its belly, and excretory aperture, where a part is separated, and the remainder absorbed by the veins for further purposes.

II. The glandular system of vessels can be divided into those that take in fluids from the circulation and those that release fluids into it. The glands that extract fluids include those that produce tears, bile, urine, sweat, and many other secretions. These glands likely have an opening to select substances, a section to process them, and an outlet to release the fluids they've processed. The heart and arteries pump blood to the openings of these glands, where the living function of the gland absorbs the blood and moves it to its processing section and outlet, where some of it is separated, while the rest is taken up by the veins for additional uses.

Some of these glands are furnished with long convoluted necks or tubes, as the seminal ones, which are curiously seen when injected with quicksilver. Others seem to consist of shorter tubes, as that great congeries of glands, which constitute the liver, and those of the kidneys. Some have their excretory apertures opening into reservoirs, as the urinary and gall-bladders. And others on the external body, as those which secrete the tears, and perspirable matter.

Some of these glands have long twisted necks or tubes, like the seminal ones, which are interestingly visible when filled with mercury. Others appear to have shorter tubes, like the large group of glands that make up the liver and those of the kidneys. Some have openings that lead into reservoirs, like the urinary and gall bladders. And others open onto the outside of the body, like those that produce tears and sweat.

Another great system of glands, which have very short necks, are the capillary vessels; by which the insensible perspiration is secreted on the skin; and the mucus of various consistences, which lubricates the interstices of the cellular membrane, of the muscular fibres, and of all the larger cavities of the body. From the want of a long convolution of vessels some have doubted, whether these capillaries should be considered as glands, and have been led to conclude, that the perspirable matter rather exuded than was secreted. But the fluid of perspiration is not simple water, though that part of it, which exhales into the air may be such; for there is another part of it, which in a state of health is absorbed again; but which, when the absorbents are diseased, remains on the surface of the skin, in the form of scurf, or indurated mucus. Another thing, which shews their similitude to other glands, is their sensibility to certain affections of the mind; as is seen in the deeper colour of the skin in the blush of shame, or the greater paleness of it from fear.

Another important system of glands, which have very short necks, is the capillary vessels. These are responsible for the insensible perspiration that is secreted on the skin, as well as the mucus of various consistencies that lubricates the spaces between the cellular membrane, muscle fibers, and larger body cavities. Because these capillaries lack a long winding structure, some have questioned whether they should be classified as glands, leading them to conclude that perspirable matter is more exuded than secreted. However, the fluid in perspiration isn't just plain water; while part of it that evaporates into the air may be, there's another part that is absorbed back into the body when healthy. In cases of illness in the absorbents, this fluid can remain on the skin's surface as flakes or hardened mucus. Another similarity to other glands is their sensitivity to certain emotions. This is evident in the way the skin deepens in color during a blush of shame or turns pale from fear.

III. Another series of glandular vessels is called the absorbent system; these open their mouths into all the cavities, and upon all those surfaces of the body, where the excretory apertures of the other glands pour out their fluids. The mouths of the absorbent system drink up a part or the whole of these fluids, and carry them forwards by their living power to their respective glands, which are called conglobate glands. There these fluids undergo some change, before they pass on into the circulation; but if they are very acrid, the conglobate gland swells, and sometimes suppurates, as in inoculation of the small-pox, in the plague, and in venereal absorptions; at other times the fluid may perhaps continue there, till it undergoes some chemical change, that renders it less noxious; or, what is more likely, till it is regurgitated by the retrograde motion of the gland in spontaneous sweats or diarrhœas, as disagreeing food is vomited from the stomach.

III. Another group of glandular vessels is known as the absorbent system; these open into all the cavities and on all the surfaces of the body where the excretory openings of other glands release their fluids. The openings of the absorbent system take in part or all of these fluids and transport them forward using their living energy to their corresponding glands, which are called conglobate glands. Here, these fluids undergo some changes before they enter the circulation; however, if they are very irritating, the conglobate gland can swell and sometimes become pus-filled, as seen in smallpox inoculation, plague, and certain venereal infections. At other times, the fluid may remain there until it undergoes a chemical change that makes it less harmful; or, more likely, until it is expelled back out by the retrograde motion of the gland during spontaneous sweating or diarrhea, similar to how disagreeable food is vomited from the stomach.

IV. As all the fluids, that pass through these glands, and capillary vessels, undergo a chemical change, acquiring new combinations, the matter of heat is at the same time given out; this is apparent, since whatever increases insensible perspiration, increases the heat of the skin; and when the action of these vessels is much increased but for a moment, as in blushing, a vivid heat on the skin is the immediate consequence. So when great bilious secretions, or those of any other gland, are produced, heat is generated in the part in proportion to the quantity of the secretion.

IV. All the fluids that flow through these glands and tiny blood vessels go through a chemical change, forming new combinations, and at the same time, heat is released. This is clear, as anything that boosts insensible sweating also raises the skin's temperature. When the activity of these vessels significantly increases, even for a moment—like during blushing—there's an immediate warmth on the skin. Similarly, when there are large amounts of bile or secretions from any other gland, heat is generated in the area proportional to the amount of secretion produced.

The heat produced on the skin by blushing may be thought by some too sudden to be pronounced a chemical effect, as the fermentations or new combinations taking place in a fluid is in general a slower process. Yet are there many chemical mixtures in which heat is given out as instantaneously; as in solutions of metals in acids, or in mixtures of essential oils and acids, as of oil of cloves and acid of nitre. So the bruised parts of an unripe apple become almost instantaneously sweet; and if the chemico-animal process of digestion be stopped for but a moment, as by fear, or even by voluntary eructation, a great quantity of air is generated, by the fermentation, which instantly succeeds the stop of digestion. By the experiments of Dr. Hales it appears, that an apple during fermentation gave up above six hundred times its bulk of air; and the materials in the stomach are such, and in such a situation, as immediately to run into fermentation, when digestion is impeded.

The heat that blushing creates on the skin might seem too quick to be called a chemical effect since fermentations or new combinations in a fluid usually happen more slowly. However, there are many chemical mixtures where heat is released almost instantly, like when metals dissolve in acids or when you mix essential oils with acids, such as clove oil and nitric acid. Similarly, the bruised parts of an unripe apple can become sweet almost right away; and if the chemical process of digestion is interrupted, even briefly, by fear or even by a voluntary burp, a large amount of air is produced due to fermentation that quickly follows the halt in digestion. Dr. Hales’ experiments show that during fermentation, an apple can release over six hundred times its volume in air; and the substances in the stomach can quickly start to ferment if digestion is blocked.

As the blood passes through the small vessels of the lungs, which connect the pulmonary artery and vein, it undergoes a change of colour from a dark to a light red; which may be termed a chemical change, as it is known to be effected by an admixture of oxygene, or vital air; which, according to a discovery of Dr. Priestley, passes through the moist membranes, which constitute the sides of these vessels. As the blood passes through the capillary vessels, and glands, which connect the aorta and its various branches with their correspondent veins in the extremities of the body, it again loses the bright red colour, and undergoes some new combinations in the glands or capillaries, in which the matter of heat is given out from the secreted fluids. This process therefore, as well as the process of respiration, has some analogy to combustion, as the vital air or oxygene seems to become united to some inflammable base, and the matter of heat escapes from the new acid, which is thus produced.

As blood flows through the small vessels in the lungs that connect the pulmonary artery and vein, it changes color from dark red to light red. This can be called a chemical change because it happens due to a mix with oxygen, or vital air, which, according to Dr. Priestley's discovery, passes through the moist membranes lining these vessels. As blood moves through the capillary vessels and glands linking the aorta and its branches to the corresponding veins in the body's extremities, it again loses its bright red color and undergoes new changes in the glands or capillaries, where heat is released from the secreted fluids. This process, along with respiration, is somewhat similar to combustion, as the vital air or oxygen seems to bond with some flammable material, resulting in heat being released from the new acid produced.

V. After the blood has passed these glands and capillaries, and parted with whatever they chose to take from it, the remainder is received by the veins, which are a set of blood-absorbing vessels in general corresponding with the ramifications of the arterial system. At the extremity of the fine convolutions of the glands the arterial force ceases; this in respect to the capillary vessels, which unite the extremities of the arteries with the commencement of the veins, is evident to the eye, on viewing the tail of a tadpole by means of a solar, or even by a common microscope, for globules of blood are seen to endeavour to pass, and to return again and again, before they become absorbed by the mouths of the veins; which returning of these globules evinces, that the arterial force behind them has ceased. The veins are furnished with valves like the lymphatic absorbents; and the great trunks of the veins, and of the lacteals and lymphatics, join together before the ingress of their fluids into the left chamber of the heart; both which evince, that the blood in the veins, and the lymph and chyle in the lacteals and lymphatics, are carried on by a similar force; otherwise the stream, which was propelled with a less power, could not enter the vessels, which contained the stream propelled with a greater power. From whence it appears, that the veins are a system of vessels absorbing blood, as the lacteals and lymphatics are a system of vessels absorbing chyle and lymph. See Sect. XXVII. 1.

V. After the blood has passed these glands and capillaries, and parted with whatever they chose to take from it, the remainder is received by the veins, which are a set of blood-absorbing vessels in general corresponding with the ramifications of the arterial system. At the extremity of the fine convolutions of the glands the arterial force ceases; this in respect to the capillary vessels, which unite the extremities of the arteries with the commencement of the veins, is evident to the eye, on viewing the tail of a tadpole by means of a solar, or even by a common microscope, for globules of blood are seen to endeavour to pass, and to return again and again, before they become absorbed by the mouths of the veins; which returning of these globules evinces, that the arterial force behind them has ceased. The veins are furnished with valves like the lymphatic absorbents; and the great trunks of the veins, and of the lacteals and lymphatics, join together before the ingress of their fluids into the left chamber of the heart; both which evince, that the blood in the veins, and the lymph and chyle in the lacteals and lymphatics, are carried on by a similar force; otherwise the stream, which was propelled with a less power, could not enter the vessels, which contained the stream propelled with a greater power. From whence it appears, that the veins are a system of vessels absorbing blood, as the lacteals and lymphatics are a system of vessels absorbing chyle and lymph. See Sect. XXVII. 1.

VI. The movements of their adapted fluids in the various vessels of the body are carried forwards by the actions of those vessels in consequence of two kinds of stimulus, one of which may be compared to a pleasurable sensation or desire inducing the vessel to seize, and, as it were, to swallow the particles thus selected from the blood; as is done by the mouths of the various glands, veins, and other absorbents, which may be called glandular appetency. The other kind of stimulus may be compared to disagreeable sensation, or aversion, as when the heart has received the blood, and is stimulated by it to push it forwards into the arteries; the same again stimulates the arteries to contract, and carry forwards the blood to their extremities, the glands and capillaries. Thus the mesenteric veins absorb the blood from the intestines by glandular appetency, and carry it forward to the vena portarum; which acting as an artery contracts itself by disagreeable stimulus, and pushes it to its ramified extremities, the various glands, which constitute the liver.

VI. The movement of their adapted fluids in the different vessels of the body is driven by the actions of those vessels in response to two types of stimulus. One type can be compared to a pleasurable feeling or desire that causes the vessel to take in and, in a sense, consume the particles picked from the blood, similar to how the mouths of various glands, veins, and other absorbents operate, which can be referred to as glandular appetite. The other type of stimulus can be likened to an unpleasant sensation or aversion; for instance, when the heart receives blood, it is activated by it to pump it forward into the arteries. This, in turn, prompts the arteries to contract and push the blood to their farthest points, the glands and capillaries. In this way, the mesenteric veins absorb blood from the intestines through glandular appetite and transport it to the vena portarum, which functions like an artery, contracting due to unpleasant stimulus and pushing blood to its branched ends, the various glands that make up the liver.

It seems probable, that at the beginning of the formation of these vessels in the embryon, an agreeable sensation was in reality felt by the glands during secretion, as is now felt in the act of swallowing palatable food; and that a disagreeable sensation was originally felt by the heart from the distention occasioned by the blood, or by its chemical stimulus; but that by habit these are all become irritative motions; that is, such motions as do not affect the whole system, except when the vessels are diseased by inflammation.

It seems likely that at the start of these vessels forming in the embryo, the glands actually experienced a pleasant sensation during secretion, similar to what we feel when swallowing good food. On the other hand, the heart probably experienced an unpleasant sensation due to the pressure from the blood or its chemical stimulation. However, over time, these sensations have turned into irritative motions, meaning they don't impact the whole system unless the vessels are affected by inflammation.



SECT. XXIV.

OF THE SECRETIONS OF SALIVA, AND OF TEARS, AND OF THE LACRYMAL SACK.

OF THE SECRETIONS OF SALIVA, AND OF TEARS, AND OF THE LACRYMAL SACK.

I. Secretion of saliva increased by mercury in the blood. 1. By the food in the mouth. Dryness of the mouth not from a deficiency of saliva. 2. By Sensitive ideas. 3. By volition. 4. By distasteful substances. It is secreted in a dilute and saline state. It then becomes more viscid. 5. By ideas of distasteful substances. 6. By nausea. 7. By aversion. 8. By catenation with stimulating substances in the ear. II. 1. Secretion of tears less in sleep. From stimulation of their excretory duct. 2. Lacrymal sack is a gland. 3. Its uses. 4. Tears are secreted, when the nasal duct is stimulated. 5. Or when it is excited by sensation. 6. Or by volition. 7. The lacrymal sack can regurgitate its contents into the eye. 8. More tears are secreted by association with the irritation of the nasal duct of the lacrymal sack, than the puncta lacrymalia can imbibe. Of the gout in the liver and stomach.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Saliva production increases due to mercury in the blood. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. From the food in the mouth. Mouth dryness isn’t caused by a lack of saliva. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. From sensitive thoughts. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. From willpower. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. From unpleasant substances. It is secreted in a diluted and salty form. Then, it becomes stickier. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. From thoughts of unpleasant substances. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. From nausea. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. From aversion. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. From connection with stimulating substances in the ear. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Tear production is lower during sleep. Resulting from the stimulation of their excretory duct. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. The lacrimal sac is a gland. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. Its purposes. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. Tears are produced when the nasal duct is stimulated. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. Or when it is triggered by sensation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. Or by willpower. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. The lacrimal sac can push its contents back into the eye. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. More tears are produced due to irritation of the nasal duct of the lacrimal sac than the puncta lacrymalia can absorb. Concerning the gout in the liver and stomach.

I. The salival glands drink up a certain fluid from the circumfluent blood, and pour it into the mouth. They are sometimes stimulated into action by the blood, that surrounds their origin, or by some part of that heterogeneous fluid: for when mercurial salts, or oxydes, are mixed with the blood, they stimulate these glands into unnatural exertions; and then an unusual quantity of saliva is separated.

I. The salivary glands absorb a specific fluid from the surrounding blood and release it into the mouth. They can be triggered into action by the blood that surrounds them or by certain components of that mixed fluid: for when mercury salts or oxides are combined with the blood, they cause these glands to work harder than normal; as a result, an unusual amount of saliva is produced.

1. As the saliva secreted by these glands is most wanted during the mastication of our food, it happens, when the terminations of their ducts in the mouth are stimulated into action, the salival glands themselves are brought into increased action at the same time by association, and separate a greater quantity of their juices from the blood; in the same manner as tears are produced in greater abundance during the stimulus of the vapour of onions, or of any other acrid material in the eye.

1. Since the saliva produced by these glands is essential when we chew our food, when the openings of their ducts in the mouth are activated, the salivary glands are also triggered to work harder, releasing more of their juices from the blood. This is similar to how we produce more tears when exposed to onion vapors or other irritating substances in the eye.

The saliva is thus naturally poured into the mouth only during the stimulus of our food in mastication; for when there is too great an exhalation of the mucilaginous secretion from the membranes, which line the mouth, or too great an absorption of it, the mouth becomes dry, though there is no deficiency in the quantity of saliva; as in those who sleep with their mouths open, and in some fevers.

The saliva is naturally released into the mouth only when we chew our food. When there’s too much release of the mucous secretion from the membranes lining the mouth, or too much absorption of it, the mouth gets dry, even if there’s enough saliva. This can happen, for example, in people who sleep with their mouths open or during certain fevers.

2. Though during the mastication of our natural food the salival glands are excited into action by the stimulus on their excretory ducts, and a due quantity of saliva is separated from the blood, and poured into the mouth; yet as this mastication of our food is always attended with a degree of pleasure; and that pleasurable sensation is also connected with our ideas of certain kinds of aliment; it follows, that when these ideas are reproduced, the pleasurable sensation arises along with them, and the salival glands are excited into action, and fill the mouth with saliva from this sensitive association, as is frequently seen in dogs, who slaver at the sight of food.

2. When we chew our natural food, the salivary glands get stimulated by the pressure on their ducts, causing a proper amount of saliva to be released from the blood into the mouth. This chewing process is always linked to a certain level of pleasure, and that pleasurable feeling is also associated with our thoughts about specific types of food. Therefore, when these thoughts come to mind, the pleasurable feeling occurs along with them, causing the salivary glands to activate and fill the mouth with saliva due to this sensitive connection, much like what we often see in dogs that drool at the sight of food.

3. We have also a voluntary power over the action of these salival glands, for we can at any time produce a flow of saliva into our mouth, and spit out, or swallow it at will.

3. We also have the ability to control the action of these salivary glands, as we can create a flow of saliva in our mouth anytime, and choose to either spit it out or swallow it as we wish.

4. If any very acrid material be held in the mouth, as the root of pyrethrum, or the leaves of tobacco, the salival glands are stimulated into stronger action than is natural, and thence secrete a much larger quantity of saliva; which is at the same time more viscid than in its natural state; because the lymphatics, that open their mouths into the ducts of the salival glands, and on the membranes, which line the mouth, are likewise stimulated into stronger action, and absorb the more liquid parts of the saliva with greater avidity; and the remainder is left both in greater quantity and more viscid.

4. When a very bitter substance, like the root of pyrethrum or tobacco leaves, is held in the mouth, the salivary glands are triggered to work harder than usual, producing a much larger amount of saliva. This saliva is also stickier than normal because the lymphatic vessels, which connect to the salivary glands and the membranes lining the mouth, are similarly stimulated and absorb more of the liquid components of the saliva more eagerly. As a result, the remaining saliva is both more abundant and stickier.

The increased absorption in the mouth by some stimulating substances, which are called astringents, as crab juice, is evident from the instant dryness produced in the mouth by a small quantity of them.

The increased absorption in the mouth by some stimulating substances, which are called astringents, like crab juice, is clear from the immediate dryness caused in the mouth by just a small amount of them.

As the extremities of the glands are of exquisite tenuity, as appears by their difficulty of injection, it was necessary for them to secrete their fluids in a very dilute state; and, probably for the purpose of stimulating them into action, a quantity of neutral salt is likewise secreted or formed by the gland. This aqueous and saline part of all secreted fluids is again reabsorbed into the habit. More than half of some secreted fluids is thus imbibed from the reservoirs, into which they are poured; as in the urinary bladder much more than half of what is secreted by the kidneys becomes reabsorbed by the lymphatics, which are thickly dispersed around the neck of the bladder. This seems to be the purpose of the urinary bladders of fish, as otherwise such a receptacle for the urine could have been of no use to an animal immersed in water.

As the ends of the glands are extremely thin, evident from how hard they are to inject, they need to release their fluids in a very diluted form. Probably to stimulate them into action, a certain amount of neutral salt is also secreted or formed by the gland. The watery and salty component of all secreted fluids is then reabsorbed into the body. More than half of some secreted fluids is absorbed again from the reservoirs where they are released; for example, in the urinary bladder, much more than half of what the kidneys secrete is reabsorbed by the lymphatics, which are densely located around the neck of the bladder. This seems to be the purpose of the urinary bladders in fish, as otherwise, such storage for urine wouldn't be useful for an animal submerged in water.

5. The idea of substances disagreeably acrid will also produce a quantity of saliva in the mouth; as when we smell very putrid vapours, we are induced to spit out our saliva, as if something disagreeable was actually upon our palates.

5. The thought of unpleasantly strong substances will also cause our mouths to produce extra saliva; like when we smell really foul odors, we instinctively want to spit out the saliva, as if something unpleasant is actually in our mouths.

6. When disagreeable food in the stomach produces nausea, a flow of saliva is excited in the mouth by association; as efforts to vomit are frequently produced by disagreeable drugs in the mouth by the same kind of association.

6. When unpleasant food in the stomach causes nausea, it triggers a flow of saliva in the mouth due to association; similarly, attempts to vomit can often be triggered by unpleasant substances in the mouth through the same type of association.

7. A preternatural flow of saliva is likewise sometimes occasioned by a disease of the voluntary power; for if we think about our saliva, and determine not to swallow it, or not to spit it out, an exertion is produced by the will, and more saliva is secreted against our wish; that is, by our aversion, which bears the same analogy to desire, as pain does to pleasure; as they are only modifications of the same disposition of the sensorium. See Class IV. 3. 2. 1.

7. An unusual increase in saliva can also sometimes occur due to a condition affecting our voluntary control; for if we focus on our saliva and decide not to swallow it or spit it out, our willpower creates a response, resulting in even more saliva being produced against our intention. This happens because of our aversion, which is similar to desire, just as pain is to pleasure; both are just different expressions of the same state of the sensory system. See Class IV. 3. 2. 1.

8. The quantity of saliva may also be increased beyond what is natural, by the catenation of the motions of these glands with other motions, or sensations, as by an extraneous body in the ear; of which I have known an instance; or by the application of stizolobium, siliqua hirsuta, cowhage, to the seat of the parotis, as some writers have affirmed.

8. The amount of saliva can also be increased beyond normal levels due to the connection of these glands' movements with other motions or sensations, like something foreign in the ear; I've seen this happen before. It can also be triggered by applying stizolobium, siliqua hirsuta, or cowhage to the area around the parotid gland, as some authors have claimed.

II. 1. The lacrymal gland drinks up a certain fluid from the circumfluent blood, and pours it on the ball of the eye, on the upper part of the external corner of the eyelids. Though it may perhaps be stimulated into the performance of its natural action by the blood, which surrounds its origin, or by some part of that heterogeneous fluid; yet as the tears secreted by this gland are more wanted at some times than at others, its secretion is variable, like that of the saliva above mentioned, and is chiefly produced when its excretory duct is stimulated; for in our common sleep there seems to be little or no secretion of tears; though they are occasionally produced by our sensations in dreams.

II. 1. The lacrimal gland absorbs a certain fluid from the surrounding blood and releases it onto the surface of the eye, specifically at the upper part of the outer corner of the eyelids. While it may be triggered to perform its natural function by the blood around it or by some part of that mixed fluid, the secretion of tears from this gland varies depending on need, much like the saliva mentioned earlier. It mainly produces tears when its duct is stimulated; during regular sleep, there seems to be little to no secretion of tears, although they can occasionally be produced by sensations experienced in dreams.

Thus when any extraneous material on the eye-ball, or the dryness of the external covering of it, or the coldness of the air, or the acrimony of some vapours, as of onions, stimulates the excretory duct of the lacrymal gland, it discharges its contents upon the ball; a quicker secretion takes place in the gland, and abundant tears succeed, to moisten, clean, and lubricate the eye. These by frequent nictitation are diffused over the whole ball, and as the external angle of the eye in winking is closed sooner than the internal angle, the tears are gradually driven forwards, and downwards from the lacrymal gland to the puncta lacrymalia.

So when something foreign gets into the eye, or when the outer layer of the eye dries out, or when the air feels cold, or when irritating substances like onion vapors stimulate the tear duct of the tear gland, it releases tears onto the surface of the eye. This triggers a faster production of tears in the gland, resulting in a lot of tears that help to moisten, clean, and lubricate the eye. As we blink frequently, these tears spread across the entire surface of the eye, and because the outer corner of the eye closes faster than the inner corner when we blink, the tears are gradually pushed forward and downward from the tear gland to the tear ducts.

2. The lacrymal sack, with its puncta lacrymalia, and its nasal duct, is a complete gland; and is singular in this respect, that it neither derives its fluid from, nor disgorges it into the circulation. The simplicity of the structure of this gland, and both the extremities of it being on the surface of the body, makes it well worthy our minuter observation; as the actions of more intricate and concealed glands may be better understood from their analogy to this.

2. The tear sac, with its tear ducts and nasal passage, is a fully functioning gland; and it's unique in that it neither takes its fluid from, nor releases it into, the bloodstream. The straightforward design of this gland, with both ends located on the surface of the body, makes it worthy of closer examination; as the functions of more complicated and hidden glands can be better understood by comparing them to this one.

3. This simple gland consists of two absorbing mouths, a belly, and an excretory duct. As the tears are brought to the internal angle of the eye, these two mouths drink them up, being stimulated into action by this fluid, which they absorb. The belly of the gland, or lacrymal sack, is thus filled, in which the saline part of the tears is absorbed, and when the other end of the gland, or nasal duct, is stimulated by the dryness, or pained by the coldness of the air, or affected by any acrimonious dust or vapour in the nostrils, it is excited into action together with the sack, and the tears are disgorged upon the membrane, which lines the nostrils; where they serve a second purpose to moisten, clean, and lubricate, the organ of smell.

3. This simple gland has two openings, a pouch, and a duct for draining. As tears collect in the inner corner of the eye, these two openings absorb them, activated by the presence of the fluid. The pouch of the gland, or lacrymal sac, fills up, soaking up the salty components of the tears. When the other end of the gland, or nasal duct, is triggered by dryness, the chill of the air, or irritated by any harsh dust or vapors in the nostrils, it activates along with the sac, releasing the tears onto the lining of the nostrils, where they also help to moisten, clean, and lubricate the sense of smell.

4. When the nasal duct of this gland is stimulated by any very acrid material, as the powder of tobacco, or volatile spirits, it not only disgorges the contents of its belly or receptacle (the lacrymal sack), and absorbs hastily all the fluid, that is ready for it in the corner of the eye; but by the association of its motions with those of the lacrymal gland, it excites that also into increased action, and a large flow of tears is poured into the eye.

4. When the nasal duct of this gland is stimulated by any harsh substance, like tobacco powder or strong spirits, it not only empties the contents of its reservoir (the lacrimal sac) and quickly takes in all the fluid that's prepared in the corner of the eye; but by coordinating its movements with those of the lacrimal gland, it also triggers that gland to become more active, resulting in a significant flow of tears into the eye.

5. This nasal duct is likewise excited into strong action by sensitive ideas, as in grief, or joy, and then also by its associations with the lacrymal gland it produces a great flow of tears without any external stimulus; as is more fully explained in Sect. XVI. 8. on Instinct.

5. This nasal duct is likewise excited into strong action by sensitive ideas, as in grief, or joy, and then also by its associations with the lacrymal gland it produces a great flow of tears without any external stimulus; as is more fully explained in Sect. XVI. 8. on Instinct.

6. There are some, famous in the arts of exciting compassion, who are said to have acquired a voluntary power of producing a flow of tears in the eye; which, from what has been said in the section on Instinct above mentioned, I should suspect, is performed by acquiring a voluntary power over the action of this nasal duct.

6. Some well-known individuals in the arts of stirring empathy are said to have gained the ability to produce tears on command; based on what was discussed in the earlier section about Instinct, I would guess that this is achieved by gaining voluntary control over the actions of the tear duct.

7. There is another circumstance well worthy our attention, that when by any accident this nasal duct is obstructed, the lacrymal sack, which is the belly or receptacle of this gland, by slight pressure of the finger is enabled to disgorge its contents again into the eye; perhaps the bile in the same manner, when the biliary ducts are obstructed, is returned into the blood by the vessels which secrete it?

7. There’s another point worth noting: when this nasal duct gets blocked for any reason, the tear sac, which is the storage area for this gland, can be squeezed lightly to release its contents back into the eye. Maybe bile works similarly—when the bile ducts are blocked, does it get sent back into the blood through the vessels that produce it?

8. A very important though minute occurrence must here be observed, that though the lacrymal gland is only excited into action, when we weep at a distressful tale, by its association with this nasal duct, as is more fully explained in Sect. XVI. 8; yet the quantity of tears secreted at once is more than the puncta lacrymalia can readily absorb; which shews that the motions occasioned by associations are frequently more energetic than the original motions, by which they were occasioned. Which we shall have occasion to mention hereafter, to illustrate, why pains frequently exist in a part distant from the cause of them, as in the other end of the urethra, when a stone stimulates the neck of the bladder. And why inflammations frequently arise in parts distant from their cause, as the gutta rosea of drinking people, from an inflamed liver.

8. A very important though minute occurrence must here be observed, that though the lacrymal gland is only excited into action, when we weep at a distressful tale, by its association with this nasal duct, as is more fully explained in Sect. XVI. 8; yet the quantity of tears secreted at once is more than the puncta lacrymalia can readily absorb; which shews that the motions occasioned by associations are frequently more energetic than the original motions, by which they were occasioned. Which we shall have occasion to mention hereafter, to illustrate, why pains frequently exist in a part distant from the cause of them, as in the other end of the urethra, when a stone stimulates the neck of the bladder. And why inflammations frequently arise in parts distant from their cause, as the gutta rosea of drinking people, from an inflamed liver.

The inflammation of a part is generally preceded by a torpor or quiescence of it; if this exists in any large congeries of glands, as in the liver, or any membranous part, as the stomach, pain is produced and chilliness in consequence of the torpor of the vessels. In this situation sometimes an inflammation of the parts succeeds the torpor; at other times a distant more sensible part becomes inflamed; whose actions have previously been associated with it; and the torpor of the first part ceases. This I apprehend happens, when the gout of the foot succeeds a pain of the biliary duct, or of the stomach. Lastly, it sometimes happens, that the pain of torpor exists without any consequent inflammation of the affected part, or of any distant part associated with it, as in the membranes about the temple and eye-brows in hemicrania, and in those pains, which occasion convulsions; if this happens to gouty people, when it affects the liver, I suppose epileptic fits are produced; and, when it affects the stomach, death is the consequence. In these cases the pulse is weak, and the extremities cold, and such medicines as stimulate the quiescent parts into action, or which induce inflammation in them, or in any distant part, which is associated with them, cures the present pain of torpor, and saves the patient.

The inflammation of a body part usually follows a period of inactivity or dormancy in that area; if this occurs in a large group of glands, like the liver, or in a membranous area, like the stomach, it causes pain and chills due to the sluggishness of the blood vessels. In this condition, sometimes the inflammation develops in the affected area after the dormancy; other times, a different area that has been connected to it becomes inflamed instead, and the dormancy of the initial area ends. This seems to happen when foot gout follows a pain in the bile duct or stomach. Finally, it can also occur that the pain from dormancy exists without any subsequent inflammation in the affected area or in any related distant area, as seen in the membranes around the temple and eyebrows during migraines, or in pain that causes convulsions; if this occurs in people with gout affecting the liver, I believe it leads to epileptic fits; and when it affects the stomach, it may result in death. In these situations, the pulse is weak, the extremities are cold, and medications that stimulate the inactive parts into action, or that cause inflammation in them or in any associated distant part, can relieve the current pain from dormancy and save the patient.

I have twice seen a gouty inflammation of the liver, attended with jaundice; the patients after a few days were both of them affected with cold fits, like ague-fits, and their feet became affected with gout, and the inflammation of their livers ceased. It is probable, that the uneasy sensations about the stomach, and indigestion, which precedes gouty paroxysms, are generally owing to torpor or slight inflammation of the liver, and biliary ducts; but where great pain with continued sickness, with feeble pulse, and sensation of cold, affect the stomach in patients debilitated by the gout, that it is a torpor of the stomach itself, and destroys the patient from the great connexion of that viscus with the vital organs. See Sect. XXV. 17.

I have twice seen a gouty inflammation of the liver, attended with jaundice; the patients after a few days were both of them affected with cold fits, like ague-fits, and their feet became affected with gout, and the inflammation of their livers ceased. It is probable, that the uneasy sensations about the stomach, and indigestion, which precedes gouty paroxysms, are generally owing to torpor or slight inflammation of the liver, and biliary ducts; but where great pain with continued sickness, with feeble pulse, and sensation of cold, affect the stomach in patients debilitated by the gout, that it is a torpor of the stomach itself, and destroys the patient from the great connexion of that viscus with the vital organs. See Sect. XXV. 17.



SECT. XXV.

OF THE STOMACH AND INTESTINES.

STOMACH AND INTESTINES.

1. Of swallowing our food. Ruminating animals. 2. Action of the stomach. 3. Action of the intestines. Irritative motions connected with these. 4. Effects of repletion. 5. Stronger action of the stomach and intestines from more stimulating food. 6. Their action inverted by still greater stimuli. Or by disgustful ideas. Or by volition. 7. Other glands strengthen or invert their motions by sympathy. 8. Vomiting performed by intervals. 9. Inversion of the cutaneous absorbents. 10. Increased secretion of bile and pancreatic juice. 11. Inversion of the lacteals. 12. And of the bile-ducts. 13. Case of a cholera. 14. Further account of the inversion of lacteals. 15. Iliac passions. Valve of the colon. 16. Cure of the iliac passion. 17. Pain of gall-stone distinguished from pain of the stomach. Gout of the stomach from torpor, from inflammation. Intermitting pulse owing to indigestion. To overdose of foxglove. Weak pulse from emetics. Death from a blow on the stomach. From gout of the stomach.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. About swallowing our food. Ruminating animals. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Function of the stomach. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Function of the intestines. Irritating movements related to these. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Consequences of being full. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Stronger activity of the stomach and intestines due to more stimulating food. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Their activity can be reversed by even stronger stimuli, unpleasant thoughts, or willpower. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Other glands can enhance or reverse their movements by sympathy. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Vomiting happening at intervals. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Reversal of the skin's absorbent actions. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Increased production of bile and pancreatic juice. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Reversal of the lacteals. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. And of the bile ducts. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. Case of cholera. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. Further details on the reversal of lacteals. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. Iliac passion. Valve of the colon. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. Treatment of iliac passion. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. Distinguishing gallstone pain from stomach pain. Stomach gout from sluggishness or inflammation. Irregular pulse due to indigestion. Effects of an overdose of foxglove. Weak pulse from emetics. Death due to a blow to the stomach. From stomach gout.

1. The throat, stomach, and intestines, may be considered as one great gland; which like the lacrymal sack above mentioned, neither begins nor ends in the circulation. Though the act of masticating our aliment belongs to the sensitive class of motions, for the pleasure of its taste induces the muscles of the jaw into action; yet the deglutition of it when masticated is generally, if not always, an irritative motion, occasioned by the application of the food already masticated to the origin of the pharinx; in the same manner as we often swallow our spittle without attending to it.

1. The throat, stomach, and intestines can be seen as one large gland; similar to the lacrimal sac mentioned earlier, it doesn't clearly start or end in the bloodstream. While chewing our food is a sensitive action, as the enjoyment of its taste makes us move our jaw muscles, the act of swallowing it afterward, once chewed, is usually, if not always, a reflex action triggered by the food's contact with the back of the throat; just like how we often swallow our saliva without really thinking about it.

The ruminating class of animals have the power to invert the motion of their gullet, and of their first stomach, from the stimulus of this aliment, when it is a little further prepared; as is their daily practice in chewing the cud; and appears to the eye of any one, who attends to them, whilst they are employed in this second mastication of their food.

The group of animals that chew their cud can reverse the movement of their throat and first stomach when their food is a bit more processed, which they do every day when they chew cud. Anyone who watches them during this second chewing of their food can easily see this happening.

2. When our natural aliment arrives into the stomach, this organ is simulated into its proper vermicular action; which beginning at the upper orifice of it, and terminating at the lower one, gradually mixes together and pushes forwards the digesting materials into the intestine beneath it.

2. When our food enters the stomach, this organ is activated to perform its natural movements; starting at the top and ending at the bottom, it gradually mixes and pushes the digesting materials into the intestine below.

At the same time the glands, that supply the gastric juices, which are necessary to promote the chemical part of the process of digestion, are stimulated to discharge their contained fluids, and to separate a further supply from the blood-vessels: and the lacteals or lymphatics, which open their mouths into the stomach, are stimulated into action, and take up some part of the digesting materials.

At the same time, the glands that produce gastric juices, which are essential for the chemical part of digestion, are prompted to release their fluids and draw in more from the blood vessels. The lacteals or lymphatics, which connect to the stomach, are also activated and absorb some of the digesting materials.

3. The remainder of these digesting materials is carried forwards into the upper intestines, and stimulates them into their peristaltic motion similar to that of the stomach; which continues gradually to mix the changing materials, and pass them along through the valve of the colon to the excretory end of this great gland, the sphincter ani.

3. The rest of these digesting materials is moved into the upper intestines, which triggers their peristaltic motion, similar to that of the stomach. This process gradually mixes the changing materials and pushes them through the valve of the colon to the excretory end of this large gland, the anal sphincter.

The digesting materials produce a flow of bile, and of pancreatic juice, as they pass along the duodenum, by stimulating the excretory ducts of the liver and pancreas, which terminate in that intestine: and other branches of the absorbent or lymphatic system, called lacteals, are excited to drink up, as it passes, those parts of the digesting materials, that are proper for their purpose, by its stimulus on their mouths.

The substances being digested create a flow of bile and pancreatic juice as they move through the duodenum. This happens by stimulating the excretory ducts of the liver and pancreas, which end in that section of the intestine. Additionally, the lacteals—part of the lymphatic system—are triggered to absorb the components of the digesting materials that are suitable for their function, thanks to the stimulation from the substances as they pass by.

4. When the stomach and intestines are thus filled with their proper food, not only the motions of the gastric glands, the pancreas, liver, and lacteal vessels, are excited into action; but at the same time the whole tribe of irritative motions are exerted with greater energy, a greater degree of warmth, colour, plumpness, and moisture, is given to the skin from the increased action of those glands called capillary vessels; pleasurable sensation is excited, the voluntary motions are less easily exerted, and at length suspended; and sleep succeeds, unless it be prevented by the stimulus of surrounding objects, or by voluntary exertion, or by an acquired habit, which was originally produced by one or other of these circumstances, as is explained in Sect. XXI. on Drunkenness.

4. When the stomach and intestines are thus filled with their proper food, not only the motions of the gastric glands, the pancreas, liver, and lacteal vessels, are excited into action; but at the same time the whole tribe of irritative motions are exerted with greater energy, a greater degree of warmth, colour, plumpness, and moisture, is given to the skin from the increased action of those glands called capillary vessels; pleasurable sensation is excited, the voluntary motions are less easily exerted, and at length suspended; and sleep succeeds, unless it be prevented by the stimulus of surrounding objects, or by voluntary exertion, or by an acquired habit, which was originally produced by one or other of these circumstances, as is explained in Sect. XXI. on Drunkenness.

At this time also, as the blood-vessels become replete with chyle, more urine is separated into the bladder, and less of it is reabsorbed; more mucus poured into the cellular membranes, and less of it reabsorbed; the pulse becomes fuller, and softer, and in general quicker. The reason why less urine and cellular mucus is absorbed after a full meal with sufficient drink is owing to the blood-vessels being fuller: hence one means to promote absorption is to decrease the resistance by emptying the vessels by venesection. From this decreased absorption the urine becomes pale as well as copious, and the skin appears plump as well as florid.

At this time, as the blood vessels fill up with chyle, more urine is produced and stored in the bladder, and less of it is reabsorbed; more mucus is released into the cellular membranes, and less of it is reabsorbed; the pulse becomes fuller, softer, and generally quicker. The reason less urine and cellular mucus is reabsorbed after a big meal with enough to drink is that the blood vessels are fuller. Therefore, one way to encourage absorption is to reduce resistance by emptying the vessels through bloodletting. As a result of this reduced absorption, the urine becomes pale and abundant, and the skin looks plump and rosy.

By daily repetition of these movements they all become connected together, and make a diurnal circle of irritative action, and if one of this chain be disturbed, the whole is liable to be put into disorder. See Sect. XX. on Vertigo.

By daily repetition of these movements they all become connected together, and make a diurnal circle of irritative action, and if one of this chain be disturbed, the whole is liable to be put into disorder. See Sect. XX. on Vertigo.

5. When the stomach and intestines receive a quantity of food, whose stimulus is greater than usual, all their motions, and those of the glands and lymphatics, are stimulated into stronger action than usual, and perform their offices with greater vigour and in less time: such are the effects of certain quantities of spice or of vinous spirit.

5. When the stomach and intestines get a larger than usual amount of food, their movement, along with that of the glands and lymphatic system, is triggered into stronger activity than normal, allowing them to work more efficiently and quickly: this is the effect of certain amounts of spices or alcoholic beverages.

6. But if the quantity or duration of these stimuli are still further increased, the stomach and throat are stimulated into a motion, whose direction is contrary to the natural one above described; and they regurgitate the materials, which they contain, instead of carrying them forwards. This retrograde motion of the stomach may be compared to the stretchings of wearied limbs the contrary way, and is well elucidated by the following experiment. Look earnestly for a minute or two on an area an inch square of pink silk, placed in a strong light, the eye becomes fatigued, the colour becomes faint, and at length vanishes, for the fatigued eye can no longer be stimulated into direct motions; then on closing the eye a green spectrum will appear in it, which is a colour directly contrary to pink, and which will appear and disappear repeatedly, like the efforts in vomiting. See Section XXIX. 11.

6. But if the quantity or duration of these stimuli are still further increased, the stomach and throat are stimulated into a motion, whose direction is contrary to the natural one above described; and they regurgitate the materials, which they contain, instead of carrying them forwards. This retrograde motion of the stomach may be compared to the stretchings of wearied limbs the contrary way, and is well elucidated by the following experiment. Look earnestly for a minute or two on an area an inch square of pink silk, placed in a strong light, the eye becomes fatigued, the colour becomes faint, and at length vanishes, for the fatigued eye can no longer be stimulated into direct motions; then on closing the eye a green spectrum will appear in it, which is a colour directly contrary to pink, and which will appear and disappear repeatedly, like the efforts in vomiting. See Section XXIX. 11.

Hence all those drugs, which by their bitter or astringent stimulus increase the action of the stomach, as camomile and white vitriol, if their quantity is increased above a certain dose become emetics.

Hence all those drugs, which by their bitter or astringent effect increase the action of the stomach, like chamomile and white vitriol, if taken in larger amounts than a certain dose, can induce vomiting.

These inverted motions of the stomach and throat are generally produced from the stimulus of unnatural food, and are attended with the sensation of nausea or sickness: but as this sensation is again connected with an idea of the distasteful food, which induced it; so an idea of nauseous food will also sometimes excite the action of nausea; and that give rise by association to the inversion of the motions of the stomach and throat. As some, who have had horse-flesh or dogs-flesh given them for beef or mutton, are said to have vomited many hours afterwards, when they have been told of the imposition.

These reversed movements of the stomach and throat usually happen because of the intake of unnatural food and come with feelings of nausea or sickness. However, since this feeling is linked to the thought of the unpleasant food that caused it, just the thought of disgusting food can sometimes trigger nausea as well. This can lead to the reversal of the stomach and throat movements. For instance, some people who have mistakenly been served horse meat or dog meat instead of beef or lamb are said to have vomited hours later when they found out they had been deceived.

I have been told of a person, who had gained a voluntary command over these inverted motions of the stomach and throat, and supported himself by exhibiting this curiosity to the public. At these exhibitions he swallowed a pint of red rough gooseberries, and a pint of white smooth ones, brought them up in small parcels into his mouth, and restored them separately to the spectators, who called for red or white as they pleased, till the whole were redelivered.

I heard about someone who had learned to control the unusual movements of their stomach and throat, and made a living by showcasing this odd talent to the public. During these shows, he would swallow a pint of red, rough gooseberries and a pint of white, smooth ones, and then bring them back up in small groups into his mouth, returning them separately to the audience. The spectators would shout for red or white as they wanted, until everything was returned.

7. At the same time that these motions of the stomach and throat are stimulated into inversion, some of the other irritative motions, that had acquired more immediate connexions with the stomach, as those of the gastric glands, are excited into stronger action by this association; and some other of these motions, which are more easily excited, as those of the gastric lymphatics, are inverted by their association with the retrograde motions of the stomach, and regurgitate their contents, and thus a greater quantity of mucus, and of lymph, or chyle, is poured into the stomach, and thrown up along with its contents.

7. At the same time that these movements of the stomach and throat are triggered to reverse, some of the other irritative movements that were more closely connected to the stomach, like those of the gastric glands, are stimulated into stronger action due to this connection; and some of these movements, which are easier to trigger, like those of the gastric lymphatics, are reversed because of their association with the backward movements of the stomach, causing them to expel their contents. As a result, a larger amount of mucus and lymph or chyle is released into the stomach and thrown up along with its contents.

8. These inversions of the motion of the stomach in vomiting are performed by intervals, for the same reason that many other motions are reciprocally exerted and relaxed; for during the time of exertion the stimulus, or sensation, which caused this exertion, is not perceived; but begins to be perceived again, as soon as the exertion ceases, and is some time in again producing its effect. As explained in Sect. XXXIV. on Volition, where it is shewn, that the contractions of the fibres, and the sensation of pain, which occasioned that exertion, cannot exist at the same time. The exertion ceases from another cause also, which is the exhaustion of the sensorial power of the part, and these two causes frequently operate together.

8. These inversions of the motion of the stomach in vomiting are performed by intervals, for the same reason that many other motions are reciprocally exerted and relaxed; for during the time of exertion the stimulus, or sensation, which caused this exertion, is not perceived; but begins to be perceived again, as soon as the exertion ceases, and is some time in again producing its effect. As explained in Sect. XXXIV. on Volition, where it is shewn, that the contractions of the fibres, and the sensation of pain, which occasioned that exertion, cannot exist at the same time. The exertion ceases from another cause also, which is the exhaustion of the sensorial power of the part, and these two causes frequently operate together.

9. At the times of these inverted efforts of the stomach not only the lymphatics, which open their mouths into the stomach, but those of the skin also, are for a time inverted; for sweats are sometimes pushed out during the efforts of vomiting without an increase of heat.

9. During these moments when the stomach is struggling, not only the lymphatic vessels that drain into the stomach are affected, but those in the skin as well; this is because sweating can occur during vomiting without an associated rise in body temperature.

10. But if by a greater stimulus the motions of the stomach are inverted still more violently or more permanently, the duodenum has its peristaltic motions inverted at the same time by their association with those of the stomach; and the bile and pancreatic juice, which it contains, are by the inverted motions brought up into the stomach, and discharged along with its contents; while a greater quantity of bile and pancreatic juice is poured into this intestine; as the glands, that secrete them, are by their association with the motions of the intestine excited into stronger action than usual.

10. But if a stronger stimulus causes the stomach to move even more violently or permanently, the duodenum also experiences reversed movements due to its connection with the stomach. This causes bile and pancreatic juice, which are in the duodenum, to be brought back up into the stomach and expelled along with its contents. At the same time, more bile and pancreatic juice are released into this intestine, as the glands that produce them are stimulated into greater activity by the duodenum's movements.

11. The other intestines are by association excited into more powerful action, while the lymphatics, that open their mouths into them, suffer an inversion of their motions corresponding with the lymphatics of the stomach, and duodenum; which with a part of the abundant secretion of bile is carried downwards, and contributes both to stimulate the bowels, and to increase the quantity of the evacuations. This inversion of the motion of the lymphatics appears from the quantity of chyle, which comes away by stools; which is otherwise absorbed as soon as produced, and by the immense quantity of thin fluid, which is evacuated along with it.

11. The other intestines respond by becoming more active, while the lymphatic vessels that connect to them reverse their movements in sync with the lymphatics of the stomach and duodenum. This, along with some of the abundant bile produced, gets pushed downwards, stimulating the bowels and increasing the amount of waste expelled. The reversal of the lymphatic movement is evidenced by the quantity of chyle that is found in the stools, which would usually be absorbed right after being produced, as well as by the large volume of thin fluid expelled alongside it.

12. But if the stimulus, which inverts the stomach, be still more powerful, or more permanent, it sometimes happens, that the motions of the biliary glands, and of their excretory ducts, are at the same time inverted, and regurgitate their contained bile into the blood-vessels, as appears by the yellow colour of the skin, and of the urine; and it is probable the pancreatic secretion may suffer an inversion at the same time, though we have yet no mark by which this can be ascertained.

12. But if the stimulus that disrupts the stomach is even stronger or more persistent, it can sometimes cause the movements of the bile glands and their ducts to also reverse, leading to the bile being released back into the bloodstream, which is indicated by the yellowing of the skin and urine. It's likely that the secretion from the pancreas may also be affected in the same way, although we currently have no way to confirm this.

13. Mr. —— eat two putrid pigeons out of a cold pigeon-pye, and drank about a pint of beer and ale along with them, and immediately rode about five miles. He was then seized with vomiting, which was after a few periods succeeded by purging; these continued alternately for two hours; and the purging continued by intervals for six or eight hours longer. During this time he could not force himself to drink more than one pint in the whole; this great inability to drink was owing to the nausea, or inverted motions of the stomach, which the voluntary exertion of swallowing could seldom and with difficulty overcome; yet he discharged in the whole at least six quarts; whence came this quantity of liquid? First, the contents of the stomach were emitted, then of the duodenum, gall-bladder, and pancreas, by vomiting. After this the contents of the lower bowels, then the chyle, that was in the lacteal vessels, and in the receptacle of chyle, was regurgitated into the intestines by a retrograde motion of these vessels. And afterwards the mucus deposited in the cellular membrane, and on the surface of all the other membranes, seems to have been absorbed; and with the fluid absorbed from the air to have been carried up their respective lymphatic branches by the increased energy of their natural motions, and down the visceral lymphatics, or lacteals, by the inversion of their motions.

13. Mr. —— ate two rotten pigeons from a cold meat pie and drank about a pint of beer and ale with them, then immediately rode about five miles. He was soon hit with vomiting, which was followed by diarrhea; these alternated for two hours, and the diarrhea continued at intervals for another six to eight hours. During this time, he couldn’t manage to drink more than a pint altogether; this inability to drink was due to nausea and the involuntary movements of his stomach, which he could hardly overcome by trying to swallow. Yet, he expelled at least six quarts in total; where did all that liquid come from? First, he vomited the contents of his stomach, then material from the duodenum, gallbladder, and pancreas. After that, the contents of the lower intestines, followed by the chyle that was in the lymphatic vessels and the chyle receptacle, were pushed back into the intestines due to a reverse motion in those vessels. Finally, the mucus that had built up in the connective tissue and on the surfaces of various membranes seems to have been absorbed, and along with the fluid absorbed from the air, was carried up through their respective lymphatic channels due to the increased activity of their normal motions, and down through the visceral lymphatics or lacteals by the reverse of their motions.

14. It may be difficult to invent experiments to demonstrate the truth of this inversion of some branches of the absorbent system, and increased absorption of others, but the analogy of these vessels to the intestinal canal, and the symptoms of many diseases, render this opinion more probable than many other received opinions of the animal œconomy.

14. It might be challenging to create experiments that prove the validity of this reversal in some parts of the absorbent system and the heightened absorption in others, but the comparison of these vessels to the intestinal tract, along with the symptoms of various diseases, makes this viewpoint more likely than many other accepted beliefs about animal physiology.

In the above instance, after the yellow excrement was voided, the fluid ceased to have any smell, and appeared like curdled milk, and then a thinner fluid, and some mucus, were evacuated; did not these seem to partake of the chyle, of the mucous fluid from all the cells of the body, and lastly, of the atmospheric moisture? All these facts may be easily observed by any one, who takes a brisk purge.

In the situation described, after the yellow waste was expelled, the fluid lost its smell and looked like curdled milk, followed by a thinner liquid and some mucus. Did these not seem to consist of chyle, the mucus from all the body’s cells, and finally, moisture from the air? Anyone who takes a strong laxative can easily observe all these facts.

15. Where the stimulus on the stomach, or on some other part of the intestinal canal, is still more permanent, not only the lacteal vessels, but the whole canal itself, becomes inverted from its associations: this is the iliac passion, in which all the fluids mentioned above are thrown up by the mouth. At this time the valve in the colon, from the inverted motions of that bowel, and the inverted action of this living valve, does not prevent the regurgitation of its contents.

15. When the stimulus on the stomach, or elsewhere in the digestive tract, becomes more constant, not just the lymphatic vessels, but the entire canal itself, gets turned upside down from its usual function: this condition is called iliac passion, where all the fluids mentioned earlier are expelled through the mouth. At this point, the valve in the colon, due to the reversed movements of that section and the contrary action of this living valve, fails to stop the backflow of its contents.

The structure of this valve may be represented by a flexile leathern pipe standing up from the bottom of a vessel of water: its sides collapse by the pressure of the ambient fluid, as a small part of that fluid passes through it; but if it has a living power, and by its inverted action keeps itself open, it becomes like a rigid pipe, and will admit the whole liquid to pass. See Sect. XXIX. 2. 5.

The structure of this valve may be represented by a flexile leathern pipe standing up from the bottom of a vessel of water: its sides collapse by the pressure of the ambient fluid, as a small part of that fluid passes through it; but if it has a living power, and by its inverted action keeps itself open, it becomes like a rigid pipe, and will admit the whole liquid to pass. See Sect. XXIX. 2. 5.

In this case the patient is averse to drink, from the constant inversion of the motions of the stomach, and yet many quarts are daily ejected from the stomach, which at length smell of excrement, and at last seem to be only a thin mucilaginous or aqueous liquor.

In this case, the patient refuses to drink because of the continuous turmoil in the stomach, yet he expels several quarts daily, which ultimately smell like feces, and eventually appear to be just a thin, slimy, or watery fluid.

From whence is it possible, that this great quantity of fluid for many successive days can be supplied, after the cells of the body have given up their fluids, but from the atmosphere? When the cutaneous branch of absorbents acts with unnatural strength, it is probable the intestinal branch has its motions inverted, and thus a fluid is supplied without entering the arterial system. Could oiling or painting the skin give a check to this disease?

From where could such a large amount of fluid be supplied for many consecutive days after the body's cells have released their fluids, if not from the atmosphere? When the skin's absorbent system works too strongly, it's likely that the intestinal absorption is reversed, allowing fluid to be supplied without going into the bloodstream. Could applying oil or paint to the skin help control this disease?

So when the stomach has its motions inverted, the lymphatics of the stomach, which are most strictly associated with it, invert their motions at the same time. But the more distant branches of lymphatics, which are less strictly associated with it, act with increased energy; as the cutaneous lymphatics in the cholera, or iliac passion, above described. And other irritative motions become decreased, as the pulsations of the arteries, from the extra-derivation or exhaustion of the sensorial power.

So when the stomach's movements are disrupted, the lymphatic vessels closely linked to it also change their movements. However, the more distant lymphatic branches, which are less closely connected, become more active; like the skin lymphatics during cholera or the conditions previously mentioned. Other irritative movements, such as the heartbeat from the overload or exhaustion of sensory power, decrease.

Sometimes when stronger vomiting takes place the more distant branches of the lymphatic system invert their motions with those of the stomach, and loose stools are produced, and cold sweats.

Sometimes when severe vomiting occurs, the more distant branches of the lymphatic system reverse their movements with those of the stomach, leading to loose stools and cold sweats.

So when the lacteals have their motions inverted, as during the operation of strong purges, the urinary and cutaneous absorbents have their motions increased to supply the want of fluid in the blood, as in great thirst; but after a meal with sufficient potation the urine is pale, that is, the urinary absorbents act weakly, no supply of water being wanted for the blood. And when the intestinal absorbents act too violently, as when too great quantities of fluid have been drank, the urinary absorbents invert their motions to carry off the superfluity, which is a new circumstance of association, and a temporary diabetes supervenes.

So when the lacteals reverse their motions, like during the use of strong laxatives, the urinary and skin absorbents increase their activity to make up for the lack of fluid in the blood, similar to experiencing intense thirst. However, after a meal with enough beverages, the urine is light, meaning the urinary absorbents are working less because there's no need for extra water in the blood. And when the intestinal absorbents become overactive, like when too much fluid has been consumed, the urinary absorbents reverse their motions to eliminate the excess, which is a new association, leading to temporary diabetes.

16. I have had the opportunity of seeing four patients in the iliac passion, where the ejected material smelled and looked like excrement. Two of these were so exhausted at the time I saw them, that more blood could not be taken from them, and as their pain had ceased, and they continued to vomit up every thing which they drank, I suspected that a mortification of the bowel had already taken place, and as they were both women advanced in life, and a mortification is produced with less preceding pain in old and weak people, these both died. The other two, who were both young men, had still pain and strength sufficient for further venesection, and they neither of them had any appearance of hernia, both recovered by repeated bleeding, and a scruple of calomel given to one, and half a dram to the other, in very small pills: the usual means of clysters, and purges joined with opiates, had been in vain attempted. I have thought an ounce or two of crude mercury in less violent diseases of this kind has been of use, by contributing to restore its natural motion to some part of the intestinal canal, either by its weight or stimulus; and that hence the whole tube recovered its usual associations of progressive peristaltic motion. I have in three cases seen crude mercury given in small doses, as one or two ounces twice a day, have great effect in stopping pertinacious vomitings.

16. I've had the chance to see four patients with iliac pain, where the expelled material smelled and looked like feces. Two of them were so weak when I examined them that no more blood could be drawn, and since their pain had stopped and they kept vomiting everything they drank, I suspected that there was already some tissue death in the bowel. Given that both were older women, and tissue death usually causes less pain in elderly and frail individuals, both passed away. The other two patients, who were young men, still had pain and enough strength for additional bloodletting. Neither showed signs of hernia, and both recovered after repeated bloodletting, with one receiving a scruple of calomel and the other half a dram in very small pills. The usual treatments of enemas and purges along with opiates had been unsuccessfully tried. I've found that one or two ounces of crude mercury in less severe cases like this has been helpful, possibly by restoring natural movement in some part of the intestines, either through its weight or stimulation; this may allow the entire digestive tract to regain its typical peristaltic motion. In three cases, I've seen small doses of crude mercury, around one or two ounces twice a day, have a significant effect in stopping persistent vomiting.

17. Besides the affections above described, the stomach is liable, like many other membranes of the body, to torpor without consequent inflammation: as happens to the membranes about the head in some cases of hemicrania, or in general head-ach. This torpor of the stomach is attended with indigestion, and consequent flatulency, and with pain, which is usually called the cramp of the stomach, and is relievable by aromatics, essential oils, alcohol, or opium.

17. In addition to the feelings mentioned above, the stomach can also experience sluggishness like many other membranes in the body, without resulting in inflammation. This can occur similarly to the membranes around the head in certain cases of migraines or general headaches. This sluggishness of the stomach leads to indigestion, gas, and pain, commonly referred to as stomach cramps, which can be alleviated with aromatics, essential oils, alcohol, or opium.

The intrusion of a gall-stone into the common bile-duct from the gall-bladder is sometimes mistaken for a pain of the stomach, as neither of them are attended with fever; but in the passage of a gall-stone, the pain is confined to a less space, which is exactly where the common bile-duct enters the duodenum, as explained in Section XXX. 1. 3. Whereas in this gastrodynia the pain is diffused over the whole stomach; and, like other diseases from torpor, the pulse is weaker, and the extremities colder, and the general debility greater, than in the passage of a gall-stone; for in the former the debility is the consequence of the pain, in the latter it is the cause of it.

The intrusion of a gall-stone into the common bile-duct from the gall-bladder is sometimes mistaken for a pain of the stomach, as neither of them are attended with fever; but in the passage of a gall-stone, the pain is confined to a less space, which is exactly where the common bile-duct enters the duodenum, as explained in Section XXX. 1. 3. Whereas in this gastrodynia the pain is diffused over the whole stomach; and, like other diseases from torpor, the pulse is weaker, and the extremities colder, and the general debility greater, than in the passage of a gall-stone; for in the former the debility is the consequence of the pain, in the latter it is the cause of it.

Though the first fits of the gout, I believe, commence with a torpor of the liver; and the ball of the toe becomes inflamed instead of the membranes of the liver in consequence of this torpor, as a coryza or catarrh frequently succeeds a long exposure of the feet to cold, as in snow, or on a moist brick-floor; yet in old or exhausted constitutions, which have been long habituated to its attacks, it sometimes commences with a torpor of the stomach, and is transferable to every membrane of the body. When the gout begins with torpor of the stomach, a painful sensation of cold occurs, which the patient compares to ice, with weak pulse, cold extremities, and sickness; this in its slighter degree is relievable by spice, wine, or opium; in its greater degree it is succeeded by sudden death, which is owing to the sympathy of the stomach with the heart, as explained below.

Though the first attacks of gout, I believe, start with sluggishness of the liver; and the big toe becomes inflamed instead of the membranes of the liver as a result of this sluggishness, similar to how a cold often follows a long exposure of the feet to cold, like in snow or on a damp brick floor; however, in older or weakened bodies that have been used to these attacks for a long time, it sometimes starts with sluggishness of the stomach and can spread to any membrane in the body. When gout starts with sluggishness of the stomach, a painful cold sensation occurs, which the patient describes as feeling like ice, accompanied by a weak pulse, cold hands and feet, and nausea; in its milder form, this can be eased with spice, wine, or opium; in its more severe form, it may lead to sudden death, which is due to the connection between the stomach and the heart, as explained below.

If the stomach becomes inflamed in consequence of this gouty torpor of it, or in consequence of its sympathy with some other part, the danger is less. A sickness and vomiting continues many days, or even weeks, the stomach rejecting every thing stimulant, even opium or alcohol, together with much viscid mucus; till the inflammation at length ceases, as happens when other membranes, as those of the joints, are the seat of gouty inflammation; as observed in Sect. XXIV. 2. 8.

If the stomach becomes inflamed in consequence of this gouty torpor of it, or in consequence of its sympathy with some other part, the danger is less. A sickness and vomiting continues many days, or even weeks, the stomach rejecting every thing stimulant, even opium or alcohol, together with much viscid mucus; till the inflammation at length ceases, as happens when other membranes, as those of the joints, are the seat of gouty inflammation; as observed in Sect. XXIV. 2. 8.

The sympathy, or association of motions, between those of the stomach and those of the heart, are evinced in many diseases. First, many people are occasionally affected with an intermission of their pulse for a few days, which then ceases again. In this case there is a stop of the motion of the heart, and at the same time a tendency to eructation from the stomach. As soon as the patient feels a tendency to the intermission of the motion of his heart, if he voluntarily brings up wind from his stomach, the stop of the heart does not occur. From hence I conclude that the stop of digestion is the primary disease; and that air is instantly generated from the aliment, which begins to ferment, if the digestive process is impeded for a moment, (see Sect. XXIII. 4.); and that the stop of the heart is in consequence of the association of the motions of these viscera, as explained in Sect. XXXV. 1. 4.; but if the little air, which is instantly generated during the temporary torpor of the stomach, be evacuated, the digestion recommences, and the temporary torpor of the heart does not follow. One patient, whom I lately saw, and who had been five or six days much troubled with this intermission of a pulsation of his heart, and who had hemicrania with some fever, was immediately relieved from them all by losing ten ounces of blood, which had what is termed an inflammatory crust on it.

The sympathy, or association of motions, between those of the stomach and those of the heart, are evinced in many diseases. First, many people are occasionally affected with an intermission of their pulse for a few days, which then ceases again. In this case there is a stop of the motion of the heart, and at the same time a tendency to eructation from the stomach. As soon as the patient feels a tendency to the intermission of the motion of his heart, if he voluntarily brings up wind from his stomach, the stop of the heart does not occur. From hence I conclude that the stop of digestion is the primary disease; and that air is instantly generated from the aliment, which begins to ferment, if the digestive process is impeded for a moment, (see Sect. XXIII. 4.); and that the stop of the heart is in consequence of the association of the motions of these viscera, as explained in Sect. XXXV. 1. 4.; but if the little air, which is instantly generated during the temporary torpor of the stomach, be evacuated, the digestion recommences, and the temporary torpor of the heart does not follow. One patient, whom I lately saw, and who had been five or six days much troubled with this intermission of a pulsation of his heart, and who had hemicrania with some fever, was immediately relieved from them all by losing ten ounces of blood, which had what is termed an inflammatory crust on it.

Another instance of this association between the motions of the stomach and heart is evinced by the exhibition of an over dose of foxglove, which induces an incessant vomiting, which is attended with very slow, and sometimes intermitting pulse.—Which continues in spite of the exhibition of wine and opium for two or three days. To the same association must be ascribed the weak pulse, which constantly attends the exhibition of emetics during their operation. And also the sudden deaths, which have been occasioned in boxing by a blow on the stomach; and lastly, the sudden death of those, who have been long debilitated by the gout, from the torpor of the stomach. See Sect. XXXV. 1. 4.

Another instance of this association between the motions of the stomach and heart is evinced by the exhibition of an over dose of foxglove, which induces an incessant vomiting, which is attended with very slow, and sometimes intermitting pulse.—Which continues in spite of the exhibition of wine and opium for two or three days. To the same association must be ascribed the weak pulse, which constantly attends the exhibition of emetics during their operation. And also the sudden deaths, which have been occasioned in boxing by a blow on the stomach; and lastly, the sudden death of those, who have been long debilitated by the gout, from the torpor of the stomach. See Sect. XXXV. 1. 4.



SECT. XXVI.

OF THE CAPILLARY GLANDS AND MEMBRANES.

OF THE CAPILLARY GLANDS AND MEMBRANES.

I. 1. The capillary vessels are glands. 2. Their excretory ducts. Experiments on the mucus of the intestines, abdomen, cellular membrane, and on the humours of the eye. 3. Scurf on the head, cough, catarrh, diarrhœa, gonorrhœa. 4. Rheumatism. Gout. Leprosy. II. 1. The most minute membranes are unorganized. 2. Larger membranes are composed of the ducts of the capillaries, and the mouths of the absorbents. 3. Mucilaginous fluid is secreted on their surfaces. III. Three kinds of rheumatism.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. The capillary vessels act as glands. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Their excretory ducts are studied. Experiments have been conducted on the mucus from the intestines, abdomen, cell membranes, and the fluids of the eye. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Conditions include scalp flakes, cough, cold symptoms, diarrhea, and gonorrhea. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Other issues include rheumatism, gout, and leprosy. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. The smallest membranes are unorganized. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Larger membranes are made up of the ducts from the capillaries and the openings of the absorbents. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. A mucilaginous fluid is secreted on their surfaces. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. There are three types of rheumatism.

I. 1. The capillary-vessels are like all the other glands except the absorbent system, inasmuch as they receive blood from the arteries, separate a fluid from it, and return the remainder by the veins.

I. 1. The capillaries are similar to the other glands, except for the lymphatic system, as they receive blood from the arteries, extract a fluid from it, and send the rest back through the veins.

2. This series of glands is of the most extensive use, as their excretory ducts open on the whole external skin forming its perspirative pores, and on the internal surfaces of every cavity of the body. Their secretion on the skin is termed insensible perspiration, which in health is in part reabsorbed by the mouths of the lymphatics, and in part evaporated in the air; the secretion on the membranes, which line the larger cavities of the body, which have external openings, as the mouth and intestinal canal, is termed mucus, but is not however coagulable by heat; and the secretion on the membranes of those cavities of the body, which have no external openings, is called lymph or water, as in the cavities of the cellular membrane, and of the abdomen; this lymph however is coagulable by the heat of boiling water. Some mucus nearly as viscid as the white of egg, which was discharged by stool, did not coagulate, though I evaporated it to one fourth of the quantity, nor did the aqueous and vitreous humours of a sheep's eye coagulate by the like experiment: but the serosity from an anasarcous leg, and that from the abdomen of a dropsical person, and the crystalline humour of a sheep's eye, coagulated in the same heat.

2. This series of glands is extremely important, as their excretory ducts open onto the entire surface of the skin, forming its sweat pores, and also on the internal surfaces of every body cavity. The secretion on the skin is called insensible perspiration, which is partly reabsorbed by the lymphatic vessels and partly evaporates into the air when a person is healthy. The secretion on the membranes lining the larger cavities of the body with external openings, like the mouth and intestines, is called mucus, but it does not coagulate with heat; the secretion in the membranes of cavities without external openings is known as lymph or water, found in places like the cellular membrane and the abdomen; this lymph, however, can coagulate when heated in boiling water. Some mucus that was nearly as thick as egg white, which was expelled by stool, did not coagulate, even after I reduced it to one fourth of its original quantity, nor did the aqueous and vitreous humors from a sheep's eye coagulate under the same experiment; but the fluid from an edematous leg, from the abdomen of a person with dropsy, and the vitreous humor of a sheep's eye all coagulated in the same heat.

3. When any of these capillary glands are stimulated into greater irritative actions, than is natural, they secrete a more copious material; and as the mouths of the absorbent system, which open in their vicinity, are at the same time stimulated into greater action, the thinner and more saline part of the secreted fluid is taken up again; and the remainder is not only more copious but also more viscid than natural. This is more or less troublesome or noxious according to the importance of the functions of the part affected: on the skin and bronchiæ, where this secretion ought naturally to evaporate, it becomes so viscid as to adhere to the membrane; on the tongue it forms a pellicle, which can with difficulty be scraped off; produces the scurf on the heads of many people; and the mucus, which is spit up by others in coughing. On the nostrils and fauces, when the secretion of these capillary glands is increased, it is termed simple catarrh; when in the intestines, a mucous diarrhœa; and in the urethra, or vagina, it has the name of gonorrhœa, or fluor albus.

3. When any of these capillary glands are stimulated to act more intensely than usual, they produce a larger amount of fluid; and as the nearby absorbent system opens up and goes into overdrive, the thinner and saltier part of this fluid gets reabsorbed, while the rest is not only larger in quantity but also stickier than normal. This can be more or less annoying or harmful depending on how important the affected area’s functions are: on the skin and bronchi, where this fluid is supposed to evaporate, it becomes so sticky that it clings to the membrane; on the tongue, it forms a film that’s hard to scrape off; it creates dandruff on many people's scalps; and the mucus that some people cough up. In the nostrils and throat, when these capillary glands produce more fluid, it’s called simple catarrh; in the intestines, it’s known as mucous diarrhea; and in the urethra or vagina, it’s referred to as gonorrhea or leukorrhea.

4. When these capillary glands become inflamed, a still more viscid or even cretaceous humour is produced upon the surfaces of the membranes, which is the cause or the effect of rheumatism, gout, leprosy, and of hard tumours of the legs, which are generally termed scorbutic; all which will be treated of hereafter.

4. When these tiny glands get inflamed, a thicker or even chalky fluid forms on the surfaces of the membranes, which can cause or result from conditions like rheumatism, gout, leprosy, and the hard lumps in the legs commonly called scurvy-related; all of which will be discussed later.

II. 1. The whole surface of the body, with all its cavities and contents, are covered with membrane. It lines every vessel, forms every cell, and binds together all the muscular and perhaps the osseous fibres of the body; and is itself therefore probably a simpler substance than those fibres. And as the containing vessels of the body from the largest to the least are thus lined and connected with membranes, it follows that these membranes themselves consisted of unorganized materials.

II. 1. The entire surface of the body, along with all its cavities and contents, is covered with a membrane. It lines every blood vessel, forms every cell, and connects all the muscle and possibly bone fibers in the body; therefore, it is likely a simpler substance than those fibers. Since the vessels in the body, from the largest to the smallest, are lined and connected with membranes, it follows that these membranes are made up of unorganized materials.

For however small we may conceive the diameters of the minutest vessels of the body, which escape our eyes and glasses, yet these vessels must consist of coats or sides, which are made up of an unorganized material, and which are probably produced from a gluten, which hardens after its production, like the silk or web of caterpillars and spiders. Of this material consist the membranes, which line the shells of eggs, and the shell itself, both which are unorganized, and are formed from mucus, which hardens after it is formed, either by the absorption of its more fluid part, or by its uniting with some part of the atmosphere. Such is also the production of the shells of snails, and of shell-fish, and I suppose of the enamel of the teeth.

For however small we may think the diameters of the tiniest vessels in the body, which are invisible to our eyes and instruments, these vessels must have walls or layers made of an unorganized material, probably produced from a substance that hardens after formation, similar to the silk or web of caterpillars and spiders. This material forms the membranes that line egg shells, as well as the shells themselves, both of which are unorganized and made from mucus that solidifies after formation, either by losing its more liquid component or by interacting with something in the atmosphere. The same process creates the shells of snails and shellfish, and I suppose it also applies to the enamel of teeth.

2. But though the membranes, that compose the sides of the most minute vessels, are in truth unorganized materials, yet the larger membranes, which are perceptible to the eye, seem to be composed of an intertexture of the mouths of the absorbent system, and of the excretory ducts of the capillaries, with their concomitant arteries, veins, and nerves: and from this construction it is evident, that these membranes must possess great irritability to peculiar stimuli, though they are incapable of any motions, that are visible to the naked eye: and daily experience shews us, that in their inflamed state they have the greatest sensibility to pain, as in the pleurisy and paronychia.

2. But while the membranes that make up the sides of even the tiniest vessels are actually unstructured materials, the larger membranes that you can see seem to be made up of a network of the openings of the absorbent system and the excretory ducts of the capillaries, along with their related arteries, veins, and nerves. From this structure, it’s clear that these membranes must have a high sensitivity to specific stimuli, even though they can’t perform any movements that are visible to the naked eye. Daily experience shows us that when they are inflamed, they are extremely sensitive to pain, as seen in conditions like pleurisy and paronychia.

3. On all these membranes a mucilaginous or aqueous fluid is secreted, which moistens and lubricates their surfaces, as was explained in Section XXIII. 2. Some have doubted, whether this mucus is separated from the blood by an appropriated set of glands, or exudes through the membranes, or is an abrasion or destruction of the surface of the membrane itself, which is continually repaired on the other side of it, but the great analogy between the capillary vessels, and the other glands, countenances the former opinion; and evinces, that these capillaries are the glands, that secrete it; to which we must add, that the blood in passing these capillary vessels undergoes a change in its colour from florid to purple, and gives out a quantity of heat; from whence, as in other glands, we must conclude that something is secreted from it.

3. On all these membranes a mucilaginous or aqueous fluid is secreted, which moistens and lubricates their surfaces, as was explained in Section XXIII. 2. Some have doubted, whether this mucus is separated from the blood by an appropriated set of glands, or exudes through the membranes, or is an abrasion or destruction of the surface of the membrane itself, which is continually repaired on the other side of it, but the great analogy between the capillary vessels, and the other glands, countenances the former opinion; and evinces, that these capillaries are the glands, that secrete it; to which we must add, that the blood in passing these capillary vessels undergoes a change in its colour from florid to purple, and gives out a quantity of heat; from whence, as in other glands, we must conclude that something is secreted from it.

III. The seat of rheumatism is in the membranes, or upon them; but there are three very distinct diseases, which commonly are confounded under this name. First, when a membrane becomes affected with torpor, or inactivity of the vessels which compose it, pain and coldness succeed, as in the hemicrania, and other head-achs, which are generally termed nervous rheumatism; they exist whether the part be at rest or in motion, and are generally attended with other marks of debility.

III. Rheumatism affects the membranes, or is associated with them; however, there are three very different diseases that are often mistakenly grouped together under this term. First, when a membrane becomes sluggish or its blood vessels don’t function properly, pain and coldness follow, similar to hemicrania and other headaches, which are commonly called nervous rheumatism; these pains occur whether the affected area is at rest or in motion, and are usually accompanied by other signs of weakness.

Another rheumatism is said to exist, when inflammation and swelling, as well as pain, affect some of the membranes of the joints, as of the ancles, wrists, knees, elbows, and sometimes of the ribs. This is accompanied with fever, is analogous to pleurisy and other inflammations, and is termed the acute rheumatism.

Another type of rheumatism is said to occur when inflammation, swelling, and pain affect the membranes around the joints, such as the ankles, wrists, knees, elbows, and sometimes the ribs. This condition comes with a fever, is similar to pleurisy and other types of inflammation, and is called acute rheumatism.

A third disease is called chronic rheumatism, which is distinguished from that first mentioned, as in this the pain only affects the patient during the motion of the part, and from the second kind of rheumatism above described, as it is not attended with quick pulse or inflammation. It is generally believed to succeed the acute rheumatism of the same part, and that some coagulable lymph, or cretaceous, or calculous material, has been left on the membrane; which gives pain, when the muscles move over it, as some extraneous body would do, which was too insoluble to be absorbed. Hence there is an analogy between this chronic rheumatism and the diseases which produce gravel or gout-stones; and it may perhaps receive relief from the same remedies, such as aerated sal soda.

A third condition is called chronic rheumatism, which is different from the first one mentioned because the pain only bothers the patient when they move the affected part. It also differs from the second type of rheumatism described earlier, as it doesn't come with a rapid pulse or inflammation. It's commonly thought to follow acute rheumatism in the same area, where some thickened lymph, or chalky or stone-like material, remains on the membrane; this causes pain when the muscles move over it, similar to how a foreign object would when it cannot be absorbed. Therefore, there’s a similarity between this chronic rheumatism and the conditions that create gravel or gout stones; it might also respond to the same treatments, such as carbonated soda.



SECT. XXVII.

OF HÆMORRHAGES.

OF HEMORRHAGES.

I. The veins are absorbent vessels. 1. Hæmorrhages from inflammation. Case of hæmorrhage from the kidney cured by cold bathing. Case of hæmorrhage from the nose cured by cold immersion. II. Hæmorrhage from venous paralysis. Of Piles. Black stools. Petechiæ. Consumption. Scurvy of the lungs. Blackness of the face and eyes in epileptic fits. Cure of hæmorrhages from venous inability.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. The veins are vessels that absorb. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Bleeding from inflammation. A case of bleeding from the kidney treated with cold baths. A case of bleeding from the nose treated with cold immersion. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Bleeding from venous paralysis. Hemorrhoids. Black stools. Small red spots on the skin. Tuberculosis. Lung scurvy. Darkening of the face and eyes during epileptic seizures. Treatment of bleeding due to vein weakness.

I. As the imbibing mouths of the absorbent system already described open on the surface, and into the larger cavities of the body, so there is another system of absorbent vessels, which are not commonly esteemed such, I mean the veins, which take up the blood from the various glands and capillaries, after their proper fluids or secretions have been separated from it.

I. As the drinking openings of the absorbent system previously mentioned open up on the surface and into the larger spaces of the body, there exists another system of absorbent vessels, which are not typically recognized as such—I'm referring to the veins. They collect blood from the various glands and capillaries after their specific fluids or secretions have been filtered out.

The veins resemble the other absorbent vessels; as the progression of their contents is carried on in the same manner in both, they alike absorb their appropriated fluids, and have valves to prevent its regurgitation by the accidents of mechanical violence. This appears first, because there is no pulsation in the very beginnings of the veins, as is seen by microscopes; which must happen, if the blood was carried into them by the actions of the arteries. For though the concurrence of various venous streams of blood from different distances must prevent any pulsation in the larger branches, yet in the very beginnings of all these branches a pulsation must unavoidably exist, if the circulation in them was owing to the intermitted force of the arteries. Secondly, the venous absorption of blood from the penis, and from the teats of female animals after their erection, is still more similar to the lymphatic absorption, as it is previously poured into cells, where all arterial impulse must cease.

The veins are similar to other absorbent vessels; both absorb their designated fluids in the same way and have valves to stop any backflow caused by physical force. This is evident for two reasons: first, there’s no pulsation at the very start of the veins, as seen through microscopes, which would occur if blood was being pushed into them by the arteries. Even though the merging of various venous blood streams from different distances prevents pulsation in the larger branches, there has to be some pulsation at the beginnings of these branches if their circulation was due to the intermittent force of the arteries. Secondly, the way veins absorb blood from the penis and from the teats of female animals after they’ve been stimulated is even more like lymphatic absorption, as the blood is first collected in cells where all arterial pressure must stop.

There is an experiment, which seems to evince this venous absorption, which consists in the external application of a stimulus to the lips, as of vinegar, by which they become instantly pale; that is, the bibulous mouths of the veins by this stimulus are excited to absorb the blood faster, than it can be supplied by the usual arterial exertion. See Sect. XXIII. 5.

There is an experiment, which seems to evince this venous absorption, which consists in the external application of a stimulus to the lips, as of vinegar, by which they become instantly pale; that is, the bibulous mouths of the veins by this stimulus are excited to absorb the blood faster, than it can be supplied by the usual arterial exertion. See Sect. XXIII. 5.

There are two kinds of hæmorrhages frequent in diseases, one is where the glandular or capillary action is too powerfully exerted, and propels the blood forwards more hastily, than the veins can absorb it; and the other is, where the absorbent power of the veins is diminished, or a branch of them is become totally paralytic.

There are two types of bleeding commonly found in illnesses. One occurs when the glandular or capillary action is too strong, pushing the blood forward faster than the veins can take it in. The other happens when the veins' ability to absorb is reduced, or a branch of the veins has become completely paralyzed.

1. The former of these cases is known by the heat of the part, and the general fever or inflammation that accompanies the hæmorrhage. An hæmorrhage from the nose or from the lungs is sometimes a crisis of inflammatory diseases, as of the hepatitis and gout, and generally ceases spontaneously, when the vessels are considerably emptied. Sometimes the hæmorrhage recurs by daily periods accompanying the hot fits of fever, and ceasing in the cold fits, or in the intermissions; this is to be cured by removing the febrile paroxysms, which will be treated of in their place. Otherwise it is cured by venesection, by the internal or external preparations of lead, or by the application of cold, with an abstemious diet, and diluting liquids, like other inflammations. Which by inducing a quiescence on those glandular parts, that are affected, prevents a greater quantity of blood from being protruded forwards, than the veins are capable of absorbing.

1. The first of these cases is recognized by the warmth of the affected area and the overall fever or inflammation that comes with the bleeding. Nosebleeds or bleeding from the lungs can sometimes indicate a crisis in inflammatory diseases like hepatitis and gout, and generally stop on their own when the blood vessels are significantly emptied. At times, the bleeding occurs in daily cycles, coinciding with the fever's hot phases and stopping during the cold phases or breaks; this can be treated by addressing the fever attacks, which will be discussed later. Otherwise, it can be treated by bloodletting, using internal or external lead preparations, or applying cold, along with a light diet and fluids, similar to other inflammatory conditions. This approach helps to calm the affected glandular areas, preventing more blood from being pushed forward than the veins can handle.

Mr. B—— had an hæmorrhage from his kidney, and parted with not less than a pint of blood a day (by conjecture) along with his urine for above a fortnight: venesections, mucilages, balsams, preparations of lead, the bark, alum, and dragon's blood, opiates, with a large blister on his loins, were separately tried, in large doses, to no purpose. He was then directed to bathe in a cold spring up to the middle of his body only, the upper part being covered, and the hæmorrhage diminished at the first, and ceased at the second immersion.

Mr. B—— had a kidney hemorrhage and was losing about a pint of blood a day (according to estimates) along with his urine for over two weeks. Treatments such as bloodletting, mucilages, balsams, lead-based medications, quinine, alum, and dragon's blood, as well as opiates and a large blister on his lower back, were all attempted in high doses, but none were effective. He was then advised to bathe in a cold spring up to his waist only, keeping the upper part of his body covered, and the bleeding decreased after the first bath and stopped completely after the second immersion.

In this case the external capillaries were rendered quiescent by the coldness of the water, and thence a less quantity of blood was circulated through them; and the internal capillaries, or other glands, became quiescent from their irritative associations with the external ones; and the hæmorrhage was stopped a sufficient time for the ruptured vessels to contract their apertures, or for the blood in those apertures to coagulate.

In this situation, the external capillaries were made inactive by the coldness of the water, resulting in a reduced flow of blood through them. The internal capillaries and other glands also became inactive due to their connection with the external ones, and the bleeding was halted long enough for the damaged vessels to close up or for the blood in those openings to clot.

Mrs. K—— had a continued haemorrhage from her nose for some days; the ruptured vessel was not to be reached by plugs up the nostrils, and the sensibility of her fauces was such that nothing could be born behind the uvula. After repeated venesection, and other common applications, she was directed to immerse her whole head into a pail of water, which was made colder by the addition of several handfuls of salt, and the hæmorrhage immediately ceased, and returned no more; but her pulse continued hard, and she was necessitated to lose blood from the arm on the succeeding day.

Mrs. K—— had a persistent nosebleed for several days; the ruptured blood vessel couldn't be reached with nasal plugs, and her throat was so sensitive that nothing could be passed behind the uvula. After multiple bloodlettings and other common treatments, she was instructed to submerge her entire head in a bucket of water, which was made colder by adding several handfuls of salt. The bleeding stopped immediately and didn’t return, but her pulse remained strong, and she had to have more blood drawn from her arm the next day.

Query, might not the cold bath instantly stop hæmorrhages from the lungs in inflammatory cases?—for the shortness of breath of those, who go suddenly into cold water, is not owing to the accumulation of blood in the lungs, but to the quiescence of the pulmonary capillaries from association, as explained in Section XXXII. 3. 2.

Query, might not the cold bath instantly stop hæmorrhages from the lungs in inflammatory cases?—for the shortness of breath of those, who go suddenly into cold water, is not owing to the accumulation of blood in the lungs, but to the quiescence of the pulmonary capillaries from association, as explained in Section XXXII. 3. 2.

II. The other kind of hæmorrhage is known from its being attended with a weak pulse, and other symptoms of general debility, and very frequently occurs in those, who have diseased livers, owing to intemperance in the use of fermented liquors. These constitutions are shewn to be liable to paralysis of the lymphatic absorbents, producing the various kinds of dropsies in Section XXIX. 5. Now if any branch of the venous system loses its power of absorption, the part swells, and at length bursts and discharges the blood, which the capillaries or other glands circulate through them.

II. The other kind of hæmorrhage is known from its being attended with a weak pulse, and other symptoms of general debility, and very frequently occurs in those, who have diseased livers, owing to intemperance in the use of fermented liquors. These constitutions are shewn to be liable to paralysis of the lymphatic absorbents, producing the various kinds of dropsies in Section XXIX. 5. Now if any branch of the venous system loses its power of absorption, the part swells, and at length bursts and discharges the blood, which the capillaries or other glands circulate through them.

It sometimes happens that the large external veins of the legs burst, and effuse their blood; but this occurs most frequently in the veins of the intestines, as the vena portarum is liable to suffer from a schirrus of the liver opposing the progression of the blood, which is absorbed from the intestines. Hence the piles are a symptom of hepatic obstruction, and hence the copious discharges downwards or upwards of a black material, which has been called melancholia, or black bile; but is no other than the blood, which is probably discharged from the veins of the intestines.

It sometimes happens that the large veins in the legs burst and release blood, but this occurs more often in the veins of the intestines. The portal vein can be affected by liver scarring, which blocks the flow of blood that’s absorbed from the intestines. Therefore, hemorrhoids are a sign of liver blockage, and that's why there are large amounts of dark material being released, referred to as melancholia or black bile, but it's actually just blood likely coming from the intestinal veins.

J.F. Meckel, in his Experimenta de Finibus Vasorum, published at Berlin, 1772, mentions his discovery of a communication of a lymphatic vessel with the gastric branch of the vena portarum. It is possible, that when the motion of the lymphatic becomes retrograde in some diseases, that blood may obtain a passage into it, where it anastomoses with the vein, and thus be poured into the intestines. A discharge of blood with the urine sometimes attends diabetes, and may have its source in the same manner.

J.F. Meckel, in his Experimenta de Finibus Vasorum, published in Berlin in 1772, discusses his finding of a connection between a lymphatic vessel and the gastric branch of the vena portarum. It's possible that when the flow of lymph becomes reversed in certain diseases, blood might enter it, where it connects with the vein, and be directed into the intestines. Blood in the urine can sometimes occur with diabetes and might originate in a similar way.

Mr. A——, who had been a hard drinker, and had the gutta rosacea on his face and breast, after a stroke of the palsy voided near a quart of a black viscid material by stool: on diluting it with water it did not become yellow, as it must have done if it had been inspissated bile, but continued black like the grounds of coffee.

Mr. A——, who had been a heavy drinker and had rosacea on his face and chest, after suffering a stroke, passed nearly a quart of a thick black substance in his stool: when diluted with water, it didn't turn yellow as it would have if it had been thickened bile, but remained black like coffee grounds.

But any other part of the venous system may become quiescent or totally paralytic as well as the veins of the intestines: all which occur more frequently in those who have diseased livers, than in any others. Hence troublesome bleedings of the nose, or from the lungs with a weak pulse; hence hæmorrhages from the kidneys, too great menstruation; and hence the oozing of blood from every part of the body, and the petechiæ in those fevers, which are termed putrid, and which is erroneously ascribed to the thinness of the blood: for the blood in inflammatory diseases is equally fluid before it coagulates in the cold air.

But any other part of the venous system can become inactive or completely paralyzed, just like the veins in the intestines. This happens more often in people with damaged livers than in others. As a result, there can be annoying nosebleeds or bleeding from the lungs along with a weak pulse; also, there can be bleeding from the kidneys, excessive menstruation, and blood oozing from various parts of the body, along with petechiae in fevers known as putrid, which is mistakenly blamed on the blood being too thin. The blood in inflammatory diseases is just as fluid before it clots in cold air.

Is not that hereditary consumption, which occurs chiefly in dark-eyed people about the age of twenty, and commences with slight pulmonary hæmorrhages without fever, a disease of this kind?—These hæmorrhages frequently begin during sleep, when the irritability of the lungs is not sufficient in these patients to carry on the circulation without the assistance of volition; for in our waking hours, the motions of the lungs are in part voluntary, especially if any difficulty of breathing renders the efforts of volition necessary. See Class I. 2. 1. 3. and Class III. 2. 1. 12. Another species of pulmonary consumption which seems more certainly of scrophulous origin is described in the next Section, No. 2.

Is not that hereditary consumption, which occurs chiefly in dark-eyed people about the age of twenty, and commences with slight pulmonary hæmorrhages without fever, a disease of this kind?—These hæmorrhages frequently begin during sleep, when the irritability of the lungs is not sufficient in these patients to carry on the circulation without the assistance of volition; for in our waking hours, the motions of the lungs are in part voluntary, especially if any difficulty of breathing renders the efforts of volition necessary. See Class I. 2. 1. 3. and Class III. 2. 1. 12. Another species of pulmonary consumption which seems more certainly of scrophulous origin is described in the next Section, No. 2.

I have seen two cases of women, of about forty years of age, both of whom were seized with quick weak pulse, with difficult respiration, and who spit up by coughing much viscid mucus mixed with dark coloured blood. They had both large vibices on their limbs, and petechiæ; in one the feet were in danger of mortification, in the other the legs were œdematous. To relieve the difficult respiration, about six ounces of blood were taken from one of them, which to my surprise was sizy, like inflamed blood: they had both palpitations or unequal pulsations of the heart. They continued four or five weeks with pale and bloated countenances, and did not cease spitting phlegm mixed with black blood, and the pulse seldom slower than 130 or 135 in a minute. This blood, from its dark colour, and from the many vibices and petechiæ, seems to have been venous blood; the quickness of the pulse, and the irregularity of the motion of the heart, are to be ascribed to debility of that part of the system; as the extravasation of blood originated from the defect of venous absorption. The approximation of these two cases to sea-scurvy is peculiar, and may allow them to be called scorbutus pulmonalis. Had these been younger subjects, and the paralysis of the veins had only affected the lungs, it is probable the disease would have been a pulmonary consumption.

I have observed two cases of women, both around forty years old, who were affected by a rapid, weak pulse, difficulty breathing, and who coughed up thick mucus mixed with dark blood. They both had large bruises on their limbs and small red spots; in one case, the feet were at risk of dying off, while in the other, the legs were swollen. To help with the breathing, about six ounces of blood were drawn from one of them, which surprisingly was thick and inflamed. Both experienced palpitations or irregular heartbeats. They continued for four or five weeks with pale, swollen faces, and kept coughing up phlegm mixed with black blood, with a pulse rarely slower than 130 or 135 beats per minute. This dark blood, along with the bruises and red spots, seems to indicate it was venous blood. The rapid pulse and irregular heartbeats can be attributed to weakness in that area of the body, as the blood leakage was due to a failure in venous absorption. The similarity of these two cases to scurvy is notable, and they might be identified as pulmonary scurvy. If these had been younger patients and the vein paralysis had only affected the lungs, it’s likely the condition would have been diagnosed as pulmonary tuberculosis.

Last week I saw a gentleman of Birmingham, who had for ten days laboured under great palpitation of his heart, which was so distinctly felt by the hand, as to discountenance the idea of there being a fluid in the pericardium. He frequently spit up mucus stained with dark coloured blood, his pulse very unequal and very weak, with cold hands and nose. He could not lie down at all, and for about ten days past could not sleep a minute together, but waked perpetually with great uneasiness. Could those symptoms be owing to very extensive adhesions of the lungs? or is this a scorbutus pulmonalis? After a few days he suddenly got so much better as to be able to sleep many hours at a time by the use of one grain of powder of foxglove twice a day, and a grain of opium at night. After a few days longer, the bark was exhibited, and the opium continued with some wine; and the palpitations of his heart became much relieved, and he recovered his usual degree of health, but died suddenly some months afterwards.

Last week, I met a man from Birmingham who had been struggling with severe heart palpitations for ten days. The rhythm was so strong that you could feel it by touch, which made it hard to believe there was any fluid in the pericardium. He often coughed up mucus that was tinged with dark blood, and his pulse was very irregular and weak, accompanied by cold hands and nose. He couldn’t lie down at all, and for about ten days, he couldn’t sleep a wink, waking up constantly in distress. Could these symptoms be due to extensive lung adhesions? Or is this some kind of lung scurvy? After a few days, he suddenly improved enough to sleep for several hours at a time by taking one grain of foxglove powder twice a day and a grain of opium at night. A few days later, he was given quinine, and the opium continued along with some wine; the heart palpitations significantly eased, and he returned to his normal health, but unfortunately, he died suddenly a few months later.

In epileptic fits the patients frequently become black in the face, from the temporary paralysis of the venous system of this part. I have known two instances where the blackness has continued many days. M. P——, who had drank intemperately, was seized with the epilepsy when he was in his fortieth year; in one of these fits the white part of his eyes was left totally black with effused blood; which was attended with no pain or heat, and was in a few weeks gradually absorbed, changing colour as is usual with vibices from bruises.

During epileptic seizures, patients often develop a dark discoloration in their faces due to temporary paralysis of the veins in that area. I've seen two cases where this darkening lasted for several days. M. P——, who drank excessively, experienced epilepsy at the age of forty; during one of these seizures, the whites of his eyes became completely black from blood pooling, without any pain or heat, and within a few weeks, it was gradually absorbed, changing color as is typical with bruising.

The hæmorrhages produced from the inability of the veins to absorb the refluent blood, is cured by opium, the preparations of steel, lead, the bark, vitriolic acid, and blisters; but these have the effect with much more certainty, if a venesection to a few ounces, and a moderate cathartic with four or six grains of calomel be premised, where the patient is not already too much debilitated; as one great means of promoting the absorption of any fluid consists in previously emptying the vessels, which are to receive it.

The bleeding caused by the veins' inability to absorb returning blood can be treated with opium, preparations of steel, lead, bark, vitriolic acid, and blisters. However, these treatments are much more effective if you start with a bloodletting of a few ounces and a moderate laxative with four to six grains of calomel, as long as the patient isn't already too weak. One important way to encourage the absorption of any fluid is to first empty the vessels that will receive it.



SECT. XXVIII.

OF THE PARALYSIS OF THE ABSORBENT SYSTEM.

OF THE PARALYSIS OF THE ABSORBENT SYSTEM.

I. Paralysis of the lacteals, atrophy. Distaste to animal food. II. Cause of dropsy. Cause of herpes. Scrophula. Mesenteric consumption. Pulmonary consumption. Why ulcers in the lungs are so difficult to heal.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Paralysis of the lymphatic vessels, loss of tissue. Dislike for meat. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Cause of swelling. Cause of skin rashes. Tuberculosis of the lymph nodes. Consumption of the intestines. Lung disease. Explanation for why lung ulcers are hard to heal.

The term paralysis has generally been used to express the loss of voluntary motion, as in the hemiplagia, but may with equal propriety be applied to express the disobediency of the muscular fibres to the other kinds of stimulus; as to those of irritation or sensation.

The term paralysis is typically used to describe the loss of voluntary movement, like in hemiplegia, but it can also properly refer to the inability of muscle fibers to respond to other types of stimuli, such as irritation or sensation.

I. There is a species of atrophy, which has not been well understood; when the absorbent vessels of the stomach and intestines have been long inured to the stimulus of too much spirituous liquor, they at length, either by the too sudden omission of fermented or spirituous potation, or from the gradual decay of nature, become in a certain degree paralytic; now it is observed in the larger muscles of the body, when one side is paralytic, the other is more frequently in motion, owing to the less expenditure of sensorial power in the paralytic limbs; so in this case the other part of the absorbent system acts with greater force, or with greater perseverance, in consequence of the paralysis of the lacteals; and the body becomes greatly emaciated in a small time.

I. There is a type of atrophy that isn't well understood; when the absorbent vessels of the stomach and intestines have been used to too much alcohol for a long time, they eventually become somewhat paralyzed, whether due to the sudden stopping of alcohol or from natural decline over time. It’s noted that in the larger muscles of the body, when one side is paralyzed, the other side often moves more because there's less use of energy in the paralyzed limbs. In this situation, the other part of the absorbent system works harder or more consistently due to the paralysis of the lacteals, leading to significant weight loss in a short period.

I have seen several patients in this disease, of which the following are the circumstances. 1. They were men about fifty years of age, and had lived freely in respect to fermented liquors. 2. They lost their appetite to animal food. 3. They became suddenly emaciated to a great degree. 4. Their skins were dry and rough. 5. They coughed and expectorated with difficulty a viscid phlegm. 6. The membrane of the tongue was dry and red, and liable to become ulcerous.

I have seen several patients with this disease, and here are the details. 1. They were men around fifty years old and had lived freely regarding alcohol consumption. 2. They lost their appetite for meat. 3. They suddenly became severely emaciated. 4. Their skin was dry and rough. 5. They coughed and had trouble bringing up thick mucus. 6. The membrane of their tongues was dry and red and prone to becoming ulcerated.

The inability to digest animal food, and the consequent distaste to it, generally precedes the dropsy, and other diseases, which originate from spirituous potation. I suppose when the stomach becomes inirritable, that there is at the same time a deficiency of gastric acid; hence milk seldom agrees with these patients, unless it be previously curdled, as they have not sufficient gastric acid to curdle it; and hence vegetable food, which is itself acescent, will agree with their stomachs longer than animal food, which requires more of the gastric acid for its digestion.

The inability to digest animal food and the resulting aversion to it usually comes before dropsy and other illnesses that stem from drinking alcohol. I think when the stomach becomes irritable, there's also a lack of gastric acid; that's why milk often doesn't sit well with these patients unless it's been curdled beforehand, as they don't have enough gastric acid to curdle it. Therefore, plant-based foods, which are naturally acidic, tend to be more agreeable to their stomachs for a longer time than animal food, which needs more gastric acid for digestion.

In this disease the skin is dry from the increased absorption of the cutaneous lymphatics, the fat is absorbed from the increased absorption of the cellular lymphatics, the mucus of the lungs is too viscid to be easily spit up by the increased absorption of the thinner parts of it, the membrana sneideriana becomes dry, covered with hardened mucus, and at length becomes inflamed and full of aphthæ, and either these sloughs, or pulmonary ulcers, terminate the scene.

In this disease, the skin is dry because of increased absorption by the lymphatic system, fat is absorbed due to increased absorption in the cellular lymphatics, and the mucus in the lungs is too thick to be easily coughed up because the thinner parts are absorbed more. The Schneiderian membrane becomes dry, coated with hardened mucus, and eventually becomes inflamed and filled with sores, leading to either these sores sloughing off or the development of pulmonary ulcers, which ultimately ends the situation.

II. The immediate cause of dropsy is the paralysis of some other branches of the absorbent system, which are called lymphatics, and which open into the larger cavities of the body, or into the cells of the cellular membrane; whence those cavities or cells become distended with the fluid, which is hourly secreted into them for the purpose of lubricating their surfaces. As is more fully explained in No. 5. of the next Section.

II. The immediate cause of dropsy is the paralysis of some other branches of the absorbent system, which are called lymphatics, and which open into the larger cavities of the body, or into the cells of the cellular membrane; whence those cavities or cells become distended with the fluid, which is hourly secreted into them for the purpose of lubricating their surfaces. As is more fully explained in No. 5. of the next Section.

As those lymphatic vessels consist generally of a long neck or mouth, which drinks up its appropriated fluid, and of a conglobate gland, in which this fluid undergoes some change, it happens, that sometimes the mouth of the lymphatic, and sometimes the belly or glandular part of it, becomes totally or partially paralytic. In the former case, where the mouths of the cutaneous lymphatics become torpid or quiescent, the fluid secreted on the skin ceases to be absorbed, and erodes the skin by its saline acrimony, and produces eruptions termed herpes, the discharge from which is as salt, as the tears, which are secreted too fast to be reabsorbed, as in grief, or when the puncta lacrymalia are obstructed, and which running down the cheek redden and inflame the skin.

As lymphatic vessels generally have a long neck or opening that absorbs specific fluid, along with a round gland where this fluid undergoes some change, it can happen that either the opening of the lymphatic vessel or the central gland part may become completely or partially paralyzed. In the first case, when the openings of the skin's lymphatics become inactive or dormant, the fluid secreted on the skin stops being absorbed, causing irritation from its salty nature, leading to eruptions known as herpes. The discharge from these eruptions is as salty as tears, which can be produced too quickly to be reabsorbed, such as in times of grief, or when the tear ducts are blocked, causing it to run down the cheek and redden and inflame the skin.

When the mouths of the lymphatics, which open on the mucous membrane of the nostrils, become torpid, as on walking into the air in a frosty morning; the mucus, which continues to be secreted, has not its aqueous and saline part reabsorbed, which running over the upper lip inflames it, and has a salt taste, if it falls on the tongue.

When the openings of the lymphatic vessels on the mucous membrane of the nostrils become sluggish, like when walking into the cold air on a frosty morning, the mucus that keeps being produced doesn't have its watery and salty components reabsorbed. This drips over the upper lip, causing inflammation, and if it touches the tongue, it has a salty taste.

When the belly, or glandular part of these lymphatics, becomes torpid, the fluid absorbed by its mouth stagnates, and forms a tumour in the gland. This disease is called the scrophula. If these glands suppurate externally, they gradually heal, as those of the neck; if they suppurate without an opening on the external habit, as the mesenteric glands, a hectic fever ensues, which destroys the patient; if they suppurate in the lungs, a pulmonary consumption ensues, which is believed thus to differ from that described in the preceding Section, in respect to its seat or proximate cause.

When the belly, or the glandular part of these lymphatics, becomes sluggish, the fluid it absorbs gets stuck and forms a lump in the gland. This condition is called scrofula. If these glands burst open externally, they gradually heal, like those in the neck; if they burst without an opening on the outside, like the mesenteric glands, a wasting fever follows that can be fatal; if they burst in the lungs, it leads to a lung disease, which is believed to differ from what was described in the previous section in terms of its location or underlying cause.

It is remarkable, that matter produced by suppuration will lie concealed in the body many weeks, or even months, without producing hectic fever; but as soon as the wound is opened, so as to admit air to the surface of the ulcer, a hectic fever supervenes, even in very few hours, which is probably owing to the azotic part of the atmosphere rather than to the oxygene; because those medicines, which contain much oxygene, as the calces or oxydes of metals, externally applied, greatly contribute to heal ulcers, of these are the solutions of lead and mercury, and copper in acids, or their precipitates.

It's interesting that pus can stay hidden in the body for many weeks or even months without causing a fever. However, once the wound is opened and air reaches the ulcer, a fever appears within just a few hours. This is likely due to the nitrogen in the air rather than the oxygen; because medications with a high oxygen content, like metal oxides, used externally, significantly help heal ulcers. Examples include solutions of lead, mercury, and copper in acids, or their precipitates.

Hence when wounds are to be healed by the first intention, as it is called, it is necessary carefully to exclude the air from them. Hence we have one cause, which prevents pulmonary ulcers from healing, which is their being perpetually exposed to the air.

Hence, when wounds need to heal by first intention, as it’s called, it’s essential to keep the air away from them. Therefore, we have one reason that prevents lung ulcers from healing, which is that they are constantly exposed to the air.

Both the dark-eyed patients, which are affected with pulmonary ulcers from deficient venous absorption, as described in Section. XXVII. 2. and the light-eyed patients from deficient lymphatic absorption, which we are now treating of, have generally large apertures of the iris; these large pupils of the eyes are a common mark of want of irritability; and it generally happens, that an increase of sensibility, that is, of motions in consequence of sensation, attends these constitutions. See Sect. XXXI. 2. Whence inflammations may occur in these from stagnated fluids more frequently than in those constitutions, which possess more irritability and less sensibility.

Both the dark-eyed patients, which are affected with pulmonary ulcers from deficient venous absorption, as described in Section. XXVII. 2. and the light-eyed patients from deficient lymphatic absorption, which we are now treating of, have generally large apertures of the iris; these large pupils of the eyes are a common mark of want of irritability; and it generally happens, that an increase of sensibility, that is, of motions in consequence of sensation, attends these constitutions. See Sect. XXXI. 2. Whence inflammations may occur in these from stagnated fluids more frequently than in those constitutions, which possess more irritability and less sensibility.

Great expectations in respect to the cure of consumptions, as well as of many other diseases, are produced by the very ingenious exertions of DR. BEDDOES; who has established an apparatus for breathing various mixtures of airs or gasses, at the hot-wells near Bristol, which well deserves the attention of the public.

High hopes for curing tuberculosis, along with many other illnesses, have been sparked by the innovative efforts of Dr. Beddoes. He has set up a system for breathing different mixes of air and gases at the hot wells near Bristol, which truly deserves the public's attention.

DR. BEDDOES very ingeniously concludes, from the florid colour of the blood of consumptive patients, that it abounds in oxygene; and that the redness of their tongues, and lips, and the fine blush of their cheeks shew the presence of the same principle, like flesh reddened by nitre. And adds, that the circumstance of the consumptions of pregnant women being stopped in their progress during pregnancy, at which time their blood may be supposed to be in part deprived of its oxygene, by oxygenating the blood of the fœtus, is a forceable argument in favour of this theory; which must soon be confirmed or confuted by his experiments. See Essay on Scurvy, Consumption, &c. by Dr. Beddoes. Murray. London. Also Letter to Dr. Darwin, by the same. Murray. London.

DR. BEDDOES cleverly concludes that the bright color of the blood in tuberculosis patients indicates a high oxygen content. He points out that the redness of their tongues and lips, along with the rosy hue of their cheeks, suggests the same thing, similar to flesh tinged by saltpeter. He also mentions that the fact that tuberculosis in pregnant women slows down during pregnancy—when their blood is likely partly depleted of oxygen due to oxygenating the fetus—is a strong argument for this theory, which will soon be validated or disproven by his experiments. See Essay on Scurvy, Consumption, &c. by Dr. Beddoes. Murray. London. Also Letter to Dr. Darwin, by the same. Murray. London.



SECT. XXIX.

ON THE RETROGRADE MOTIONS OF THE ABSORBENT SYSTEM.

ON THE BACKWARD MOVEMENTS OF THE ABSORBENT SYSTEM.

I. Account of the absorbent system. II. The valves of the absorbent vessels may suffer their fluids to regurgitate in some diseases. III. Communication from the alimentary canal to the bladder by means of the absorbent vessels. IV. The phenomena of diabetes explained. V. 1. The phenomena of dropsies explained. 2. Cases of the use of foxglove. VI. Of cold sweats. VII. Translations of matter, of chyle, of milk, of urine, operation of purging drugs applied externally. VIII. Circumstances by which the fluids, that are effused by the retrograde motions of the absorbent vessels, are distinguished. IX. Retrograde motions of vegetable juices. X. Objections answered. XI. The causes, which induce the retrograde motions of animal vessels, and the medicines by which the natural motions are restored.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Overview of the absorbent system. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. The valves of the absorbent vessels can allow their fluids to flow backward in certain diseases. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Connection between the digestive tract and the bladder through the absorbent vessels. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. The phenomena of diabetes explained. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. The phenomena of dropsy explained. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Cases of using foxglove. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. About cold sweats. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Translations of matter, chyle, milk, urine, and the effects of purging drugs applied externally. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Factors that differentiate the fluids that are released by the backward motions of the absorbent vessels. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Backward movements of plant juices. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. Responses to objections. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. The causes that lead to the backward motions of animal vessels and the medicines that restore their natural movements.

N.B. The following Section is a translation of a part of a Latin thesis written by the late Mr. Charles Darwin, which was printed with his prize-dissertation on a criterion between matter and mucus in 1780. Sold by Cadell, London.

Note: The following section is a translation of part of a Latin thesis by the late Mr. Charles Darwin, printed with his award-winning dissertation on the distinction between matter and mucus in 1780. Sold by Cadell, London.

I. Account of the Absorbent System.

I. Overview of the Absorbent System.

1. The absorbent system of vessels in animal bodies consists of several branches, differing in respect to their situations, and to the fluids, which they absorb.

1. The system of vessels that absorbs fluids in animal bodies has several branches that vary depending on their locations and the types of fluids they absorb.

The intestinal absorbents open their mouths on the internal surfaces of the intestines; their office is to drink up the chyle and the other fluids from the alimentary canal; and they are termed lacteals, to distinguish them from the other absorbent vessels, which have been termed lymphatics.

The intestinal absorbents open their mouths on the inner surfaces of the intestines; their job is to sip up the chyle and other fluids from the digestive tract; and they are called lacteals to set them apart from the other absorbent vessels, which are called lymphatics.

Those, whose mouths are dispersed on the external skin, imbibe a great quantity of water from the atmosphere, and a part of the perspirable matter, which does not evaporate, and are termed cutaneous absorbents.

Those with mouths spread across their outer skin take in a significant amount of water from the air, as well as some of the moisture that doesn’t evaporate, and are called skin absorbents.

Those, which arise from the internal surface of the bronchia, and which imbibe moisture from the atmosphere, and a part of the bronchial mucus, are called pulmonary absorbents.

Those that come from the inner surface of the bronchi, absorb moisture from the air, and some of the bronchial mucus, are called pulmonary absorbents.

Those, which open their innumerable mouths into the cells of the whole cellular membrane; and whose use is to take up the fluid, which is poured into those cells, after it has done its office there; may be called cellular absorbents.

Those that open their countless mouths into the cells of the entire cellular membrane, and whose function is to absorb the fluid that is poured into those cells after it has served its purpose there, can be called cellular absorbents.

Those, which arise from the internal surfaces of the membranes, which line the larger cavities of the body, as the thorax, abdomen, scrotum, pericardium, take up the mucus poured into those cavities; and are distinguished by the names of their respective cavities.

Those that come from the inner surfaces of the membranes lining the larger body cavities, like the chest, abdomen, scrotum, and pericardium, absorb the mucus that gathers in those spaces; they are named after their respective cavities.

Whilst those, which arise from the internal surfaces of the urinary bladder, gall-bladder, salivary ducts, or other receptacles of secreted fluids, may take their names from those fluids; the thinner parts of which it is their office to absorb: as urinary, bilious, or salivary absorbents.

While those that come from the inner surfaces of the urinary bladder, gall-bladder, salivary ducts, or other containers of secreted fluids may be named after those fluids, the thinner parts whose job it is to absorb them are referred to as urinary, bilious, or salivary absorbents.

2. Many of these absorbent vessels, both lacteals and lymphatics, like some of the veins, are replete with valves: which seem designed to assist the progress of their fluids, or at least to prevent their regurgitation; where they are subjected to the intermitted pressure of the muscular, or arterial actions in their neighbourhood.

2. Many of these absorbent vessels, such as lacteals and lymphatics, similar to some veins, are filled with valves: which appear to help move their fluids forward, or at least prevent backflow; especially when they experience the intermittent pressure from nearby muscular or arterial movements.

These valves do not however appear to be necessary to all the absorbents, any more than to all the veins; since they are not found to exist in the absorbent system of fish; according to the discoveries of the ingenious, and much lamented Mr. Hewson. Philos. Trans. v. 59, Enquiries into the Lymph. Syst. p. 94.

These valves don’t seem to be necessary for all the absorbents, just like they aren’t for all the veins; they aren’t found in the absorbent system of fish, according to the findings of the brilliant and greatly missed Mr. Hewson. Philos. Trans. v. 59, Enquiries into the Lymph. Syst. p. 94.

3. These absorbent vessels are also furnished with glands, which are called conglobate glands; whose use is not at present sufficiently investigated; but it is probable that they resemble the conglomerate glands both in structure and in use, except that their absorbent mouths are for the conveniency of situation placed at a greater distance from the body of the gland. The conglomerate glands open their mouths immediately into the sanguiferous vessels, which bring the blood, from whence they absorb their respective fluids, quite up to the gland: but these conglobate glands collect their adapted fluids from very distant membranes, or cysts, by means of mouths furnished with long necks for this purpose; and which are called lacteals, or lymphatics.

3. These absorbent vessels also have glands known as conglobate glands; their exact function isn’t fully understood yet, but it’s likely they are similar in structure and function to conglomerate glands. The main difference is that their absorbent openings are positioned further away from the body of the gland for convenience. The conglomerate glands have their openings directly into the blood vessels that carry blood, from which they absorb fluids right up to the gland. In contrast, the conglobate glands gather their specific fluids from far-off membranes or sacs through openings equipped with long necks for this purpose, which are called lacteals or lymphatics.

4. The fluids, thus collected from various parts of the body, pass by means of the thoracic duct into the left subclavian near the jugular vein; except indeed that those collected from the right side of the head and neck, and from the right arm, are carried into the right subclavian vein: and sometimes even the lymphatics from the right side of the lungs are inserted into the right subclavian vein; whilst those of the left side of the head open but just into the summit of the thoracic duct.

4. The fluids collected from different parts of the body flow through the thoracic duct into the left subclavian vein near the jugular vein; however, those from the right side of the head, neck, and right arm go to the right subclavian vein. Sometimes, even the lymphatics from the right side of the lungs connect to the right subclavian vein, while the lymphatics from the left side of the head drain mainly into the top of the thoracic duct.

5. In the absorbent system there are many anastomoses of the vessels, which seem of great consequence to the preservation of health. These anastomoses are discovered by dissection to be very frequent between the intestinal and urinary lymphatics, as mentioned by Mr. Hewson, (Phil. Trans. v. 58.)

5. In the absorbent system, there are many connections between the vessels, which appear to be very important for maintaining health. Dissection reveals that these connections are quite common between the intestinal and urinary lymphatics, as noted by Mr. Hewson, (Phil. Trans. v. 58.)

6. Nor do all the intestinal absorbents seem to terminate in the thoracic duct, as appears from some curious experiments of D. Munro, who gave madder to some animals, having previously put a ligature on the thoracic duct, and found their bones, and the serum of their blood, coloured red.

6. Not all the intestinal absorbents seem to end in the thoracic duct, as shown by some interesting experiments by D. Munro. He gave madder to some animals after tying off the thoracic duct and discovered that their bones and blood serum were colored red.

II. The Valves of the Absorbent System may suffer their Fluids to regurgitate in some Diseases.

II. The valves in the absorbent system might allow their fluids to flow back in certain diseases.

1. The many valves, which occur in the progress of the lymphatic and lacteal vessels, would seem insuperable obstacles to the regurgitation of their contents. But as these valves are placed in vessels, which are indued with life, and are themselves indued with life also; and are very irritable into those natural motions, which absorb, or propel the fluids they contain; it is possible, in some diseases, where these valves or vessels are stimulated into unnatural exertions, or are become paralytic, that during the diastole of the part of the vessel to which the valve is attached, the valve may not so completely close, as to prevent the relapse of the lymph or chyle. This is rendered more probable, by the experiments of injecting mercury, or water, or suet, or by blowing air down these vessels: all which pass the valves very easily, contrary to the natural course of their fluids, when the vessels are thus a little forcibly dilated, as mentioned by Dr. Haller, Elem. Physiol. t. iii. s. 4.

1. The numerous valves found in the lymphatic and lacteal vessels might seem like major barriers to the backflow of their contents. However, since these valves are located in living vessels that are themselves alive and responsive, they can react naturally to absorb or push the fluids they contain. In certain diseases, when these valves or vessels are overly stimulated or become paralyzed, it's possible that during the relaxation phase of the section of the vessel connected to the valve, the valve might not close completely, allowing the lymph or chyle to flow back. This is further supported by experiments involving the injection of mercury, water, or fat, or by blowing air into these vessels, all of which can easily pass through the valves, going against the normal flow of their fluids when the vessels are slightly forced open, as noted by Dr. Haller, Elem. Physiol. t. iii. s. 4.

"The valves of the thoracic duct are few, some assert they are not more than twelve, and that they do not very accurately perform their office, as they do not close the whole area of the duct, and thence may permit chyle to repass them downwards. In living animals, however, though not always, yet more frequently than in the dead, they prevent the chyle from returning. The principal of these valves is that, which presides over the insertion of the thoracic duct, into the subclavian vein; many have believed this also to perform the office of a valve, both to admit the chyle into the vein, and to preclude the blood from entering the duct; but in my opinion it is scarcely sufficient for this purpose." Haller, Elem. Phys. t. vii. p. 226.

"The valves of the thoracic duct are few; some say there are no more than twelve, and they don't function very well since they don't close off the entire area of the duct, which may allow chyle to flow back down. However, in living animals, although not always, they more often prevent chyle from returning. The main valve is located where the thoracic duct connects to the subclavian vein; many have believed this also serves as a valve that allows chyle into the vein and stops blood from entering the duct, but in my view, it’s not really adequate for that purpose." Haller, Elem. Phys. t. vii. p. 226.

2. The mouths of the lymphatics seem to admit water to pass through them after death, the inverted way, easier than the natural one; since an inverted bladder readily lets out the water with which it is filled; whence it may be inferred, that there is no obstacle at the mouths of these vessels to prevent the regurgitation of their contained fluids.

2. The openings of the lymphatic vessels seem to allow water to flow through them after death more easily in the reverse direction than in the natural one; since a turned-over bladder can easily release the water it contains. This suggests that there is no barrier at the openings of these vessels that would stop the backflow of their fluids.

I was induced to repeat this experiment, and having accurately tied the ureters and neck of a fresh ox's bladder, I made an opening at the fundus of it; and then, having turned it inside outwards, filled it half full with water, and was surprised to see it empty itself so hastily. I thought the experiment more apposite to my purpose by suspending the bladder with its neck downwards, as the lymphatics are chiefly spread upon this part of it, as shewn by Dr. Watson, Philos. Trans. v. 59. p. 392.

I was encouraged to repeat this experiment, and after carefully tying the ureters and the neck of a fresh ox's bladder, I made a small opening at the base of it. Then, after turning it inside out, I filled it halfway with water and was surprised to see it empty so quickly. I thought the experiment would be more relevant to my goal by suspending the bladder neck down, since the lymphatic vessels are mainly located in that area, as shown by Dr. Watson, Philos. Trans. v. 59. p. 392.

3. In some diseases, as in the diabetes and scrophula, it is probable the valves themselves are diseased, and are thence incapable of preventing the return of the fluids they should support. Thus the valves of the aorta itself have frequently been found schirrous, according to the dissections of Mons. Lieutaud, and have given rise to an interrupted pulse, and laborious palpitations, by suffering a return of part of the blood into the heart. Nor are any parts of the body so liable to schirrosity as the lymphatic glands and vessels, insomuch that their schirrosities have acquired a distinct name, and been termed scrophula.

3. In some diseases, like diabetes and scrofula, the valves themselves may be affected, making them unable to stop the fluids they’re supposed to support from flowing back. For example, the valves of the aorta have often been found to be hardened, according to the dissections by Mons. Lieutaud, leading to an irregular pulse and difficult heartbeats because they allow some blood to flow back into the heart. No part of the body is more prone to hardening than the lymphatic glands and vessels, so much so that these hardenings have been given a specific name and are referred to as scrofula.

4. There are valves in other parts of the body, analogous to those of the absorbent system, and which are liable, when diseased, to regurgitate their contents: thus the upper and lower orifices of the stomach are closed by valves, which, when too great quantities of warm water have been drank with a design to promote vomiting, have sometimes resisted the utmost efforts of the abdominal muscles, and diaphragm: yet, at other times, the upper valve, or cardia, easily permits the evacuation of the contents of the stomach; whilst the inferior valve, or pylorus, permits the bile, and other contents of the duodenum, to regurgitate into the stomach.

4. There are valves in other areas of the body that are similar to those in the absorption system and can, when they are diseased, release their contents backward. For example, the upper and lower openings of the stomach are secured by valves that, when too much warm water is consumed with the intention of inducing vomiting, may sometimes resist the strongest efforts of the abdominal muscles and diaphragm. However, at other times, the upper valve, or cardia, easily allows the contents of the stomach to be expelled, while the lower valve, or pylorus, allows bile and other materials from the duodenum to flow back into the stomach.

5. The valve of the colon is well adapted to prevent the retrograde motion of the excrements; yet, as this valve is possessed of a living power, in the iliac passion, either from spasm, or other unnatural exertions, it keeps itself open, and either suffers or promotes the retrograde movements of the contents of the intestines below; as in ruminating animals the mouth of the first stomach seems to be so constructed, as to facilitate or assist the regurgitation of the food; the rings of the œsophagus afterwards contracting themselves in inverted order. De Haeu, by means of a syringe, forced so much water into the rectum intestinum of a dog, that he vomited it in a full stream from his mouth; and in the iliac passion above mentioned, excrements and clyster are often evacuated by the mouth. See Section XXV. 15.

5. The valve of the colon is well adapted to prevent the retrograde motion of the excrements; yet, as this valve is possessed of a living power, in the iliac passion, either from spasm, or other unnatural exertions, it keeps itself open, and either suffers or promotes the retrograde movements of the contents of the intestines below; as in ruminating animals the mouth of the first stomach seems to be so constructed, as to facilitate or assist the regurgitation of the food; the rings of the œsophagus afterwards contracting themselves in inverted order. De Haeu, by means of a syringe, forced so much water into the rectum intestinum of a dog, that he vomited it in a full stream from his mouth; and in the iliac passion above mentioned, excrements and clyster are often evacuated by the mouth. See Section XXV. 15.

6. The puncta lacrymalia, with the lacrymal sack and nasal duct, compose a complete gland, and much resemble the intestinal canal: the puncta lacrymalia are absorbent mouths, that take up the tears from the eye, when they have done their office there, and convey them into the nostrils; but when the nasal duct is obstructed, and the lacrymal sack distended with its fluid, on pressure with the finger the mouths of this gland (puncta lacrymalia) will readily disgorge the fluid, they had previously absorbed, back into the eye.

6. The lacrimal puncta, along with the lacrimal sac and nasal duct, form a complete gland that closely resembles the intestinal canal: the lacrimal puncta are absorbent openings that collect tears from the eye once they’ve served their purpose and transport them into the nostrils. However, when the nasal duct is blocked and the lacrimal sac becomes swollen with fluid, applying pressure with a finger can easily push the fluid back into the eye through the openings of this gland (lacrimal puncta).

7. As the capillary vessels receive blood from the arteries, and separating the mucus, or perspirable matter from it, convey the remainder back by the veins; these capillary vessels are a set of glands, in every respect similar to the secretory vessels of the liver, or other large congeries of glands. The beginnings of these capillary vessels have frequent anastomoses into each other, in which circumstance they are resembled by the lacteals; and like the mouths or beginnings of other glands, they are a set of absorbent vessels, which drink up the blood which is brought to them by the arteries, as the chyle is drank up by the lacteals: for the circulation of the blood through the capillaries is proved to be independent of arterial impulse; since in the blush of shame, and in partial inflammations, their action is increased, without any increase of the motion of the heart.

7. As the capillary vessels take in blood from the arteries, separating the mucus or sweat from it, they send the rest back through the veins; these capillary vessels function like a network of glands, much like the secretory vessels of the liver or other large glandular groups. The beginnings of these capillary vessels frequently connect with each other, similar to the way lacteals do; and like the openings of other glands, they act as absorbent vessels, soaking up the blood that the arteries supply, just as the lacteals absorb chyle. The blood circulation through the capillaries operates independently of arterial pressure, as evidenced by the increase in their activity during blushing or localized inflammation, without a corresponding increase in heart rate.

8. Yet not only the mouths, or beginnings of these anastomosing capillaries are frequently seen by microscopes, to regurgitate some particles of blood, during the struggles of the animal; but retrograde motion of the blood, in the veins of those animals, from the very heart of the extremity of the limbs, is observable, by intervals, during the distresses of the dying creature. Haller, Elem. Physiol. t. i. p. 216. Now, as the veins have perhaps all of them a valve somewhere between their extremities and the heart, here is ocular demonstration of the fluids in this diseased condition of the animal, repassing through venous valves: and it is hence highly probable, from the strictest analogy, that if the course of the fluids, in the lymphatic vessels, could be subjected to microscopic observation, they would also, in the diseased state of the animal, be seen to repass the valves, and the mouths of those vessels, which had previously absorbed them, or promoted their progression.

8. However, not only are the mouths or beginnings of these connecting capillaries often seen by microscopes to push some blood particles back during the animal's struggles, but it's also noticeable that blood flows backward in the veins of these animals—from the very heart to the tips of the limbs—at times during the creature's distress. Haller, Elem. Physiol. t. i. p. 216. Since the veins likely have a valve somewhere between their ends and the heart, this provides clear evidence of fluids in this diseased state of the animal going back through venous valves. Therefore, it's highly likely, based on strong comparisons, that if we could observe the flow of fluids in the lymphatic vessels under a microscope, they would similarly be seen reversing through the valves and openings of those vessels that previously absorbed them or helped them move along.

III. Communication from the Alimentary Canal to the Bladder, by means of the Absorbent Vessels.

III. Communication from the Digestive System to the Bladder, through the Absorbent Vessels.

Many medical philosophers, both ancient and modern, have suspected that there was a nearer communication between the stomach and the urinary bladder, than that of the circulation: they were led into this opinion from the great expedition with which cold water, when drank to excess, passes off by the bladder; and from the similarity of the urine, when produced in this hasty manner, with the material that was drank.

Many medical thinkers, both in the past and today, have believed that there is a closer connection between the stomach and the urinary bladder than just through the bloodstream. They reached this conclusion based on how quickly cold water, when consumed in large amounts, is expelled through the bladder, and because the urine produced in this quick process resembles the liquid that was ingested.

The former of these circumstances happens perpetually to those who drink abundance of cold water, when they are much heated by exercise, and to many at the beginning of intoxication.

The first of these situations happens all the time to those who drink a lot of cold water when they're really hot from exercising, and to many people at the start of getting drunk.

Of the latter, many instances are recorded by Etmuller, t. xi. p. 716. where simple water, wine, and wine with sugar, and emulsions, were returned by urine unchanged.

Of the latter, many instances are recorded by Etmuller, t. xi. p. 716. where plain water, wine, and wine with sugar, as well as emulsions, were passed in urine unchanged.

There are other experiments, that seem to demonstrate the existence of another passage to the bladder, besides that through the kidneys. Thus Dr. Kratzenstein put ligatures on the ureters of a dog, and then emptied the bladder by a catheter; yet in a little time the dog drank greedily, and made a quantity of water, (Disputat. Morbor. Halleri. t. iv. p. 63.) A similar experiment is related in the Philosophical Transactions, with the same event, (No. 65, 67, for the year 1670.)

There are other experiments that seem to show there’s another way for urine to reach the bladder, aside from the kidneys. For example, Dr. Kratzenstein tied off the ureters of a dog and then drained the bladder using a catheter; however, soon after, the dog drank eagerly and produced a significant amount of urine (Disputat. Morbor. Halleri. t. iv. p. 63). A similar experiment is mentioned in the Philosophical Transactions, with the same outcome (No. 65, 67, for the year 1670).

Add to this, that in some morbid cases the urine has continued to pass, after the suppuration or total destruction of the kidneys; of which many instances are referred to in the Elem. Physiol. t. vii. p. 379. of Dr. Haller.

Add to this that in some severe cases, urine has continued to flow even after the pus formation or complete destruction of the kidneys; many examples of this are mentioned in Elem. Physiol. t. vii. p. 379 of Dr. Haller.

From all which it must be concluded, that some fluids have passed from the stomach or abdomen, without having gone through the sanguiferous circulation: and as the bladder is supplied with many lymphatics, as described by Dr. Watson, in the Philos. Trans. v. 59. p. 392. and as no other vessels open into it besides these and the ureters, it seems evident, that the unnatural urine, produced as above described, when the ureters were tied, or the kidneys obliterated, was carried into the bladder by the retrograde motions of the urinary branch of the lymphatic system.

From all this, we can conclude that some fluids have moved from the stomach or abdomen without going through the blood circulation. Since the bladder has many lymphatic vessels, as noted by Dr. Watson in the Philos. Trans. v. 59. p. 392, and since no other vessels connect to it aside from these and the ureters, it seems clear that the unusual urine, generated as described earlier, when the ureters were tied or the kidneys were removed, was transported to the bladder through the backward flow of the urinary branch of the lymphatic system.

The more certainly to ascertain the existence of another communication between the stomach and bladder, besides that of the circulation, the following experiment was made, to which I must beg your patient attention:—A friend of mine (June 14, 1772) on drinking repeatedly of cold small punch, till he began to be intoxicated, made a quantity of colourless urine. He then drank about two drams of nitre dissolved in some of the punch, and eat about twenty stalks of boiled asparagus: on continuing to drink more of the punch, the next urine that he made was quite clear, and without smell; but in a little time another quantity was made, which was not quite so colourless, and had a strong smell of the asparagus: he then lost about four ounces of blood from the arm.

To determine if there’s another connection between the stomach and bladder beyond just circulation, I conducted the following experiment, and I appreciate your patience: A friend of mine (June 14, 1772) drank a lot of cold small punch until he started feeling tipsy, resulting in a large amount of clear urine. He then consumed about two drams of nitre dissolved in some of the punch, along with around twenty stalks of boiled asparagus. After drinking more of the punch, his next urine production was completely clear and odorless; however, shortly after, he produced another batch that was not quite as clear and had a strong smell of asparagus. He then lost about four ounces of blood from his arm.

The smell of asparagus was not at all perceptible in the blood, neither when fresh taken, nor the next morning, as myself and two others accurately attended to; yet this smell was strongly perceived in the urine, which was made just before the blood was taken from his arm.

The smell of asparagus was not detectable in the blood, neither when freshly taken nor the next morning, as I and two others carefully noted; however, this smell was strongly noticeable in the urine, which was produced just before the blood was drawn from his arm.

Some bibulous paper, moistened in the serum of this blood, and suffered to dry, shewed no signs of nitre by its manner of burning. But some of the same paper, moistened in the urine, and dried, on being ignited, evidently shewed the presence of nitre. This blood and the urine stood some days exposed to the sun in the open air, till they were evaporated to about a fourth of their original quantity, and began to stink: the paper, which was then moistened with the concentrated urine, shewed the presence of much nitre by its manner of burning; whilst that moistened with the blood shewed no such appearance at all.

Some absorbent paper, dampened with this blood serum and allowed to dry, showed no signs of nitrate when burned. However, some of the same paper, dampened with urine and dried, clearly indicated the presence of nitrate when ignited. This blood and urine were left exposed to the sun in the open air for several days until they had evaporated to about a quarter of their original volume and began to smell bad: the paper, which was then dampened with the concentrated urine, demonstrated a significant presence of nitrate when burned; whereas the paper moistened with the blood showed no such signs at all.

Hence it appears, that certain fluids at the beginning of intoxication, find another passage to the bladder besides the long course of the arterial circulation; and as the intestinal absorbents are joined with the urinary lymphatics by frequent anastomoses, as Hewson has demonstrated; and as there is no other road, we may justly conclude, that these fluids pass into the bladder by the urinary branch of the lymphatics, which has its motions inverted during the diseased state of the animal.

Hence, it seems that certain fluids at the start of intoxication find a different route to the bladder besides the lengthy path of the arterial circulation. As Hewson has shown, the intestinal absorbents connect with the urinary lymphatics through frequent anastomoses; and since there is no other route, we can reasonably conclude that these fluids enter the bladder through the urinary branch of the lymphatics, which has its movements reversed during the diseased state of the animal.

A gentleman, who had been some weeks affected with jaundice, and whose urine was in consequence of a very deep yellow, took some cold small punch, in which was dissolved about a dram of nitre; he then took repeated draughts of the punch, and kept himself in a cool room, till on the approach of slight intoxication he made a large quantity of water; this water had a slight yellow tinge, as might be expected from a small admixture of bile secreted from the kidneys; but if the whole of it had passed through the sanguiferous vessels, which were now replete with bile (his whole skin being as yellow as gold) would not this urine also, as well as that he had made for weeks before, have been of a deep yellow? Paper dipped in this water, and dryed, and ignited, shewed evident marks of the presence of nitre, when the flame was blown out.

A gentleman, who had been dealing with jaundice for several weeks, and whose urine was consequently a very deep yellow, drank some cold small punch that had about a dram of nitre dissolved in it. He then took several large sips of the punch and kept himself in a cool room until he started to feel slightly intoxicated, at which point he urinated a large amount. This urine had a slight yellow tint, as might be expected from a small amount of bile secreted by the kidneys. However, if all of it had passed through the blood vessels, which were now full of bile (his entire skin was as yellow as gold), wouldn’t this urine, just like the urine he had produced for weeks prior, also have been a deep yellow? Paper that was dipped in this urine, dried, and then ignited showed clear signs of the presence of nitre when the flame was blown out.

IV. The Phænomena of the Diabetes explained, and of some Diarrhœas.

IV. The Phenomena of Diabetes Explained, and Some Diarrheas.

The phenomena of many diseases are only explicable from the retrograde motions of some of the branches of the lymphatic system; as the great and immediate flow of pale urine in the beginning of drunkenness; in hysteric paroxysms; from being exposed to cold air; or to the influence of fear or anxiety.

The occurrence of many diseases can only be explained by the reverse movements of certain branches of the lymphatic system, such as the sudden and excessive flow of pale urine at the onset of drunkenness; during hysterical episodes; from being exposed to cold air; or due to the effects of fear or anxiety.

Before we endeavour to illustrate this doctrine, by describing the phænomena of these diseases, we must premise one circumstance; that all the branches of the lymphatic system have a certain sympathy with each other, insomuch that when one branch is stimulated into unusual kinds or quantities of motion, some other branch has its motions either increased, or decreased, or inverted at the same time. This kind of sympathy can only be proved by the concurrent testimony of numerous facts, which will be related in the course of the work. I shall only add here, that it is probable, that this sympathy does not depend on any communication of nervous filaments, but on habit; owing to the various branches of this system having frequently been stimulated into action at the same time.

Before we attempt to explain this theory by describing the phenomena of these diseases, we need to mention one important point: all parts of the lymphatic system have a certain connection with each other. When one part is triggered into unusual types or amounts of movement, another part either increases, decreases, or changes its movement at the same time. This connection can only be demonstrated by the supporting evidence of many facts, which will be discussed throughout this work. I will just add that it’s likely this connection doesn’t rely on any communication through nerve fibers, but rather on habit; because the various parts of this system have often been activated simultaneously.

There are a thousand instances of involuntary motions associated in this manner; as in the act of vomiting, while the motions of the stomach and œsophagus are inverted, the pulsations of the arterial system by a certain sympathy become weaker; and when the bowels or kidneys are stimulated by poison, a stone, or inflammation, into more violent action; the stomach and œsophagus by sympathy invert their motions.

There are countless examples of involuntary movements linked in this way; for instance, during vomiting, the movements of the stomach and esophagus are reversed, and the pulsations of the arterial system, due to a certain sympathy, weaken. Similarly, when the bowels or kidneys are triggered into greater activity by poison, a stone, or inflammation, the stomach and esophagus also reverse their movements in response.

1. When any one drinks a moderate quantity of vinous spirit, the whole system acts with more energy by consent with the stomach and intestines, as is seen from the glow on the skin, and the increase of strength and activity; but when a greater quantity of this inebriating material is drank, at the same time that the lacteals are excited into greater action to absorb it; it frequently happens, that the urinary branch of absorbents, which is connected with the lacteals by many anastomoses, inverts its motions, and a great quantity of pale unanimalized urine is discharged. By this wise contrivance too much of an unnecessary fluid is prevented from entering the circulation—This may be called the drunken diabetes, to distinguish it from the other temporary diabetes, which occur in hysteric diseases, and from continued fear or anxiety.

1. When someone drinks a moderate amount of alcoholic beverage, the entire body functions more energetically, working in sync with the stomach and intestines, which is evident from the flush on the skin and the boost in strength and activity. However, when larger amounts of this intoxicating substance are consumed, while the lymphatic system is stimulated to absorb it more actively, it often happens that the urinary system, which connects with the lymphatics through various pathways, reverses its processes and a significant amount of pale, unrefined urine is expelled. This clever mechanism prevents excessive fluid from entering the bloodstream—this can be referred to as drunken diabetes, to differentiate it from other temporary diabetes episodes, which occur in hysterical conditions or due to prolonged fear or anxiety.

2. If this idle ingurgitation of too much vinous spirit be daily practised, the urinary branch of absorbents at length gains an habit of inverting its motions, whenever the lacteals are much stimulated; and the whole or a great part of the chyle is thus daily carried to the bladder without entering the circulation, and the body becomes emaciated. This is one kind of chronic diabetes, and may be distinguished from the others by the taste and appearance of the urine; which is sweet, and the colour of whey, and may be termed the chyliferous diabetes.

2. If this lazy habit of consuming too much alcohol continues every day, the urinary system eventually gets used to reversing its normal functions whenever the lymphatic system is heavily stimulated; as a result, a large portion of the digestive fluid is sent to the bladder without entering the bloodstream, leading to weight loss. This is a type of chronic diabetes, and it can be identified by the taste and appearance of the urine, which is sweet and resembles the color of whey; this condition can be called chyliferous diabetes.

3. Many children have a similar deposition of chyle in their urine, from the irritation of worms in their intestines, which stimulating the mouths of the lacteals into unnatural action, the urinary branch of the absorbents becomes inverted, and carries part of the chyle to the bladder: part of the chyle also has been carried to the iliac and lumbar glands, of which instances are recorded by Haller, t. vii. 225. and which can be explained on no other theory: but the dissections of the lymphatic system of the human body, which have yet been published, are not sufficiently extensive for our purpose; yet if we may reason from comparative anatomy, this translation of chyle to the bladder is much illustrated by the account given of this system of vessels in a turtle, by Mr. Hewson, who observed, "That the lacteals near the root of the mesentery anastomose, so as to form a net-work, from which several large branches go into some considerable lymphatics lying near the spine; and which can be traced almost to the anus, and particularly to the kidneys." Philos. Trans. v. 59. p. 199—Enquiries, p. 74.

3. Many children show a similar presence of chyle in their urine, caused by irritation from worms in their intestines. This irritation stimulates the openings of the lacteals into abnormal activity, leading the urinary branch of the absorbents to invert and transport some chyle to the bladder. Additionally, some chyle is directed to the iliac and lumbar glands, as noted by Haller, t. vii. 225. This can only be explained by the dissections of the human lymphatic system published so far, which are not extensive enough for our needs. However, if we consider comparative anatomy, the movement of chyle to the bladder is well illustrated by Mr. Hewson's observations on the network of vessels in a turtle. He noted, "That the lacteals near the base of the mesentery connect to form a network, from which several large branches lead to significant lymphatics near the spine; and these can be traced almost to the anus, particularly to the kidneys." Philos. Trans. v. 59. p. 199—Enquiries, p. 74.

4. At the same time that the urinary branch of absorbents, in the beginning of diabetes, is excited into inverted action, the cellular branch is excited by the sympathy above mentioned, into more energetic action; and the fat, that was before deposited, is reabsorbed and thrown into the blood vessels; where it floats, and was mistaken for chyle, till the late experiments of the ingenious Mr. Hewson demonstrated it to be fat.

4. At the same time that the urinary part of the absorbent system is triggered into reversing its action at the onset of diabetes, the cellular part becomes more active due to the aforementioned connection; the fat that was previously stored is reabsorbed and released into the bloodstream, where it circulates and was previously thought to be chyle until the recent experiments by the clever Mr. Hewson proved it to be fat.

This appearance of what was mistaken for chyle in the blood, which was drawn from these patients, and the obstructed liver, which very frequently accompanies this disease, seems to have led Dr. Mead to suspect the diabetes was owing to a defect of sanguification; and that the schirrosity of the liver was the original cause of it: but as the schirrhus of the liver is most frequently owing to the same causes, that produce the diabetes and dropsies; namely, the great use of fermented liquors; there is no wonder they should exist together, without being the consequence of each other.

This appearance of what was mistaken for chyle in the blood taken from these patients, along with the blocked liver that often comes with this disease, seems to have led Dr. Mead to think that diabetes was due to a problem with blood formation and that the liver scarring was the original cause of it. However, since the liver scarring is usually due to the same factors that cause diabetes and fluid retention—specifically, excessive consumption of alcoholic drinks—it’s not surprising that they occur together without one causing the other.

5. If the cutaneous branch of absorbents gains a habit of being excited into stronger action, and imbibes greater quantities of moisture from the atmosphere, at the same time that the urinary branch has its motions inverted, another kind of diabetes is formed, which may be termed the aqueous diabetes. In this diabetes the cutaneous absorbents frequently imbibe an amazing quantity of atmospheric moisture; insomuch that there are authentic histories, where many gallons a day, for many weeks together, above the quantity that has been drank, have been discharged by urine.

5. If the skin's lymphatic system becomes accustomed to being more active and absorbs more moisture from the air, while the urinary system starts functioning in the opposite way, it creates a different type of diabetes that can be called aqueous diabetes. In this condition, the skin's lymphatic vessels often absorb an incredible amount of moisture from the atmosphere, to the extent that there are verified cases where several gallons a day, for many weeks, have been expelled through urine, exceeding the amount consumed.

Dr. Keil, in his Medicina Statica, found that he gained eighteen ounces from the moist air of one night; and Dr. Percival affirms, that one of his hands imbibed, after being well chafed, near an ounce and half of water, in a quarter of an hour. (Transact. of the College, London, vol. ii. p. 102.) Home's Medic. Facts, p. 2. sect. 3.

Dr. Keil, in his Medicina Statica, discovered that he absorbed eighteen ounces from the humid air in one night; and Dr. Percival states that one of his hands absorbed about an ounce and a half of water after being thoroughly rubbed for a quarter of an hour. (Transact. of the College, London, vol. ii. p. 102.) Home's Medic. Facts, p. 2. sect. 3.

The pale urine in hysterical women, or which is produced by fear or anxiety, is a temporary complaint of this kind; and it would in reality be the same disease, if it was confirmed by habit.

The pale urine in anxious women, or that which is caused by fear or worry, is a temporary issue of this nature; and it would essentially be the same condition if it became habitual.

6. The purging stools, and pale urine, occasioned by exposing the naked body to cold air, or sprinkling it with cold water, originate from a similar cause; for the mouths of the cutaneous lymphatics being suddenly exposed to cold become torpid, and cease, or nearly cease, to act; whilst, by the sympathy above described, not only the lymphatics of the bladder and intestines cease also to absorb the more aqueous and saline part of the fluids secreted into them; but it is probable that these lymphatics invert their motions, and return the fluids, which were previously absorbed, into the intestines and bladder. At the very instant that the body is exposed naked to the cold air, an unusual movement is felt in the bowels; as is experienced by boys going into the cold bath: this could not occur from an obstruction of the perspirable matter, since there is not time, for that to be returned to the bowels by the course of the circulation.

6. The purging stools and pale urine, caused by exposing the naked body to cold air or splashing it with cold water, come from a similar reason; when the openings of the skin lymphatics are suddenly exposed to cold, they become sluggish and almost stop functioning. Due to the described connection, not only do the lymphatics of the bladder and intestines stop absorbing the more watery and salty parts of the fluids secreted into them, but it's likely that these lymphatics reverse their actions and send back the fluids that were previously absorbed into the intestines and bladder. At the moment the body is fully exposed to the cold air, an unusual movement is felt in the bowels, just like what boys experience when entering a cold bath; this couldn’t be due to a blockage of the perspiration because there isn’t enough time for that to return to the bowels through the circulation.

There is also a chronic aqueous diarrhœa, in which the atmospheric moisture, drank up by the cutaneous and pulmonary lymphatics, is poured into the intestines, by the retrograde motions of the lacteals. This disease is most similar to the aqueous diabetes, and is frequently exchanged for it: a distinct instance of this is recorded by Benningerus, Cent. v. Obs. 98. in which an aqueous diarrhœa succeeded an aqueous diabetes, and destroyed the patient. There is a curious example of this, described by Sympson (De Re Medica)—"A young man (says he) was seized with a fever, upon which a diarrhœa came on, with great stupor; and he refused to drink any thing, though he was parched up with excessive heat: the better to supply him with moisture, I directed his feet to be immersed in cold water; immediately I observed a wonderful decrease of water in the vessel, and then an impetuous stream of a fluid, scarcely coloured, was discharged by stool, like a cataract."

There is also a chronic watery diarrhea, where the moisture from the atmosphere, absorbed by the skin and lungs, is released into the intestines through the backward movement of the lacteals. This condition is very similar to watery diabetes and is often mistaken for it: a distinct case of this is recorded by Benningerus, Cent. v. Obs. 98, where watery diarrhea followed watery diabetes and ultimately killed the patient. There's an interesting example of this described by Sympson (De Re Medica)—"A young man (he says) was struck with a fever, which was followed by diarrhea and intense stupor; he refused to drink anything, even though he was extremely thirsty from excessive heat: to better provide him with moisture, I had his feet soaked in cold water; immediately I noticed a remarkable reduction of water in the vessel, and then a powerful stream of fluid, barely colored, was discharged by stool, like a waterfall."

7. There is another kind of diarrhœa, which has been called cæliaca; in this disease the chyle, drank up by the lacteals of the small intestines, is probably poured into the large intestines, by the retrograde motions of their lacteals: as in the chyliferous diabetes, the chyle is poured into the bladder, by the retrograde motions of the urinary branch of absorbents.

7. There’s another type of diarrhea called celiac disease; in this condition, the chyle absorbed by the lacteals in the small intestine is likely pushed into the large intestine due to the backward movement of those lacteals. In chylous diabetes, the chyle is sent into the bladder through the reverse motion of the urinary absorbent vessels.

The chyliferous diabetes, like this chyliferous diarrhœa, produces sudden atrophy; since the nourishment, which ought to supply the hourly waste of the body, is expelled by the bladder, or rectum: whilst the aqueous diabetes, and the aqueous diarrhœa produce excessive thirst; because the moisture, which is obtained from the atmosphere, is not conveyed to the thoracic receptacle, as it ought to be, but to the bladder, or lower intestines; whence the chyle, blood, and whole system of glands, are robbed of their proportion of humidity.

Chyliferous diabetes, like chyliferous diarrhea, causes rapid weight loss because the nutrients that should replace the body's daily losses are expelled through the bladder or rectum. On the other hand, aqueous diabetes and aqueous diarrhea lead to extreme thirst, as the moisture that should be stored in the chest cavity instead goes to the bladder or lower intestines. This results in the chyle, blood, and entire glandular system losing their necessary moisture.

8. There is a third species of diabetes, in which the urine is mucilaginous, and appears ropy in pouring it from one vessel into another; and will sometimes coagulate over the fire. This disease appears by intervals, and ceases again, and seems to be occasioned by a previous dropsy in some part of the body. When such a collection is reabsorbed, it is not always returned into the circulation; but the same irritation that stimulates one lymphatic branch to reabsorb the deposited fluid, inverts the urinary branch, and pours it into the bladder. Hence this mucilaginous diabetes is a cure, or the consequence of a cure, of a worse disease, rather than a disease itself.

8. There’s a third type of diabetes, where the urine is thick and looks sticky when you pour it from one container to another; it can even solidify when heated. This condition comes and goes and seems to be triggered by a previous buildup of fluid in part of the body. When that buildup is reabsorbed, it doesn’t always go back into the bloodstream; instead, the same irritation that prompts one lymphatic system to absorb the excess fluid causes the urinary system to release it into the bladder. Therefore, this thick diabetes is more of a result or a treatment of a more serious condition, rather than a disease on its own.

Dr. Cotunnius gave half an ounce of cream of tartar, every morning, to a patient, who had the anasarca; and he voided a great quantity of urine; a part of which, put over the fire, coagulated, on the evaporation of half of it, so as to look like the white of an egg. De Ischiade Nervos.

Dr. Cotunnius gave half an ounce of cream of tartar every morning to a patient who had anasarca, and the patient produced a large amount of urine. When a portion of it was heated, it coagulated, looking like egg whites after half of it evaporated. De Ischiade Nervos.

This kind of diabetes frequently precedes a dropsy; and has this remarkable circumstance attending it, that it generally happens in the night; as during the recumbent state of the body, the fluid, that was accumulated in the cellular membrane, or in the lungs, is more readily absorbed, as it is less impeded by its gravity. I have seen more than one instance of this disease. Mr. D. a man in the decline of life, who had long accustomed himself to spirituous liquor, had swelled legs, and other symptoms of approaching anasarca; about once in a week, or ten days, for several months, he was seized, on going to bed, with great general uneasiness, which his attendants resembled to an hysteric fit; and which terminated in a great discharge of viscid urine; his legs became less swelled, and he continued in better health for some days afterwards. I had not the opportunity to try if this urine would coagulate over the fire, when part of it was evaporated, which I imagine would be the criterion of this kind of diabetes; as the mucilaginous fluid deposited in the cells and cysts of the body, which have no communication with the external air, seems to acquire, by stagnation, this property of coagulation by heat, which the secreted mucus of the intestines and bladder do not appear to possess; as I have found by experiment: and if any one should suppose this coagulable urine was separated from the blood by the kidneys, he may recollect, that in the most inflammatory diseases, in which the blood is most replete or most ready to part with the coagulable lymph, none of this appears in the urine.

This type of diabetes often comes before swelling; and there’s this interesting point about it: it usually occurs at night. When the body is lying down, the fluid that has built up in the tissues or lungs is more easily absorbed because gravity isn’t hindering it as much. I’ve seen this condition more than once. Mr. D., an older man who had a long history of drinking alcohol, experienced swollen legs and other signs of developing severe swelling. About once a week or every ten days, for several months, he would feel very uncomfortable right before going to bed, which his caregivers compared to a hysterical episode; this would end with a large release of thick urine, after which his legs would swell less, and he would feel healthier for a few days. I never got the chance to see if this urine would thicken when heated, which I think would be an indicator of this type of diabetes. The thick fluid that collects in the body’s cells and sacs, which don’t connect to the outside air, seems to gain this ability to thicken from heat when it stagnates, unlike the mucus secreted in the intestines and bladder, which I’ve found doesn’t have this property. And if anyone thinks this thick urine comes from the blood through the kidneys, they should remember that in the most inflammatory diseases, where the blood is full or most ready to release the thick lymph, none of this shows up in the urine.

9. Different kinds of diabetes require different methods of cure. For the first kind, or chyliferous diabetes, after clearing the stomach and intestines, by ipecacuanha and rhubarb, to evacuate any acid material, which may too powerfully stimulate the mouths of the lacteals, repeated and large doses of tincture of cantharides have been much recommended. The specific stimulus of this medicine, on the neck of the bladder, is likely to excite the numerous absorbent vessels, which are spread on that part, into stronger natural actions, and by that means prevent their retrograde ones; till, by persisting in the use of the medicine, their natural habits of motions might again be established. Another indication of cure, requires such medicines, as by lining the intestines with mucilaginous substances, or with such as consist of smooth particles, or which chemically destroy the acrimony of their contents, may prevent the too great action of the intestinal absorbents. For this purpose, I have found the earth precipitated from a solution of alum, by means of fixed alcali, given in the dose of half a dram every six hours, of great advantage, with a few grains of rhubarb, so as to produce a daily evacuation.

9. Different types of diabetes require different treatments. For the first type, or chyliferous diabetes, after cleaning out the stomach and intestines with ipecacuanha and rhubarb to eliminate any acidic materials that might overly stimulate the lacteals, large and repeated doses of tincture of cantharides have been highly recommended. The specific effect of this medicine on the bladder neck is likely to activate the many absorbent vessels in that area, encouraging them to function more effectively and preventing their backward actions; with continued use of the medicine, their natural motion habits might be restored. Another treatment goal requires medicines that coat the intestines with mucilaginous substances or smooth particles, or those that chemically neutralize the acidity of their contents, in order to curb the excessive action of the intestinal absorbents. For this purpose, I have found that the earth precipitated from a solution of alum using fixed alkali, given in a dose of half a dram every six hours, is very beneficial, especially when combined with a few grains of rhubarb to ensure a daily bowel movement.

The food should consist of materials that have the least stimulus, with calcareous water, as of Bristol and Matlock; that the mouths of the lacteals may be as little stimulated as is necessary for their proper absorption; lest with their greater exertions, should be connected by sympathy, the inverted motions of the urinary lymphatics.

The food should be made up of ingredients that cause the least irritation, along with mineral-rich water like that from Bristol and Matlock; this way, the mouths of the lacteals will be stimulated just enough for proper absorption; otherwise, increased effort might trigger the reversed actions of the urinary lymphatics.

The same method may be employed with equal advantage in the aqueous diabetes, so great is the sympathy between the skin and the stomach. To which, however, some application to the skin might be usefully added; as rubbing the patient all over with oil, to prevent the too great action of the cutaneous absorbents. I knew an experiment of this kind made upon one patient with apparent advantage.

The same method can be used just as effectively for watery diabetes because of the strong connection between the skin and the stomach. However, adding some treatment to the skin could be beneficial, such as massaging the patient with oil to reduce excessive activity of the skin's absorbent pores. I know of an experiment like this performed on one patient that showed clear benefits.

The mucilaginous diabetes will require the same treatment, which is most efficacious in the dropsy, and will be described below. I must add, that the diet and medicines above mentioned, are strongly recommended by various authors, as by Morgan, Willis, Harris, and Etmuller; but more histories of the successful treatment of these diseases are wanting to fully ascertain the most efficacious methods of cure.

The slimy diabetes will need the same treatment that's most effective for dropsy, which will be described below. I should mention that the diet and medicines mentioned earlier are highly recommended by various authors, including Morgan, Willis, Harris, and Etmuller; however, we need more accounts of successful treatments for these conditions to really determine the best methods of cure.

In a letter from Mr. Charles Darwin, dated April 24, 1778, Edinburgh, is the subsequent passage:—"A man who had long laboured under a diabetes died yesterday in the clinical ward. He had for some time drank four, and passed twelve pounds of fluid daily; each pound of urine contained an ounce of sugar. He took, without considerable relief, gum kino, sanguis diaconis melted with alum, tincture of cantharides, isinglass, gum arabic, crabs eyes, spirit of hartshorn, and eat ten or fifteen oysters thrice a day. Dr. Home, having read my thesis, bled him, and found that neither the fresh blood nor the serum tasted sweet. His body was opened this morning—every viscus appeared in a sound and natural state, except that the left kidney had a very small pelvis, and that there was a considerable enlargement of most of the mesenteric lymphatic glands. I intend to insert this in my thesis, as it coincides with the experiment, where some asparagus was eaten at the beginning of intoxication, and its smell perceived in the urine, though not in the blood."

In a letter from Mr. Charles Darwin, dated April 24, 1778, Edinburgh, is the following passage:—"A man who had been suffering from diabetes for a long time died yesterday in the clinical ward. He had been drinking four pounds and passing twelve pounds of fluid daily; each pound of urine contained an ounce of sugar. He took, without much relief, gum kino, sanguis diaconis mixed with alum, tincture of cantharides, isinglass, gum arabic, crab's eyes, spirit of hartshorn, and ate ten or fifteen oysters three times a day. Dr. Home, after reading my thesis, bled him and found that neither the fresh blood nor the serum tasted sweet. His body was examined this morning—every organ looked healthy and normal, except that the left kidney had a very small pelvis, and there was significant enlargement of most of the mesenteric lymphatic glands. I plan to include this in my thesis, as it aligns with the experiment where some asparagus was eaten at the start of intoxication, and its smell was noticeable in the urine, though not in the blood."

The following case of chyliferous diabetes is extracted from some letters of Mr. Hughes, to whose unremitted care the infirmary at Stafford for many years was much indebted. Dated October 10, 1778.

The following case of chyliferous diabetes is taken from some letters of Mr. Hughes, whose dedicated care greatly benefited the infirmary at Stafford for many years. Dated October 10, 1778.

Richard Davis, aged 33, a whitesmith by trade, had drank hard by intervals; was much troubled with sweating of his hands, which incommoded him in his occupation, but which ceased on his frequently dipping them in lime. About seven months ago he began to make large quantities of water; his legs are œdematous, his belly tense, and he complains of a rising in his throat, like the globus hystericus: he eats twice as much as other people, drinks about fourteen pints of small beer a day, besides a pint of ale, some milk-porridge, and a bason of broth, and he makes about eighteen pints of water a day.

Richard Davis, 33, a white smith by trade, had been drinking heavily at times; he was often troubled by sweating hands, which made it difficult for him to work, but this stopped when he frequently dipped them in lime. About seven months ago, he started to produce large amounts of urine; his legs are swollen, his stomach feels tight, and he complains of a sensation in his throat, similar to globus hystericus. He eats twice as much as most people, drinks about fourteen pints of small beer a day, plus a pint of ale, some milk porridge, and a bowl of broth, and he urinates around eighteen pints a day.

He tried alum, dragon's blood, steel, blue vitriol, and cantharides in large quantities, and duly repeated, under the care of Dr. Underhill, but without any effect; except that on the day after he omitted the cantharides, he made but twelve pints of water, but on the next day this good effect ceased again.

He tried alum, dragon's blood, steel, blue vitriol, and cantharides in large amounts, and repeated these under the supervision of Dr. Underhill, but nothing worked; except that on the day after he stopped taking the cantharides, he produced only twelve pints of water, but the next day that positive effect went away again.

November 21.—He made eighteen pints of water, and he now, at Dr. Darwin's request, took a grain of opium every four hours, and five grains of aloes at night; and had a flannel shirt given him.

November 21.—He produced eighteen pints of urine, and now, at Dr. Darwin's request, he took a grain of opium every four hours and five grains of aloes at night; and was given a flannel shirt.

22.—Made sixteen pints. 23.—Thirteen pints: drinks less.

22.—Made sixteen pints. 23.—Thirteen pints: drinks less.

24.—Increased the opium to a grain and quarter every four hours: he made twelve pints.

24.—Increased the opium to a grain and a quarter every four hours: he made twelve pints.

25.—Increased the opium to a grain and half: he now makes ten pints; and drinks eight pints in a day.

25.—Increased the opium to one and a half grains: he now makes ten pints; and drinks eight pints in a day.

The opium was gradually increased during the next fortnight, till he took three grains every four hours, but without any further diminution of his water. During the use of the opium he sweat much in the nights, so as to have large drops stand on his face and all over him. The quantity of opium was then gradually decreased, but not totally omitted, as he continued to take about a grain morning and evening.

The amount of opium was gradually increased over the next two weeks until he was taking three grains every four hours, but this did not lessen his need for water. While using the opium, he sweated a lot at night, with large drops forming on his face and body. The dosage of opium was then gradually lowered, but it wasn't completely stopped, as he kept taking about a grain in the morning and evening.

January 17.—He makes fourteen pints of water a day. Dr. Underhill now directed him two scruples of common rosin triturated with as much sugar, every six hours; and three grains of opium every night.

January 17.—He produces fourteen pints of water a day. Dr. Underhill now prescribed two scruples of common rosin ground up with the same amount of sugar, every six hours; and three grains of opium each night.

19.—Makes fifteen pints of water: sweats at night.

19.—Makes fifteen pints of water: sweats at night.

21.—Makes seventeen pints of water; has twitchings of his limbs in a morning, and pains of his legs: he now takes a dram of rosin for a dose, and continues the opium.

21.—Produces seventeen pints of water; experiences twitching in his limbs in the morning, and leg pain: he now takes a shot of rosin as a dose, and continues using the opium.

23.—Water more coloured, and reduced to sixteen pints, and he thinks has a brackish taste.

23.—The water appears more colorful and has been reduced to sixteen pints, and he thinks it has a salty taste.

26.—Water reduced to fourteen pints.

Water cut to fourteen pints.

28.—Water thirteen pints: he continues the opium, and takes four scruples of the rosin for a dose.

28.—Water thirteen pints: he keeps taking the opium and takes four scruples of the rosin for a dose.

February 1.—Water twelve pints.

February 1.—Drink twelve pints.

4.—Water eleven pints: twitchings less; takes five scruples for a dose.

4.—Water eleven pints: twitching reduced; takes five scruples for a dose.

8.—Water ten pints: has had many stools.

8.—Drink ten pints of water: has had multiple stools.

12.—Appetite less: purges very much.

12.—Reduced appetite: purges significantly.

After this the rosin either purged him, or would not stay on his stomach; and he gradually relapsed nearly to his former condition, and in a few months sunk under the disease.

After this, the rosin either made him sick or wouldn’t stay in his stomach; and he slowly fell back into his previous condition, and in a few months succumbed to the illness.

October 3, Mr. Hughes evaporated two quarts of the water, and obtained from it four ounces and half of a hard and brittle saccharine mass, like treacle which had been some time boiled. Four ounces of blood, which he took from his arm with design to examine it, had the common appearances, except that the serum resembled cheese-whey; and that on the evidence of four persons, two of whom did not know what it was they tasted, the serum had a saltish taste.

October 3, Mr. Hughes evaporated two quarts of water and obtained four ounces of a hard, brittle sugary mass that looked like treacle that had been boiled for a while. He drew four ounces of blood from his arm to examine it, which appeared normal except that the serum looked like cheese whey. According to four people, two of whom were unaware of what they were tasting, the serum had a salty flavor.

From hence it appears, that the saccharine matter, with which the urine of these patients so much abounds, does not enter the blood-vessels like the nitre and asparagus mentioned above; but that the process of digestion resembles the process of the germination of vegetables, or of making barley into malt; as the vast quantity of sugar found in the urine must be made from the food which he took (which was double that taken by others), and from the fourteen pints of small beer which he drank. And, secondly, as the serum of the blood was not sweet, the chyle appears to have been conveyed to the bladder without entering the circulation of the blood, since so large a quantity of sugar, as was found in the urine, namely, twenty ounces a day, could not have previously existed in the blood without being perceptible to the taste.

From this, it seems that the sugary substance found in the urine of these patients doesn't enter the bloodstream like the nitrate or asparagus mentioned earlier; instead, the digestion process is similar to how plants germinate or how barley is turned into malt. The large amount of sugar detected in the urine must come from the food they consumed (which was twice the amount eaten by others) and the fourteen pints of beer they drank. Additionally, since the blood serum wasn't sweet, it appears that the chyle was sent to the bladder without passing through the bloodstream, as such a large amount of sugar—specifically twenty ounces a day—couldn't have been present in the blood without being noticeable in taste.

November 1. Mr. Hughes dissolved two drams of nitre in a pint of a decoction of the roots of asparagus, and added to it two ounces of tincture of rhubarb: the patient took a fourth part of this mixture every five minutes, till he had taken the whole.—In about half an hour he made eighteen ounces of water, which was very manifestly tinged with the rhubarb; the smell of asparagus was doubtful.

November 1. Mr. Hughes dissolved two drams of saltpetre in a pint of asparagus root tea and added two ounces of rhubarb tincture to it. The patient took one-fourth of this mixture every five minutes until he finished it. In about half an hour, he produced eighteen ounces of urine, which was clearly colored by the rhubarb; the scent of asparagus was uncertain.

He then lost four ounces of blood, the serum of which was not so opake as that drawn before, but of a yellowish cast, as the serum of the blood usually appears.

He then lost four ounces of blood, which was not as cloudy as the blood taken before, but had a yellowish tint, like the serum of blood typically does.

Paper, dipped three or four times in the tinged urine and dried again, did not scintillate when it was set on fire; but when the flame was blown out, the fire ran along the paper for half an inch; which, when the same paper was unimpregnated, it would not do; nor when the same paper was dipped in urine made before he took the nitre, and dried in the same manner.

Paper, dipped three or four times in the colored urine and then dried, didn’t sparkle when set on fire; but when the flame was blown out, the fire traveled along the paper for half an inch; which wouldn’t happen with the same paper if it wasn’t soaked, nor would it occur if the same paper was dipped in urine made before he took the saltpeter and dried the same way.

Paper, dipped in the serum of the blood and dried in the same manner as in the urine, did not scintillate when the flame was blown out, but burnt exactly in the same manner as the same paper dipped in the serum of blood drawn from another person.

Paper, soaked in blood serum and dried like it is with urine, didn’t sparkle when the flame was extinguished, but burned in exactly the same way as paper soaked in blood serum taken from someone else.

This experiment, which is copied from a letter of Mr. Hughes, as well as the former, seems to evince the existence of another passage from the intestines to the bladder, in this disease, besides that of the sanguiferous system; and coincides with the curious experiment related in section the third, except that the smell of the asparagus was not here perceived, owing perhaps to the roots having been made use of instead of the heads.

This experiment, which is taken from a letter by Mr. Hughes, just like the previous one, seems to show that there’s another way for substances to move from the intestines to the bladder in this disease, apart from the blood system. It aligns with the interesting experiment mentioned in section three, except that the smell of the asparagus wasn’t detected here, possibly because the roots were used instead of the tops.

The rising in the throat of this patient, and the twitchings of his limbs, seem to indicate some similarity between the diabetes and the hysteric disease, besides the great flow of pale urine, which is common to them both.

The swelling in this patient's throat and the twitching in his limbs seem to show some similarities between diabetes and hysterical disorders, in addition to the large amount of pale urine, which is common to both.

Perhaps if the mesenteric glands were nicely inspected in the dissections of these patients; and if the thoracic duct, and the larger branches of the lacteals, and if the lymphatics, which arise from the bladder, were well examined by injection, or by the knife, the cause of diabetes might be more certainly understood.

Perhaps if the mesenteric glands were thoroughly examined during the dissections of these patients; and if the thoracic duct, the larger branches of the lacteals, and the lymphatics that come from the bladder were carefully investigated through injection or dissection, the cause of diabetes might be more clearly understood.

The opium alone, and the opium with the rosin, seem much to have served this patient, and might probably have effected a cure, if the disease had been slighter, or the medicine had been exhibited, before it had been confirmed by habit during the seven months it had continued. The increase of the quantity of water on beginning the large doses of rosin was probably owing to his omitting the morning doses of opium.

The opium by itself, and the opium mixed with rosin, seemed to have helped this patient a lot and might have actually cured him if the illness had been less severe or if the medicine had been given before it became a habit over the seven months it lasted. The increase in the amount of water when he started taking large doses of rosin was likely because he skipped the morning doses of opium.

V. The Phænomena of Dropsies explained.

V. The Phenomena of Edema Explained.

I. Some inebriates have their paroxysms of inebriety terminated by much pale urine, or profuse sweats, or vomiting, or stools; others have their paroxysms terminated by stupor, or sleep, without the above evacuations.

I. Some drinkers end their bouts of drunkenness with a lot of pale urine, heavy sweating, vomiting, or diarrhea; others finish their episodes with a daze or sleep, without any of those other symptoms.

The former kind of these inebriates have been observed to be more liable to diabetes and dropsy; and the latter to gout, gravel, and leprosy. Evoe! attend ye bacchanalians! start at this dark train of evils, and, amid your immodest jests, and idiot laughter, recollect,

The first type of these drinkers has been noticed to be more likely to develop diabetes and swelling; the second type is more prone to gout, kidney stones, and leprosy. Evoe! Listen up, party animals! Shudder at this dark list of problems, and, in the midst of your inappropriate jokes and foolish laughter, remember,

Quem Deus vult perdere, prius dementat.

Quem Deus vult perdere, prius dementat.

In those who are subject to diabetes and dropsy, the absorbent vessels are naturally more irritable than in the latter; and by being frequently disturbed or inverted by violent stimulus, and by their too great sympathy with each other, they become at length either entirely paralytic, or are only susceptible of motion from the stimulus of very acrid materials; as every part of the body, after having been used to great irritations, becomes less affected by smaller ones. Thus we cannot distinguish objects in the night, for some time after we come out of a strong light, though the iris is presently dilated; and the air of a summer evening appears cold, after we have been exposed to the heat of the day.

In people with diabetes and edema, the absorbent vessels are naturally more sensitive than in others; and when they are often disturbed or disrupted by strong stimuli, and by being overly connected to each other, they eventually become either completely paralyzed or can only move in response to very irritating substances. This is similar to how every part of the body, after being used to intense irritations, becomes less responsive to smaller ones. For example, we can't see well in the dark for a while after coming out of bright light, even though our pupils quickly adjust; and the air on a summer evening feels cold after we've been in the heat all day.

There are no cells in the body, where dropsy may not be produced, if the lymphatics cease to absorb that mucilaginous fluid, which is perpetually deposited in them, for the purpose of lubricating their surfaces.

There are no cells in the body where swelling can't occur if the lymphatic system stops absorbing the sticky fluid that constantly collects in them to keep their surfaces lubricated.

If the lymphatic branch, which opens into the cellular membrane, either does its office imperfectly, or not at all; these cells become replete with a mucilaginous fluid, which, after it has stagnated some time in the cells, will coagulate over the fire; and is erroneously called water. Wherever the seat of this disease is, (unless in the lungs or other pendent viscera) the mucilaginous liquid above mentioned will subside to the most depending parts of the body, as the feet and legs, when those are lower than the head and trunk; for all these cells have communications with each other.

If the lymphatic branch that connects to the cell membrane doesn't do its job properly or at all, these cells fill up with a thick fluid that, after sitting for a while, will harden when heated and is mistakenly called water. No matter where this disease is located (except in the lungs or other hanging organs), the thick liquid will settle in the lowest parts of the body, like the feet and legs, when those areas are lower than the head and torso, since all these cells are connected to each other.

When the cellular absorbents are become insensible to their usual irritations, it most frequently happens, but not always, that the cutaneous branch of absorbents, which is strictly associated with them, suffers the like inability. And then, as no water is absorbed from the atmosphere, the urine is not only less diluted at the time of its secretion, and consequently in less quantity and higher coloured: but great thirst is at the same time induced, for as no water is absorbed from the atmosphere to dilute the chyle and blood, the lacteals and other absorbent vessels, which have not lost their powers, are excited into more constant or more violent action, to supply this deficiency; whence the urine becomes still less in quantity, and of a deeper colour, and turbid like the yolk of an egg, owing to a greater absorption of its thinner parts. From this stronger action of those absorbents, which still retain their irritability, the fat is also absorbed, and the whole body becomes emaciated. This increased exertion of some branches of the lymphatics, while others are totally or partially paralytic, is resembled by what constantly occurs in the hemiplagia; when the patient has lost the use of the limbs on one side, he is incessantly moving those of the other; for the moving power, not having access to the paralytic limbs, becomes redundant in those which are not diseased.

When the cellular absorbents become unresponsive to their usual stimuli, it often happens, though not always, that the skin's absorbent branches, closely linked to them, experience the same issue. As a result, since no water is absorbed from the atmosphere, the urine is not only less diluted when secreted, but is also produced in smaller quantities and is more concentrated. This leads to increased thirst, as the lack of water absorption from the atmosphere means the chyle and blood aren't diluted. The lacteals and other absorbent vessels that haven't lost their function become more active to compensate for this deficiency; consequently, the urine is produced in even lesser amounts, becomes deeper in color, and appears cloudy like egg yolk due to the increased absorption of its thinner components. This heightened activity in the absorbents that remain responsive also results in the absorption of fat, leading to overall body wasting. This situation, where some lymphatic branches become overactive while others are completely or partially paralyzed, is similar to what happens in hemiplegia; when a patient loses the use of limbs on one side, they constantly move the limbs on the unaffected side because the moving power, unable to reach the paralyzed limbs, becomes excessive in those that are still healthy.

The paucity of urine and thirst cannot be explained from a greater quantity of mucilaginous fluid being deposited in the cellular membrane: for though these symptoms have continued many weeks, or even months, this collection frequently does not amount to more than very few pints. Hence also the difficulty of promoting copious sweats in anasarca is accounted for, as well as the great thirst, paucity of urine, and loss of fat; since, when the cutaneous branch of absorbents is paralytic, or nearly so, there is already too small a quantity of aqueous fluid in the blood: nor can these torpid cutaneous lymphatics be readily excited into retrograde motions.

The low amount of urine and constant thirst can't be explained by a larger quantity of thick fluid being stored in the tissue: even though these symptoms can last for weeks or even months, this buildup often amounts to very little—like just a few pints. This also explains why it's hard to induce heavy sweating in cases of anasarca, along with the intense thirst, low urine output, and loss of fat. When the skin's lymphatic system is paralyzed or close to it, there isn't enough water in the blood. Plus, these sluggish lymphatic vessels can't easily be stimulated to work in reverse.

Hence likewise we understand, why in the ascites, and some other dropsies, there is often no thirst, and no paucity of urine; in these cases the cutaneous absorbents continue to do their office.

Hence, we also understand why in ascites and some other types of edema, there is often no thirst and no decrease in urine output; in these cases, the skin’s absorbent vessels continue to function properly.

Some have believed, that dropsies were occasioned by the inability of the kidneys, from having only observed the paucity of urine; and have thence laboured much to obtain diuretic medicines; but it is daily observable, that those who die of a total inability to make water, do not become dropsical in consequence of it: Fernelius mentions one, who laboured under a perfect suppression of urine during twenty days before his death, and yet had no symptoms of dropsy. Pathol. 1. vi. c. 8. From the same idea many physicians have restrained their patients from drinking, though their thirst has been very urgent; and some cases have been published, where this cruel regimen has been thought advantageous: but others of nicer observation are of opinion, that it has always aggravated the distresses of the patient; and though it has abated his swellings, yet by inducing a fever it has hastened his dissolution. See Transactions of the College, London, vol. ii. p. 235. Cases of Dropsy by Dr. G. Baker.

Some have believed that dropsy was caused by the kidneys not functioning properly, just because they noticed a low amount of urine; and so they've worked hard to find diuretic medicines. However, it is clear that those who die from complete inability to urinate do not develop dropsy as a result. Fernelius mentions a case of someone who went without urine for twenty days before dying, yet showed no signs of dropsy. Pathol. 1. vi. c. 8. From this same belief, many doctors have told their patients to stop drinking, even when their thirst was intense; some cases have been reported where this harsh treatment was thought to be helpful. Yet others with more careful observation believe it has always worsened the patient's suffering; and although it may have reduced swelling, it has, by causing a fever, sped up their death. See Transactions of the College, London, vol. ii. p. 235. Cases of Dropsy by Dr. G. Baker.

The cure of anasarca, so far as respects the evacuation of the accumulated fluid, coincides with the idea of the retrograde action of the lymphatic system. It is well known that vomits, and other drugs, which induce sickness or nausea; at the same time that they evacuate the stomach, produce a great absorption of the lymph accumulated in the cellular membrane. In the operation of a vomit, not only the motions of the stomach and duodenum become inverted, but also those of the lymphatics and lacteals, which belong to them; whence a great quantity of chyle and lymph is perpetually poured into the stomach and intestines, during the operation, and evacuated by the mouth. Now at the same time, other branches of the lymphatic system, viz. those which open on the cellular membrane, are brought into more energetic action, by the sympathy above mentioned, and an increase of their absorption is produced.

The treatment of anasarca, in terms of getting rid of the excess fluid, relates to the reverse action of the lymphatic system. It's well understood that medications that cause vomiting or nausea, while emptying the stomach, also lead to a significant absorption of the lymph that's built up in the cellular membrane. During vomiting, not only do the movements of the stomach and small intestine reverse, but so do those of the lymphatic and lacteal systems connected to them; this results in a large amount of chyle and lymph being continuously poured into the stomach and intestines during the process and expelled through the mouth. At the same time, other parts of the lymphatic system, specifically those that connect to the cellular membrane, become more active due to the previously mentioned symbiotic response, leading to an increase in their absorption.

Hence repeated vomits, and cupreous salts, and small doses of squill or foxglove, are so efficacious in this disease. And as drastic purges act also by inverting the motions of the lacteals; and thence the other branches of lymphatics are induced into more powerful natural action, by sympathy, and drink up the fluids from all the cells of the body; and by their anastomoses, pour them into the lacteal branches; which, by their inverted actions, return them into the intestines; and they are thus evacuated from the body:—these purges also are used with success in discharging the accumulated fluid in anasarca.

Therefore, repeated vomiting, copper salts, and small doses of squill or foxglove are very effective for this illness. Drastic purges also work by reversing the movements of the lacteals; this leads the other branches of the lymphatic system to engage in stronger natural activity, drawing fluids from all the cells in the body. Through their connections, they channel these fluids into the lacteal branches, which, through their reversed actions, send them back into the intestines, where they are then expelled from the body. These purges are also successfully used to remove the excess fluid in cases of anasarca.

II. The following cases are related with design to ascertain the particular kinds of dropsy in which the digitalis purpurea, or common foxglove, is preferable to squill, or other evacuants, and were first published in 1780, in a pamphlet entitled Experiments on mucilaginous and purulent Matter, &c. Cadell. London. Other cases of dropsy, treated with digitalis, were afterwards published by Dr. Darwin in the Medical Transactions, vol. iii. in which there is a mistake in respect to the dose of the powder of foxglove, which should have been from five grains to one, instead of from five grains to ten.

II. The following cases are related to the specific types of dropsy for which digitalis purpurea, or common foxglove, is more effective than squill or other diuretics. These were first published in 1780 in a pamphlet titled "Experiments on Mucilaginous and Purulent Matter, &c." by Cadell, London. Additional cases of dropsy treated with digitalis were later published by Dr. Darwin in the Medical Transactions, vol. iii., which contains an error regarding the dosage of foxglove powder; it should have been listed as five grains to one, rather than five grains to ten.

Anasarca of the Lungs.

Lung Edema.

1. A lady, between forty and fifty years of age, had been indisposed some time, was then seized with cough and fever, and afterwards expectorated much digested mucus. This expectoration suddenly ceased, and a considerable difficulty of breathing supervened, with a pulse very irregular both in velocity and strength; she was much distressed at first lying down, and at first rising; but after a minute or two bore either of those attitudes with ease. She had no pain or numbness in her arms; she had no hectic fever, nor any cold shiverings, and the urine was in due quantity, and of the natural colour.

1. A woman, between forty and fifty years old, had been unwell for some time, then was hit with a cough and fever, and later coughed up a lot of thick mucus. This coughing suddenly stopped, and she began to have a lot of trouble breathing, with a pulse that was very irregular in both speed and strength; she felt very uncomfortable when lying down at first and when getting up, but after a minute or two, she was able to manage either position with ease. She didn’t have any pain or numbness in her arms; she didn’t have a fever or any chills, and her urine was the right amount and the usual color.

The difficulty of breathing was twice considerably relieved by small doses of ipecacuanha, which operated upwards and downwards, but recurred in a few days: she was then directed a decoction of foxglove, (digitalis purpurea) prepared by boiling four ounces of the fresh leaves from two pints of water to one pint; to which was added two ounces of vinous spirit: she took three large spoonfuls of this mixture every two hours, till she had taken it four times; a continued sickness supervened, with frequent vomiting, and a copious flow of urine: these evacuations continued at intervals for two or three days, and relieved the difficulty of breathing—She had some relapses afterwards, which were again relieved by the repetition of the decoction of foxglove.

The difficulty in breathing was significantly eased by small doses of ipecac, which had effects both upwards and downwards, but it came back after a few days. She was then prescribed a decoction of foxglove (digitalis purpurea), made by boiling four ounces of fresh leaves in two pints of water until only one pint remained; two ounces of vinous spirit were added to it. She took three large spoonfuls of this mixture every two hours until she had taken it four times; this led to persistent nausea, frequent vomiting, and a heavy flow of urine. These issues continued intermittently for two or three days and helped alleviate the breathing difficulty. She had some relapses later, which were again managed by repeating the foxglove decoction.

2. A gentleman, about sixty years of age, who had been addicted to an immoderate use of fermented liquors, and had been very corpulent, gradually lost his strength and flesh, had great difficulty of breathing, with legs somewhat swelled, and a very irregular pulse. He was very much distressed at first lying down, and at first rising from his bed, yet in a minute or two was easy in both those attitudes. He made straw-coloured urine in due quantity, and had no pain or numbness of his arms.

2. A man, around sixty years old, who had a serious habit of drinking too much alcohol and had been quite overweight, slowly lost his strength and weight, experienced heavy breathing, had slightly swollen legs, and had an irregular heartbeat. He felt a lot of discomfort when lying down and when getting up from bed at first, but after a minute or two, he felt comfortable in both positions. He produced light-colored urine in normal amounts and didn’t have any pain or numbness in his arms.

He took a large spoonful of the decoction of foxglove, as above, every hour, for ten or twelve successive hours, had incessant sickness for about two days, and passed a large quantity of urine; upon which his breath became quite easy, and the swelling of his legs subsided; but as his whole constitution was already sinking from the previous intemperance of his life, he did not survive more than three or four months.

He took a big spoonful of the foxglove concoction, as mentioned above, every hour for ten or twelve straight hours. He experienced constant nausea for about two days and urinated a lot; after that, his breathing became much easier, and the swelling in his legs went down. However, since his overall health was already deteriorating due to his past excesses, he didn’t last more than three or four months.

Hydrops Pericardii.

Pericardial effusion.

3. A gentleman of temperate life and sedulous application to business, between thirty and forty years of age, had long been subject, at intervals, to an irregular pulse: a few months ago he became weak, with difficulty of breathing, and dry cough. In this situation a physician of eminence directed him to abstain from all animal food and fermented liquor, during which regimen all his complaints increased; he now became emaciated, and totally lost his appetite; his pulse very irregular both in velocity and strength; with great difficulty of breathing, and some swelling of his legs; yet he could lie down horizontally in his bed, though he got little sleep, and passed a due quantity of urine, and of the natural colour: no fullness or hardness could be perceived about the region of the liver; and he had no pain or numbness in his arms.

3. A man living a moderate lifestyle and dedicated to his work, between thirty and forty years old, had experienced episodes of an irregular heartbeat for a long time. A few months ago, he became weak, had trouble breathing, and developed a dry cough. At this point, a well-known doctor advised him to avoid all animal products and alcoholic drinks, but during this time, all his symptoms worsened. He became very thin and completely lost his appetite; his pulse was very irregular in both speed and strength, along with severe breathing difficulties and some swelling in his legs. Still, he could lie flat in bed, even though he slept very little and produced a normal amount of urine that was its usual color. There was no swelling or hardness in the area of his liver, and he felt no pain or numbness in his arms.

One night he had a most profuse sweat all over his body and limbs, which quite deluged his bed, and for a day or two somewhat relieved his difficulty of breathing, and his pulse became less irregular: this copious sweat recurred three or four times at the intervals of five or six days, and repeatedly alleviated his symptoms.

One night, he sweated heavily all over his body and limbs, soaking his bed completely. For a day or two, this offered some relief to his breathing difficulties, and his pulse became less erratic. This intense sweating happened three or four times, with breaks of five or six days in between, and it consistently eased his symptoms.

He was directed one large spoonful of the above decoction of foxglove every hour, till it procured some considerable evacuation: after he had taken it eleven successive hours he had a few liquid stools, attended with a great flow of urine, which last had a dark tinge, as if mixed with a few drops of blood: he continued sick at intervals for two days, but his breath became quite easy, and his pulse quite regular, the swelling of his legs disappeared, and his appetite and sleep returned.

He was given a large spoonful of the foxglove mixture every hour until it caused a significant amount of bowel movements. After taking it for eleven hours straight, he had a few liquid stools accompanied by a large amount of urine, which had a dark color, as if mixed with a few drops of blood. He felt unwell intermittently for two days, but his breathing became easy, his pulse became regular, the swelling in his legs went down, and his appetite and sleep returned.

He then took three grains of white vitriol twice a day, with some bitter medicines, and a grain of opium with five grains of rhubarb every night; was advised to eat flesh meat, and spice, as his stomach would bear it, with small beer, and a few glasses of wine; and had issues made in his thighs; and has suffered no relapse.

He then took three grains of white vitriol twice a day, along with some bitter medications, and a grain of opium with five grains of rhubarb every night; he was advised to eat meat and spices, as his stomach could handle it, along with some small beer and a few glasses of wine; he also had issues made in his thighs; and he has not experienced any relapse.

4. A lady, about fifty years of age, had for some weeks great difficulty of breathing, with very irregular pulse, and considerable general debility: she could lie down in bed, and the urine was in due quantity and of the natural colour, and she had no pain or numbness of her arms.

4. A woman, around fifty years old, had been experiencing severe breathing issues for several weeks, with an irregular pulse and significant overall weakness. She was able to lie down in bed, her urine was the right amount and normal color, and she felt no pain or numbness in her arms.

She took one large spoonful of the above decoction of foxglove every hour, for ten or twelve successive hours; was sick, and made a quantity of pale urine for about two days, and was quite relieved both of the difficulty of breathing, and the irregularity of her pulse. She then took a grain of opium, and five grains of rhubarb, every night, night, for many weeks; with some slight chalybeate and bitter medicines, and has suffered no relapse.

She took a big spoonful of the foxglove mixture mentioned above every hour for ten or twelve hours straight. She felt nauseous and produced a lot of light-colored urine for about two days, but she felt much better both in terms of her breathing and her pulse irregularities. Then she started taking a grain of opium and five grains of rhubarb every night for several weeks, along with some mild iron and bitter medicines, and she hasn’t had any setbacks.

Hydrops Thoracis.

Pleural effusion.

5. A tradesman, about fifty years of age, became weak and short of breath, especially on increase of motion, with pain in one arm, about the insertion of the biceps muscle. He observed he sometimes in the night made an unusual quantity of pale water. He took calomel, alum, and peruvian bark, and all his symptoms increased: his legs began to swell considerably; his breath became more difficult, and he could not lie down in bed; but all this time he made a due quantity of straw-coloured water.

5. A tradesman, around fifty years old, started feeling weak and short of breath, especially when moving more. He also experienced pain in one arm, near where the biceps muscle is attached. He noticed that sometimes at night he produced an unusual amount of pale urine. He took calomel, alum, and Peruvian bark, but all his symptoms worsened: his legs swelled a lot; breathing became harder, and he couldn’t lie down in bed; yet during all this time, he still produced a normal amount of straw-colored urine.

The decoction of foxglove was given as in the preceding cases, which operated chiefly by purging, and seemed to relieve his breath for a day or two; but also seemed to contribute to weaken him.—He became after some weeks universally dropsical, and died comatous.

The decoction of foxglove was given just like in the earlier cases, which mainly worked by purging and seemed to relieve his breathing for a day or two; however, it also appeared to weaken him. After a few weeks, he became completely dropsical and died in a coma.

6. A young lady of delicate constitution, with light eyes and hair, and who had perhaps lived too abstemiously both in respect to the quantity and quality of what she eat and drank, was seized with great difficulty of breathing, so as to threaten immediate death. Her extremities were quite cold, and her breath felt cold to the back of one's hand. She had no sweat, nor could be down for a single moment; and had previously, and at present, complained of great weakness and pain and numbness of both her arms; had no swelling of her legs, no thirst, water in due quantity and colour. Her sister, about a year before, was afflicted with similar symptoms, was repeatedly blooded, and died universally dropsical.

6. A young woman with a fragile constitution, light eyes, and hair, who may have lived too strictly in terms of both the amount and quality of food and drink, experienced severe breathing difficulties that threatened immediate death. Her limbs were very cold, and her breath felt chilly against the back of one’s hand. She had no sweat and couldn’t lie down for even a moment, and she had complained in the past, and was still complaining, of intense weakness, pain, and numbness in both arms; her legs showed no swelling, she had no thirst, and her urine was normal in both quantity and color. Her sister, about a year earlier, had similar symptoms, was repeatedly bled, and eventually died with widespread edema.

A grain of opium was given immediately, and repeated every six hours with evident and amazing advantage; afterwards a blister, with chalybeates, bitters, and essential oils, were exhibited, but nothing had such eminent effect in relieving the difficulty of breathing and coldness of her extremities as opium, by the use of which in a few weeks she perfectly regained her health, and has suffered no relapse.

A grain of opium was given right away and repeated every six hours with clear and impressive results; then a blister, along with iron supplements, bitters, and essential oils, were used, but nothing worked as effectively in easing her breathing troubles and coldness in her limbs as opium. By using it, she fully recovered her health in a few weeks and hasn’t had any relapse since.

Ascites.

Fluid buildup in abdomen.

7. A young lady of delicate constitution having been exposed to great fear, cold, and fatigue, by the overturn of a chaise in the night, began with pain and tumour in the right hypochondrium: in a few months a fluctuation was felt throughout the whole abdomen, more distinctly perceptible indeed about the region of the stomach; since the integuments of the lower part of the abdomen generally become thickened in this disease by a degree of anasarca. Her legs were not swelled, no thirst, water in due quantity and colour.—She took the foxglove so as to induce sickness and stools, but without abating the swelling, and was obliged at length to submit to the operation of tapping.

7. A young woman with a fragile constitution, after experiencing severe fear, cold, and exhaustion from the overturn of a carriage at night, began to suffer from pain and swelling in her right side. A few months later, a noticeable fluctuation was felt throughout her entire abdomen, particularly around her stomach; additionally, the skin on the lower part of her abdomen had thickened due to a degree of swelling. Her legs were not swollen, she had no thirst, and her water intake was normal in quantity and color. She took foxglove to induce vomiting and diarrhea, but it didn’t reduce the swelling, and eventually, she had to undergo the procedure of tapping.

8. A man about sixty-seven, who had long been accustomed to spirituous potation, had some time laboured under ascites; his legs somewhat swelled; his breath easy in all attitudes; no appetite; great thirst; urine in exceedingly small quantity, very deep coloured, and turbid; pulse equal. He took the foxglove in such quantity as vomited him, and induced sickness for two days; but procured no flow of urine, or diminution of his swelling; but was thought to leave him considerably weaker.

8. A man around sixty-seven, who had been used to drinking alcohol for a long time, had been suffering from fluid retention; his legs were a bit swollen; he could breathe easily in any position; he had no appetite; he was very thirsty; his urine was very minimal, dark, and cloudy; and his pulse was steady. He took foxglove in such a quantity that it made him vomit and caused nausea for two days; however, it did not increase his urine flow or reduce his swelling, and it was believed to have left him significantly weaker.

9. A corpulent man, accustomed to large potation of fermented liquors, had vehement cough, difficult breathing, anasarca of his legs, thighs, and hands, and considerable tumour, with evident fluctuation of his abdomen; his pulse was equal; his urine in small quantity, of deep colour, and turbid. These swellings had been twice considerably abated by drastic cathartics. He took three ounces of a decoction of foxglove (made by boiling one ounce of the fresh leaves in a pint of water) every three hours, for two whole days; it then began to vomit and purge him violently, and promoted a great flow of urine; he was by these evacuations completely emptied in twelve hours. After two or three months all these symptoms returned, and were again relieved by the use of the foxglove; and thus in the space of about three years he was about ten times evacuated, and continued all that time his usual potations: excepting at first, the medicine operated only by urine, and did not appear considerably to weaken him—The last time he took it, it had no effect; and a few weeks afterwards he vomited a great quantity of blood, and expired.

9. A overweight man, used to drinking large amounts of alcoholic beverages, had a severe cough, trouble breathing, swelling in his legs, thighs, and hands, and a noticeable mass with evident fluid in his abdomen; his pulse was steady; his urine was low in volume, dark in color, and cloudy. These swellings had been significantly reduced twice by strong laxatives. He took three ounces of a foxglove decoction (made by boiling one ounce of fresh leaves in a pint of water) every three hours for two full days; it then caused him to vomit and purge violently and greatly increased his urine output; he was completely emptied out in twelve hours from these eliminations. After two or three months, all these symptoms returned and were again eased by the use of foxglove; thus, over about three years, he was treated with it around ten times and continued his usual drinking habits during that time, except initially, the medicine worked only through urine and did not seem to significantly weaken him—The last time he took it, it had no effect; and a few weeks later, he vomited a large amount of blood, and passed away.

QUERIES.

1. As the first six of these patients had a due discharge of urine, and of the natural colour, was not the feat of the disease confined to some part of the thorax, and the swelling of the legs rather a symptom of the obstructed circulation of the blood, than of a paralysis of the cellular lymphatics of those parts?

1. Since the first six of these patients were able to urinate normally and their urine was the usual color, wasn't the issue from the disease limited to a specific area of the chest, and was the swelling in their legs more a sign of blocked blood circulation rather than paralysis of the lymphatic system in those areas?

2. When the original disease is a general anasarca, do not the cutaneous lymphatics always become paralytic at the same time with the cellular ones, by their greater sympathy with each other? and hence the paucity of urine, and the great thirst, distinguish this kind of dropsy?

2. When the initial disease is a general swelling, don’t the skin lymphatics always become paralyzed at the same time as the cellular ones, due to their closer connection with each other? And that’s why the low urine output and intense thirst characterize this type of edema?

3. In the anasarca of the lungs, when the disease is not very great, though the patients have considerable difficulty of breathing at their first lying down, yet after a minute or two their breath becomes easy again; and the same occurs at their first rising. Is not this owing to the time necessary for the fluid in the cells of the lungs to change its place, so as the least to incommode respiration in the new attitude?

3. In lung swelling, when the condition isn't too severe, patients might struggle to breathe when they first lie down, but after a minute or two, their breathing returns to normal. The same happens when they first get up. Isn't this due to the time it takes for the fluid in the lung cells to shift, so it minimally disrupts breathing in the new position?

4. In the dropsy of the pericardium does not the patient bear the horizontal or perpendicular attitude with equal ease? Does this circumstance distinguish the dropsy of the pericardium from that of the lungs and of the thorax?

4. In pericardial effusion, doesn’t the patient find it equally easy to lie down flat or sit up? Does this difference set pericardial effusion apart from that of the lungs and thorax?

5. Do the universal sweats distinguish the dropsy of the pericardium, or of the thorax? and those, which cover the upper parts of the body only, the anasarca of the lungs?

5. Do the general sweats indicate the swelling of the pericardium, or of the chest? And do those that only cover the upper parts of the body indicate swelling of the lungs?

6. When in the dropsy of the thorax, the patient endeavours to lie down, does not the extravasated fluid compress the upper parts of the bronchia, and totally preclude the access of air to every part of the lungs; whilst in the perpendicular attitude the inferior parts of the lungs only are compressed? Does not something similar to this occur in the anasarca of the lungs, when the disease is very great, and thus prevent those patients also from lying down?

6. When someone has fluid buildup in their chest, if they try to lie down, doesn’t the fluid press on the upper parts of the bronchi and completely block air from reaching all areas of the lungs? Meanwhile, when standing upright, only the lower parts of the lungs are compressed. Doesn’t a similar situation happen in cases of widespread fluid buildup in the lungs, especially when the condition is severe, making it difficult for those patients to lie down as well?

7. As a principal branch of the fourth cervical nerve of the left side, after having joined a branch of the third and of the second cervical nerves, descending between the subclavian vein and artery, is received in a groove formed for it in the pericardium, and is obliged to make a considerable turn outwards to go over the prominent part of it, where the point of the heart is lodged, in its course to the diaphragm; and as the other phrenic nerve of the right side has a straight course to the diaphragm; and as many other considerable branches of this fourth pair of cervical nerves are spread on the arms; does not a pain in the left arm distinguish a disease of the pericardium, as in the angina pectoris, or in the dropsy of the pericardium? and does not a pain or weakness in both arms distinguish the dropsy of the thorax?

7. As a main branch of the fourth cervical nerve on the left side, after it joins a branch from the third and second cervical nerves, it descends between the subclavian vein and artery, fitting into a groove made for it in the pericardium. It has to make a significant turn outward to go over the bulging part of it, where the point of the heart is located, on its way to the diaphragm. Meanwhile, the phrenic nerve on the right side travels straight to the diaphragm. Since many other important branches from this fourth pair of cervical nerves spread to the arms, doesn’t pain in the left arm indicate a condition of the pericardium, like in angina pectoris or pericardial effusion? And doesn’t pain or weakness in both arms indicate pleural effusion?

8. Do not the dropsies of the thorax and pericardium frequently exist together, and thus add to the uncertainty and fatality of the disease?

8. Don't the fluid accumulations in the chest and around the heart often occur together, making the disease even more uncertain and deadly?

9. Might not the foxglove be serviceable in hydrocephalus internus, in hydrocele, and in white swellings of the joints?

9. Could the foxglove be useful for internal hydrocephalus, for hydrocele, and for white swellings in the joints?

VI. Of cold Sweats.

VI. Cold Sweats.

There have been histories given of chronical immoderate sweatings, which bear some analogy to the diabetes. Dr. Willis mentions a lady then living, whose sweats where for many years so profuse, that all her bed-clothes were not only moistened, but deluged with them every night; and that many ounces, and sometimes pints, of this sweat, were received in vessels properly placed, as it trickled down her body. He adds, that she had great thirst, had taken many medicines, and submitted to various rules of life, and changes of climate, but still continued to have these immoderate sweats. Pharmac. ration. de sudore anglico.

There are accounts of chronic excessive sweating that seem similar to diabetes. Dr. Willis mentions a woman who, for many years, sweated so much that her bedclothes were not just damp but soaked through every night. He notes that several ounces, and sometimes pints, of sweat were collected in containers placed to catch it as it dripped down her body. He adds that she experienced intense thirst, tried numerous medications, and followed different lifestyle changes and climates, but she still suffered from these excessive sweats. Pharmac. ration. de sudore anglico.

Dr. Willis has also observed, that the sudor anglicanus which appeared in England, in 1483, and continued till 1551, was in some respects similar to the diabetes; and as Dr. Caius, who saw this disease, mentions the viscidity, as well as the quantity of these sweats, and adds, that the extremities were often cold, when the internal parts were burnt up with heat and thirst, with great and speedy emaciation and debility: there is great reason to believe, that the fluids were absorbed from the cells of the body by the cellular and cystic branches of the lymphatics, and poured on the skin by the retrograde motions of the cutaneous ones.

Dr. Willis also noted that the sweating sickness, which appeared in England in 1483 and lasted until 1551, was somewhat similar to diabetes. Dr. Caius, who witnessed this illness, mentioned the stickiness and the large amount of sweat, and he added that the extremities were often cold while the internal parts were overheated and thirsty, leading to rapid weight loss and weakness. There is strong reason to believe that the fluids were being absorbed from the body's cells by the lymphatic system's cellular and cystic branches and expelled onto the skin through the reverse movement of the skin's lymphatics.

Sydenham has recorded, in the stationary fever of the year 1685, the viscid sweats flowing from the head, which were probably from the same source as those in the sweating plague above mentioned.

Sydenham noted that in the persistent fever of 1685, there were thick sweats coming from the head, likely originating from the same cause as those in the previously mentioned sweating plague.

It is very common in dropsies of the chest or lungs to have the difficulty of breathing relieved by copious sweats, flowing from the head and neck. Mr. P. about 50 years of age, had for many weeks been afflicted with anasarca of his legs and thighs, attended with difficulty of breathing; and had repeatedly been relieved by squill, other bitters, and chalybeates.—One night the difficulty of breathing became so great, that it was thought he must have expired; but so copious a sweat came out of his head and neck, that in a few hours some pints, by estimation, were wiped off from those parts, and his breath was for a time relieved. This dyspnœa and these sweats recurred at intervals, and after some weeks he ceased to exist. The skin of his head and neck felt cold to the hand, and appeared pale at the time these sweats flowed so abundantly; which is a proof, that they were produced by an inverted motion of the absorbents of those parts: for sweats, which are the consequence of an increased action of the sanguiferous system, are always attended with a warmth of the skin, greater than is natural, and a more florid colour; as the sweats from exercise, or those that succeed the cold fits of agues. Can any one explain how these partial sweats should relieve the difficulty of breathing in anasarca, but by supposing that the pulmonary branch of absorbents drank up the fluid in the cavity of the thorax, or in the cells of the lungs, and threw it on the skin, by the retrograde motions of the cutaneous branch? for, if we could suppose, that the increased action of the cutaneous glands or capillaries poured upon the skin this fluid, previously absorbed from the lungs; why is not the whole surface of the body covered with sweat? why is not the skin warm? Add to this, that the sweats above mentioned were clammy or glutinous, which the condensed perspirable matter is not; whence it would seem to have been a different fluid from that of common perspiration.

It's common for those with chest or lung fluid to find relief from breathing difficulties through heavy sweating from the head and neck. Mr. P., around 50 years old, had suffered for weeks with swelling in his legs and thighs, which made it hard for him to breathe; he often found relief from squill, various bitters, and iron-containing remedies. One night, his breathing became so labored that it seemed he might die, but he started sweating heavily from his head and neck, losing several pints of fluid in just a few hours, which temporarily eased his breathing. This shortness of breath and sweating returned at intervals, and after a few weeks, he passed away. His head and neck felt cold to the touch and appeared pale during these intense sweating episodes; this suggests that the sweating was caused by the backward movement of the absorbent vessels in those areas. Sweating resulting from increased blood flow typically makes the skin warmer and redder, as seen after exercise or during the chills of fevers. Can anyone explain how these localized sweats eased his breath in the case of swelling, except to assume that the lymph vessels in the lungs absorbed the fluid in the chest cavity or lung cells and expelled it through the skin by reverse action of the skin vessels? If we consider that the heightened activity of the skin glands or tiny blood vessels brought this fluid to the skin from the lungs, why isn't the whole body sweating? Why isn't the skin warm? Additionally, the sweats mentioned were sticky or slimy, unlike regular sweating, suggesting they were a different type of fluid altogether.

Dr. Dobson, of Liverpool, has given a very ingenious explanation of the acid sweats, which he observed in a diabetic patient—he thinks part of the chyle is secreted by the skin, and afterwards undergoes an acetous fermentation.—Can the chyle get thither, but by an inverted motion of the cutaneous lymphatics? in the same manner as it is carried to the bladder, by the inverted motions of the urinary lymphatics. Medic. Observat. and Enq. London, vol. v.

Dr. Dobson from Liverpool has provided a clever explanation for the acid sweats he noticed in a diabetic patient—he believes that some of the chyle is secreted through the skin and then goes through an acid fermentation process. Can the chyle reach there except through a reversed motion of the skin's lymphatics, just like it is transported to the bladder by the reversed motions of the urinary lymphatics? Medic. Observat. and Enq. London, vol. v.

Are not the cold sweats in some fainting fits, and in dying people, owing to an inverted motion of the cutaneous lymphatics? for in these there can be no increased arterial or glandular action.

Aren't the cold sweats during fainting spells and in dying individuals caused by a reverse movement of the skin's lymphatic system? Because in these cases, there can't be any increased activity from the arteries or glands.

Is the difficulty of breathing, arising from anasarca of the lungs, relieved by sweats from the head and neck; whilst that difficulty of breathing, which arises from a dropsy of the thorax, or pericardium, is never attended with these sweats of the head? and thence can these diseases be distinguished from each other? Do the periodic returns of nocturnal asthma rise from a temporary dropsy of the lungs, collected during their more torpid state in sound deep, and then re-absorbed by the vehement efforts of the disordered organs of respiration, and carried off by the copious sweats about the head and neck?

Is the difficulty in breathing, caused by fluid buildup in the lungs, relieved by sweating from the head and neck, while the breathing issues from fluid around the chest or heart never come with these head sweats? Can we distinguish between these conditions based on this? Do the recurring bouts of nighttime asthma stem from a temporary buildup of fluid in the lungs that occurs while we are in deep sleep and is then reabsorbed by the intense efforts of the struggling respiratory organs, and eliminated through heavy sweating in the head and neck?

More extensive and accurate dissections of the lymphatic system are wanting to enable us to unravel these knots of science.

More detailed and precise studies of the lymphatic system are needed to help us untangle these complexities of science.

VII. Translations of Matter, of Chyle, of Milk, of Urine. Operation of purging Drugs applied externally.

VII. Translations of Matter, of Chyle, of Milk, of Urine. Effect of purging drugs used externally.

1. The translations of matter from one part of the body to another, can only receive an explanation from the doctrine of the occasional retrograde motions of some branches of the lymphatic system: for how can matter, absorbed and mixed with the whole mass of blood, be so hastily collected again in any one part? and is it not an immutable law, in animal bodies, that each gland can secrete no other, but its own proper fluid? which is, in part, fabricated in the very gland by an animal process, which it there undergoes: of these purulent translations innumerable and very remarkable instances are recorded.

1. The movement of substances from one part of the body to another can only be understood through the theory of the occasional backward motions of certain branches of the lymphatic system: how can substances, absorbed and mixed with the entire blood supply, be so quickly gathered again in any specific area? And isn't it a fundamental rule in living organisms that each gland can only produce its own specific fluid? This fluid is partly created in the gland itself through a biological process that occurs there: there are countless well-documented instances of these unusual movements of pus.

2. The chyle, which is seen among the materials thrown up by violent vomiting, or in purging stools, can only come thither by its having been poured into the bowels by the inverted motions of the lacteals: for our aliment is not converted into chyle in the stomach or intestines by a chemical process, but is made in the very mouths of the lacteals; or in the mesenteric glands; in the same manner as other secreted fluids are made by an animal process in their adapted glands.

2. The chyle, which can be found among the materials expelled by severe vomiting or in diarrhea, can only arrive there by being transferred into the intestines through the reverse movements of the lacteals. Our food isn't turned into chyle in the stomach or intestines through a chemical process; instead, it's produced right at the openings of the lacteals or in the mesenteric glands, similar to how other secreted fluids are generated by biological processes in their specific glands.

Here a curious phænomenon in the exhibition of mercury is worth explaining:—If a moderate dose of calomel, as six or ten grains, be swallowed, and within one or two days a cathartic is given, a salivation is prevented: but after three or four days, a salivation having come on, repeated purges every day, for a week or two, are required to eliminate the mercury from the constitution. For this acrid metallic preparation, being absorbed by the mouth of the lacteals, continues, for a time arrested by the mesenteric glands, (as the variolous or venereal poisons swell the subaxillar or inguinal glands): which, during the operation of a cathartic, is returned into the intestines by the inverted action of the lacteals, and thus carried out of the system.

Here, a curious phenomenon related to mercury is worth explaining: If a moderate dose of calomel, around six or ten grains, is swallowed, and a laxative is given within one or two days, salivation can be prevented. However, after three or four days, if salivation occurs, repeated purges every day for a week or two are needed to eliminate the mercury from the body. This acrid metallic preparation, once absorbed through the lacteals, is temporarily held up by the mesenteric glands (just like variolous or venereal poisons cause swelling in the subaxillary or inguinal glands). During the action of a laxative, the mercury is returned to the intestines by the reversed movement of the lacteals, and is then expelled from the system.

Hence we understand the use of vomits or purges, to those who have swallowed either contagious or poisonous materials, even though exhibited a day or even two days after such accidents; namely, that by the retrograde motions of the lacteals and lymphatics, the material still arrested in the mesenteric, or other glands, may be eliminated from the body.

Therefore, we recognize the purpose of inducing vomiting or using purges for those who have ingested either contagious or toxic substances, even if it’s done a day or two after the incident. This is because, through the reverse movement of the lacteals and lymphatic system, any material still trapped in the mesenteric or other glands can be removed from the body.

3. Many instances of milk and chyle found in ulcers are given by Haller, El. Physiol. t. vii. p. 12, 23, which admit of no other explanation than by supposing, that the chyle, imbibed by one branch of the absorbent system, was carried to the ulcer, by the inverted motions of another branch of the same system.

3. Many cases of milk and chyle found in ulcers are reported by Haller, El. Physiol. t. vii. p. 12, 23, which can only be explained by assuming that the chyle, absorbed by one part of the absorbent system, was sent to the ulcer through the reverse actions of another part of the same system.

4. Mrs. P. on the second day after delivery, was seized with a violent purging, in which, though opiates, mucilages, the bark, and testacea were profusely used, continued many days, till at length she recovered. During the time of this purging, no milk could be drawn from her breasts; but the stools appeared like the curd of milk broken into small pieces. In this case, was not the milk taken up from the follicles of the pectoral glands, and thrown on the intestines, by a retrogression of the intestinal absorbents? for how can we for a moment suspect that the mucous glands of the intestines could separate pure milk from the blood? Doctor Smelly has observed, that loose stools, mixed with milk, which is curdled in the intestines, frequently relieves the turgescency of the breasts of those who studiously repel their milk. Cases in Midwifery, 43, No. 2. 1.

4. Mrs. P., on the second day after giving birth, experienced a severe bout of diarrhea. Despite the extensive use of opiates, mucilages, bark, and various shell substances, it persisted for several days until she finally recovered. During this time, she was unable to produce any milk from her breasts; however, her stools resembled curdled milk broken into small pieces. In this situation, could it be that the milk was taken from the glands in her breasts and transmitted to the intestines due to a reverse flow from the intestinal absorbents? How could we even consider that the intestinal mucous glands could separate pure milk from the blood? Doctor Smelly has noted that loose stools that are mixed with curdled milk often relieve the swelling in the breasts of women who actively try to suppress their milk. Cases in Midwifery, 43, No. 2. 1.

5. J.F. Meckel observed in a patient, whose urine was in small quantity and high coloured, that a copious sweat under the arm-pits, of a perfectly urinous smell, stained the linen; which ceased again when the usual quantity of urine was discharged by the urethra. Here we must believe from analogy, that the urine was first secreted in the kidneys, then re-absorbed by the increased action of the urinary lymphatics, and lastly carried to the axillae by the retrograde motions of the lymphatic branches of those parts. As in the jaundice it is necessary, that the bile should first be secreted by the liver, and re-absorbed into the circulation, to produce the yellowness of the skin; as was formerly demonstrated by the late Dr. Munro, (Edin. Medical Essays) and if in this patient the urine had been re-absorbed into the mass of blood, as the bile in the jaundice, why was it not detected in other parts of the body, as well as in the arm-pits?

5. J.F. Meckel noticed in a patient whose urine was scant and dark that there was a strong-smelling sweat under the armpits that stained the linen. This sweat stopped when the patient urinated the normal amount through the urethra. From this, we can infer that the urine was first produced in the kidneys, then reabsorbed by the increased activity of the urinary lymphatics, and finally transported to the armpits through the retrograde movement of the lymphatic branches in those areas. Similarly, in jaundice, bile must first be produced by the liver and then reabsorbed into the bloodstream to cause the skin to turn yellow, as was previously shown by the late Dr. Munro (Edin. Medical Essays). If in this patient the urine had been reabsorbed into the bloodstream like bile in jaundice, then why wasn’t it found in other parts of the body besides the armpits?

6. Cathartic and vermifuge medicines applied externally to the abdomen, seem to be taken up by the cutaneous branch of lymphatics, and poured on the intestines by the retrograde motions of the lacteals, without having passed the circulation.

6. Cathartic and worm-expelling medications applied externally to the abdomen appear to be absorbed by the skin's lymphatic system and delivered to the intestines through the backward flow of the lacteals, without going through the bloodstream.

For when the drastic purges are taken by the mouth, they excite the lacteals of the intestines into retrograde motions, as appears from the chyle, which is found coagulated among the fæces, as was shewn above, (sect. 2 and 4.) And as the cutaneous lymphatics are joined with the lacteals of the intestines, by frequent anastomoses; it would be more extraordinary, when a strong purging drug, absorbed by the skin, is carried to the anastomosing branches of the lacteals unchanged, if it should not excite them into retrograde action as efficaciously, as if it was taken by the mouth, and mixed with the food of the stomach.

When strong purgatives are taken orally, they stimulate the lymphatic vessels in the intestines to move backward, which is evident from the chyle found solidified among the feces, as shown earlier (sects. 2 and 4). Since the lymphatic vessels in the skin are connected to the lymphatics of the intestines through frequent interconnections, it would be surprising if a powerful purging medication absorbed through the skin traveled to these interconnected lymphatic branches unchanged, without causing them to react in the same backward motion as if it had been taken orally and mixed with the food in the stomach.

VIII. Circumstances by which the Fluids, that are effused by the retrograde Motions of the absorbent Vessels, are distinguished.

VIII. Conditions that differentiate the fluids released by the reverse movements of the absorbent vessels.

1. We frequently observe an unusual quantity of mucus or other fluids in some diseases, although the action of the glands, by which those fluids are separated from the blood, is not unusually increased; but when the power of absorption alone is diminished. Thus the catarrhal humour from the nostrils of some, who ride in frosty weather; and the tears, which run down the cheeks of those, who have an obstruction of the puncta lacrymalia; and the ichor of those phagedenic ulcers, which are not attended with inflammation, are all instances of this circumstance.

1. We often see an excessive amount of mucus or other fluids in certain illnesses, even though the glands that produce those fluids from the blood aren’t usually more active; it’s just that the ability to absorb is reduced. For example, the nasal discharge from some people who ride in cold weather; the tears that flow down the cheeks of those with blocked tear ducts; and the fluid from those non-inflamed ulcers—these are all cases that illustrate this situation.

These fluids however are easily distinguished from others by their abounding in ammoniacal or muriatic salts; whence they inflame the circumjacent skin: thus in the catarrh the upper lip becomes red and swelled from the acrimony of the mucus, and patients complain of the saltness of its taste. The eyes and cheeks are red with the corrosive tears, and the ichor of some herpetic eruptions erodes far and wide the contiguous parts, and is pungently salt to the taste, as some patients have informed me.

These fluids, however, are easily recognized from others by their high content of ammonium or hydrochloric salts, which irritate the surrounding skin. In cases of a cold, for instance, the upper lip turns red and swells due to the harshness of the mucus, and patients often describe its taste as salty. The eyes and cheeks become red from the irritating tears, and the discharge from some herpes sores severely damages the nearby areas and is notably salty, as some patients have told me.

Whilst, on the contrary, those fluids, which are effused by the retrograde action of the lymphatics, are for the most part mild and innocent; as water, chyle, and the natural mucus: or they take their properties from the materials previously absorbed, as in the coloured or vinous urine, or that scented with asparagus, described before.

While, on the other hand, the fluids released by the backward action of the lymphatic system are mostly mild and harmless; like water, chyle, and natural mucus: or they get their properties from the substances absorbed earlier, such as the colored or wine-like urine, or that smelling of asparagus, mentioned earlier.

2. Whenever the secretion of any fluid is increased, there is at the same time an increased heat in the part; for the secreted fluid, as the bile, did not previously exist in the mass of blood, but a new combination is produced in the gland. Now as solutions are attended with cold, so combinations are attended with heat; and it is probable the sum of the heat given out by all the secreted fluids of animal bodies may be the cause of their general heat above that of the atmosphere.

2. Whenever the amount of any fluid being produced increases, there is also an increase in heat in that area; because the fluid being produced, like bile, wasn't present in the bloodstream before, but rather a new combination is created in the gland. Just as dissolving substances tends to produce cold, forming new combinations leads to heat; and it’s likely that the total heat generated by all the fluids secreted by living organisms contributes to their overall temperature being higher than that of the surrounding air.

Hence the fluids derived from increased secretions are readily distinguished from those originating from the retrograde motions of the lymphatics: thus an increase of heat either in the diseased parts, or diffused over the whole body, is perceptible, when copious bilious stools are consequent to an inflamed liver; or a copious mucous salivation from the inflammatory angina.

Thus, the fluids resulting from increased secretions can be easily differentiated from those that come from the backward movement of the lymphatics: therefore, an increase in heat, either in the affected areas or throughout the body, can be noticed when there are large amounts of bile in the stools due to an inflamed liver, or when there is excessive mucus production from inflammatory sore throat.

3. When any secreted fluid is produced in an unusual quantity, and at the same time the power of absorption is increased in equal proportion, not only the heat of the gland becomes more intense, but the secreted fluid becomes thicker and milder, its thinner and saline parts being re-absorbed: and these are distinguishable both by their greater consistence, and by their heat, from the fluids, which are effused by the retrograde motions of the lymphatics; as is observable towards the termination of gonorrhœa, catarrh, chincough, and in those ulcers, which are said to abound with laudable pus.

3. When any fluid produced by the body is secreted in larger amounts than usual, and at the same time the absorption ability increases proportionally, the heat in the gland becomes more intense, and the secreted fluid becomes thicker and milder, as its thinner and saltier components are reabsorbed. These components can be identified by their greater thickness and warmth compared to the fluids that are released due to the backward motion of the lymphatic vessels; this is noticeable towards the end of conditions like gonorrhea, a cold, whooping cough, and in ulcers that are described as having acceptable pus.

4. When chyle is observed in stools, or among the materials ejected by vomit, we may be confident it must have been brought thither by the retrograde motions of the lacteals; for chyle does not previously exist amid the contents of the intestines, but is made in the very mouths of the lacteals, as was before explained.

4. When we see chyle in stools or in vomit, we can be sure it got there through the backward movement of the lacteals; chyle doesn't exist in the intestines beforehand but is actually produced right in the mouths of the lacteals, as explained earlier.

5. When chyle, milk, or other extraneous fluids are found in the urinary bladder, or in any other excretory receptacle of a gland; no one can for a moment believe, that these have been collected from the mass of blood by a morbid secretion, as it contradicts all analogy.

5. When chyle, milk, or other foreign fluids are found in the urinary bladder or in any other excretory organ of a gland, no one can reasonably believe that these have been gathered from the blood through some abnormal secretion, as it goes against all logic.

—— Aurea duræ

—— Golden hard

Mala ferant quercus? Narcisco floreat alnus?

Mala ferant quercus? Narciso floreat alnus?

Pinguia corticibus sudent electra myricæ?—VIRGIL.

Pinguia sounds like myrica wax?—VIRGIL.

IX. Retrograde Motions of Vegetable juices.

IX. Backward Movement of Plant Juices.

There are besides some motions of the sap in vegetables, which bear analogy to our present subject; and as the vegetable tribes are by many philosophers held to be inferior animals, it may be a matter of curiosity at least to observe, that their absorbent vessels seem evidently, at times, to be capable of a retrograde motion. Mr. Perault cut off a forked branch of a tree, with the leaves on; and inverting one of the forks into a vessel of water, observed, that the leaves on the other branch continued green much longer than those of a similar branch, cut off from the same tree; which shews, that the water from the vessel was carried up one part of the forked branch, by the retrograde motion of its vessels, and supplied nutriment some time to the other part of the branch, which was out of the water. And the celebrated Dr. Hales found, by numerous very accurate experiments, that the sap of trees rose upwards during the warmer hours of the day, and in part descended again during the cooler ones. Vegetable Statics.

There are also some movements of sap in plants that relate to our current topic; and since many philosophers consider plants to be lesser animals, it might be interesting to note that their absorbent vessels sometimes seem capable of moving backwards. Mr. Perault cut off a forked branch from a tree, leaves intact, and when he placed one fork into a container of water, he observed that the leaves on the other fork remained green much longer than those on a similar branch cut from the same tree. This shows that water from the container was drawn up one part of the forked branch through the reverse motion of its vessels, providing nutrients for a while to the other part of the branch, which was out of the water. The renowned Dr. Hales discovered through many precise experiments that the sap of trees rises during the warmer parts of the day and partially descends again during the cooler times. Vegetable Statics.

It is well known that the branches of willows, and of many other trees, will either take root in the earth or engraft on other trees, so as to have their natural direction inverted, and yet flourish with vigour.

It’s well known that the branches of willows and many other trees can either take root in the ground or graft onto other trees, allowing them to grow in an inverted manner and still thrive energetically.

Dr. Hope has also made this pleasing experiment, after the manner of Hales—he has placed a forked branch, cut from one tree, erect between two others; then cutting off a part of the bark from one fork applied it to a similar branch of one of the trees in its vicinity; and the same of the other fork; so that a tree is seen to grow suspended in the air, between two other trees; which supply their softer friend with due nourishment.

Dr. Hope has also conducted this interesting experiment, similar to Hales—he placed a forked branch, cut from one tree, standing upright between two others; then he trimmed some bark off one fork and attached it to a corresponding branch of one of the nearby trees; he did the same with the other fork, so that a tree can be seen growing suspended in the air, between two other trees, which provide their gentler companion with the necessary nutrients.

Miranturque novas frondes, et non sua poma.

Mirantur new leaves and not their own fruit.

All these experiments clearly evince, that the juices of vegetables can occasionally pass either upwards or downwards in their absorbent system of vessels.

All these experiments clearly demonstrate that plant juices can sometimes move either up or down in their system of absorbing vessels.

X. Objections answered.

X. Responses to objections.

The following experiment, at first view, would seem to invalidate this opinion of the retrograde motions of the lymphatic vessels, in some diseases.

The following experiment, at first glance, would appear to challenge this view of the backward motions of the lymphatic vessels in certain diseases.

About a gallon of milk having been giving to an hungry swine, he was suffered to live about an hour, and was then killed by a stroke or two on his head with an axe.—On opening his belly the lacteals were well seen filled with chyle; on irritating many of the branches of them with a knife, they did not appear to empty themselves hastily; but they did however carry forwards their contents in a little time.

About a gallon of milk was given to a hungry pig, and he was allowed to live for about an hour before being killed with a couple of blows to the head with an axe. When we opened his belly, we could see the lacteals filled with chyle. When we irritated many of their branches with a knife, they didn’t empty out quickly, but they did eventually move their contents forward after a little while.

I then passed a ligature round several branches of lacteals, and irritated them much with a knife beneath the ligature, but could not make them regurgitate their contained fluid into the bowels.

I then wrapped a band around several branches of lacteals and irritated them a lot with a knife beneath the band, but I couldn't make them release their contained fluid into the bowels.

I am not indeed certain, that the nerve was not at the same time included in the ligature, and thus the lymphatic rendered unirritable or lifeless; but this however is certain, that it is not any quantity of any stimulus, which induces the vessels of animal bodies to revert their motions; but a certain quantity of a certain stimulus, as appears from wounds in the stomach, which do not produce vomiting; and wounds of the intestines, which do not produce the cholera morbus.

I’m not really sure if the nerve was also caught in the ligature, which might have made the lymphatic system inactive or lifeless; however, what is clear is that it’s not just any amount of any stimulus that causes the vessels in animal bodies to change their movements, but a specific amount of a specific stimulus. This is evident from stomach wounds that don’t cause vomiting and intestinal wounds that don’t lead to cholera.

At Nottingham, a few years ago, two shoemakers quarrelled, and one of them with a knife, which they use in their occupation, stabbed his companion about the region of the stomach. On opening the abdomen of the wounded man after his death the food and medicines he had taken were in part found in the cavity of the belly, on the outside of the bowels; and there was a wound about half an inch long at the bottom of the stomach; which I suppose was distended with liquor and food at the time of the accident; and thence was more liable to be injured at its bottom: but during the whole time he lived, which was about ten days, he had no efforts to vomit, nor ever even complained of being sick at the stomach! Other cases similar to this are mentioned in the philosophical transactions.

At Nottingham, a few years ago, two shoemakers had a fight, and one of them stabbed his coworker in the stomach with a knife they use for their work. After the wounded man died, doctors opened his abdomen and found some of the food and medicine he had taken inside his belly, just outside the intestines. There was a wound about half an inch long at the bottom of his stomach, which I believe was full of food and drink at the time of the incident, making it more vulnerable to damage. However, during the entire ten days he was alive after the stabbing, he never threw up nor complained of feeling nauseous! Other similar cases have been noted in the philosophical transactions.

Thus, if you vellicate the throat with a feather, nausea is produced; if you wound it with a penknife, pain is induced, but not sickness. So if the soles of the feet of children or their armpits are tickled, convulsive laughter is excited, which ceases the moment the hand is applied, so as to rub them more forcibly.

Therefore, if you tickle the throat with a feather, it can cause nausea; if you scratch it with a penknife, it causes pain but not sickness. Similarly, when you tickle children's feet or armpits, it provokes uncontrollable laughter, which stops as soon as you start rubbing those areas more firmly.

The experiment therefore above related upon the lacteals of a dead pig, which were included in a strict ligature, proves nothing; as it is not the quantity, but the kind of stimulus, which excites the lymphatic vessels into retrograde motion.

The experiment mentioned above involving the lacteals of a dead pig, which were tightly bound, shows nothing; it's not the amount, but the type of stimulus that triggers the lymphatic vessels to move backward.

XI. The Causes which induce the retrograde Motions of animal Vessels; and the Medicines by which the natural Motions are restored.

XI. The Reasons That Cause the Backward Movements of Animal Vessels; and the Medicines That Restore Their Natural Movements.

1. Such is the construction of animal bodies, that all their parts, which are subjected to less stimuli than nature designed, perform their functions with less accuracy: thus, when too watery or too acescent food is taken into the stomach, indigestion, and flatulency, and heartburn succeed.

1. The way animal bodies are built means that all their parts, which are exposed to less stimulation than they were meant to handle, don’t work as accurately: therefore, when overly watery or acidic food is consumed, it leads to indigestion, gas, and heartburn.

2. Another law of irritation, connate with our existence, is, that all those parts of the body, which have previously been exposed to too great a quantity of such stimuli, as strongly affect them, become for some time afterwards disobedient to the natural quantity of their adapted stimuli.—Thus the eye is incapable of seeing objects in an obscure room, though the iris is quite dilated, after having been exposed to the meridian sun.

2. Another irritating rule that comes with our existence is that any part of the body that has been exposed to an excessive amount of stimuli that strongly affect it becomes temporarily unresponsive to the normal amount of stimuli it typically handles. For example, after being in bright sunlight, the eye struggles to see objects in a dimly lit room, even though the iris is fully dilated.

3. There is a third law of irritation, that all the parts of our bodies, which have been lately subjected to less stimulus, than they have been accustomed to, when they are exposed to their usual quantity of stimulus, are excited into more energetic motions: thus when we come from a dusky cavern into the glare of daylight, our eyes are dazzled; and after emerging from the cold bath, the skin becomes warm and red.

3. There's a third law of irritation that states when any part of our body has experienced less stimulation than it's used to, it'll react more intensely when it gets back to its normal level of stimulation. For example, when we step out of a dark cave into bright sunlight, our eyes are overwhelmed; and after getting out of a cold bath, our skin warms up and turns red.

4. There is a fourth law of irritation, that all the parts of our bodies, which are subjected to still stronger stimuli for a length of time, become torpid, and refuse to obey even these stronger stimuli; and thence do their offices very imperfectly.—Thus, if any one looks earnestly for some minutes on an area, an inch diameter, of red silk, placed on a sheet of white paper, the image of the silk will gradually become pale, and at length totally vanish.

4. There’s a fourth rule about irritation that says when parts of our bodies are exposed to stronger stimuli for a long time, they can become numb and stop responding to those stimuli; as a result, they don’t function properly. For example, if someone stares intently at a one-inch circle of red silk on a white sheet of paper for several minutes, the image of the silk will slowly fade and eventually disappear completely.

5. Nor is it the nerves of sense alone, as the optic and auditory nerves, that thus become torpid, when the stimulus is withdrawn or their irritability decreased; but the motive muscles, when they are deprived of their natural stimuli, or of their irritability, become torpid and paralytic; as is seen in the tremulous hand of the drunkard in a morning; and in the awkward step of age.

5. It's not just the sensory nerves, like those for sight and sound, that become inactive when the stimulus is removed or their sensitivity is lowered; the muscles responsible for movement also become inactive and weak when they lack their normal stimulation or responsiveness. This can be observed in the shaky hands of someone who has been drinking the night before and in the unsteady gait of the elderly.

The hollow muscles also, of which the various vessels of the body are constructed, when they are deprived of their natural stimuli, or of their due degree of irritability, not only become tremulous, as the arterial pulsations of dying people; but also frequently invert their motions, as in vomiting, in hysteric suffocations, and diabetes above described.

The hollow muscles that make up the various vessels in the body, when they lack their natural stimuli or proper level of sensitivity, not only become shaky, like the heartbeat of dying individuals, but often reverse their movements, as seen in vomiting, hysterical attacks, and the diabetes mentioned earlier.

I must beg your patient attention, for a few moments whilst I endeavour to explain, how the retrograde actions of our hollow muscles are the consequence of their debility; as the tremulous actions of the solid muscles are the consequence of their debility. When, through fatigue, a muscle can act no longer; the antagonist muscles, either by their inanimate elasticity, or by their animal action, draw the limb into a contrary direction: in the solid muscles, as those of locomotion, their actions are associated in tribes, which have been accustomed to synchronous action only; hence when they are fatigued, only a single contrary effort takes place; which is either tremulous, when the fatigued muscles are again immediately brought into action; or it is a pandiculation, or stretching, where they are not immediately again brought into action.

I ask for your patience for a few moments while I try to explain how the backward movements of our hollow muscles result from their weakness, just like the shaky movements of solid muscles are due to their weakness. When a muscle gets too tired to work anymore, the opposing muscles, either through their natural elasticity or through their biological function, pull the limb in the opposite direction. In solid muscles, like those used for movement, their actions are grouped together, having always worked in sync. So, when they get tired, only one opposing effort occurs, which can be shaky if the tired muscles are immediately called back into action, or it can be a stretch or pandiculation if they’re not immediately used again.

Now the motions of the hollow muscles, as they in general propel a fluid along their cavities, are associated in trains, which have been accustomed to successive actions: hence when one ring of such a muscle is fatigued from its too great debility, and is brought into retrograde action, the next ring from its association falls successively into retrograde action; and so on throughout the whole canal. See Sect. XXV. 6.

Now the motions of the hollow muscles, as they in general propel a fluid along their cavities, are associated in trains, which have been accustomed to successive actions: hence when one ring of such a muscle is fatigued from its too great debility, and is brought into retrograde action, the next ring from its association falls successively into retrograde action; and so on throughout the whole canal. See Sect. XXV. 6.

6. But as the retrograde motions of the stomach, œsophagus, and fauces in vomiting are, as it were, apparent to the eye; we shall consider this operation more minutely, that the similar operations in the more recondite parts of our system may be easier understood.

6. But since the backwards movements of the stomach, esophagus, and throat during vomiting are, so to speak, visible; we will take a closer look at this process, so that the similar functions in the deeper parts of our system may be better understood.

From certain nauseous ideas of the mind, from an ungrateful taste in the mouth, or from fœtid smells, vomiting is sometimes instantly excited; or even from a stroke on the head, or from the vibratory motions of a ship; all which originate from association, or sympathy. See Sect. XX. on Vertigo.

From certain nauseous ideas of the mind, from an ungrateful taste in the mouth, or from fœtid smells, vomiting is sometimes instantly excited; or even from a stroke on the head, or from the vibratory motions of a ship; all which originate from association, or sympathy. See Sect. XX. on Vertigo.

But when the stomach is subjected to a less stimulus than is natural, according to the first law of irritation mentioned above, its motions become disturbed, as in hunger; first pain is produced, then sickness, and at length vain efforts to vomit, as many authors inform us.

But when the stomach experiences less stimulation than usual, following the first law of irritation mentioned above, its movements become disrupted, like when you're hungry; first, it causes pain, then nausea, and eventually futile attempts to vomit, as many authors have noted.

But when a great quantity of wine, or of opium, is swallowed, the retrograde motions of the stomach do not occur till after several minutes, or even hours; for when the power of so strong a stimulus ceases, according to the second law of irritation, mentioned above, the peristaltic motions become tremulous, and at length retrograde; as is well known to the drunkard, who on the next morning has sickness and vomitings.

But when a large amount of wine or opium is consumed, the backward movements of the stomach don't happen for several minutes, or even hours. This is because, when the effect of such a strong stimulus wears off, as stated in the second law of irritation mentioned above, the peristaltic movements become shaky and eventually reverse. This is something a heavy drinker knows all too well, experiencing nausea and vomiting the next morning.

When a still greater quantity of wine, or of opium, or when nauseous vegetables, or strong bitters, or metallic salts, are taken into the stomach, they quickly induce vomiting; though all these in less doses excite the stomach into more energetic action, and strengthen the digestion; as the flowers of chamomile, and the vitriol of zinc: for, according to the fourth law of irritation, the stomach will not long be obedient to a stimulus so much greater than is natural; but its action becomes first tremulous and then retrograde.

When a larger amount of wine, opium, unpleasant vegetables, strong bitters, or metallic salts is consumed, they quickly cause vomiting. In smaller doses, however, these substances stimulate the stomach to work harder and improve digestion, like chamomile flowers and zinc sulfate. According to the fourth law of irritation, the stomach can't keep responding to a stimulus that’s much stronger than what it's used to; its response first becomes shaky and then reverses.

7. When the motions of any vessels become retrograde, less heat of the body is produced; for in paroxysms of vomiting, of hysteric affections, of diabetes, of asthma, the extremities of the body are cold: hence we may conclude, that these symptoms arise from the debility of the parts in action; for an increase of muscular action is always attended with increase of heat.

7. When the movements of any vessels slow down, less heat is generated in the body; during episodes of vomiting, hysteric conditions, diabetes, and asthma, the body's extremities feel cold: therefore, we can conclude that these symptoms come from the weakness of the active parts; because an increase in muscle activity always leads to an increase in heat.

8. But as animal debility is owing to defect of stimulus, or to defect of irritability, as shewn above, the method of cure is easily deduced: when the vascular muscles are not excited into their due action by the natural stimuli, we should exhibit those medicines, which possess a still greater degree of stimulus; amongst these are the fœtids, the volatiles, aromatics, bitters, metallic salts, opiates, wine, which indeed should be given in small doses, and frequently repeated. To these should be added constant, but moderate exercise, cheerfulness of mind, and change of country to a warmer climate; and perhaps occasionally the external stimulus of blisters.

8. But since animal weakness comes from a lack of stimulus or a lack of irritability, as shown above, the treatment method is clear: when the vascular muscles aren't triggered into proper action by natural stimuli, we should use medicines that provide an even stronger stimulus. These include things like strong-smelling substances, volatiles, aromatics, bitters, metallic salts, opiates, and wine, which should be given in small doses and repeated often. Additionally, we should include regular but moderate exercise, a positive mindset, and moving to a warmer climate; and possibly the occasional use of external stimuli like blisters.

It is also frequently useful to diminish the quantity of natural stimulus for a short time, by which afterwards the irritability of the system becomes increased; according to the third law of irritation above-mentioned, hence the use of baths somewhat colder than animal heat, and of equitation in the open air.

It’s often helpful to reduce the amount of natural stimulation for a short while, which can then increase the irritability of the system. This aligns with the third law of irritation mentioned earlier, explaining the benefits of baths that are slightly cooler than body temperature and riding outdoors.

The catalogue of diseases owing to the retrograde motions of lymphatics is here omitted, as it will appear in the second volume of this work. The following is the conclusion to this thesis of Mr. CHARLES DARWIN.

The list of diseases caused by the backward flow of lymphatics is not included here, as it will be presented in the second volume of this work. The following is the conclusion to this thesis by Mr. CHARLES DARWIN.

Thus have I endeavoured in a concise manner to explain the numerous diseases, which deduce their origin from the inverted motions of the hollow muscles of our bodies: and it is probable, that Saint Vitus's dance, and the stammering of speech, originate from a similar, inverted order of the associated motions of some of the solid muscles; which, as it is foreign to my present purpose, I shall not here discuss.

I’ve tried to briefly explain the various diseases that arise from the reversed movements of the hollow muscles in our bodies. It’s likely that Saint Vitus's dance and speech stammering come from a similar, reversed pattern in the movements of some solid muscles. However, since this isn't the focus of my current discussion, I won’t delve into it here.

I beg, illustrious professors, and ingenious fellow-students, that you will recollect how difficult a talk I have attempted, to evince the retrograde motions of the lymphatic vessels, when the vessels themselves for so many ages escaped the eyes and glasses of philosophers: and if you are not yet convinced of the truth of this theory, hold, I entreat you, your minds in suspense, till ANATOMY draws her sword with happier omens, cuts asunder the knots, which entangle PHYSIOLOGY; and, like an augur inspecting the immolated victim, announces to mankind the wisdom of HEAVEN.

I ask you, distinguished professors and clever classmates, to remember how challenging it has been for me to discuss the backward movements of the lymphatic vessels, especially since these vessels have eluded the observation of thinkers for so long. If you're not yet convinced of this theory, I urge you to keep an open mind until AANATOMY wields her sword with more favorable signs, cuts through the complexities that confuse PPHYSIOLOGY, and like a fortune-teller examining a sacrificial offering, reveals to humanity the wisdom of HEAVEN.



SECT. XXX.

PARALYSIS OF THE LIVER AND KIDNEYS.

PARALYSIS OF THE LIVER AND KIDNEYS.

I. 1.Bile-ducts less irritable after having been stimulated much. 2. Jaundice from paralysis of the bile-ducts cured by electric shocks. 3. From bile-stones. Experiments on bile-stones. Oil vomit. 4. Palsy of the liver, two cases. 5. Schirrosity of the liver. 6. Large livers of geese. II. Paralysis of the kidneys. III. Story of Prometheus.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.Bile ducts are less sensitive after being stimulated frequently. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Jaundice caused by bile duct paralysis is treated with electric shocks. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Due to gallstones. Research on gallstones. Oil induced vomiting. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Liver paralysis, two cases. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Liver cirrhosis. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Large livers from geese. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Kidney paralysis. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. The story of Prometheus.

I. 1. From the ingurgitation of spirituous liquors into the stomach and duodenum, the termination of the common bile-duct in that bowel becomes stimulated into unnatural action, and a greater quantity of bile is produced from all the secretory vessels of the liver, by the association of their motions with those of their excretory ducts; as has been explained in Section XXIV. and XXV. but as all parts of the body, that have been affected with stronger stimuli for any length of time, become less susceptible of motion, from their natural weaker stimuli, it follows, that the motions of the secretory vessels, and in consequence the secretion of bile, is less than is natural during the intervals of sobriety. 2. If this ingurgitation of spirituous liquors has been daily continued in considerable quantity, and is then suddenly intermitted, a languor or paralysis of the common bile-duct is induced; the bile is prevented from being poured into the intestines; and as the bilious absorbents are stimulated into stronger action by its accumulation, and by the acrimony or viscidity, which it acquires by delay, it is absorbed, and carried to the receptacle of the chyle; or otherwise the secretory vessels of the liver, by the above-mentioned stimulus, invert their motions, and regurgitate their contents into the blood, as sometimes happens to the tears in the lachrymal sack, see Sect. XXIV. 2. 7. and one kind of jaundice is brought on.

I. 1. From the ingurgitation of spirituous liquors into the stomach and duodenum, the termination of the common bile-duct in that bowel becomes stimulated into unnatural action, and a greater quantity of bile is produced from all the secretory vessels of the liver, by the association of their motions with those of their excretory ducts; as has been explained in Section XXIV. and XXV. but as all parts of the body, that have been affected with stronger stimuli for any length of time, become less susceptible of motion, from their natural weaker stimuli, it follows, that the motions of the secretory vessels, and in consequence the secretion of bile, is less than is natural during the intervals of sobriety. 2. If this ingurgitation of spirituous liquors has been daily continued in considerable quantity, and is then suddenly intermitted, a languor or paralysis of the common bile-duct is induced; the bile is prevented from being poured into the intestines; and as the bilious absorbents are stimulated into stronger action by its accumulation, and by the acrimony or viscidity, which it acquires by delay, it is absorbed, and carried to the receptacle of the chyle; or otherwise the secretory vessels of the liver, by the above-mentioned stimulus, invert their motions, and regurgitate their contents into the blood, as sometimes happens to the tears in the lachrymal sack, see Sect. XXIV. 2. 7. and one kind of jaundice is brought on.

There is reason to believe, that the bile is most frequently returned into the circulation by the inverted motions of these hepatic glands, for the bile does not seem liable to be absorbed by the lymphatics, for it soaks through the gall-ducts, and is frequently found in the cellular membrane. This kind of jaundice is not generally attended with pain, neither at the extremity of the bile-duct, where it enters the duodenum, nor on the region of the gall-bladder.

There’s good reason to think that bile is often reintroduced into the bloodstream due to the reverse movements of these liver glands, since bile doesn’t seem to be absorbed by the lymphatic system. Instead, it seeps through the bile ducts and is often found in the surrounding tissue. This type of jaundice usually doesn’t come with pain, either at the end of the bile duct where it enters the small intestine or in the area of the gallbladder.

Mr. S. a gentleman between 40 and 50 years of age, had had the jaundice about six weeks, without pain, sickness, or fever; and had taken emetics, cathartics, mercurials, bitters, chalybeates, essential oil, and ether, without apparent advantage. On a supposition that the obstruction of the bile might be owing to the paralysis, or torpid action of the common bile-duct, and the stimulants taken into the stomach seeming to have no effect, I directed half a score smart electric shocks from a coated bottle, which held about a quart, to be passed through the liver, and along the course of the common gall-duct, as near as could be guessed, and on that very day the stools became yellow; he continued the electric shocks a few days more, and his skin gradually became clear.

Mr. S., a man between 40 and 50 years old, had been experiencing jaundice for about six weeks, without any pain, nausea, or fever. He had tried various treatments including emetics, laxatives, mercury-based medications, bitters, iron supplements, essential oils, and ether, all without any noticeable benefit. Thinking that the bile obstruction might be due to the paralysis or sluggish action of the common bile duct, and since the stimulants he took had no effect, I recommended administering a series of smart electric shocks from a glass jar that held about a quart through his liver and along the path of the common gall duct, as accurately as possible. That same day, his stools turned yellow; he continued the electric shocks for a few more days, and gradually his skin cleared up.

3. The bilious vomiting and purging, that affects some people by intervals of a few weeks, is a less degree of this disease; the bile-duct is less irritable than natural, and hence the bile becomes accumulated in the gall-bladder, and hepatic ducts, till by its quantity, acrimony or viscidity, a greater degree of irritation is produced, and it is suddenly evacuated, or lastly from the absorption of the more liquid parts of the bile, the remainder becomes inspissated, and chrystallizes into masses too large to pass, and forms another kind of jaundice, where the bile-duct is not quite paralytic, or has regained its irritability.

3. The severe vomiting and purging that happens to some people every few weeks is a milder version of this disease. The bile duct is less sensitive than usual, causing bile to build up in the gallbladder and hepatic ducts. Eventually, due to its amount, sharpness, or thickness, this leads to increased irritation, resulting in a sudden release. Additionally, as the more liquid components of the bile get absorbed, the rest thickens and crystallizes into large masses that can't pass through, creating another type of jaundice where the bile duct isn’t completely paralyzed or has regained some sensitivity.

This disease is attended with much pain, which at first is felt at the pit of the stomach, exactly in the centre of the body, where the bile-duct enters the duodenum; afterwards, when the size of the bile-stones increase, it is also felt on the right side, where the gall-bladder is situated. The former pain at the pit of the stomach recurs by intervals, as the bile-stone is pushed against the neck of the duct; like the paroxysms of the stone in the urinary bladder, the other is a more dull and constant pain.

This disease comes with a lot of pain, which is initially felt in the pit of the stomach, right in the center of the body, where the bile duct connects to the duodenum. As the size of the bile stones increases, the pain is also felt on the right side, where the gallbladder is located. The initial pain in the pit of the stomach occurs intermittently as the bile stone presses against the duct's neck; similar to the attacks of a stone in the urinary bladder, the other pain is more dull and constant.

Where these bile-stones are too large to pass, and the bile-ducts possess their sensibility, this becomes a very painful and hopeless disease. I made the following experiments with a view to their chemical solution.

Where these gallstones are too large to pass, and the bile ducts are sensitive, this becomes a very painful and hopeless condition. I conducted the following experiments to find a chemical solution.

Some fragments of the same bile-stone were put into the weak spirit of marine salt, which is sold in the shops, and into solution of mild alcali; and into a solution of caustic alcali; and into oil of turpentine; without their being dissolved. All these mixtures were after some time put into a heat of boiling water, and then the oil of turpentine dissolved its fragments of bile-stone, but no alteration was produced upon those in the other liquids except some change of their colour.

Some pieces of the same bile stone were added to a weak solution of marine salt sold in stores, as well as to a mild alkali solution, a caustic alkali solution, and oil of turpentine, but none of them dissolved. After a while, all these mixes were heated in boiling water, and then the oil of turpentine dissolved its bile stone fragments, but the ones in the other liquids only changed color a bit.

Some fragments of the same bile-stone were put into vitriolic æther, and were quickly dissolved without additional heat. Might not æther mixed with yolk of egg or with honey be given advantageously in bilious concretions?

Some pieces of the same bile-stone were placed in vitriolic ether, and quickly dissolved without extra heat. Could ether mixed with egg yolk or honey be used beneficially for bile concretions?

I have in two instances seen from 30 to 50 bile-stones come away by stool, about the size of large peas, after having given six grains of calomel in the evening, and four ounces of oil of almonds or olives on the succeeding morning. I have also given half a pint of good olive or almond oil as an emetic during the painful fit, and repeated it in half an hour, if the first did not operate, with frequent good effect.

I have seen 30 to 50 gallstones pass through stool twice, about the size of large peas, after giving six grains of calomel in the evening and four ounces of almond or olive oil the next morning. I've also used half a pint of good olive or almond oil as an emetic during a painful episode, and repeated it in half an hour if the first dose didn't work, with positive results.

4. Another disease of the liver, which I have several times observed, consists in the inability or paralysis of the secretory vessels. This disease has generally the same cause as the preceding one, the too frequent potation of spirituous liquors, or the too sudden omission of them, after the habit is confined; and is greater or less in proportion, as the whole or a part of the liver is affected, and as the inability or paralysis is more or less complete.

4. Another liver disease I've seen several times is the inability or paralysis of the secretory vessels. This condition usually has the same cause as the previous one: either drinking alcoholic beverages too frequently or suddenly stopping after developing a habit. Its severity varies depending on whether the entire liver or just part of it is affected, and whether the inability or paralysis is more or less complete.

This palsy of the liver is known from these symptoms, the patients have generally passed the meridian of life, have drank fermented liquors daily, but perhaps not been opprobrious drunkards; they lose their appetite, then their flesh and strength diminish in consequence, there appears no bile in their stools, nor in their urine, nor is any hardness or swelling perceptible on the region of the liver. But what is peculiar to this disease, and distinguishes it from all others at the first glance of the eye, is the bombycinous colour of the skin, which, like that of full-grown silkworms, has a degree of transparency with a yellow tint not greater than is natural to the serum of the blood.

This liver condition is identified by these symptoms: the patients are usually over middle age and have consumed alcoholic beverages daily, but are not necessarily heavy drinkers. They lose their appetite, which leads to a decrease in their weight and strength. There is no bile present in their stools or urine, and there's no noticeable hardness or swelling in the liver area. What is unique to this illness, and sets it apart from others at first glance, is the silky color of the skin, which resembles that of fully grown silkworms; it has a certain transparency with a yellow tint that is no more intense than what is naturally found in blood serum.

Mr. C. and Mr. B. both very strong men, between 50 and 60 years of age, who had drank ale at their meals instead of small beer, but were not reputed hard-drinkers, suddenly became weak, lost their appetite, flesh, and strength, with all the symptoms above enumerated, and died in about two months from the beginning of their malady. Mr. C. became anasarcous a few days before his death, and Mr. B. had frequent and great hæmorrhages from an issue, and some parts of his mouth, a few days before his death. In both these cases calomel, bitters and chalybeates were repeatedly used without effect.

Mr. C. and Mr. B., both strong men in their 50s and 60s, who usually drank ale with their meals instead of light beer but weren’t considered heavy drinkers, suddenly became weak, lost their appetite, weight, and strength, showing all the symptoms mentioned earlier, and died about two months after their illness began. Mr. C. developed swelling in his body a few days before he died, and Mr. B. experienced frequent and severe bleeding from a sore and some areas in his mouth shortly before his death. In both cases, calomel, bitters, and iron supplements were used repeatedly with no results.

One of the patients described above, Mr. C, was by trade a plumber; both of them could digest no food, and died apparently for want of blood. Might not the transfusion of blood be used in these cases with advantage?

One of the patients mentioned earlier, Mr. C, worked as a plumber; both of them couldn't digest any food and died seemingly due to lack of blood. Could blood transfusion be beneficial in these situations?

5. When the paralysis of the hepatic glands is less complete, or less universal, a schirrosity of some part of the liver is induced; for the secretory vessels retaining some of their living power take up a fluid from the circulation, without being sufficiently irritable to carry it forwards to their excretory ducts; hence the body, or receptacle of each gland, becomes inflated, and this distension increases, till by its very great stimulus inflammation is produced, or till those parts of the viscus become totally paralytic. This disease is distinguishable from the foregoing by the palpable hardness or largeness of the liver; and as the hepatic glands are not totally paralytic, or the whole liver not affected, some bile continues to be made. The inflammations of this viscus, consequent to the schirrosity of it, belong to the diseases of the sensitive motions, and will be treated of hereafter.

5. When the paralysis of the liver glands is not completely or universally affected, it causes scarring in some parts of the liver. The secretory vessels retain some of their ability to function and absorb fluid from the bloodstream, but they lack the sensitivity to transport it to their excretory ducts. As a result, the body or receptacle of each gland becomes swollen, and this swelling continues until the intense pressure leads to inflammation or those parts of the organ become fully paralyzed. This condition can be identified by the noticeable hardness or enlargement of the liver, and since the liver glands are not completely paralyzed and the whole liver is not affected, some bile continues to be produced. The inflammations of the liver that follow from the scarring will be discussed later as part of the diseases related to sensitive motions.

6. The ancients are said to have possessed an art of increasing the livers of geese to a size greater than the remainder of the goose. Martial. l. 13. epig. 58.—This is said to have been done by fat and figs. Horace, l. 2. sat. 8.—Juvenal sets these large livers before an epicure as a great rarity. Sat. 5. l. 114; and Persius, sat. 6. l. 71. Pliny says these large goose-livers were soaked in mulled milk, that is, I suppose, milk mixed with honey and wine; and adds, "that it is uncertain whether Scipio Metellus, of consular dignity, or M. Sestius, a Roman knight, was the great discoverer of this excellent dish." A modern traveller, I believe Mr. Brydone, asserts that the art of enlarging the livers of geese still exists in Sicily; and it is to be lamented that he did not import it into his native country, as some method of affecting the human liver might perhaps have been collected from it; besides the honour he might have acquired in improving our giblet pies.

6. It's said that ancient people had a way of making goose livers much larger than the rest of the bird. Martial. l. 13. epig. 58.—This was supposedly achieved using fat and figs. Horace, l. 2. sat. 8.—Juvenal showcases these oversized livers as a rare delicacy for food lovers. Sat. 5. l. 114; and Persius, sat. 6. l. 71. Pliny mentions that these large goose livers were soaked in mulled milk, which I guess means milk mixed with honey and wine; he also notes, "it's unclear whether Scipio Metellus, a consul, or M. Sestius, a Roman knight, was the first to create this delicious dish." A modern traveler, I believe Mr. Brydone, claims that the technique for enlarging goose livers still exists in Sicily; it's unfortunate he didn't bring it back to his home country, as some way to influence the human liver might have been discovered from it, along with the recognition he could have gained by enhancing our giblet pies.

Our wiser caupones, I am told, know how to fatten their fowls, as well as their geese, for the London markets, by mixing gin instead of figs and fat with their food; by which they are said to become sleepy, and to fatten apace, and probably acquire enlarged livers; as the swine are asserted to do, which are fed on the sediments of barrels in the distilleries; and which so frequently obtains in those, who ingurgitate much ale, or wine, or drams.

Our smarter innkeepers, I’ve heard, know how to fatten their chickens, as well as their geese, for the London markets by mixing gin instead of figs and fat into their food; this supposedly makes them sleepy and helps them gain weight quickly, probably leading to enlarged livers, much like the pigs that are said to be fed on the leftovers from barrels in distilleries; this also often happens to those who consume a lot of ale, wine, or spirits.

II. The irritative diseases of the kidneys, pancreas, spleen, and other glands, are analogous to those of the liver above described, differing only in the consequences attending their inability to action. For instance, when the secretory vessels of the kidneys become disobedient to the stimulus of the passing current of blood, no urine is separated or produced by them; their excretory mouths become filled with concreted mucus, or calculus matter, and in eight or ten days stupor and death supervenes in consequence of the retention of the feculent part of the blood.

II. The irritating diseases of the kidneys, pancreas, spleen, and other glands are similar to those of the liver described above, differing only in the effects of their inability to function. For example, when the secretory vessels of the kidneys no longer respond to the flow of blood, no urine is produced. Their excretory openings become clogged with hardened mucus or stones, and within eight to ten days, confusion and death can occur due to the buildup of waste in the blood.

This disease in a slighter degree, or when only a part of the kidney is affected, is succeeded by partial inflammation of the kidney in consequence of previous torpor. In that case greater actions of the secretory vessels occur, and the nucleus of gravel is formed by the inflamed mucous membranes of the tubuli uriniferi, as farther explained in its place.

This disease, to a lesser extent, or when only part of the kidney is affected, is followed by partial inflammation of the kidney due to prior sluggishness. In that case, there is increased activity of the secretory vessels, and the core of gravel is created by the inflamed mucous membranes of the renal tubules, as explained further in its section.

This torpor, or paralysis of the secretory vessels of the kidneys, like that of the liver, owes its origin to their being previously habituated to too great stimulus; which in this country is generally owing to the alcohol contained in ale or wine; and hence must be registered amongst the diseases owing to inebriety; though it may be caused by whatever occasionally inflames the kidney; as too violent riding on horseback, or the cold from a damp bed, or by sleeping on the cold ground; or perhaps by drinking in general too little aqueous fluids.

This sluggishness, or paralysis of the kidney's secretory vessels, similar to that of the liver, comes from being used to excessive stimulation. In this country, this is usually due to the alcohol found in beer or wine, and therefore should be included among the diseases related to drunkenness. However, it can also be caused by anything that occasionally irritates the kidneys, such as intense horseback riding, cold from a damp bed, sleeping on cold ground, or possibly by not drinking enough fluids in general.

III. I shall conclude this section on the diseases of the liver induced by spirituous liquors, with the well known story of Prometheus, which seems indeed to have been invented by physicians in those ancient times, when all things were clothed in hieroglyphic, or in fable. Prometheus was painted as stealing fire from heaven, which might well represent the inflammable spirit produced by fermentation; which may be said to animate or enliven the man of clay: whence the conquests of Bacchus, as well as the temporary mirth and noise of his devotees. But the after punishment of those, who steal this accursed fire, is a vulture gnawing the liver; and well allegorises the poor inebriate lingering for years under painful hepatic diseases. When the expediency of laying a further tax on the distillation of spirituous liquors from grain was canvassed before the House of Commons some years ago, it was said of the distillers, with great truth, "They take the bread from the people, and convert it into poison!" Yet is this manufactory of disease permitted to continue, as appears by its paying into the treasury above 900,000l. near a million of money annually. And thus, under the names of rum, brandy, gin, whisky, usquebaugh, wine, cyder, beer, and porter, alcohol is become the bane of the Christian world, as opium of the Mahometan.

III. I'll wrap up this section on liver diseases caused by alcoholic drinks with the well-known story of Prometheus, which seems to have been created by doctors in ancient times when everything was framed in hieroglyphics or fables. Prometheus is depicted as stealing fire from the heavens, which could easily symbolize the flammable spirit produced by fermentation; this fire brings life to the man made of clay, leading to the victories of Bacchus, along with the temporary joy and noise of his followers. However, the punishment for those who steal this cursed fire is a vulture eating away at the liver, which symbolizes the unfortunate drunkard suffering for years with painful liver diseases. When the idea of imposing an additional tax on the distillation of alcoholic drinks from grain was discussed in the House of Commons some years ago, it was truthfully said of the distillers, "They take the bread from the people and turn it into poison!" Yet this factory of disease continues to operate, contributing over £900,000, nearly a million pounds, to the treasury annually. Thus, under the names of rum, brandy, gin, whisky, usquebaugh, wine, cider, beer, and porter, alcohol has become a curse for the Christian world, just as opium is for the Muslim world.

Evoe! parce, liber?

Evoe! party, free?

Parce, gravi metuende thirso!—Hor.

Parce, gravi metuende thirso!—Hor.



SECT. XXXI.

OF TEMPERAMENTS.

OF PERSONALITIES.

I. The temperament of decreased irritability known by weak pulse, large pupils of the eyes, cold extremities. Are generally supposed to be too irritable. Bear pain better than labour. Natives of North-America contrasted with those upon the coast of Africa. Narrow and broad shouldered people. Irritable constitutions bear labour better than pain. II. Temperament of increased sensibility. Liable to intoxication, to inflammation, hæmoptoe, gutta serena, enthusiasm, delirium, reverie. These constitutions are indolent to voluntary exertions, and dull to irritations. The natives of South-America, and brute animals of this temperament. III. Of increased voluntarity; these are subject to locked jaw, convulsions, epilepsy, mania. Are very active, bear cold, hunger, fatigue. Are suited to great exertions. This temperament distinguishes mankind from other animals. IV. Of increased association. These have great memories, are liable to quartan agues, and stronger sympathies of parts with each other. V. Change of temperaments into one another.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. The temperament marked by decreased irritability is identified by a weak pulse, large pupils, and cold extremities. It's generally believed that people with this temperament are overly irritable. They handle pain better than labor. This is seen when comparing those from North America with those from the African coast, as well as between narrow-shouldered and broad-shouldered individuals. Those with irritable constitutions handle labor better than they do pain. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. This temperament is characterized by increased sensitivity. Individuals may be prone to intoxication, inflammation, coughing up blood, vision issues, enthusiasm, delirium, and daydreaming. These individuals tend to be sluggish in their voluntary actions and unresponsive to irritations. Natives of South America and animals with this temperament fall into this category. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. This temperament shows increased willpower; individuals may experience locked jaw, convulsions, epilepsy, or mania. They are highly active, can withstand cold, hunger, and fatigue, and are suited for strenuous activities. This temperament sets humans apart from other animals. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. This is characterized by increased associations. Individuals with this temperament have excellent memories and are susceptible to recurring fevers and a stronger connection between their bodily parts. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Transformation of temperaments into one another.

Antient writers have spoken much of temperaments, but without sufficient precision. By temperament of the system should be meant a permanent predisposition to certain classes of diseases: without this definition a temporary predisposition to every distinct malady might be termed a temperament. There are four kinds of constitution, which permanently deviate from good health, and are perhaps sufficiently marked to be distinguished from each other, and constitute the temperaments or predispositions to the irritative, sensitive, voluntary, and associate classes of diseases.

Ancient writers have talked a lot about temperaments, but not with enough clarity. By "temperament of the system," we should mean a lasting tendency toward certain types of diseases; without this definition, a temporary tendency toward any specific illness could also be called a temperament. There are four types of constitution that consistently differ from good health, and are likely distinct enough to be identified separately. These represent the temperaments or predispositions to irritative, sensitive, voluntary, and associative classes of diseases.

I. The Temperament of decreased Irritability.

I. The Temperament of Lowered Irritability.

The diseases, which are caused by irritation, most frequently originate from the defect of it; for those, which are immediately owing to the excess of it, as the hot fits of fever, are generally occasioned by an accumulation of sensorial power in consequence of a previous defect of irritation, as in the preceding cold fits of fever. Whereas the diseases, which are caused by sensation and volition, most frequently originate from the excess of those sensorial powers, as will be explained below.

The diseases caused by irritation usually stem from a lack of it. Those that are directly due to having too much irritation, like high fevers, are typically a result of a buildup of sensory energy following a previous lack of irritation, as seen in earlier cold fevers. On the other hand, diseases caused by sensation and willpower often arise from an excess of those sensory powers, as will be explained later.

The temperament of decreased irritability appears from the following circumstances, which shew that the muscular fibres or organs of sense are liable to become torpid or quiescent from less defect of stimulation than is productive of torpor or quiescence in other constitutions.

The temperament of reduced irritability comes from the following circumstances, which show that the muscle fibers or sensory organs can become inactive or calm from less stimulation than what causes inactivity or calmness in other individuals.

1. The first is the weak pulse, which in some constitutions is at the same time quick. 2. The next most marked criterion of this temperament is the largeness of the aperture of the iris, or pupil of the eye, which has been reckoned by some a beautiful feature in the female countenance, as an indication of delicacy, but to an experienced observer it is an indication of debility, and is therefore a defect, not an excellence. The third most marked circumstance in this constitution is, that the extremities, as the hands and feet, or nose and ears, are liable to become cold and pale in situations in respect to warmth, where those of greater strength are not affected. Those of this temperament are subject to hysteric affections, nervous fevers, hydrocephalus, scrophula, and consumption, and to all other diseases of debility.

1. The first sign is a weak pulse, which can sometimes be fast in certain individuals. 2. The next noticeable characteristic of this temperament is the size of the iris or pupil of the eye, which some people consider an attractive trait in women, suggesting delicacy. However, to a trained observer, it indicates weakness and is therefore seen as a flaw, not a quality. The third notable aspect of this constitution is that the extremities, like the hands, feet, nose, and ears, tend to get cold and pale in warm situations, where stronger individuals are not affected. Those with this temperament often experience hysterical issues, nervous fevers, hydrocephalus, scrofula, and tuberculosis, along with other illnesses related to weakness.

Those, who possess this kind of constitution, are popularly supposed to be more irritable than is natural, but are in reality less so.

Those who have this kind of constitution are commonly thought to be more irritable than is normal, but they're actually less so.

This mistake has arisen from their generally having a greater quickness of pulse, as explained in Sect. XII. 1. 4. XII. 3. 3.; but this frequency of pulse is not necessary to the temperament, like the debility of it.

This mistake has arisen from their generally having a greater quickness of pulse, as explained in Sect. XII. 1. 4. XII. 3. 3.; but this frequency of pulse is not necessary to the temperament, like the debility of it.

Persons of this temperament are frequently found amongst the softer sex, and amongst narrow-shouldered men; who are said to bear labour worse, and pain better than others. This last circumstance is supposed to have prevented the natives of North America from having been made slaves by the Europeans. They are a narrow-shouldered race of people, and will rather expire under the lash, than be made to labour. Some nations of Asia have small hands, as may be seen by the handles of their scymetars; which with their narrow shoulders shew, that they have not been accustomed to so great labour with their hands and arms, as the European nations in agriculture, and those on the coasts of Africa in swimming and rowing. Dr. Maningham, a popular accoucheur in the beginning of this century, observes in his aphorisms, that broad-shouldered men procreate broad-shouldered children. Now as labour strengthens the muscles employed, and increases their bulk, it would seem that a few generations of labour or of indolence may in this respect change the form and temperament of the body.

People with this temperament are often found among women and among men with narrow shoulders, who are said to handle work poorly and endure pain better than others. This last point is believed to have stopped the natives of North America from being enslaved by Europeans. They are a narrow-shouldered group and would rather die than be forced to work. Some Asian nations have small hands, as seen in the handles of their scimitars; along with their narrow shoulders, this indicates they haven't been used to as much manual labor with their hands and arms as European nations in agriculture and those in Africa who swim and row. Dr. Maningham, a well-known childbirth expert at the beginning of this century, notes in his aphorisms that broad-shouldered men tend to have broad-shouldered children. Since labor strengthens the muscles involved and increases their size, it seems that a few generations of hard work or laziness could change the body’s shape and temperament in this way.

On the contrary, those who are happily possessed of a great degree of irritability, bear labour better than pain; and are strong, active, and ingenious. But there is not properly a temperament of increased irritability tending to disease, because an increased quantity of irritative motions generally induces an increase of pleasure or pain, as in intoxication, or inflammation; and then the new motions are the immediate consequences of increased sensation, not of increased irritation; which have hence been so perpetually confounded with each other.

On the other hand, those who are naturally more irritable handle work better than discomfort; they are strong, energetic, and creative. However, there isn’t actually a condition of heightened irritability that leads to illness, because a greater amount of irritative actions usually results in increased pleasure or pain, like with intoxication or inflammation. In these cases, the new actions are direct results of heightened sensations, not of increased irritability; which is why these two have often been confused with each other.

II. Temperament of Sensibility.

II. Sensitive Temperament.

There is not properly a temperament, or predisposition to disease, from decreased sensibility, since irritability and not sensibility is immediately necessary to bodily health. Hence it is the excess of sensation alone, as it is the defect of irritation, that most frequently produces disease. This temperament of increased sensibility is known from the increased activity of all those motions of the organs of sense and muscles, which are exerted in consequence of pleasure or pain, as in the beginning of drunkenness, and in inflammatory fever. Hence those of this constitution are liable to inflammatory diseases, as hepatitis; and to that kind of consumption which is hereditary, and commences with slight repeated hæmoptoe. They have high-coloured lips, frequently dark hair and dark eyes with large pupils, and are in that case subject to gutta serena. They are liable to enthusiasm, delirium, and reverie. In this last circumstance they are liable to start at the clapping of a door; because the more intent any one is on the passing current of his ideas, the greater surprise he experiences on their being dissevered by some external violence, as explained in Sect. XIX. on reverie.

There is not properly a temperament, or predisposition to disease, from decreased sensibility, since irritability and not sensibility is immediately necessary to bodily health. Hence it is the excess of sensation alone, as it is the defect of irritation, that most frequently produces disease. This temperament of increased sensibility is known from the increased activity of all those motions of the organs of sense and muscles, which are exerted in consequence of pleasure or pain, as in the beginning of drunkenness, and in inflammatory fever. Hence those of this constitution are liable to inflammatory diseases, as hepatitis; and to that kind of consumption which is hereditary, and commences with slight repeated hæmoptoe. They have high-coloured lips, frequently dark hair and dark eyes with large pupils, and are in that case subject to gutta serena. They are liable to enthusiasm, delirium, and reverie. In this last circumstance they are liable to start at the clapping of a door; because the more intent any one is on the passing current of his ideas, the greater surprise he experiences on their being dissevered by some external violence, as explained in Sect. XIX. on reverie.

As in these constitutions more than the natural quantities of sensitive motions are produced by the increased quantity of sensation existing in the habit, it follows, that the irritative motions will be performed in some degree with less energy, owing to the great expenditure of sensorial power on the sensitive ones. Hence those of this temperament do not attend to slight stimulations, as explained in Sect. XIX. But when a stimulus is so great as to excite sensation, it produces greater sensitive actions of the system than in others; such as delirium or inflammation. Hence they are liable to be absent in company; sit or lie long in one posture; and in winter have the skin of their legs burnt into various colours by the fire. Hence also they are fearful of pain; covet music and sleep; and delight in poetry and romance.

As in these constitutions more than the natural quantities of sensitive motions are produced by the increased quantity of sensation existing in the habit, it follows, that the irritative motions will be performed in some degree with less energy, owing to the great expenditure of sensorial power on the sensitive ones. Hence those of this temperament do not attend to slight stimulations, as explained in Sect. XIX. But when a stimulus is so great as to excite sensation, it produces greater sensitive actions of the system than in others; such as delirium or inflammation. Hence they are liable to be absent in company; sit or lie long in one posture; and in winter have the skin of their legs burnt into various colours by the fire. Hence also they are fearful of pain; covet music and sleep; and delight in poetry and romance.

As the motions in consequence of sensation are more than natural, it also happens from the greater expenditure of sensorial power on them, that the voluntary motions are less easily exerted. Hence the subjects of this temperament are indolent in respect to all voluntary exertions, whether of mind or body.

As the movements caused by sensation are beyond what is typical, it also occurs that because more sensorial energy is spent on them, voluntary movements become harder to perform. Therefore, people with this temperament are lazy when it comes to any voluntary efforts, whether mental or physical.

A race of people of this description seems to have been found by the Spaniards in the islands of America, where they first landed, ten of whom are said not to have consumed more food than one Spaniard, nor to have been capable of more than one tenth of the exertion of a Spaniard. Robertson's History.—In a state similar to this the greatest part of the animal world pass their lives, between sleep or inactive reverie, except when they are excited by the call of hunger.

A group of people with this description seems to have been discovered by the Spaniards in the islands of America, where they first landed. It's said that ten of them ate no more food than one Spaniard and were capable of only one-tenth of a Spaniard's effort. Robertson's History.—Most of the animal world lives in a similar state, mostly sleeping or in a passive daze, except when driven by hunger.

III. The Temperament of increased Voluntarity.

III. The Mindset of Greater Willingness.

Those of this constitution differ from both the last mentioned in this, that the pain, which gradually subsides in the first, and is productive of inflammation or delirium in the second, is in this succeeded by the exertion of the muscles or ideas, which are most frequently connected with volition; and they are thence subject to locked jaw, convulsions, epilepsy, and mania, as explained in Sect. XXXIV. Those of this temperament attend to the slightest irritations or sensations, and immediately exert themselves to obtain or avoid the objects of them; they can at the same time bear cold and hunger better than others, of which Charles the Twelfth of Sweden was an instance. They are suited and generally prompted to all great exertions of genius or labour, as their desires are more extensive and more vehement, and their powers of attention and of labour greater. It is this facility of voluntary exertion, which distinguishes men from brutes, and which has made them lords of the creation.

Those of this constitution differ from both the last mentioned in this, that the pain, which gradually subsides in the first, and is productive of inflammation or delirium in the second, is in this succeeded by the exertion of the muscles or ideas, which are most frequently connected with volition; and they are thence subject to locked jaw, convulsions, epilepsy, and mania, as explained in Sect. XXXIV. Those of this temperament attend to the slightest irritations or sensations, and immediately exert themselves to obtain or avoid the objects of them; they can at the same time bear cold and hunger better than others, of which Charles the Twelfth of Sweden was an instance. They are suited and generally prompted to all great exertions of genius or labour, as their desires are more extensive and more vehement, and their powers of attention and of labour greater. It is this facility of voluntary exertion, which distinguishes men from brutes, and which has made them lords of the creation.

IV. The Temperament of increased Association.

IV. *The Temperament of Increased Association.*

This constitution consists in the too great facility, with which the fibrous motions acquire habits of association, and by which these associations become proportionably stronger than in those of the other temperaments. Those of this temperament are slow in voluntary exertions, or in those dependent on sensation, or on irritation. Hence great memories have been said to be attended with less sense and less imagination from Aristotle down to the present time; for by the word memory these writers only understood the unmeaning repetition of words or numbers in the order they were received, without any voluntary efforts of the mind.

This constitution is characterized by the ease with which the fibrous motions form habits of association, and how these associations become stronger than those in other temperaments. People with this temperament are slow in voluntary actions or those based on sensation or irritation. Because of this, it has been said from Aristotle to today that great memories come with less understanding and less imagination; these writers have only regarded memory as the mindless repetition of words or numbers in the order they were received, without any conscious effort from the mind.

In this temperament those associations of motions, which are commonly termed sympathies, act with greater certainty and energy, as those between disturbed vision and the inversion of the motion of the stomach, as in sea-sickness; and the pains in the shoulder from hepatic inflammation. Add to this, that the catenated circles of actions are of greater extent than in the other constitutions. Thus if a strong vomit or cathartic be exhibited in this temperament, a smaller quantity will produce as great an effect, if it be given some weeks afterwards; whereas in other temperaments this is only to be expected, if it be exhibited in a few days after the first dose. Hence quartan agues are formed in those of this temperament, as explained in Section XXXII. on diseases from irritation, and other intermittents are liable to recur from slight causes many weeks after they have been cured by the bark.

In this temperament those associations of motions, which are commonly termed sympathies, act with greater certainty and energy, as those between disturbed vision and the inversion of the motion of the stomach, as in sea-sickness; and the pains in the shoulder from hepatic inflammation. Add to this, that the catenated circles of actions are of greater extent than in the other constitutions. Thus if a strong vomit or cathartic be exhibited in this temperament, a smaller quantity will produce as great an effect, if it be given some weeks afterwards; whereas in other temperaments this is only to be expected, if it be exhibited in a few days after the first dose. Hence quartan agues are formed in those of this temperament, as explained in Section XXXII. on diseases from irritation, and other intermittents are liable to recur from slight causes many weeks after they have been cured by the bark.

V. The first of these temperaments differs from the standard of health from defect, and the others from excess of sensorial power; but it sometimes happens that the same individual, from the changes introduced into his habit by the different seasons of the year, modes or periods of life, or by accidental diseases, passes from one of these temperaments to another. Thus a long use of too much fermented liquor produces the temperament of increased sensibility; great indolence and solitude that of decreased irritability; and want of the necessaries of life that of increased voluntarity.

V. The first of these temperaments differs from the standard of health due to a deficiency, while the others stem from an excess of sensory power; however, it's not uncommon for the same person to shift between these temperaments due to changes in their habits brought on by different seasons, lifestyle choices, or illnesses. For example, prolonged consumption of excessive alcohol leads to heightened sensitivity; extreme laziness and isolation contribute to reduced irritability; and scarcity of life's essentials results in heightened willfulness.



SECT. XXXII.

DISEASES OF IRRITATION.

Irritation-related diseases.

I. Irritative fevers with strong pulse. With weak pulse. Symptoms of fever, Their source. II. 1. Quick pulse is owing to decreased irritability. 2. Not in sleep or in apoplexy. 3. From inanition. Owing to deficiency of sensorial power. III. 1. Causes of fever. From defect of heat. Heat from secretions. Pain of cold in the loins and forehead. 2. Great expense of sensorial power in the vital motions. Immersion in cold water. Succeeding glow of heat. Difficult respiration in cold bathing explained. Why the cold bath invigorates. Bracing and relaxation are mechanical terms. 3. Uses of cold bathing. Uses of cold air in fevers. 4. Ague fits from cold air. Whence their periodical returns. IV. Defect of distention a cause of fever. Deficiency of blood. Transfusion of blood. V. 1. Defect of momentum of the blood from mechanic stimuli. 2. Air injected into the blood-vessels. 3. Exercise increases the momentum of the blood. 4. Sometimes bleeding increases the momentum of it. VI. Influence of the sun and moon on diseases. The chemical stimulus of the blood. Menstruation obeys the lunations. Queries. VII. Quiesence of large glands a cause of fever. Swelling of the præcordia. VIII. Other causes of quiescence, as hunger, bad air, fear, anxiety. IX. 1. Symptoms of the cold fit. 2. Of the hot fit. 3. Second cold fit why. 4. Inflammation introduced, or delirium, or stupor. X. Recapitulation. Fever not an effort of nature to relieve herself. Doctrine of spasm.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Irritative fevers with a strong pulse. With a weak pulse. Symptoms of fever and their source. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. A quick pulse is due to decreased irritability. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Not while asleep or in a state of apoplexy. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Caused by inanition, due to lack of sensorial power. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Causes of fever. Due to a lack of heat. Heat from secretions. Pain caused by cold in the lower back and forehead. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. High expenditure of sensorial power in vital functions. Immersion in cold water. The subsequent warming sensation explained. Difficulty in breathing during cold baths explained. Why cold baths are invigorating. Bracing and relaxation are mechanical terms. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Benefits of cold bathing. Uses of cold air in fevers. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Chills from cold air. The reason for their periodic returns. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. A lack of blood distention as a cause of fever. Blood deficiency. Blood transfusion. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. A lack of blood momentum from mechanical stimuli. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. Air injected into blood vessels. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. Exercise increases blood momentum. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. Sometimes bleeding can increase blood momentum. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. The influence of the sun and moon on diseases. The chemical stimulus of blood. Menstruation follows the lunar cycle. Questions. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. Inactivity of large glands as a cause of fever. Swelling of the chest area. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_18__. Other causes of inactivity, such as hunger, poor air, fear, anxiety. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_19__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_20__. Symptoms of the cold phase. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_21__. Of the hot phase. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_22__. Why a second cold phase occurs. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_23__. Introduction of inflammation, or delirium, or stupor. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_24__. Summary. Fever is not a natural effort to heal. The theory of spasm.

I. When the contractile sides of the heart and arteries perform a greater number of pulsations in a given time, and move through a greater area at each pulsation, whether these motions are occasioned by the stimulus of the acrimony or quantity of the blood, or by their association with other irritative motions, or by the increased irritability of the arterial system, that is, by an increased quantity of sensorial power, one kind of fever is produced; which may be called Synocha irritativa, or Febris irritativa pulsu forti, or irritative fever with strong pulse.

I. When the contracting sides of the heart and arteries beat more frequently within a certain time period and move through a larger area with each beat—whether these movements are caused by irritation from the blood’s acidity or volume, their connection with other stimulating motions, or by heightened sensitivity in the arterial system, meaning an increased level of sensory energy—a type of fever is created. This can be referred to as Synocha irritativa, or Febris irritativa pulsu forti, or irritative fever with a strong pulse.

When the contractile sides of the heart and arteries perform a greater number of pulsations in a given time, but move through a much less area at each pulsation, whether these motions are occasioned by defect of their natural stimuli, or by the defect of other irritative motions with which they are associated, or from the inirritability of the arterial system, that is, from a decreased quantity of sensorial power, another kind of fever arises; which may be termed, Typhus irritativus, or Febris irritativa pulsu debili, or irritative fever with weak pulse. The former of these fevers is the synocha of nosologists, and the latter the typhus mitior, or nervous fever. In the former there appears to be an increase of sensorial power, in the latter a deficiency of it; which is shewn to be the immediate cause of strength and weakness, as defined in Sect. XII. 1. 3.

When the contractile sides of the heart and arteries perform a greater number of pulsations in a given time, but move through a much less area at each pulsation, whether these motions are occasioned by defect of their natural stimuli, or by the defect of other irritative motions with which they are associated, or from the inirritability of the arterial system, that is, from a decreased quantity of sensorial power, another kind of fever arises; which may be termed, Typhus irritativus, or Febris irritativa pulsu debili, or irritative fever with weak pulse. The former of these fevers is the synocha of nosologists, and the latter the typhus mitior, or nervous fever. In the former there appears to be an increase of sensorial power, in the latter a deficiency of it; which is shewn to be the immediate cause of strength and weakness, as defined in Sect. XII. 1. 3.

It should be added, that a temporary quantity of strength or debility may be induced by the defect or excess of stimulus above what is natural; and that in the same fever debility always exists during the cold fit, though strength does not always exist during the hot fit.

It should be noted that a temporary amount of strength or weakness can be caused by too much or too little stimulation beyond what is normal; and that in the same fever weakness always occurs during the chill, even though strength doesn't always occur during the heat.

These fevers are always connected with, and generally induced by, the disordered irritative motions of the organs of sense, or of the intestinal canal, or of the glandular system, or of the absorbent system; and hence are always complicated with some or many of these disordered motions, which are termed the symptoms of the fever, and which compose the great variety in these diseases.

These fevers are always linked to, and usually triggered by, the chaotic irritative movements of the sensory organs, the digestive system, the glandular system, or the lymphatic system; therefore, they are always associated with some or many of these disordered movements, which are called the symptoms of the fever and account for the wide range of variations in these illnesses.

The irritative fevers both with strong and with weak pulse, as well as the sensitive fevers with strong and with weak pulse, which are to be described in the next section, are liable to periodical remissions, and then they take the name of intermittent fevers, and are distinguished by the periodical times of their access.

The irritating fevers, whether with a strong or weak pulse, as well as the sensitive fevers with a strong or weak pulse, which will be described in the next section, can have periodic remissions. When this happens, they are called intermittent fevers and are characterized by their periodic onset times.

II. For the better illustration of the phenomena of irritative fevers we must refer the reader to the circumstances of irritation explained in Sect. XII. and shall commence this intricate subject by speaking of the quick pulse, and proceed by considering many of the causes, which either separately or in combination most frequently produce the cold fits of fevers.

II. For the better illustration of the phenomena of irritative fevers we must refer the reader to the circumstances of irritation explained in Sect. XII. and shall commence this intricate subject by speaking of the quick pulse, and proceed by considering many of the causes, which either separately or in combination most frequently produce the cold fits of fevers.

1. If the arteries are dilated but to half their usual diameters, though they contract twice as frequently in a given time, they will circulate only half their usual quantity of blood: for as they are cylinders, the blood which they contain must be as the squares of their diameters. Hence when the pulse becomes quicker and smaller in the same proportion, the heart and arteries act with less energy than in their natural state. See Sect. XII. 1. 4.

1. If the arteries are dilated but to half their usual diameters, though they contract twice as frequently in a given time, they will circulate only half their usual quantity of blood: for as they are cylinders, the blood which they contain must be as the squares of their diameters. Hence when the pulse becomes quicker and smaller in the same proportion, the heart and arteries act with less energy than in their natural state. See Sect. XII. 1. 4.

That this quick small pulse is owing to want of irritability, appears, first, because it attends other symptoms of want of irritability; and, secondly, because on the application of a stimulus greater than usual, it becomes slower and larger. Thus in cold fits of agues, in hysteric palpitations of the heart, and when the body is much exhausted by hæmorrhages, or by fatigue, as well as in nervous fevers, the pulse becomes quick and small; and secondly, in all those cases if an increase of stimulus be added, by giving a little wine or opium; the quick small pulse becomes slower and larger, as any one may easily experience on himself, by counting his pulse after drinking one or two glasses of wine, when he is faint from hunger or fatigue.

That this quick, weak pulse is due to a lack of irritability is clear for two reasons: first, because it occurs alongside other signs of low irritability; and second, because when a stronger stimulus is applied, the pulse becomes slower and stronger. For example, during cold fits of fevers, in cases of hysterical heart palpitations, and when the body is significantly weakened from blood loss or fatigue, as well as in nervous fevers, the pulse tends to be quick and weak. Furthermore, in all these situations, if a little wine or opium is introduced as a stimulus, the quick, weak pulse slows down and strengthens. Anyone can easily notice this for themselves by checking their pulse after having one or two glasses of wine when feeling faint from hunger or fatigue.

Now nothing can so strongly evince that this quick small pulse is owing to defect of irritability, than that an additional stimulus, above what is natural, makes it become slower and larger immediately: for what is meant by a defect of irritability, but that the arteries and heart are not excited into their usual exertions by their usual quantity of stimulus? but if you increase the quantity of stimulus, and they immediately act with their usual energy, this proves their previous want of their natural degree of irritability. Thus the trembling hands of drunkards in a morning become steady, and acquire strength to perform their usual offices, by the accustomed stimulus of a glass or two of brandy.

Now, nothing demonstrates more clearly that this quick, weak pulse is due to a lack of irritability than the fact that adding an extra stimulus, beyond what is normal, causes it to become slower and stronger right away. A lack of irritability means that the arteries and heart aren’t responding to their usual levels of stimulus. However, if you increase the stimulus, and they respond with their usual energy immediately, it shows that they were lacking their normal level of irritability before. For example, the shaking hands of drunkards in the morning become steady and gain the strength to perform their regular tasks after they consume a glass or two of brandy.

2. In sleep and in apoplexy the pulse becomes slower, which is not owing to defect of irritability, for it is at the same time larger; and thence the quantity of the circulation is rather increased than diminished. In these cases the organs of sense are closed, and the voluntary power is suspended, while the motions dependent on internal irritations, as those of digestion and secretion, are carried on with more than their usual vigour; which has led superficial observers to confound these cases with those arising from want of irritability. Thus if you lift up the eyelid of an apoplectic patient, who is not actually dying, the iris will, as usual, contract itself, as this motion is associated with the stimulus of light; but it is not so in the last stages of nervous fevers, where the pupil of the eye continues expanded in the broad day-light: in the former case there is a want of voluntary power, in the latter a want of irritability.

2. During sleep and in a stroke, the pulse slows down, but this isn’t due to a lack of sensitivity; rather, it’s still strong. This actually increases blood flow rather than decreases it. In these situations, the sensory organs are shut off, and voluntary control is paused, while movements driven by internal signals, like digestion and secretion, occur with more intensity than usual. This has led some casual observers to mistake these cases for those caused by a lack of sensitivity. For example, if you lift the eyelid of a stroke patient who isn’t close to death, the iris will still contract in response to light, as it normally would. However, this isn't the case in the final stages of nervous fevers, where the pupil remains dilated even in bright daylight. In the first case, there’s a lack of voluntary control; in the second, there’s a lack of sensitivity.

Hence also those constitutions which are deficient in quantity of irritability, and which possess too great sensibility, as during the pain of hunger, of hysteric spasms, or nervous headachs, are generally supposed to have too much irritability; and opium, which in its due dose is a most powerful stimulant, is erroneously called a sedative; because by increasing the irritative motions it decreases the pains arising from defect of them.

Hence, those bodies that have a low level of irritability but a high level of sensitivity—like during hunger pains, hysterical spasms, or nerve headaches—are often thought to have too much irritability. Opium, which is a powerful stimulant when taken in the right amount, is mistakenly referred to as a sedative because it boosts irritative motions while reducing the pain caused by their absence.

Why the pulse should become quicker both from an increase of irritation, as in the synocha irritativa, or irritative fever with strong pulse; and from the decrease of it, as in the typhus irritativus, or irritative fever with weak pulse; seems paradoxical. The former circumstance needs no illustration; since if the stimulus of the blood, or the irritability of the sanguiferous system be increased, and the strength of the patient not diminished, it is plain that the motions must be performed quicker and stronger.

Why the pulse speeds up with increased irritation, like in irritative fever with a strong pulse, or slows down, as in typhus with a weak pulse, seems contradictory. The first situation is straightforward; when the stimulation of the blood or the reactivity of the circulatory system goes up, and the patient's strength is not reduced, it’s clear that the movements must happen faster and with more force.

In the latter circumstance the weakness of the muscular power of the heart is soon over-balanced by the elasticity of the coats of the arteries, which they possess besides a muscular power of contraction; and hence the arteries are distended to less than their usual diameters. The heart being thus stopped, when it is but half emptied, begins sooner to dilate again; and the arteries being dilated to less than their usual diameters, begin so much sooner to contract themselves; insomuch, that in the last stages of fevers with weakness the frequency of pulsation of the heart and arteries becomes doubled; which, however, is never the case in fevers with strength, in which they seldom exceed 118 or 120 pulsations in a minute. It must be added, that in these cases, while the pulse is very small and very quick, the heart often feels large, and labouring to one's hand; which coincides with the above explanation, shewing that it does not completely empty itself.

In this situation, the heart's muscle power is quickly countered by the elasticity of the artery walls, which also have their own ability to contract. As a result, the arteries are stretched to less than their normal widths. When the heart is temporarily halted and only partially emptied, it begins to expand again more quickly; the arteries, being stretched to less than their usual sizes, also start to contract sooner. This leads to a significant increase in the heart and artery pulse rate during the final stages of fevers with weakness, sometimes doubling in frequency. However, this doesn't happen in fevers with strength, where the pulse rarely exceeds 118 or 120 beats per minute. Additionally, in these cases, while the pulse is very weak and rapid, the heart often feels enlarged and strained to the touch, supporting the explanation that it doesn't fully empty itself.

3. In cases however of debility from paucity of blood, as in animals which are bleeding to death in the slaughter-house, the quick pulsations of the heart and arteries may be owing to their not being distended to more than half their usual diastole; and in consequence they must contract sooner, or more frequently, in a given time. As weak people are liable to a deficient quantity of blood, this cause may occasionally contribute to quicken the pulse in fevers with debility, which may be known by applying one's hand upon the heart as above; but the principal cause I suppose to consist in the diminution of sensorial power. When a muscle contains, or is supplied with but little sensorial power, its contraction soon ceases, and in consequence may soon recur, as is seen in the trembling hands of people weakened by age or by drunkenness. See Sect. XII. 1. 4. XII. 3. 4.

3. In cases however of debility from paucity of blood, as in animals which are bleeding to death in the slaughter-house, the quick pulsations of the heart and arteries may be owing to their not being distended to more than half their usual diastole; and in consequence they must contract sooner, or more frequently, in a given time. As weak people are liable to a deficient quantity of blood, this cause may occasionally contribute to quicken the pulse in fevers with debility, which may be known by applying one's hand upon the heart as above; but the principal cause I suppose to consist in the diminution of sensorial power. When a muscle contains, or is supplied with but little sensorial power, its contraction soon ceases, and in consequence may soon recur, as is seen in the trembling hands of people weakened by age or by drunkenness. See Sect. XII. 1. 4. XII. 3. 4.

It may nevertheless frequently happen, that both the deficiency of stimulus, as where the quantity of blood is lessened (as described in No. 4. of this section), and the deficiency of sensorial power, as in those of the temperament of irritability, described in Sect. XXXI. occur at the same time; which will thus add to the quickness of the pulse and to the danger of the disease.

It may nevertheless frequently happen, that both the deficiency of stimulus, as where the quantity of blood is lessened (as described in No. 4. of this section), and the deficiency of sensorial power, as in those of the temperament of irritability, described in Sect. XXXI. occur at the same time; which will thus add to the quickness of the pulse and to the danger of the disease.

III. 1. A certain degree of heat is necessary to muscular motion, and is, in consequence, essential to life. This is observed in those animals and insects which pass the cold season in a torpid state, and which revive on being warmed by the fire. This necessary stimulus of heat has two sources; one from the fluid atmosphere of heat, in which all things are immersed, and the other from the internal combinations of the particles, which form the various fluids, which are produced in the extensive systems of the glands. When either the external heat, which surrounds us, or the internal production of it, becomes lessened to a certain degree, the pain of cold is perceived.

III. 1. A certain amount of heat is needed for muscle movement and is, therefore, vital for life. This can be seen in animals and insects that go into a dormant state during cold seasons and come back to life when warmed by fire. This necessary heat comes from two sources: one from the warm atmosphere that surrounds us and the other from the internal reactions of particles that create various fluids produced in the body's glands. When either the external heat around us or the internal heat production decreases beyond a certain point, we feel the discomfort of cold.

This pain of cold is experienced most sensibly by our teeth, when ice is held in the mouth; or by our whole system after having been previously accustomed to much warmth. It is probable, that this pain does not arise from the mechanical or chemical effects of a deficiency of heat; but that, like the organs of sense by which we perceive hunger and thirst, this sense of heat suffers pain, when the stimulus of its object is wanting to excite the irritative motions of the organ; that is, when the sensorial power becomes too much accumulated in the quiescent fibres. See Sect. XII. 5. 3. For as the peristaltic motions of the stomach are lessened, when the pain of hunger is great, so the action of the cutaneous capillaries are lessened during the pain of cold; as appears by the paleness of the skin, as explained in Sect. XIV. 6. on the production of ideas.

This pain of cold is experienced most sensibly by our teeth, when ice is held in the mouth; or by our whole system after having been previously accustomed to much warmth. It is probable, that this pain does not arise from the mechanical or chemical effects of a deficiency of heat; but that, like the organs of sense by which we perceive hunger and thirst, this sense of heat suffers pain, when the stimulus of its object is wanting to excite the irritative motions of the organ; that is, when the sensorial power becomes too much accumulated in the quiescent fibres. See Sect. XII. 5. 3. For as the peristaltic motions of the stomach are lessened, when the pain of hunger is great, so the action of the cutaneous capillaries are lessened during the pain of cold; as appears by the paleness of the skin, as explained in Sect. XIV. 6. on the production of ideas.

The pain in the small of the back and forehead in the cold fits of the ague, in nervous hemicrania, and in hysteric paroxysms, when all the irritative motions are much impaired, seems to arise from this cause; the vessels of these membranes or muscles become torpid by their irritative associations with other parts of the body, and thence produce less of their accustomed secretions, and in consequence less heat is evolved, and they experience the pain of cold; which coldness may often be felt by the hand applied upon the affected part.

The pain in the lower back and forehead during the chills of fever, in nervous headaches, and in hysterical episodes, when all the irritating movements are significantly decreased, seems to come from this reason; the blood vessels of these membranes or muscles become sluggish due to their connection with other parts of the body, resulting in less of their usual secretions, and therefore, less heat is generated, causing them to feel the pain of cold; this coldness can often be felt by placing a hand on the affected area.

2. The importance of a greater or less deduction of heat from the system will be more easy to comprehend, if we first consider the great expense of sensorial power used in carrying on the vital motions; that is, which circulates, absorbs, secretes, aerates, and elaborates the whole mass of fluids with unceasing assiduity. The sensorial power, or spirit of animation, used in giving perpetual and strong motion to the heart, which overcomes the elasticity and vis inertiæ of the whole arterial system; next the expense of sensorial power in moving with great force and velocity the innumerable trunks and ramifications of the arterial system; the expense of sensorial power in circulating the whole mass of blood through the long and intricate intortions of the very fine vessels, which compose the glands and capillaries; then the expense of sensorial power in the exertions of the absorbent extremities of all the lacteals, and of all the lymphatics, which open their mouths on the external surface of the skin, and on the internal surfaces of every cell or interstice of the body; then the expense of sensorial power in the venous absorption, by which the blood is received from the capillary vessels, or glands, where the arterial power ceases, and is drank up, and returned to the heart; next the expense of sensorial power used by the muscles of respiration in their office of perpetually expanding the bronchia, or air-vessels, of the lungs; and lastly in the unceasing peristaltic motions of the stomach and whole system of intestines, and in all the secretions of bile, gastric juice, mucus, perspirable matter, and the various excretions from the system. If we consider the ceaseless expense of sensorial power thus perpetually employed, it will appear to be much greater in a day than all the voluntary exertions of our muscles and organs of sense consume in a week; and all this without any sensible fatigue! Now, if but a part of these vital motions are impeded, or totally stopped for but a short time, we gain an idea, that there must be a great accumulation of sensorial power; as its production in these organs, which are subject to perpetual activity, is continued during their quiescence, and is in consequence accumulated.

2. Understanding the significance of how much heat is lost from the system becomes easier when we first consider the huge amount of energy used to maintain vital functions. This includes the circulation, absorption, secretion, aeration, and constant management of all the fluids in the body. The energy, or life force, required to keep the heart beating strongly and consistently overcomes the elasticity and inertia of the entire arterial system. Then, there's the energy used to move the countless arteries with great force and speed, and the energy needed to circulate blood through the long and complex tiny vessels that make up the glands and capillaries. We also consider the energy spent by the absorbent ends of the lacteals and lymphatic vessels, which open at the surface of the skin and within every cell and space in the body. The energy is also required for venous absorption, where blood is drawn from the capillaries or glands, where arterial pressure ends, back to the heart. Additionally, there's the energy used by the respiratory muscles to constantly expand the airways of the lungs, and finally, the continuous peristaltic movements of the stomach and intestines, along with all the secretions like bile, gastric juice, mucus, sweat, and other waste products from the body. When we think about the relentless energy expenditure constantly at work, it becomes evident that this is far greater in a single day than all the voluntary actions of our muscles and senses use in a week, all without showing any noticeable fatigue! Now, if any part of these vital functions is hindered or entirely halted for even a brief period, it's clear that a significant amount of energy must accumulate since its production in those actively working organs continues even during their rest, leading to a buildup of energy.

While, on the contrary, where those vital organs act too forcibly by increase of stimulus without a proportionally-increased production of sensorial power in the brain, it is evident, that a great deficiency of action, that is torpor, must soon follow, as in fevers; whereas the locomotive muscles, which act only by intervals, are neither liable to so great accumulation of sensorial power during their times of inactivity, nor to so great an exhaustion of it during their times of action.

While, on the other hand, when those vital organs work too hard due to increased stimulation without a proportional increase in sensory power in the brain, it's clear that a significant lack of action, or torpor, will soon result, as seen in fevers. In contrast, the muscles used for movement, which only operate at intervals, are not subject to such a large buildup of sensory power during their inactive periods, nor do they experience such extreme depletion during their active periods.

Thus, on going into a very cold bath, suppose at 33 degrees of heat on Fahrenheit's scale, the action of the subcutaneous capillaries, or glands, and of the mouths of the cutaneous absorbents is diminished, or ceases for a time. Hence less or no blood passes these capillaries, and paleness succeeds. But soon after emerging from the bath, a more florid colour and a greater degree of heat is generated on the skin than was possessed before immersion; for the capillary glands, after this quiescent state, occasioned by the want of stimulus, become more irritable than usual to their natural stimuli, owing to the accumulation of sensorial power, and hence a greater quantity of blood is transmitted through them, and a greater secretion of perspirable matter; and, in consequence, a greater degree of heat succeeds. During the continuance in cold water the breath is cold, and the act of respiration quick and laborious; which have generally been ascribed to the obstruction of the circulating fluid by a spasm of the cutaneous vessels, and by a consequent accumulation of blood in the lungs, occasioned by the pressure as well as by the coldness of the water. This is not a satisfactory account of this curious phænomenon, since at this time the whole circulation is less, as appears from the smallness of the pulse and coldness of the breath; which shew that less blood passes through the lungs in a given time; the same laborious breathing immediately occurs when the paleness of the skin is produced by fear, where no external cold or pressure are applied.

So, when you jump into a really cold bath, say at 33 degrees on the Fahrenheit scale, the activity of the small blood vessels just under the skin and the skin's ability to absorb liquids decreases or stops for a bit. As a result, less blood flows through these vessels, leading to paleness. However, soon after getting out of the bath, your skin becomes redder and warmer than it was before you went in. This happens because, after being inactive from the lack of stimulation, these small glands become more sensitive to their usual triggers due to a buildup of energy, allowing more blood to flow through them and producing more sweat. Consequently, this leads to a higher body temperature. While you're in cold water, your breath feels cold, and breathing becomes fast and hard; this has usually been attributed to a spasm in the skin's blood vessels causing blood to pool in the lungs, due to both the pressure and the cold water. However, this explanation doesn’t fully capture the interesting phenomenon at play since the overall circulation also slows down, as indicated by a weaker pulse and cold breath, showing that less blood is moving through the lungs in a given time. The same kind of hard breathing happens when the skin turns pale from fear, even when there's no external cold or pressure involved.

The minute vessels of the bronchia, through which the blood passes from the arterial to the venal system, and which correspond with the cutaneous capillaries, have frequently been exposed to cold air, and become quiescent along with those of the skin; and hence their motions are so associated together, that when one is affected either with quiescence or exertion, the other sympathizes with it, according to the laws of irritative association. See Sect. XXVII. 1. on hæmorrhages.

The minute vessels of the bronchia, through which the blood passes from the arterial to the venal system, and which correspond with the cutaneous capillaries, have frequently been exposed to cold air, and become quiescent along with those of the skin; and hence their motions are so associated together, that when one is affected either with quiescence or exertion, the other sympathizes with it, according to the laws of irritative association. See Sect. XXVII. 1. on hæmorrhages.

Besides the quiescence of the minute vessels of the lungs, there are many other systems of vessels which become torpid from their irritative associations with those of the skin, as the absorbents of the bladder and intestines; whence an evacuation of pale urine occurs, when the naked skin is exposed only to the coldness of the atmosphere; and sprinkling the naked body with cold water is known to remove even pertinacious constipation of the bowels. From the quiescence of such extensive systems of vessels as the glands and capillaries of the skin, and the minute vessels of the lungs, with their various absorbent series of vessels, a great accumulation of sensorial powers is occasioned; part of which is again expended in the increased exertion of all these vessels, with an universal glow of heat in consequence of this exertion, and the remainder of it adds vigour to both the vital and voluntary exertions of the whole day.

Besides the inactivity of the tiny blood vessels in the lungs, there are many other systems of vessels that become sluggish due to their irritating connections with those in the skin, like the absorbent vessels of the bladder and intestines. This leads to the release of pale urine when bare skin is exposed only to the cold air. Sprinkling cold water on the bare body is known to relieve even stubborn constipation. Because of the inactivity of extensive systems of vessels, such as the glands and capillaries in the skin and the tiny vessels in the lungs, along with their various absorbent vessels, there’s a significant buildup of sensory energy. Some of this energy is used up in the increased activity of these vessels, causing a general warmth due to this activity, while the rest contributes to both vital and voluntary actions throughout the day.

If the activity of the subcutaneous vessels, and of those with which their actions are associated, was too great before cold immersion, as in the hot days of summer, and by that means the sensorial power was previously diminished, we see the cause why the cold bath gives such present strength; namely, by stopping the unnecessary activity of the subcutaneous vessels, and thus preventing the too great exhaustion of sensorial power; which, in metaphorical language, has been called bracing the system: which is, however, a mechanical term, only applicable to drums, or musical strings: as on the contrary the word relaxation, when applied to living animal bodies, can only mean too small a quantity of stimulus, or too small a quantity of sensorial power; as explained in Sect. XII. 1.

If the activity of the subcutaneous vessels, and of those with which their actions are associated, was too great before cold immersion, as in the hot days of summer, and by that means the sensorial power was previously diminished, we see the cause why the cold bath gives such present strength; namely, by stopping the unnecessary activity of the subcutaneous vessels, and thus preventing the too great exhaustion of sensorial power; which, in metaphorical language, has been called bracing the system: which is, however, a mechanical term, only applicable to drums, or musical strings: as on the contrary the word relaxation, when applied to living animal bodies, can only mean too small a quantity of stimulus, or too small a quantity of sensorial power; as explained in Sect. XII. 1.

3. This experiment of cold bathing presents us with a simple fever-fit; for the pulse is weak, small, and quick during the cold immersion; and becomes strong, full, and quick during the subsequent glow of heat; till in a few minutes these symptoms subside, and the temporary fever ceases.

3. This cold bathing experiment gives us a simple fever fit; the pulse is weak, small, and fast during the cold immersion, and then becomes strong, full, and fast during the warming period that follows. After a few minutes, these symptoms fade away, and the temporary fever stops.

In those constitutions where the degree of inirritability, or of debility, is greater than natural, the coldness and paleness of the skin with the quick and weak pulse continue a long time after the patient leaves the bath; and the subsequent heat approaches by unequal flushings, and he feels himself disordered for many hours. Hence the bathing in a cold spring of water, where the heat is but forty-eight degrees on Fahrenheit's thermometer, much disagrees with those of weak or inirritable habits of body; who possess so little sensorial power, that they cannot without injury bear to have it diminished even for a short time; but who can nevertheless bear the more temperate coldness of Buxton bath, which is about eighty degrees of heat, and which strengthens them, and makes them by habit less liable to great quiescence from small variations of cold, and thence less liable to be disordered by the unavoidable accidents of life. Hence it appears, why people of these inirritable constitutions, which is another expression for sensorial deficiency, are often much injured by bathing in a cold spring of water; and why they should continue but a very short time in baths, which are colder than their bodies; and should gradually increase both the degree of coldness of the water, and the time of their continuance in it, if they would obtain salutary effects from cold immersions. See Sect. XII. 2. 1.

In those constitutions where the degree of inirritability, or of debility, is greater than natural, the coldness and paleness of the skin with the quick and weak pulse continue a long time after the patient leaves the bath; and the subsequent heat approaches by unequal flushings, and he feels himself disordered for many hours. Hence the bathing in a cold spring of water, where the heat is but forty-eight degrees on Fahrenheit's thermometer, much disagrees with those of weak or inirritable habits of body; who possess so little sensorial power, that they cannot without injury bear to have it diminished even for a short time; but who can nevertheless bear the more temperate coldness of Buxton bath, which is about eighty degrees of heat, and which strengthens them, and makes them by habit less liable to great quiescence from small variations of cold, and thence less liable to be disordered by the unavoidable accidents of life. Hence it appears, why people of these inirritable constitutions, which is another expression for sensorial deficiency, are often much injured by bathing in a cold spring of water; and why they should continue but a very short time in baths, which are colder than their bodies; and should gradually increase both the degree of coldness of the water, and the time of their continuance in it, if they would obtain salutary effects from cold immersions. See Sect. XII. 2. 1.

On the other hand, in all cases where the heat of the external surface of the body, or of the internal surface of the lungs, is greater than natural, the use of exposure to cool air may be deduced. In fever-fits attended with strength, that is with great quantity of sensorial power, it removes the additional stimulus of heat from the surfaces above mentioned, and thus prevents their excess of useless motion; and in fever-fits attended with debility, that is with a deficiency of the quantity of sensorial power, it prevents the great and dangerous waste of sensorial power expended in the unnecessary increase of the actions of the glands and capillaries of the skin and lungs.

On the other hand, in all cases where the heat of the body's outer surface or the inner surface of the lungs is higher than normal, exposure to cool air may be beneficial. In fever conditions accompanied by strength, meaning a high level of sensory energy, it helps to remove the extra heat stimulus from the surfaces mentioned above, thereby preventing excessive and unproductive movement. In fever conditions with weakness, which involves a low level of sensory energy, it helps prevent the significant and dangerous loss of sensory energy that occurs due to the unnecessary increase in the activities of the glands and capillaries in the skin and lungs.

4. In the same manner, when any one is long exposed to very cold air, a quiescence is produced of the cutaneous and pulmonary capillaries and absorbents, owing to the deficiency of their usual stimulus of heat; and this quiescence of so great a quantity of vessels affects, by irritative association, the whole absorbent and glandular system, which becomes in a greater or less degree quiescent, and a cold fit of fever is produced.

4. Similarly, when someone is exposed to very cold air for a long time, the capillaries and absorbent vessels in the skin and lungs become inactive because they lack their usual heat stimulus. This inactivity of so many vessels impacts the entire absorbent and gland system through a chain reaction, leading to varying degrees of inactivity, and a cold fever episode occurs.

If the deficiency of the stimulus of heat is very great, the quiescence becomes so general as to extinguish life, as in those who are frozen to death.

If the lack of heat stimulus is extreme, the body becomes so inactive that it leads to death, like in cases of freezing to death.

If the deficiency of heat be in less degree, but yet so great as in some measure to disorder the system, and should occur the succeeding day, it will induce a greater degree of quiescence than before, from its acting in concurrence with the period of the diurnal circle of actions, explained in Sect. XXXVI. Hence from a small beginning a greater and greater degree of quiescence may be induced, till a complete fever-fit is formed; and which will continue to recur at the periods by which it was produced. See Sect. XVII. 3. 6.

If the deficiency of heat be in less degree, but yet so great as in some measure to disorder the system, and should occur the succeeding day, it will induce a greater degree of quiescence than before, from its acting in concurrence with the period of the diurnal circle of actions, explained in Sect. XXXVI. Hence from a small beginning a greater and greater degree of quiescence may be induced, till a complete fever-fit is formed; and which will continue to recur at the periods by which it was produced. See Sect. XVII. 3. 6.

If the degree of quiescence occasioned by defect of the stimulus of heat be very great, it will recur a second time by a slighter cause, than that which first induced it. If the cause, which induces the second fit of quiescence, recurs the succeeding day, the quotidian fever is produced; if not till the alternate day, the tertian fever; and if not till after seventy-two hours from the first fit of quiescence, the quartan fever is formed. This last kind of fever recurs less frequently than the other, as it is a disease only of those of the temperament of associability, as mentioned in Sect. XXXI.; for in other constitutions the capability of forming a habit ceases, before the new cause of quiescence is again applied, if that does not occur sooner than in seventy-two hours.

If the degree of quiescence occasioned by defect of the stimulus of heat be very great, it will recur a second time by a slighter cause, than that which first induced it. If the cause, which induces the second fit of quiescence, recurs the succeeding day, the quotidian fever is produced; if not till the alternate day, the tertian fever; and if not till after seventy-two hours from the first fit of quiescence, the quartan fever is formed. This last kind of fever recurs less frequently than the other, as it is a disease only of those of the temperament of associability, as mentioned in Sect. XXXI.; for in other constitutions the capability of forming a habit ceases, before the new cause of quiescence is again applied, if that does not occur sooner than in seventy-two hours.

And hence those fevers, whose cause is from cold air of the night or morning, are more liable to observe the solar day in their periods; while those from other causes frequently observe the lunar day in their periods, their paroxysms returning near an hour later every day, as explained in Sect. XXXVI.

And hence those fevers, whose cause is from cold air of the night or morning, are more liable to observe the solar day in their periods; while those from other causes frequently observe the lunar day in their periods, their paroxysms returning near an hour later every day, as explained in Sect. XXXVI.

IV. Another frequent cause of the cold fits of fever is the defect of the stimulus of distention. The whole arterial system would appear, by the experiments of Haller, to be irritable by no other stimulus, and the motions of the heart and alimentary canal are certainly in some measure dependant on the same cause. See Sect. XIV. 7. Hence there can be no wonder, that the diminution of distention should frequently induce the quiescence, which constitutes the beginning of fever-fits.

IV. Another frequent cause of the cold fits of fever is the defect of the stimulus of distention. The whole arterial system would appear, by the experiments of Haller, to be irritable by no other stimulus, and the motions of the heart and alimentary canal are certainly in some measure dependant on the same cause. See Sect. XIV. 7. Hence there can be no wonder, that the diminution of distention should frequently induce the quiescence, which constitutes the beginning of fever-fits.

Monsieur Leiutaud has judiciously mentioned the deficiency of the quantity of blood amongst the causes of diseases, which he says is frequently evident in dissections: fevers are hence brought on by great hæmorrhages, diarrhœas, or other evacuations; or from the continued use of diet, which contains but little nourishment; or from the exhaustion occasioned by violent fatigue, or by those chronic diseases in which the digestion is much impaired; as where the stomach has been long affected with the gout or schirrus; or in the paralysis of the liver, as described in Sect. XXX. Hence a paroxysm of gout is liable to recur on bleeding or purging; as the torpor of some viscus, which precedes the inflammation of the foot, is thus induced by the want of the stimulus of distention. And hence the extremities of the body, as the nose and fingers, are more liable to become cold, when we have long abstained from food; and hence the pulse is increased both in strength and velocity above the natural standard after a full meal by the stimulus of distention.

Monsieur Leiutaud has judiciously mentioned the deficiency of the quantity of blood amongst the causes of diseases, which he says is frequently evident in dissections: fevers are hence brought on by great hæmorrhages, diarrhœas, or other evacuations; or from the continued use of diet, which contains but little nourishment; or from the exhaustion occasioned by violent fatigue, or by those chronic diseases in which the digestion is much impaired; as where the stomach has been long affected with the gout or schirrus; or in the paralysis of the liver, as described in Sect. XXX. Hence a paroxysm of gout is liable to recur on bleeding or purging; as the torpor of some viscus, which precedes the inflammation of the foot, is thus induced by the want of the stimulus of distention. And hence the extremities of the body, as the nose and fingers, are more liable to become cold, when we have long abstained from food; and hence the pulse is increased both in strength and velocity above the natural standard after a full meal by the stimulus of distention.

However, this stimulus of distention, like the stimulus of heat above described, though it contributes much to the due action not only of the heart, arteries, and alimentary canal, but seems necessary to the proper secretion of all the various glands; yet perhaps it is not the sole cause of any of these numerous motions: for as the lacteals, cutaneous absorbents, and the various glands appear to be stimulated into action by the peculiar pungency of the fluids they absorb, so in the intestinal canal the pungency of the digesting aliment, or the acrimony of the fæces, seem to contribute, as well as their bulk, to promote the peristaltic motions; and in the arterial system, the momentum of the particles of the circulating blood, and their acrimony, stimulate the arteries, as well as the distention occasioned by it. Where the pulse is small this defect of distention is present, and contributes much to produce the febris irritativa pulsu debili, or irritative fever with weak pulse, called by modern writers nervous fever, as a predisponent cause. See Sect. XII. 1. 4. Might not the transfusion of blood, suppose of four ounces daily from a strong man, or other healthful animal, as a sheep or an ass, be used in the early state of nervous or putrid fevers with great prospect of success?

However, this stimulus of distention, like the stimulus of heat above described, though it contributes much to the due action not only of the heart, arteries, and alimentary canal, but seems necessary to the proper secretion of all the various glands; yet perhaps it is not the sole cause of any of these numerous motions: for as the lacteals, cutaneous absorbents, and the various glands appear to be stimulated into action by the peculiar pungency of the fluids they absorb, so in the intestinal canal the pungency of the digesting aliment, or the acrimony of the fæces, seem to contribute, as well as their bulk, to promote the peristaltic motions; and in the arterial system, the momentum of the particles of the circulating blood, and their acrimony, stimulate the arteries, as well as the distention occasioned by it. Where the pulse is small this defect of distention is present, and contributes much to produce the febris irritativa pulsu debili, or irritative fever with weak pulse, called by modern writers nervous fever, as a predisponent cause. See Sect. XII. 1. 4. Might not the transfusion of blood, suppose of four ounces daily from a strong man, or other healthful animal, as a sheep or an ass, be used in the early state of nervous or putrid fevers with great prospect of success?

V. 1. The defect of the momentum of the particles of the circulating blood is another cause of the quiescence, with which the cold fits of fever commence. This stimulus of the momentum of the progressive particles of the blood does not act over the whole body like those of heat and distention above described, but is confined to the arterial system; and differs from the stimulus of the distention of the blood, as much as the vibration of the air does from the currents of it. Thus are the different organs of our bodies stimulated by four different mechanic properties of the external world: the sense of touch by the pressure of solid bodies so as to distinguish their figure; the muscular system by the distention, which they occasion; the internal surface of the arteries, by the momentum of their moving particles; and the auditory nerves, by the vibration of them: and these four mechanic properties are as different from each other as the various chemical ones, which are adapted to the numerous glands, and to the other organs of sense.

V. 1. The lack of momentum in the particles of circulating blood is another reason for the stillness that occurs when cold fits of fever begin. This momentum of the moving blood particles doesn’t affect the entire body like the heat and expansion mentioned earlier, but rather is limited to the arterial system; it differs from the stimulus of blood expansion in the same way that air vibrations differ from air currents. Thus, the various organs in our bodies are stimulated by four different mechanical properties of the outside world: the sense of touch by the pressure of solid objects to distinguish their shape; the muscular system by the expansion they cause; the inner surface of the arteries by the momentum of the moving particles; and the auditory nerves by their vibrations. These four mechanical properties are as distinct from one another as the various chemical properties that are suited to the many glands and other sensory organs.

2. The momentum of the progressive particles of blood is compounded of their velocity and their quantity of matter: hence whatever circumstances diminish either of these without proportionally increasing the other, and without superadding either of the general stimuli of heat or distention, will tend to produce a quiescence of the arterial system, and from thence of all the other irritative motions, which are connected with it.

2. The movement of progressive blood particles is a combination of their speed and mass: therefore, any situation that reduces either of these factors without proportionally increasing the other, and without adding any general stimuli like heat or expansion, will likely lead to a lull in the arterial system, and consequently in all other related involuntary movements.

Hence in all those constitutions or diseases where the blood contains a greater proportion of serum, which is the lightest part of its composition, the pulsations of the arteries are weaker, as in nervous fevers, chlorosis, and hysteric complaints; for in these cases the momentum of the progressive particles of blood is less: and hence, where the denser parts of its composition abound, as the red part of it, or the coagulable lymph, the arterial pulsations are stronger; as in those of robust health, and in inflammatory diseases.

Thus, in all those conditions or diseases where the blood has a higher amount of serum, which is the lightest part of its makeup, the pulses of the arteries are weaker, such as in nervous fevers, chlorosis, and hysterical issues; this is because the movement of the flowing blood particles is reduced. Conversely, when the denser components, like the red blood cells or the coagulable lymph, are more prevalent, the arterial pulses are stronger, as seen in cases of good health and inflammatory diseases.

That this stimulus of the momentum of the particles of the circulating fluid is of the greatest consequence to the arterial action, appears from the experiment of injecting air into the blood vessels, which seems to destroy animal life from the want of this stimulus of momentum; for the distention of the arteries is not diminished by it, it possesses no corrosive acrimony, and is less liable to repass the valves than the blood itself; since air-valves in all machinery require much less accuracy of construction than those which are opposed to water.

The impact of the momentum of the particles in the circulating fluid is crucial for arterial function, as shown by experiments where air is injected into blood vessels. This injection appears to harm or destroy life due to the lack of this momentum stimulus; the arteries remain distended, the air has no corrosive properties, and it is less likely to pass back through the valves compared to blood. In fact, air valves in machinery need much less precision in their construction than valves that deal with water.

3. One method of increasing the velocity of the blood, and in consequence the momentum of its particles, is by the exercise of the body, or by the friction of its surface: so, on the contrary, too great indolence contributes to decrease this stimulus of the momentum of the particles of the circulating blood, and thus tends to induce quiescence; as is seen in hysteric cases, and chlorosis, and the other diseases of sedentary people.

3. One way to increase the speed of the blood, and therefore the momentum of its particles, is through physical exercise or by the friction of its surface. Conversely, excessive laziness reduces this stimulation of the momentum of the circulating blood particles, which can lead to inactivity, as seen in cases of hysteria, chlorosis, and other diseases common in sedentary individuals.

4. The velocity of the particles of the blood in certain circumstances is increased by venesection, which, by removing a part of it, diminishes the resistance to the motion of the other part, and hence the momentum of the particles of it is increased. This may be easily understood by considering it in the extreme, since, if the resistance was greatly increased, so as to overcome the propelling power, there could be no velocity, and in consequence no momentum at all. From this circumstance arises that curious phænomenon, the truth of which I have been more than once witness to, that venesection will often instantaneously relieve those nervous pains, which attend the cold periods of hysteric, asthmatic, or epileptic diseases; and that even where large doses of opium have been in vain exhibited. In these cases the pulse becomes stronger after the bleeding, and the extremities regain their natural warmth; and an opiate then given acts with much more certain effect.

4. In certain situations, the speed of blood particles increases due to venesection, which removes some blood, reducing the resistance to the flow of the remaining blood and thus increasing the momentum of those particles. This is easier to grasp in extreme cases because if the resistance were greatly heightened, overpowering the driving force, there would be no speed and consequently no momentum at all. This leads to the interesting phenomenon, which I have witnessed more than once, that venesection can often provide immediate relief from the nerve pain associated with the cold phases of hysterical, asthmatic, or epileptic conditions, even when large doses of opium have failed. In these instances, the pulse becomes stronger after bleeding, and the limbs regain their natural warmth; an opiate administered afterward is then much more effective.

VI. There is another cause, which seems occasionally to induce quiescence into some part of our system, I mean the influence of the sun and moon; the attraction of these luminaries, by decreasing the gravity of the particles of the blood, cannot affect their momentum, as their vis inertiæ remains the same; but it may nevertheless produce some chemical change in them, because whatever affects the general attractions of the particles of matter may be supposed from analogy to affect their specific attractions or affinities: and thus the stimulus of the particles of blood may be diminished, though not their momentum. As the tides of the sea obey the southing and northing of the moon (allowing for the time necessary for their motion, and the obstructions of the shores), it is probable, that there are also atmospheric tides on both sides of the earth, which to the inhabitants of another planet might so deflect the light as to resemble the ring of Saturn. Now as these tides of water, or of air, are raised by the diminution of their gravity, it follows, that their pressure on the surface of the earth is no greater than the pressure of the other parts of the ocean, or of the atmosphere, where no such tides exist; and therefore that they cannot affect the mercury in the barometer. In the same manner, the gravity of all other terrestrial bodies is diminished at the times of the southing and northing of the moon, and that in a greater degree when this coincides with the southing and northing of the sun, and this in a still greater degree about the times of the equinoxes. This decrease of the gravity of all bodies during the time the moon passes our zenith or nadir might possibly be shewn by the slower vibrations of a pendulum, compared with a spring clock, or with astronomical observation. Since a pendulum of a certain length moves slower at the line than near the poles, because the gravity being diminished and the vis inertiæ continuing the same, the motive power is less, but the resistance to be overcome continues the same. The combined powers of the lunar and solar attraction is estimated by Sir Isaac Newton not to exceed one 7,868,850th part of the power of gravitation, which seems indeed but a small circumstance to produce any considerable effect on the weight of sublunary bodies, and yet this is sufficient to raise the tides at the equator above ten feet high; and if it be considered, what small impulses of other bodies produce their effects on the organs of sense adapted to the perception of them, as of vibration on the auditory nerves, we shall cease to to be surprised, that so minute a diminution in the gravity of the particles of blood should so far affect their chemical changes, or their stimulating quality, as, joined with other causes, sometimes to produce the beginnings of diseases.

VI. There's another factor that seems to occasionally calm some part of our system, which is the influence of the sun and moon. The attraction of these celestial bodies decreases the gravity on the blood particles, but it can't change their momentum since their inertia stays the same. However, it could lead to some chemical changes in them because anything that affects the overall attraction of matter particles might also affect their specific attractions or affinities. Thus, the stimulation of blood particles might be reduced, even if their momentum remains unchanged. Just like how the tides of the sea follow the movements of the moon (considering the time it takes for their motion and the barriers created by shores), it's likely that there are also atmospheric tides on both sides of the Earth, which could bend the light enough to mimic the rings of Saturn to the inhabitants of another planet. Since these tides in water or air are caused by decreased gravity, their pressure on the Earth's surface is no greater than the pressure from other parts of the ocean or atmosphere where such tides don't occur. Therefore, they can't affect the mercury in the barometer. Similarly, the gravity of all other earthly bodies is reduced when the moon is at its highest or lowest point, and this reduction is more significant when it coincides with the sun's position, particularly around the equinoxes. This decrease in gravity when the moon is directly overhead or below could potentially be demonstrated by the slower vibrations of a pendulum compared to a spring clock or through astronomical observations. A pendulum of a specific length moves slower at the equator than near the poles because, while gravity is reduced, inertia remains constant; thus, the force acting to move it is less, but the resistance remains the same. Sir Isaac Newton estimated that the combined effects of lunar and solar attraction do not exceed one 7,868,850th of the force of gravitation, which seems like a minor factor for significantly affecting the weight of earthly bodies. Yet, this is enough to raise the tides at the equator by over ten feet. When we consider how small impulses from other bodies affect our sensory organs, like vibrations on auditory nerves, we should not be surprised that such a tiny decrease in the gravity of blood particles can significantly impact their chemical changes or stimulating qualities, sometimes contributing to the onset of diseases when combined with other factors.

Add to this, that if the lunar influence produces a very small degree of quiescence at first, and if that recurs at certain periods even with less power to produce quiescence than at first, yet the quiescence will daily increase by the acquired habit acting at the same time, till at length so great a degree of quiescence is induced as to produce phrensy, canine madness, epilepsy, hysteric pains or cold fits of fever, instances of many of which are to be found in Dr. Mead's work on this subject. The solar influence also appears daily in several diseases; but as darkness, silence, sleep, and our periodical meals mark the parts of the solar circle of actions, it is sometimes dubious to which of these the periodical returns of these diseases are to be ascribed.

Additionally, if the influence of the moon initially causes a very slight state of calm, and if that calm returns at certain times even with less strength than before, it will gradually increase due to the habits formed over time until it eventually leads to such a high level of calm that it can cause madness, rabies, epilepsy, hysterical pains, or chills from fever. There are many examples of this in Dr. Mead's work on the subject. The influence of the sun also becomes evident in various illnesses; however, since darkness, silence, sleep, and our regular meals mark the solar cycle of activities, it can sometimes be unclear to which of these factors the recurring episodes of these diseases should be attributed.

As far as I have been able to observe, the periods of inflammatory diseases observe the solar day; as the gout and rheumatism have their greatest quiescence about noon and midnight, and their exacerbations some hours after; as they have more frequently their immediate cause from cold air, inanition, or fatigue, than from the effects of lunations: whilst the cold fits of hysteric patients, and those in nervous fevers, more frequently occur twice a day, later by near half an hour each time, according to the lunar day; whilst some fits of intermittents, which are undisturbed by medicines, return at regular solar periods, and others at lunar ones; which may, probably, be owing to the difference of the periods of those external circumstances of cold, inanition, or lunation, which immediately caused them.

From what I've been able to see, inflammatory diseases follow the solar day. For example, gout and rheumatism are usually less active around noon and midnight, with flare-ups happening a few hours later. Their triggers often come from cold air, lack of food, or fatigue rather than the effects of the moon. In contrast, cold spells in people with hysteria and those suffering from nervous fevers tend to happen twice a day, occurring about half an hour later each time based on the lunar cycle. Some intermittent fevers, which don’t respond to treatment, return at regular solar intervals, while others align with lunar cycles. This might be due to the different timings of external factors like cold, lack of food, or moon phases that directly cause these issues.

We must, however, observe, that the periods of quiescence and exacerbation in diseases do not always commence at the times of the syzygies or quadratures of the moon and sun, or at the times of their passing the zenith or nadir; but as it is probable, that the stimulus of the particles of the circumfluent blood is gradually diminished from the time of the quadratures to that of the syzygies, the quiescence may commence at any hour, when co-operating with other causes of quiescence, it becomes great enough to produce a disease: afterwards it will continue to recur at the same period of the lunar or solar influence; the same cause operating conjointly with the acquired habit, that is with the catenation of this new motion with the dissevered links of the lunar or solar circles of animal action.

We need to recognize that the periods of calm and flare-ups in diseases don't always start during the alignments or phases of the moon and sun, or when they reach their highest or lowest points; however, it seems likely that the effect of the particles in the surrounding blood gradually decreases from the time of these alignments to when they coincide. Therefore, a state of calm can begin at any moment when combined with other factors that promote calmness, becoming strong enough to trigger a disease. After that, it will keep recurring at the same time influenced by the moon or sun, with the same factor working alongside the established patterns, meaning the new activity is linked to the separated cycles of the moon or sun's effect on bodily functions.

In this manner the periods of menstruation obey the lunar month with great exactness in healthy patients (and perhaps the venereal orgasm in brute animals does the same), yet these periods do not commence either at the syzygies or quadratures of the lunations, but at whatever time of the lunar periods they begin, they observe the same in their returns till some greater cause disturbs them.

In this way, menstruation cycles closely follow the lunar month in healthy individuals (and possibly the sexual climax in animals does too). However, these cycles do not start at the syzygies or quadratures of the lunar phases. Instead, no matter when in the lunar cycle they begin, they continue to return at the same intervals until some significant factor disrupts them.

Hence, though the best way to calculate the time of the expected returns of the paroxysms of periodical diseases is to count the number of hours between the commencement of the two preceding fits, yet the following observations may be worth attending to, when we endeavour to prevent the returns of maniacal or epileptic diseases; whose periods (at the beginning of them especially) frequently observe the syzygies of the moon and sun, and particularly about the equinox.

Hence, while the best way to estimate the timing of expected returns of periodic diseases is to count the hours between the onset of the last two episodes, the following observations may be worth considering when we try to prevent the recurrence of maniacal or epileptic conditions. Their cycles, especially at the start, often align with the phases of the moon and sun, particularly around the equinox.

The greatest of the two tides happening in every revolution of the moon, is that when the moon approaches nearest to the zenith or nadir; for this reason, while the sun is in the northern signs, that is during the vernal and summer months, the greater of the two diurnal tides in our latitude is that, when the moon is above the horizon; and when the sun is in the southern signs, or during the autumnal and winter months, the greater tide is that, which arises when the moon is below the horizon: and as the sun approaches somewhat nearer the earth in winter than in summer, the greatest equinoctial tides are observed to be a little before the vernal equinox, and a little after the autumnal one.

The highest of the two tides that occur with every cycle of the moon happens when the moon is closest to its highest point or lowest point in the sky. Because of this, while the sun is in the northern zodiac signs, which is during spring and summer, the bigger of the two daily tides in our area is when the moon is above the horizon. When the sun is in the southern signs during fall and winter, the larger tide occurs when the moon is below the horizon. Since the sun is a bit closer to the Earth in winter than in summer, the strongest equinox tides are seen just before the spring equinox and just after the fall equinox.

Do not the cold periods of lunar diseases commence a few hours before the southing of the moon during the vernal and summer months, and before the northing of the moon during the autumnal and winter months? Do not palsies and apoplexies, which occur about the equinoxes, happen a few days before the vernal equinoctial lunation, and after the autumnal one? Are not the periods of those diurnal diseases more obstinate, that commence many hours before the southing or northing of the moon, than of those which commence at those times? Are not those palsies and apoplexies more dangerous which commence many days before the syzygies of the moon, than those which happen at those times? See Sect. XXXVI. on the periods of diseases.

Do not the cold periods of lunar diseases commence a few hours before the southing of the moon during the vernal and summer months, and before the northing of the moon during the autumnal and winter months? Do not palsies and apoplexies, which occur about the equinoxes, happen a few days before the vernal equinoctial lunation, and after the autumnal one? Are not the periods of those diurnal diseases more obstinate, that commence many hours before the southing or northing of the moon, than of those which commence at those times? Are not those palsies and apoplexies more dangerous which commence many days before the syzygies of the moon, than those which happen at those times? See Sect. XXXVI. on the periods of diseases.

VII. Another very frequent cause of the cold fit of fever is the quiescence of some of those large congeries of glands, which compose the liver, spleen, or pancreas; one or more of which are frequently so enlarged in the autumnal intermittents as to be perceptible to the touch externally, and are called by the vulgar ague-cakes. As these glands are stimulated into action by the specific pungency of the fluids, which they absorb, the general cause of their quiescence seems to be the too great insipidity of the fluids of the body, co-operating perhaps at the same time with other general causes of quiescence.

VII. Another common reason for the cold fit of fever is the inactivity of some of the large clusters of glands that make up the liver, spleen, or pancreas; one or more of which often become so enlarged during autumnal fevers that they can be felt externally and are commonly referred to as ague-cakes. As these glands are prompted into action by the specific sharpness of the fluids they absorb, the underlying cause of their inactivity seems to be the overall blandness of the body's fluids, possibly working together with other general reasons for inactivity.

Hence, in marshy countries at cold seasons, which have succeeded hot ones, and amongst those, who have lived on innutritious and unstimulating diet, these agues are most frequent. The enlargement of these quiescent viscera, and the swelling of the præcordia in many other fevers, is, most probably, owing to the same cause; which may consist in a general deficiency of the production of sensorial power, as well as in the diminished stimulation of the fluids; and when the quiescence of so great a number of glands, as constitute one of those large viscera, commences, all the other irritative motions are affected by their connection with it, and the cold fit of fever is produced.

Therefore, in marshy regions during the cold seasons that follow hot ones, and among those who have lived on poor and unexciting diets, these fevers are most common. The swelling of these inactive organs and the enlargement of the chest in many other fevers is likely due to the same reason, which could involve a general lack of sensory energy production, as well as reduced stimulation of bodily fluids. When the inactivity of so many glands, which make up one of these large organs, begins, all other stimulating actions are influenced by their link to it, leading to the cold phase of fever.

VIII. There are many other causes, which produce quiescence of some part of the animal system, as fatigue, hunger, thirst, bad diet, disappointed love, unwholesome air, exhaustion from evacuations, and many others; but the last cause, that we shall mention, as frequently productive of cold fits of fever, is fear or anxiety of mind. The pains, which we are first and most generally acquainted with, have been produced by defect of some stimulus; thus, soon after our nativity we become acquainted with the pain from the coldness of the air, from the want of respiration, and from the want of food. Now all these pains occasioned by defect of stimulus are attended with quiescence of the organ, and at the same time with a greater or less degree of quiescence of other parts of the system: thus, if we even endure the pain of hunger so as to miss one meal instead of our daily habit of repletion, not only the peristaltic motions of the stomach and bowels are diminished, but we are more liable to coldness of our extremities, as of our noses, and ears, and feet, than at other times.

VIII. There are many other factors that can cause a part of the animal system to become inactive, such as fatigue, hunger, thirst, poor diet, unrequited love, bad air, exhaustion from bodily functions, and many others; but the last factor we’ll mention, which often leads to chills during fever, is fear or anxiety. The pains we first become familiar with are usually caused by a lack of some stimulus; right after we're born, we experience pain from the cold air, from not being able to breathe well, and from not having enough to eat. All these pains caused by a lack of stimulus are associated with inactivity in the organ and also a greater or lesser degree of inactivity in other parts of the system: for example, if we endure hunger by skipping a meal that we usually have, not only do the movements of the stomach and intestines slow down, but we are also more likely to feel cold in our extremities, like our noses, ears, and feet, than we usually do.

Now, as fear is originally excited by our having experienced pain, and is itself a painful affection, the same quiescence of other fibrous motions accompany it, as have been most frequently connected with this kind of pain, as explained in Sect. XVI. 8. 1. as the coldness and paleness of the skin, trembling, difficult respiration, indigestion, and other symptoms, which contribute to form the cold fit of fevers. Anxiety is fear continued through a longer time, and, by producing chronical torpor of the system, extinguishes life slowly, by what is commonly termed a broken heart.

Now, as fear is originally excited by our having experienced pain, and is itself a painful affection, the same quiescence of other fibrous motions accompany it, as have been most frequently connected with this kind of pain, as explained in Sect. XVI. 8. 1. as the coldness and paleness of the skin, trembling, difficult respiration, indigestion, and other symptoms, which contribute to form the cold fit of fevers. Anxiety is fear continued through a longer time, and, by producing chronical torpor of the system, extinguishes life slowly, by what is commonly termed a broken heart.

IX. 1. We now step forwards to consider the other symptoms in consequence of the quiescence which begins the fits of fever. If by any of the circumstances before described, or by two or more of them acting at the same time, a great degree of quiescence is induced on any considerable part of the circle of irritative motions, the whole class of them is more or less disturbed by their irritative associations. If this torpor be occasioned by a deficient supply of sensorial power, and happens to any of those parts of the system, which are accustomed to perpetual activity, as the vital motions, the torpor increases rapidly, because of the great expenditure of sensorial power by the incessant activity of those parts of the system, as shewn in No. 3. 2. of this Section. Hence a deficiency of all the secretions succeeds, and as animal heat is produced in proportion to the quantity of those secretions, the coldness of the skin is the first circumstance, which is attended to. Dr. Martin asserts, that some parts of his body were warmer than natural in the cold fit of fever; but it is certain, that those, which are uncovered, as the fingers, and nose, and ears, are much colder to the touch, and paler in appearance. It is possible, that his experiments were made at the beginning of the subsequent hot fits; which commence with partial distributions of heat, owing to some parts of the body regaining their natural irritability sooner than others.

IX. 1. We now step forwards to consider the other symptoms in consequence of the quiescence which begins the fits of fever. If by any of the circumstances before described, or by two or more of them acting at the same time, a great degree of quiescence is induced on any considerable part of the circle of irritative motions, the whole class of them is more or less disturbed by their irritative associations. If this torpor be occasioned by a deficient supply of sensorial power, and happens to any of those parts of the system, which are accustomed to perpetual activity, as the vital motions, the torpor increases rapidly, because of the great expenditure of sensorial power by the incessant activity of those parts of the system, as shewn in No. 3. 2. of this Section. Hence a deficiency of all the secretions succeeds, and as animal heat is produced in proportion to the quantity of those secretions, the coldness of the skin is the first circumstance, which is attended to. Dr. Martin asserts, that some parts of his body were warmer than natural in the cold fit of fever; but it is certain, that those, which are uncovered, as the fingers, and nose, and ears, are much colder to the touch, and paler in appearance. It is possible, that his experiments were made at the beginning of the subsequent hot fits; which commence with partial distributions of heat, owing to some parts of the body regaining their natural irritability sooner than others.

From the quiescence of the anastomosing capillaries a paleness of the skin succeeds, and a less secretion of the perspirable matter; from the quiescence of the pulmonary capillaries a difficulty of respiration arises; and from the quiescence of the other glands less bile, less gastric and pancreatic juice, are secreted into the stomach and intestines, and less mucus and saliva are poured into the mouth; whence arises the dry tongue, costiveness, dry ulcers, and paucity of urine. From the quiescence of the absorbent system arises the great thirst, as less moisture is absorbed from the atmosphere. The absorption from the atmosphere was observed by Dr. Lyster to amount to eighteen ounces in one night, above what he had at the same time insensibly perspired. See Langrish. On the same account the urine is pale, though in small quantity, for the thinner part is not absorbed from it; and when repeated ague-fits continue long, the legs swell from the diminished absorption of the cellular absorbents.

From the inactivity of the interconnected capillaries, the skin becomes pale, and there’s less secretion of sweat. When the pulmonary capillaries cease to function properly, it leads to difficulty in breathing. Additionally, because of reduced activity in other glands, there’s less bile, gastric juice, and pancreatic juice released into the stomach and intestines, as well as less mucus and saliva in the mouth; this results in a dry tongue, constipation, dry sores, and reduced urine output. The lack of activity in the absorption system causes intense thirst since there’s less moisture absorbed from the atmosphere. Dr. Lyster noted that moisture absorbed from the atmosphere could reach up to eighteen ounces in one night, more than what he unknowingly sweated out at the same time. See Langrish. For the same reason, the urine becomes pale, even though it’s produced in small amounts, because the thinner part is not absorbed from it; and when episodes of fever continue for a long time, the legs swell due to decreased absorption by the cellular absorbents.

From the quiescence of the intestinal canal a loss of appetite and flatulencies proceed. From the partial quiescence of the glandular viscera a swelling and tension about the præcordia becomes sensible to the touch; which is occasioned by the delay of the fluids from the defect of venous or lymphatic absorption. The pain of the forehead, and of the limbs, and of the small of the back, arises from the quiescence of the membranous fascia, or muscles of those parts, in the same manner as the skin becomes painful, when the vessels, of which it is composed, become quiescent from cold. The trembling in consequence of the pain of coldness, the restlessness, and the yawning, and stretching of the limbs, together with the shuddering, or rigours, are convulsive motions; and will be explained amongst the diseases of volition; Sect. XXXIV.

From the quiescence of the intestinal canal a loss of appetite and flatulencies proceed. From the partial quiescence of the glandular viscera a swelling and tension about the præcordia becomes sensible to the touch; which is occasioned by the delay of the fluids from the defect of venous or lymphatic absorption. The pain of the forehead, and of the limbs, and of the small of the back, arises from the quiescence of the membranous fascia, or muscles of those parts, in the same manner as the skin becomes painful, when the vessels, of which it is composed, become quiescent from cold. The trembling in consequence of the pain of coldness, the restlessness, and the yawning, and stretching of the limbs, together with the shuddering, or rigours, are convulsive motions; and will be explained amongst the diseases of volition; Sect. XXXIV.

Sickness and vomiting is a frequent symptom in the beginnings of fever-fits, the muscular fibres of the stomach share the general torpor and debility of the system; their motions become first lessened, and then stop, and then become retrograde; for the act of vomiting, like the globus hystericus and the borborigmi of hypochondriasis, is always a symptom of debility, either from want of stimulus, as in hunger; or from want of sensorial power, as after intoxication; or from sympathy with some other torpid irritative motions, as in the cold fits of ague. See Sect. XII. 5. 5. XXIX. 11. and XXXV. 1. 3. where this act of vomiting is further explained.

Sickness and vomiting is a frequent symptom in the beginnings of fever-fits, the muscular fibres of the stomach share the general torpor and debility of the system; their motions become first lessened, and then stop, and then become retrograde; for the act of vomiting, like the globus hystericus and the borborigmi of hypochondriasis, is always a symptom of debility, either from want of stimulus, as in hunger; or from want of sensorial power, as after intoxication; or from sympathy with some other torpid irritative motions, as in the cold fits of ague. See Sect. XII. 5. 5. XXIX. 11. and XXXV. 1. 3. where this act of vomiting is further explained.

The small pulse, which is said by some writers to be slow at the commencement of ague-fits, and which is frequently trembling and intermittent, is owing to the quiescence of the heart and arterial system, and to the resistance opposed to the circulating fluid from the inactivity of all the glands and capillaries. The great weakness and inability to voluntary motions, with the insensibility of the extremities, are owing to the general quiescence of the whole moving system; or, perhaps, simply to the deficient production of sensorial power.

The weak pulse, which some writers say is slow at the start of fever episodes, and is often shaky and irregular, happens because the heart and blood vessels are inactive, along with the resistance to blood flow from the inactivity of all the glands and capillaries. The severe weakness and inability to move voluntarily, along with the numbness in the extremities, result from the overall inactivity of the entire movement system; or maybe, it's just due to the insufficient production of sensory power.

If all these symptoms are further increased, the quiescence of all the muscles, including the heart and arteries, becomes complete, and death ensues. This is, most probably, the case of those who are starved to death with cold, and of those who are said to die in Holland from long skaiting on their frozen canals.

If all these symptoms worsen, the rest of the muscles, including the heart and arteries, completely shut down, leading to death. This is likely what happens to those who freeze to death from starvation, and to those who are said to die in Holland from skating for too long on their frozen canals.

2. As soon as this general quiescence of the system ceases, either by the diminution of the cause, or by the accumulation of sensorial power, (as in syncope, Sect. XII. 7. 1.) which is the natural consequence of previous quiescence, the hot fit commences. Every gland of the body is now stimulated into stronger action than is natural, as its irritability is increased by accumulation of sensorial power during its late quiescence, a superabundance of all the secretions is produced, and an increase of heat in consequence of the increase of these secretions. The skin becomes red, and the perspiration great, owing to the increased action of the capillaries during the hot part of the paroxysm. The secretion of perspirable matter is perhaps greater during the hot fit than in the sweating fit which follows; but as the absorption of it also is greater, it does not stand on the skin in visible drops: add to this, that the evaporation of it also is greater, from the increased heat of the skin. But at the decline of the hot fit, as the mouths of the absorbents of the skin are exposed to the cooler air, or bed-clothes, these vessels sooner lose their increased activity, and cease to absorb more than their natural quantity: but the secerning vessels for some time longer, being kept warm by the circulating blood, continue to pour out an increased quantity of perspirable matter, which now stands on the skin in large visible drops; the exhalation of it also being lessened by the greater coolness of the skin, as well as its absorption by the diminished action of the lymphatics. See Class I. 1. 2. 3.

2. As soon as this general quiescence of the system ceases, either by the diminution of the cause, or by the accumulation of sensorial power, (as in syncope, Sect. XII. 7. 1.) which is the natural consequence of previous quiescence, the hot fit commences. Every gland of the body is now stimulated into stronger action than is natural, as its irritability is increased by accumulation of sensorial power during its late quiescence, a superabundance of all the secretions is produced, and an increase of heat in consequence of the increase of these secretions. The skin becomes red, and the perspiration great, owing to the increased action of the capillaries during the hot part of the paroxysm. The secretion of perspirable matter is perhaps greater during the hot fit than in the sweating fit which follows; but as the absorption of it also is greater, it does not stand on the skin in visible drops: add to this, that the evaporation of it also is greater, from the increased heat of the skin. But at the decline of the hot fit, as the mouths of the absorbents of the skin are exposed to the cooler air, or bed-clothes, these vessels sooner lose their increased activity, and cease to absorb more than their natural quantity: but the secerning vessels for some time longer, being kept warm by the circulating blood, continue to pour out an increased quantity of perspirable matter, which now stands on the skin in large visible drops; the exhalation of it also being lessened by the greater coolness of the skin, as well as its absorption by the diminished action of the lymphatics. See Class I. 1. 2. 3.

The increased secretion of bile and of other fluids poured into the intestines frequently induce a purging at the decline of the hot fit; for as the external absorbent vessels have their mouths exposed to the cold air, as above mentioned, they cease to be excited into unnatural activity sooner than the secretory vessels, whose mouths are exposed to the warmth of the blood: now, as the internal absorbents sympathize with the external ones, these also, which during the hot fit drank up the thinner part of the bile, or of other secreted fluids, lose their increased activity before the gland loses its increased activity, at the decline of the hot fit; and the loose dejections are produced from the same cause, that the increased perspiration stands on the surface of the skin, from the increased absorption ceasing sooner than the increased secretion.

The increased secretion of bile and other fluids that enter the intestines often leads to a purge as the hot episode starts to fade. This is because the external absorbent vessels, exposed to the cold air as mentioned earlier, stop being overstimulated sooner than the secretory vessels, which are warmed by the blood. Since the internal absorbents respond to the external ones, those internal absorbents, which during the hot episode absorbed the thinner parts of bile or other secreted fluids, lose their heightened activity before the gland does at the end of the hot episode. Loose stools occur for the same reason that increased sweat collects on the surface of the skin: the increased absorption ends sooner than the heightened secretion.

The urine during the cold fit is in small quantity and pale, both from a deficiency of the secretion and a deficiency of the absorption.

The urine during the cold fit is in small amounts and light in color, due to both a lack of secretion and a lack of absorption.

During the hot fit it is in its usual quantity, but very high coloured and turbid, because a greater quantity had been secreted by the increased action of the kidnies, and also a greater quantity of its more aqueous part had been absorbed from it in the bladder by the increased action of the absorbents; and lastly, at the decline of the hot fit it is in large quantity and less coloured, or turbid, because the absorbent vessels of the bladder, as observed above, lose their increased action by sympathy with the cutaneous ones sooner than the secretory vessels of the kidnies lose their increased activity. Hence the quantity of the sediment, and the colour of the urine, in fevers, depend much on the quantity secreted by the kidnies, and the quantity absorbed from it again in the bladder: the kinds of sediment, as the lateritious, purulent, mucous, or bloody sediments, depend on other causes. It should be observed, that if the sweating be increased by the heat of the room, or of the bed-clothes, that a paucity of turbid urine will continue to be produced, as the absorbents of the bladder will have their activity increased by their sympathy with the vessels of the skin, for the purpose of supplying the fluid expended in perspiration.

During a hot fever, urine is typically produced in its usual amount but appears very dark and cloudy. This is because the kidneys are working extra hard to secrete more urine, and a larger amount of the watery part is being absorbed in the bladder due to increased activity in the absorbent vessels. As the fever starts to decline, the urine volume is still large but less colored and less cloudy. This happens because the absorbent vessels in the bladder, as mentioned earlier, stop their increased activity sooner than the kidneys do. Therefore, the amount and color of urine during fevers largely depend on how much is secreted by the kidneys and how much is reabsorbed in the bladder. The types of sediment, such as brick-red, pus, mucus, or blood, are influenced by different factors. It's important to note that if sweating increases due to room temperature or bedding, less turbidity in the urine will be produced, as the bladder's absorbent vessels will be more active in response to the skin’s vessels, helping to replace the fluid lost through sweating.

The pulse becomes strong and full owing to the increased irritability of the heart and arteries, from the accumulation of sensorial power during their quiescence, and to the quickness of the return of the blood from the various glands and capillaries. This increased action of all the secretory vessels does not occur very suddenly, nor universally at the same time. The heat seems to begin about the center, and to be diffused from thence irregularly to the other parts of the system. This may be owing to the situation of the parts which first became quiescent and caused the fever-fit, especially when a hardness or tumour about the præcordia can be felt by the hand; and hence this part, in whatever viscus it is seated, might be the first to regain its natural or increased irritability.

The pulse gets stronger and fuller because of the heart and arteries becoming more sensitive due to the buildup of energy during their rest, and the faster return of blood from the various glands and tiny blood vessels. This increased activity in all the secretory vessels doesn’t happen all at once or everywhere at the same time. The warmth seems to start in the center and spreads out unevenly to other parts of the body. This could be due to the location of the areas that first became inactive and triggered the fever, especially when there’s a hardness or swelling felt in the chest area; so this region, regardless of which organ it’s affecting, might be the first to regain its normal or heightened sensitivity.

3. It must be here noted, that, by the increased quantity of heat, and of the impulse of the blood at the commencement of the hot fit, a great increase of stimulus is induced, and is now added to the increased irritability of the system, which was occasioned by its previous quiescence. This additional stimulus of heat and momentum of the blood augments the violence of the movements of the arterial and glandular system in an increasing ratio. These violent exertions still producing more heat and greater momentum of the moving fluids, till at length the sensoral power becomes wasted by this great stimulus beneath its natural quantity, and predisposes the system to a second cold fit.

3. It should be noted that the increase in temperature and blood flow at the start of the hot phase creates a significant boost in stimulation, which adds to the heightened sensitivity of the body that was caused by its earlier state of rest. This extra stimulation from heat and blood pressure escalates the intensity of activity in the arterial and glandular systems at an increasing rate. These intense efforts continue to generate more heat and higher blood momentum, until eventually the sensory power is depleted by this intense stimulation, falling below its natural level and making the system more susceptible to a subsequent cold phase.

At length all these unnatural exertions spontaneously subside with the increased irritability that produced them; and which was itself produced by the preceding quiescence, in the same manner as the eye, on coming from darkness into day-light, in a little time ceases to be dazzled and pained, and gradually recovers its natural degree of irritability.

Eventually, all these unnatural efforts naturally fade away along with the heightened irritation that caused them, which was in turn caused by the earlier calmness, much like how the eye, after moving from darkness to light, soon stops feeling dazzled and painful, gradually returning to its normal sensitivity.

4. But if the increase of irritability, and the consequent increase of the stimulus of heat and momentum, produce more violent exertions than those above described; great pain arises in some part of the moving system, as in the membranes of the brain, pleura, or joints; and new motions of the vessels are produced in consequence of this pain, which are called inflammation; or delirium or stupor arises; as explained in Sect. XXI. and XXXIII.: for the immediate effect is the same, whether the great energy of the moving organs arises from an increase of stimulus or an increase of irritability; though in the former case the waste of sensorial power leads to debility, and in the latter to health.

4. But if the increase of irritability, and the consequent increase of the stimulus of heat and momentum, produce more violent exertions than those above described; great pain arises in some part of the moving system, as in the membranes of the brain, pleura, or joints; and new motions of the vessels are produced in consequence of this pain, which are called inflammation; or delirium or stupor arises; as explained in Sect. XXI. and XXXIII.: for the immediate effect is the same, whether the great energy of the moving organs arises from an increase of stimulus or an increase of irritability; though in the former case the waste of sensorial power leads to debility, and in the latter to health.

Recapitulation.

Summary.

X. Those muscles, which are less frequently exerted, and whose actions are interrupted by sleep, acquire less accumulation of sensorial power during their quiescent state, as the muscles of locomotion. In these muscles after great exertion, that is, after great exhaustion of sensorial power, the pain of fatigue ensues; and during rest there is a renovation of the natural quantity of sensorial power; but where the rest, or quiescence of the muscle, is long continued, a quantity of sensorial power becomes accumulated beyond what is necessary; as appears by the uneasiness occasioned by want of exercise; and which in young animals is one cause exciting them into action, as is seen in the play of puppies and kittens.

X. The muscles that aren't used often and whose activity is interrupted by sleep build up less sensory power while they're at rest, unlike the muscles used for movement. After intense use, which drains their sensory power, these muscles experience fatigue. During rest, they regain their natural amount of sensory power, but if the muscles stay inactive for too long, they accumulate more sensory power than needed. This surplus causes restlessness due to a lack of exercise, which is one reason young animals feel the urge to move, as seen in the playful behavior of puppies and kittens.

But when those muscles, which are habituated to perpetual actions, as those of the stomach by the stimulus of food, those of the vessels of the skin by the stimulus of heat, and those which constitute the arteries and glands by the stimulus of the blood, become for a time quiescent, from the want of their appropriated stimuli, or by their associations with other quiescent parts of the system; a greater accumulation of sensorial power is acquired during their quiescence, and a greater or quicker exhaustion of it is produced during their increased action.

But when those muscles that are used to constant activity, like the stomach from food stimuli, the blood vessels in the skin from heat stimuli, and the muscles in the arteries and glands from blood stimuli, become inactive for a while due to lack of their usual stimuli or because they are connected to other inactive parts of the system, they build up a greater amount of sensorial power during this rest. Then, when they start working more, that power is exhausted more quickly or intensely.

This accumulation of sensorial power from deficient action, if it happens to the stomach from want of food, occasions the pain of hunger; if it happens to the vessels of the skin from want of heat, it occasions the pain of cold; and if to the arterial system from the want of its adapted stimuli, many disagreeable sensations are occasioned, such as are experienced in the cold fits of intermittent fevers, and are as various, as there are glands or membranes in the system, and are generally termed universal uneasiness.

This buildup of sensory power from lack of action, if it happens in the stomach due to not having enough food, causes the pain of hunger; if it affects the skin's blood vessels from not having enough warmth, it causes the pain of being cold; and if it impacts the arterial system from the lack of appropriate stimuli, it leads to many unpleasant sensations, like those felt during the chills of intermittent fevers, which vary as much as the different glands or membranes in the body, and are commonly referred to as universal discomfort.

When the quiescence of the arterial system is not owing to defect of stimulus as above, but to the defective quantity of sensorial power, as in the commencement of nervous fever, or irritative fever with weak pulse, a great torpor of this system is quickly induced; because both the irritation from the stimulus of the blood, and the association of the vascular motions with each other, continue to excite the arteries into action, and thence quickly exhaust the ill-supplied vascular muscles; for to rest is death; and therefore those vascular muscles continue to proceed, though with feebler action, to the extreme of weariness or faintness: while nothing similar to this affects the locomotive muscles, whose actions are generally caused by volition, and not much subject either to irritation or to other kinds of associations besides the voluntary ones, except indeed when they are excited by the lash of slavery.

When the calm of the arterial system isn't due to a lack of stimulus as mentioned above but instead to a shortage of sensory power, like at the beginning of nervous fever or irritative fever with a weak pulse, a significant sluggishness in this system can quickly develop. This happens because both the irritation from the blood's stimulus and the connection of the vascular motions together continue to drive the arteries into action, which quickly drains the poorly supplied vascular muscles. Because resting leads to death, these vascular muscles keep going, although with weaker action, until they reach complete exhaustion or faintness. In contrast, the locomotive muscles aren't affected in the same way; their actions are generally driven by voluntary control and aren't heavily influenced by irritation or other forms of associations except when they are forced by the whip of slavery.

In these vascular muscles, which are subject to perpetual action, and thence liable to great accumulation of sensorial power during their quiescence from want of stimulus, a great increase of activity occurs, either from the renewal of their accustomed stimulus, or even from much less quantities of stimulus than usual. This increase of action constitutes the hot fit of fever, which is attended with various increased secretions, with great concomitant heat, and general uneasiness. The uneasiness attending this hot paroxysm of fever, or fit of exertion, is very different from that, which attends the previous cold fit, or fit of quiescence, and is frequently the cause of inflammation, as in pleurisy, which is treated of in the next section.

In these vascular muscles, which are constantly active and can build up a lot of sensorial power during their downtime due to lack of stimulus, there is a significant increase in activity either from the return of their usual stimulus or even from much smaller amounts of stimulus than normal. This heightened activity leads to the hot phase of a fever, accompanied by various heightened secretions, significant heat, and overall discomfort. The discomfort experienced during this hot episode of fever, or period of exertion, is quite different from that of the preceding cold phase, or period of rest, and often results in inflammation, as seen in pleurisy, which will be discussed in the next section.

A similar effect occurs after the quiescence of our organs of sense; those which are not subject to perpetual action, as the taste and smell, are less liable to an exuberant accumulation of sensorial power after their having for a time been inactive; but the eye, which is in perpetual action during the day, becomes dazzled, and liable to inflammation after a temporary quiescence.

A similar effect happens after our senses have been inactive. Those that aren't constantly active, like taste and smell, are less likely to build up excess sensory power after a period of inactivity. However, the eye, which is always active during the day, tends to get dazzled and can become inflamed after a short break.

Where the previous quiescence has been owing to a defect of sensorial power, and not to a defect of stimulus, as in the irritative fever with weak pulse, a similar increase of activity of the arterial system succeeds, either from the usual stimulus of the blood, or from a stimulus less than usual; but as there is in general in these cases of fever with weak pulse a deficiency of the quantity of the blood, the pulse in the hot fit is weaker than in health, though it is stronger than in the cold fit, as explained in No. 2. of this section. But at the same time in those fevers, where the defect of irritation is owing to the defect of the quantity of sensorial power, as well as to the defect of stimulus, another circumstance occurs; which consists in the partial distribution of it, as appears in partial flushings, as of the face or bosom, while the extremities are cold; and in the increase of particular secretions, as of bile, saliva, insensible perspiration, with great heat of the skin, or with partial sweats, or diarrhœa.

Where the previous quiescence has been owing to a defect of sensorial power, and not to a defect of stimulus, as in the irritative fever with weak pulse, a similar increase of activity of the arterial system succeeds, either from the usual stimulus of the blood, or from a stimulus less than usual; but as there is in general in these cases of fever with weak pulse a deficiency of the quantity of the blood, the pulse in the hot fit is weaker than in health, though it is stronger than in the cold fit, as explained in No. 2. of this section. But at the same time in those fevers, where the defect of irritation is owing to the defect of the quantity of sensorial power, as well as to the defect of stimulus, another circumstance occurs; which consists in the partial distribution of it, as appears in partial flushings, as of the face or bosom, while the extremities are cold; and in the increase of particular secretions, as of bile, saliva, insensible perspiration, with great heat of the skin, or with partial sweats, or diarrhœa.

There are also many uneasy sensations attending these increased actions, which, like those belonging to the hot fit of fever with strong pulse, are frequently followed by inflammation, as in scarlet fever; which inflammation is nevertheless accompanied with a pulse weaker, though quicker, than the pulse during the remission or intermission of the paroxysms, though stronger than that of the previous cold fit.

There are also many uncomfortable feelings that come with these heightened activities, which, similar to the intense phase of a fever with a strong pulse, are often followed by inflammation, like in scarlet fever; this inflammation is, however, accompanied by a pulse that is weaker, although faster, than the pulse during the calm periods or breaks of the episodes, but stronger than that of the earlier chill phase.

From hence I conclude, that both the cold and hot fits of fever are necessary consequences of the perpetual and incessant action of the arterial and glandular system; since those muscular fibres and those organs of sense, which are most frequently exerted, become necessarily most affected both with defect and accumulation of sensorial power: and that hence fever-fits are not an effort of nature to relieve herself, and that therefore they should always be prevented or diminished as much as possible, by any means which decrease the general or partial vascular actions, when they are greater, or by increasing them when they are less than in health, as described in Sect. XII. 6. 1.

From hence I conclude, that both the cold and hot fits of fever are necessary consequences of the perpetual and incessant action of the arterial and glandular system; since those muscular fibres and those organs of sense, which are most frequently exerted, become necessarily most affected both with defect and accumulation of sensorial power: and that hence fever-fits are not an effort of nature to relieve herself, and that therefore they should always be prevented or diminished as much as possible, by any means which decrease the general or partial vascular actions, when they are greater, or by increasing them when they are less than in health, as described in Sect. XII. 6. 1.

Thus have I endeavoured to explain, and I hope to the satisfaction of the candid and patient reader, the principal symptoms or circumstances of fever without the introduction of the supernatural power of spasm. To the arguments in favour of the doctrine of spasm it may be sufficient to reply, that in the evolution of medical as well as of dramatic catastrophe,

Thus, I have tried to explain, and I hope to satisfy the honest and patient reader, the main symptoms or aspects of fever without bringing in the supernatural idea of spasm. In response to the arguments supporting the doctrine of spasm, it may be enough to say that in the development of both medical and dramatic disasters,

Nec Deus intersit, nisi dignus vindice nodus inciderit.—HOR.

Nec Deus intersit, nisi dignus vindice nodus inciderit.—HEither.



SECT. XXXIII.

DISEASES OF SENSATION.

Sensation Disorders.

I. 1. Motions excited by sensation. Digestion. Generation. Pleasure of existence. Hypochondriacism. 2. Pain introduced. Sensitive fevers of two kinds. 3. Two sensorial powers exerted in sensitive fevers. Size of the blood. Nervous fevers distinguished from putrid ones. The septic and antiseptic theory. 4. Two kinds of delirium. 5. Other animals are less liable to delirium, cannot receive our contagious diseases, and are less liable to madness. II. 1. Sensitive motions generated. 2. Inflammation explained. 3. Its remote causes from excess of irritation, or of irritability, not from those pains which are owing to defect of irritation. New vessels produced, and much heat. 4. Purulent matter secreted. 5. Contagion explained. 6. Received but once. 7. If common matter be contagious? 8. Why some contagions are received but once. 9. Why others may be received frequently. Contagions of small-pox and measles do not act at the same times. Two cases of such patients. 10. The blood from patients in the small-pox will not infect others. Cases of children thus inoculated. The variolous contagion is not received into the blood. It acts by sensitive association between the stomach and skin. III. 1. Absorption of solids and fluids. 2. Art of healing ulcers. 3. Mortification attended with less pain in weak people.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Movements triggered by sensation. Digestion. Reproduction. Joy of existence. Hypochondria. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Pain introduced. Two types of sensitive fevers. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Two sensory powers involved in sensitive fevers. Volume of blood. Nervous fevers different from putrid ones. The septic and antiseptic theory. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Two types of delirium. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Other animals are less prone to delirium, cannot catch our contagious diseases, and are less prone to madness. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Sensitive movements generated. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Inflammation explained. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Its distant causes are due to excessive irritation or irritability, not from pains caused by insufficient irritation. New vessels form, and a lot of heat. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Purulent matter produced. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. Contagion explained. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. Received only once. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. Is common matter contagious? __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. Why some contagions are received only once. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. Why others can be received multiple times. Contagions of smallpox and measles do not act at the same time. Two cases of such patients. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. The blood from smallpox patients will not infect others. Cases of children inoculated in this way. The variolous contagion is not introduced into the blood. It works through a sensitive link between the stomach and skin. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_18__. Absorption of solids and fluids. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_19__. Techniques for healing ulcers. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_20__. Mortification causes less pain in weak individuals.

I. 1. As many motions of the body are excited and continued by irritations, so others require, either conjunctly with these, or separately, the pleasurable or painful sensations, for the purpose of producing them with due energy. Amongst these the business of digestion supplies us with an instance: if the food, which we swallow, is not attended with agreeable sensation, it digests less perfectly; and if very disagreeable sensation accompanies it, such as a nauseous idea, or very disgustful taste, the digestion becomes impeded; or retrograde motions of the stomach and œsophagus succeed, and the food is ejected.

I. 1. Many movements of the body are triggered and maintained by irritations, while others need either these irritations along with them or separately, the pleasurable or painful feelings to happen effectively. Digestion is a good example of this: if the food we eat doesn’t come with a pleasant sensation, it doesn't digest as well; and if there’s a really unpleasant feeling, like a nauseating thought or a revolting taste, digestion is hindered, leading to backward movements of the stomach and esophagus, and the food is expelled.

The business of generation depends so much on agreeable sensation, that, where the object is disgustful, neither voluntary exertion nor irritation can effect the purpose; which is also liable to be interrupted by the pain of fear or bashfulness.

The process of creating relies heavily on pleasant feelings, so if the object is unappealing, neither willing effort nor annoyance can achieve the goal; this can also be disrupted by the discomfort of fear or embarrassment.

Besides the pleasure, which attends the irritations produced by the objects of lust and hunger, there seems to be a sum of pleasurable affection accompanying the various secretions of the numerous glands, which constitute the pleasure of life, in contradistinction to the tedium vitæ. This quantity or sum of pleasurable affection, seems to contribute to the due or energetic performance of the whole moveable system, as well that of the heart and arteries, as of digestion and of absorption; since without the due quantity of pleasurable sensation, flatulency and hypochondriacism affect the intestines, and a languor seizes the arterial pulsations and secretions; as occurs in great and continued anxiety of the mind.

Aside from the pleasure that comes from the frustrations caused by desires and hunger, there seems to be a collection of enjoyable feelings that accompany the different secretions from various glands, which make up the joy of life, as opposed to life's monotony. This amount of pleasurable feeling appears to help the proper and vigorous functioning of the whole movable system, including the heart and arteries, as well as digestion and absorption. If there isn't enough pleasurable sensation, issues like bloating and anxiety can affect the intestines, and a sluggishness can take over the pulse and secretions, similar to what happens during prolonged mental distress.

2. Besides the febrile motions occasioned by irritation, described in Sect. XXXII. and termed irritative fever, it frequently happens that pain is excited by the violence of the fibrous contractions; and other new motions are then superadded, in consequence of sensation, which we shall term febris sensitiva, or sensitive fever. It must be observed, that most irritative fevers begin with a decreased exertion of irritation, owing to defect of stimulus; but that on the contrary the sensitive fevers, or inflammations, generally begin with the increased exertion of sensation, as mentioned in Sect. XXXI. on temperaments: for though the cold fit, which introduces inflammation, commences with decreased irritation, yet the inflammation itself commences in the hot fit during the increase of sensation. Thus a common pustule, or phlegmon, in a part of little sensibility does not excite an inflammatory fever; but if the stomach, intestines, or the tender substance beneath the nails, be injured, great sensation is produced, and the whole system is thrown into that kind of exertion, which constitutes inflammation.

2. Besides the febrile motions occasioned by irritation, described in Sect. XXXII. and termed irritative fever, it frequently happens that pain is excited by the violence of the fibrous contractions; and other new motions are then superadded, in consequence of sensation, which we shall term febris sensitiva, or sensitive fever. It must be observed, that most irritative fevers begin with a decreased exertion of irritation, owing to defect of stimulus; but that on the contrary the sensitive fevers, or inflammations, generally begin with the increased exertion of sensation, as mentioned in Sect. XXXI. on temperaments: for though the cold fit, which introduces inflammation, commences with decreased irritation, yet the inflammation itself commences in the hot fit during the increase of sensation. Thus a common pustule, or phlegmon, in a part of little sensibility does not excite an inflammatory fever; but if the stomach, intestines, or the tender substance beneath the nails, be injured, great sensation is produced, and the whole system is thrown into that kind of exertion, which constitutes inflammation.

These sensitive fevers, like the irritative ones, resolve themselves into those with arterial strength, and those with arterial debility, that is with excess or defect of sensorial power; these may be termed the febris sensitiva pulsu forti, sensitive fever with strong pulse, which is the synocha, or inflammatory fever; and the febris sensitiva pulsu debili, sensitive fever with weak pulse, which is the typhus gravior, or putrid fever of some writers.

These sensitive fevers, similar to the irritative ones, can be divided into those with strong arterial strength and those with weak arterial strength, which represent either an excess or a deficiency of sensory power. We can call these febris sensitiva pulsu forti, sensitive fever with a strong pulse, known as synocha or inflammatory fever; and febris sensitiva pulsu debili, sensitive fever with a weak pulse, referred to as typhus gravior or putrid fever by some authors.

3. The inflammatory fevers, which are here termed sensitive fevers with strong pulse, are generally attended with some topical inflammation, as pleurisy, peripneumony, or rheumatism, which distinguishes them from irritative fevers with strong pulse. The pulse is strong, quick, and full; for in this fever there is great irritation, as well as great sensation, employed in moving the arterial system. The size, or coagulable lymph, which appears on the blood, is probably an increased secretion from the inflamed internal lining of the whole arterial system, the thinner part being taken away by the increased absorption of the inflamed lymphatics.

3. The inflammatory fevers, referred to here as sensitive fevers with a strong pulse, typically come with some kind of local inflammation, such as pleurisy, pneumonia, or rheumatism, which sets them apart from irritative fevers that also have a strong pulse. The pulse is strong, fast, and full because this fever causes significant irritation and sensation, affecting the arterial system. The size, or coagulable lymph, that appears in the blood is probably due to increased secretion from the inflamed inner lining of the entire arterial system, with the thinner component being removed by heightened absorption from the inflamed lymphatics.

The sensitive fevers with weak pulse, which are termed putrid or malignant fevers, are distinguished from irritative fevers with weak pulse, called nervous fevers, described in the last section, as the former consist of inflammation joined with debility, and the latter of debility alone. Hence there is greater heat and more florid colour of the skin in the former, with petechiæ, or purple spots, and aphthæ, or sloughs in the throat, and generally with previous contagion.

The sensitive fevers with a weak pulse, known as putrid or malignant fevers, are different from irritative fevers with a weak pulse, referred to as nervous fevers, described in the last section. The former involves inflammation along with weakness, while the latter consists of weakness alone. As a result, the former has higher heat and a more vibrant skin color, along with petechiae, or purple spots, and aphthae, or sores in the throat, often following a previous infection.

When animal matter dies, as a slough in the throat, or the mortified part of a carbuncle, if it be kept moist and warm, as during its abhesion to a living body, it will soon putrify. This, and the origin of contagion from putrid animal substances, seem to have given rise to the septic and antiseptic theory of these fevers.

When animal tissue dies, like in a throat infection or the decayed part of a carbuncle, if it stays moist and warm, just like when it's still attached to a living body, it will quickly rot. This, along with how diseases can spread from decaying animal matter, seems to have led to the septic and antiseptic theory of these fevers.

The matter in pustules and ulcers is thus liable to become putrid, and to produce microscopic animalcula; the urine, if too long retained, may also gain a putrescent smell, as well as the alvine feces; but some writers have gone so far as to believe, that the blood itself in these fevers has smelt putrid, when drawn from the arm of the patient: but this seems not well founded; since a single particle of putrid matter taken into the blood can produce fever, how can we conceive that the whole mass could continue a minute in a putrid state without destroying life? Add to this, that putrid animal substances give up air, as in gangrenes; and that hence if the blood was putrid, air should be given out, which in the blood-vessels is known to occasion immediate death.

The material in pustules and ulcers can easily become rotten and produce tiny microscopic organisms; if urine is held for too long, it may also develop a foul smell, as can feces. However, some writers have even claimed that the blood itself in these fevers has a putrid odor when taken from the patient's arm. This seems unfounded; if even a small amount of rotten matter can cause a fever, how could the entire volume of blood remain in a putrid state for even a moment without harming the person? Additionally, rotting animal substances release gases, as seen in gangrene; if the blood were truly rotten, it would release gases, which are known to cause immediate death in the blood vessels.

In these sensitive fevers with strong pulse (or inflammations) there are two sensorial faculties concerned in producing the disease, viz. irritation and sensation; and hence, as their combined action is more violent, the general quantity of sensorial power becomes further exhausted during the exacerbation, and the system more rapidly weakened than in irritative fever with strong pulse; where the spirit of animation is weakened by but one mode of its exertion: so that this febris sensitiva pulsu forti (or inflammatory fever,) may be considered as the febris irritativa pulsu forti, with the addition of inflammation; and the febris sensitiva pulsu debili (or malignant fever) may be considered as the febris irritativa pulsu debili (or nervous fever), with the addition of inflammation.

In these sensitive fevers with a strong pulse (or inflammations), there are two sensory faculties involved in causing the disease: irritation and sensation. Because their combined action is more intense, the overall amount of sensory power gets further depleted during the worsening of the illness, and the body becomes weakened more quickly than in irritative fever with a strong pulse, where the spirit of vitality is diminished by just one way it functions. Therefore, this febris sensitiva pulsu forti (or inflammatory fever) can be seen as the febris irritativa pulsu forti, but with inflammation added; and the febris sensitiva pulsu debili (or malignant fever) can be viewed as the febris irritativa pulsu debili (or nervous fever), but with inflammation added.

4. In these putrid or malignant fevers a deficiency of irritability accompanies the increase of sensibility; and by this waste of sensorial power by the excess of sensation, which was already too small, arises the delirium and stupor which so perpetually attend these inflammatory fevers with arterial debility. In these cases the voluntary power first ceases to act from deficiency of sensorial spirit; and the stimuli from external bodies have no effect on the exhausted sensorial power, and a delirium like a dream is the consequence. At length the internal stimuli cease to excite sufficient irritation, and the secretions are either not produced at all, or too parsimonious in quantity. Amongst these the secretion of the brain, or production of the sensorial power, becomes deficient, till at last all sensorial power ceases, except what is just necessary to perform the vital motions, and a stupor succeeds; which is thus owing to the same cause as the preceding delirium exerted in a greater degree.

4. In these severe or harmful fevers, there's a lack of irritability that comes with an increase in sensitivity; this loss of sensory power due to heightened sensation, which was already too low, leads to the delirium and stupor that constantly accompany these inflammatory fevers with weakened arteries. In these situations, voluntary control first stops working because of a lack of sensory energy; external stimuli from the environment have no impact on the drained sensory power, resulting in a dream-like delirium. Eventually, internal stimuli fail to generate enough irritation, and secretions either aren't produced at all or are in very small amounts. Among these is the secretion from the brain, or the production of sensory energy, which becomes insufficient, until eventually all sensory power ceases, except for what is minimally needed for vital functions, leading to stupor, which arises from the same cause as the previous delirium but in a more extreme form.

This kind of delirium is owing to a suspension of volition, and to the disobedience of the senses to external stimuli, and is always occasioned by great debility, or paucity of sensorial power; it is therefore a bad sign at the end of inflammatory fevers, which had previous arterial strength, as rheumatism, or pleurisy, as it shews the presence of great exhaustion of sensorial power in a system, which having lately been exposed to great excitement, is not so liable to be stimulated into its healthy action, either by additional stimulus of food and medicines, or by the accumulation of sensorial power during its present torpor. In inflammatory fevers with debility, as those termed putrid fevers, delirium is sometimes, as well as stupor, rather a favourable sign; as less sensorial power is wasted during its continuance (see Class II. 1. 6. 8.), and the constitution not having been previously exposed to excess of stimulation, is more liable to be excited after previous quiescence.

This kind of delirium happens when there's a break in willpower, and the senses don’t respond to outside triggers. It's usually caused by extreme weakness or a lack of sensory energy. Therefore, it’s a bad sign at the end of inflammatory fevers that had strong arterial responses, like rheumatism or pleurisy, because it indicates a significant depletion of sensory energy in a system that has recently been under a lot of stress. As a result, it’s less likely to be triggered back into healthy function, either by more food and medicine or by building up sensory energy while it’s still lethargic. In inflammatory fevers with weakness, such as what we call putrid fevers, delirium as well as stupor can be somewhat encouraging because less sensory energy is used up while they last (see Class II. 1. 6. 8.), and since the body hasn’t previously faced too much stimulation, it’s more likely to be activated after a period of inactivity.

When the sum of general pleasurable sensation becomes too great, another kind of delirium supervenes, and the ideas thus excited are mistaken for the irritations of external objects: such a delirium is produced for a time by intoxicating drugs, as fermented liquors, or opium: a permanent delirium of this kind is sometimes induced by the pleasures of inordinate vanity, or by the enthusiastic hopes of heaven. In these cases the power of volition is incapable of exertion, and in a great degree the external senses become incapable of perceiving their adapted stimuli, because the whole sensorial power is employed or expended on the ideas excited by pleasurable sensation.

When the overall feeling of pleasure becomes overwhelming, a different kind of delirium sets in, and the ideas that arise are confused with the effects of outside factors. This kind of delirium can temporarily occur due to intoxicating substances, like alcoholic drinks or opium. A lasting state of this delirium can sometimes be caused by excessive vanity or by unrealistic hopes of paradise. In these situations, the ability to make decisions is severely impaired, and the external senses largely lose their ability to respond to relevant stimuli because all sensory energy is focused on the ideas triggered by pleasure.

This kind of delirium is distinguished from that which attends the fevers above mentioned from its not being accompanied with general debility, but simply with excess of pleasurable sensation; and is therefore in some measure allied to madness or to reverie; it differs from the delirium of dreams, as in this the power of volition is not totally suspended, nor are the senses precluded from external stimulation; there is therefore a degree of consistency, in this kind of delirium, and a degree of attention to external objects, neither of which exist in the delirium of fevers or in dreams.

This type of delirium is different from the fevers mentioned earlier because it doesn’t come with overall weakness, but instead is marked by an overwhelming sense of pleasure. It's somewhat similar to madness or daydreaming. This delirium is unlike the kind experienced in dreams because, in this case, a person’s ability to choose actions isn’t completely lost, and their senses can still respond to outside stimuli. So, there’s a level of consistency in this kind of delirium and a certain awareness of external objects, which isn’t seen in fever-related delirium or in dreams.

5. It would appear, that the vascular system of other animals are less liable to be put into action by their general sum of pleasurable or painful sensation; and that the trains of their ideas, and the muscular motions usually associated with them, are less powerfully connected than in the human system. For other animals neither weep, nor smile, nor laugh; and are hence seldom subject to delirium, as treated of in Sect. XVI. on Instinct. Now as our epidemic and contagious diseases are probably produced by disagreeable sensation, and not simply by irritation; there appears a reason, why brute animals are less liable to epidemic or contagious diseases; and secondly, why none of our contagions, as the small-pox or measles, can be communicated to them, though one of theirs, viz. the hydrophobia, as well as many of their poisons, as those of snakes and of in insects, communicate their deleterious or painful effects to mankind.

5. It would appear, that the vascular system of other animals are less liable to be put into action by their general sum of pleasurable or painful sensation; and that the trains of their ideas, and the muscular motions usually associated with them, are less powerfully connected than in the human system. For other animals neither weep, nor smile, nor laugh; and are hence seldom subject to delirium, as treated of in Sect. XVI. on Instinct. Now as our epidemic and contagious diseases are probably produced by disagreeable sensation, and not simply by irritation; there appears a reason, why brute animals are less liable to epidemic or contagious diseases; and secondly, why none of our contagions, as the small-pox or measles, can be communicated to them, though one of theirs, viz. the hydrophobia, as well as many of their poisons, as those of snakes and of in insects, communicate their deleterious or painful effects to mankind.

Where the quantity of general painful sensation is too great in the system, inordinate voluntary exertions are produced either of our ideas, as in melancholy and madness, or of our muscles, as in convulsion. From these maladies also brute animals are much more exempt than mankind, owing to their greater inaptitude to voluntary exertion, as mentioned in Sect. XVI. on Instinct.

Where the quantity of general painful sensation is too great in the system, inordinate voluntary exertions are produced either of our ideas, as in melancholy and madness, or of our muscles, as in convulsion. From these maladies also brute animals are much more exempt than mankind, owing to their greater inaptitude to voluntary exertion, as mentioned in Sect. XVI. on Instinct.

II. 1. When any moving organ is excited into such violent motions, that a quantity of pleasurable or painful sensation is produced, it frequently happens (but not always) that new motions of the affected organ are generated in consequence of the pain or pleasure, which are termed inflammation.

II. 1. When any moving part of the body is pushed into such intense movements that it creates a feeling of pleasure or pain, it often happens (though not always) that new movements of the affected part arise as a result of that pain or pleasure, which we call inflammation.

These new motions are of a peculiar kind, tending to distend the old, and to produce new fibres, and thence to elongate the straight muscles, which serve locomotion, and to form new vessels at the extremities or sides of the vascular muscles.

These new movements are unique, aimed at expanding the old, creating new fibers, and thereby stretching the straight muscles that enable movement, as well as forming new vessels at the ends or sides of the vascular muscles.

2. Thus the pleasurable sensations produce an enlargement of the nipples of nurses, of the papillæ of the tongue, of the penis, and probably produce the growth of the body from its embryon state to its maturity; whilst the new motions in consequence of painful sensation, with the growth of the fibres or vessels, which they occasion, are termed inflammation.

2. So, enjoyable feelings lead to an increase in the size of nurses' nipples, the bumps on the tongue, the penis, and likely contribute to the body's growth from an embryo to maturity; while the new movements resulting from painful sensations, along with the growth of fibers or blood vessels that they cause, are referred to as inflammation.

Hence when the straight muscles are inflamed, part of their tendons at each extremity gain new life and sensibility, and thus the muscle is for a time elongated; and inflamed bones become soft, vascular, and sensible. Thus new vessels shoot over the cornea of inflamed eyes, and into scirrhous tumours, when they become inflamed; and hence all inflamed parts grow together by intermixture, and inosculation of the new and old vessels.

Hence, when the straight muscles get inflamed, part of their tendons at each end gain new life and sensitivity, causing the muscle to temporarily stretch. Inflamed bones become soft, more vascular, and sensitive. Consequently, new blood vessels form over the cornea of inflamed eyes, and into hard tumours when they become inflamed; thus, all inflamed parts heal together through the mixture and connection of the new and old vessels.

The heat is occasioned from the increased secretions either of mucus, or of the fibres, which produce or elongate the vessels. The red colour is owing to the pellucidity of the newly formed vessels, and as the arterial parts of them are probably formed before their correspondent venous parts.

The heat comes from the increased secretions of either mucus or fibers that create or extend the vessels. The red color is due to the transparency of the newly formed vessels, and since the arterial parts are likely formed before their corresponding venous parts.

3. These new motions are excited either from the increased quantity of sensation in consequence of greater fibrous contractions, or from increased sensibility, that is, from the increased quantity of sensorial power in the moving organ. Hence they are induced by great external stimuli, as by wounds, broken bones; and by acrid or infectious materials; or by common stimuli on those organs, which have been some time quiescent; as the usual light of the day inflames the eyes of those, who have been confined in dungeons; and the warmth of a common fire inflames those, who have been previously exposed to much cold.

3. These new movements are triggered either by an increase in sensation due to stronger muscle contractions or by heightened sensitivity, meaning there's more sensory power in the moving part. As a result, they're caused by intense external stimuli, such as wounds, broken bones, or irritating or infectious materials; or by regular stimuli affecting organs that have been inactive for a while, like how the usual light of day hurts the eyes of those who have been locked away in dark places, and how the heat of a normal fire affects those who have been exposed to extreme cold.

But these new motions are never generated by that pain, which arises from defect of stimulus, as from hunger, thirst, cold, or inanition, with all those pains, which are termed nervous. Where these pains exist, the motions of the affected part are lessened; and if inflammation succeeds, it is in some distant parts; as coughs are caused by coldness and moisture being long applied to the feet; or it is in consequence of the renewal of the stimulus, as of heat or food, which excites our organs into stronger action after their temporary quiescence; as kibed heels after walking in snow.

But these new movements don't come from that pain, which comes from a lack of stimulation, like hunger, thirst, cold, or starvation, along with all those pains called nervous. When these pains are present, the movements of the affected area decrease; and if inflammation follows, it occurs in some distant areas, just like coughs can be caused by prolonged cold and dampness on the feet. Or it might happen because of the return of the stimulus, like heat or food, which energizes our organs into stronger activity after a brief rest, similar to sore heels after walking in the snow.

4. But when these new motions of the vascular muscles are exerted with greater violence, and these vessels are either elongated too much or too hastily, a new material is secreted from their extremities, which is of various kinds according to the peculiar animal motions of this new kind of gland, which secretes it; such is the pus laudabile or common matter, the variolous matter, venereal matter, catarrhous matter, and many others.

4. However, when these new movements of the blood vessels are pushed to extremes, and these vessels are either stretched too much or too quickly, a new substance is produced at their ends. This substance varies depending on the specific animal motions of this new type of gland that produces it; examples include good pus or common matter, variola matter, venereal matter, catarrhal matter, and several others.

5. These matters are the product of an animal process; they are secreted or produced from the blood by certain diseased motions of the extremities of the blood-vessels, and are on that account all of them contagious; for if a portion of any of these matters is transmitted into the circulation, or perhaps only inserted into the skin, or beneath the cuticle of an healthy person, its stimulus in a certain time produces the same kind of morbid motions, by which itself was produced; and hence a similar kind is generated. See Sect. XXXIX. 6. 1.

5. These matters are the product of an animal process; they are secreted or produced from the blood by certain diseased motions of the extremities of the blood-vessels, and are on that account all of them contagious; for if a portion of any of these matters is transmitted into the circulation, or perhaps only inserted into the skin, or beneath the cuticle of an healthy person, its stimulus in a certain time produces the same kind of morbid motions, by which itself was produced; and hence a similar kind is generated. See Sect. XXXIX. 6. 1.

6. It is remarkable, that many of these contagious matters are capable of producing a similar disease but once; as the small-pox and measles; and I suppose this is true of all those contagious diseases, which are spontaneously cured by nature in a certain time; for if the body was capable of receiving the disease a second time, the patient must perpetually infect himself by the very matter, which he has himself produced, and is lodged about him; and hence he could never become free from the disease. Something similar to this is seen in the secondary fever of the confluent small-pox; there is a great absorption of variolous matter, a very minute part of which would give the genuine small-pox to another person; but here it only stimulates the system into common fever; like that which common puss, or any other acrid material might occasion.

6. It's remarkable that many of these contagious illnesses can only cause similar diseases once, like smallpox and measles. I think this applies to all contagious diseases that our bodies can naturally cure over time. If the body could catch the disease again, the patient would constantly reinfect themselves with the very matter they produced, which would be around them, so they could never really get rid of the disease. A similar situation can be seen in the secondary fever associated with confluent smallpox; there's a significant absorption of variolous matter, a tiny bit of which could cause genuine smallpox in someone else. But in this case, it just triggers the body to experience a common fever, similar to what common pus or any other irritating substance might cause.

7. In the pulmonary consumption, where common matter is daily absorbed, an irritative fever only, without new inflammation, is generally produced; which is terminated like other irritative fevers by sweats, or loose stools. Hence it does not appear, that this absorbed matter always acts as a contagious material producing fresh inflammation or new abscesses. Though there is reason to believe, that the first time any common matter is absorbed, it has this effect, but not the second time, like the variolous matter above mentioned.

7. In pulmonary consumption, where common substances are absorbed daily, an irritating fever typically occurs, without causing new inflammation; this usually resolves like other irritating fevers through sweating or loose stools. Therefore, it seems that this absorbed material doesn't always act as a contagious factor, leading to fresh inflammation or new abscesses. However, there's reason to believe that the first time any common substance is absorbed, it has this effect, but not the second time, similar to the variolous material mentioned earlier.

This accounts for the opinion, that the pulmonary consumption is sometimes infectious, which opinion was held by the ancients, and continues in Italy at present; and I have myself seen three or four instances, where a husband and wife, who have slept together, and have thus much received each other's breath, who have infected each other, and both died in consequence of the original taint of only one of them. This also accounts for the abscesses in various parts of the body, that are sometimes produced after the inoculated small-pox is terminated; for this second absorption of variolous matter acts like common matter, and produces only irritative fever in those children, whose constitutions have already experienced the absorption of common matter; and inflammation with a tendency to produce new abscesses in those, whose constitutions have not experienced the absorptions of common matter.

This explains the belief that tuberculosis can sometimes be contagious, a view held by ancient scholars and still present in Italy today. I've personally seen three or four cases where a husband and wife who slept together shared each other’s breath, infected one another, and both died due to the initial illness of just one of them. This also explains the abscesses that can occur in various parts of the body after smallpox vaccination has been completed. The second absorption of the infectious material acts like regular matter, causing only irritative fever in children whose bodies have already dealt with common infectious material, and leading to inflammation with a possibility of new abscesses in those whose bodies have not dealt with such material before.

It is probable, that more certain proofs might have been found to shew, that common matter is infectious the first time it is absorbed, tending to produce similar abscesses, but not the second time of its absorption, if this subject had been attended to.

It’s likely that clearer evidence could have been found to show that common matter is infectious the first time it’s absorbed, leading to similar abscesses, but not the second time it’s absorbed, if this topic had been given more attention.

8. These contagious diseases are very numerous, as the plague, small-pox, chicken-pox, measles, scarlet-fever, pemphigus, catarrh, chincough, venereal disease, itch, trichoma, tinea. The infectious material does not seem to be dissolved by the air, but only mixed with it perhaps in fine powder, which soon subsides; since many of these contagions can only be received by actual contact; and others of them only at small distances from the infected person; as is evident from many persons having been near patients of the small-pox without acquiring the disease.

8. There are many contagious diseases, like the plague, smallpox, chickenpox, measles, scarlet fever, pemphigus, colds, whooping cough, sexually transmitted infections, scabies, trachoma, and ringworm. The infectious material doesn’t seem to dissolve in the air but rather mixes with it, possibly in fine particles that quickly settle. Many of these infections can only be transmitted through direct contact, while others can be spread only at close distances from the infected person. This is clear from the fact that many people have been near smallpox patients without getting the disease.

The reason, why many of these diseases are received but once, and others repeatedly, is not well understood; it appears to me, that the constitution becomes so accustomed to the stimuli of these infectious materials, by having once experienced them, that though irritative motions, as hectic fevers, may again be produced by them, yet no sensation, and in consequence no general inflammation succeeds; as disagreeable smells or tastes by habit cease to be perceived; they continue indeed to excite irritative ideas on the organs of sense, but these are not succeeded by sensation.

The reason why some diseases are caught just once while others are caught multiple times isn’t fully understood. It seems to me that the body becomes so familiar with the triggers of these infections after experiencing them once that, even if irritating symptoms like fevers can occur again, no actual sensation or general inflammation follows. Just as unpleasant smells or tastes eventually stop being noticeable, these infections might still provoke irritating thoughts in our sensory organs, but they don’t lead to any sensation.

There are many irritative motions, which were at first succeeded by sensation, but which by frequent repetition cease to excite sensation, as explained in Sect. XX. on Vertigo. And, that this circumstance exists in respect to infectious matter appears from a known fact; that nurses, who have had the small-pox, are liable to experience small ulcers on their arms by the contact of variolous matter in lifting their patients; and that when patients, who have formerly had the small-pox have been inoculated in the arm, a phlegmon, or inflamed sore, has succeeded, but no subsequent fever. Which shews, that the contagious matter of the small-pox has not lost its power of stimulating the part it is applied to, but that the general system is not affected in consequence. See Section XII. 7. 6. XIX. 9.

There are many irritative motions, which were at first succeeded by sensation, but which by frequent repetition cease to excite sensation, as explained in Sect. XX. on Vertigo. And, that this circumstance exists in respect to infectious matter appears from a known fact; that nurses, who have had the small-pox, are liable to experience small ulcers on their arms by the contact of variolous matter in lifting their patients; and that when patients, who have formerly had the small-pox have been inoculated in the arm, a phlegmon, or inflamed sore, has succeeded, but no subsequent fever. Which shews, that the contagious matter of the small-pox has not lost its power of stimulating the part it is applied to, but that the general system is not affected in consequence. See Section XII. 7. 6. XIX. 9.

9. From the accounts of the plague, virulent catarrh, and putrid dysentery, it seems uncertain, whether these diseases are experienced more than once; but the venereal disease and itch are doubtless repeatedly infectious; and as these diseases are never cured spontaneously, but require medicines, which act without apparent operation, some have suspected, that the contagious material produces similar matter rather by a chemical change of the fluids, than by an animal process; and that the specific medicines destroy their virus by chemically combining with it. This opinion is successfully combated by Mr. Hunter, in his Treatise on Venereal Disease, Part I. c. i.

9. Based on the reports of the plague, severe respiratory infections, and severe dysentery, it's unclear whether these illnesses occur more than once; however, it's certain that sexually transmitted infections and scabies are definitely contagious multiple times. Since these diseases never go away on their own and need medicines that work without obvious effects, some have theorized that the contagious agents create similar matter through a chemical change in the fluids rather than a biological process. They believe that the specific medications eliminate their viruses by chemically interacting with them. This view is effectively challenged by Mr. Hunter in his Treatise on Venereal Disease, Part I. c. i.

But this opinion wants the support of analogy, as there is no known process in animal bodies, which is purely chemical, not even digestion; nor can any of these matters be produced by chemical processes. Add to this, that it is probable, that the insects, observed in the pustules of the itch, and in the stools of dysenteric patients, are the consequences, and not the causes of these diseases. And that the specific medicines, which cure the itch and lues venerea, as brimstone and mercury, act only by increasing the absorption of the matter in the ulcuscles of those diseases, and thence disposing them to heal; which would otherwise continue to spread.

But this viewpoint needs support through analogy, since there's no known process in animal bodies that's purely chemical, not even digestion; nor can any of these substances be created through chemical processes. Furthermore, it's likely that the insects found in the pustules of scabies and in the stools of dysentery patients are effects, not causes of these illnesses. Additionally, the specific treatments that cure scabies and syphilis, like sulfur and mercury, work by enhancing the absorption of the matter in the ulcers of those diseases, which helps them heal; otherwise, they would just keep spreading.

Why the venereal disease, and itch, and tenia, or scald head, are repeatedly contagious, while those contagions attended with fever can be received but once, seems to depend on their being rather local diseases than universal ones, and are hence not attended with fever, except the purulent fever in their last stages, when the patient is destroyed by them. On this account the whole of the system does not become habituated to these morbid actions, so as to cease to be affected with sensation by a repetition of the contagion. Thus the contagious matter of the venereal disease, and of the tenia, affects the lymphatic glands, as the inquinal glands, and those about the roots of the hair and neck, where it is arrested, but does not seem to affect the blood-vessels, since no fever ensues.

Why the venereal disease, itch, and ringworm, or scalded head, are repeatedly contagious, while other infections that come with a fever can only be caught once, seems to depend on them being more local diseases than universal ones. Because of this, they don’t usually lead to fever, except for the purulent fever in their later stages, when the patient is severely affected. As a result, the entire system doesn’t become accustomed to these diseases, so they continue to produce symptoms each time the infection occurs. Thus, the contagious agents of venereal disease and ringworm impact the lymph nodes, like the inguinal nodes and those near the hair roots and neck, where they are halted, but they don’t seem to affect the blood vessels, since no fever develops.

Hence it would appear, that these kinds of contagion are propagated not by means of the circulation, but by sympathy of distant parts with each other; since if a distant part, as the palate, should be excited by sensitive association into the same kind of motions, as the parts originally affected by the contact of infectious matter; that distant part will produce the same kind of infectious matter; for every secretion from the blood is formed from it by the peculiar motions of the fine extremities of the gland, which secretes it; the various secreted fluids, as the bile, saliva, gastric juice, not previously existing, as such, in the blood-vessels.

So it seems that these types of contagion spread not through circulation but by the sympathetic connection between distant parts. If a remote area, like the palate, is stimulated through sensitive association to engage in the same motions as the originally affected areas that came into contact with infectious material, that distant area will create the same kind of infectious material. This happens because every secretion from the blood is formed through the specific motions of the tiny ends of the gland that secretes it; the various secreted fluids, like bile, saliva, and gastric juice, did not previously exist in that form in the blood vessels.

And this peculiar sympathy between the genitals and the throat, owing to sensitive association, appears not only in the production of venereal ulcers in the throat, but in variety of other instances, as in the mumps, in the hydrophobia, some coughs, strangulation, the production of the beard, change of voice at puberty. Which are further described in Class IV. 1. 2. 7.

And this strange connection between the genitals and the throat, due to sensitive associations, shows up not only in the formation of venereal ulcers in the throat but also in various other cases, like mumps, rabies, certain coughs, strangulation, beard growth, and voice changes during puberty. These are covered in Class IV. 1. 2. 7.

To evince that the production of such large quantities of contagious matter, as are seen in some variolous patients, so as to cover the whole skin almost with pustules, does not arise from any chemical fermentation in the blood, but that it is owing to morbid motions of the fine extremities of the capillaries, or glands, whether these be ruptured or not, appears from the quantity of this matter always corresponding with the quantity of the fever; that is, with the violent exertions of those glands and capillaries, which are the terminations of the arterial system.

To demonstrate that the production of such large amounts of contagious material, as seen in some smallpox patients, which can almost cover the entire skin with pustules, does not result from any chemical fermentation in the blood, but instead is due to unhealthy movements of the tiny ends of the capillaries or glands, whether they are ruptured or not, is evident from how the amount of this material always matches the intensity of the fever; that is, with the strong actions of those glands and capillaries, which are the end points of the arterial system.

The truth of this theory is evinced further by a circumstance observed by Mr. J. Hunter, in his Treatise on Venereal Disease; that in a patient, who was inoculated for the small-pox, and who appeared afterwards to have been previously infested with the measles, the progress of the small-pox was delayed till the measles had run their course, and that then the small-pox went through its usual periods.

The truth of this theory is further shown by an observation made by Mr. J. Hunter in his Treatise on Venereal Disease. He noted that in a patient inoculated for smallpox, who also seemed to have had measles beforehand, the progression of smallpox was delayed until the measles had completed its course, after which smallpox followed its usual timeline.

Two similar cases fell under my care, which I shall here relate, as it confirms that of Mr. Hunter, and contributes to illustrate this part of the theory of contagious diseases. I have transcribed the particulars from a letter of Mr. Lightwood of Yoxal, the surgeon who daily attended them, and at my request, after I had seen them, kept a kind of journal of their cases.

Two similar cases came to my attention, and I want to share them here because they support Mr. Hunter's case and help illustrate this aspect of contagious disease theory. I’ve copied the details from a letter by Mr. Lightwood of Yoxal, the surgeon who took care of them daily. At my request, after I had seen the patients, he kept a sort of journal of their cases.

Miss H. and Miss L. two sisters, the one about four and the other about three years old, were inoculated Feb. 7, 1791. On the 10th there was a redness on both arms discernible by a glass. On the 11th their arms were so much inflamed as to leave no doubt of the infection having taken place. On the 12th less appearance of inflammation on their arms. In the evening Miss L. had an eruption, which resembled the measles. On the 12th the eruption on Miss L. was very full on the face and breast, like the measles, with considerable fever. It was now known, that the measles were in a farm house in the neighbourhood. Miss H.'s arm less inflamed than yesterday. On the 14th Miss L.'s fever great, and the eruption universal. The arm appears to be healed. Miss H.'s arm somewhat redder. They were now put into separate rooms. On the 15th Miss L.'s arms as yesterday. Eruption continues. Miss H.'s arms have varied but little. 16th, the eruptions on Miss L. are dying away, her fever gone. Begins to have a little redness in one arm at the place of inoculation. Miss H.'s arms get redder, but she has no appearance of complaint. 20th, Miss L.'s arms have advanced slowly till this day, and now a few pustules appear. Miss H.'s arm has made little progress from the 16th to this day, and now she has some fever. 21st, Miss L. as yesterday. Miss H. has much inflammation, and an increase of the red circle on one arm to the size of half a crown, and had much fever at night, with fetid breath. 22d, Miss L.'s pustules continue advancing. Miss H.'s inflammation of her arm and red circle increases. A few red spots appear in different parts with some degree of fever this morning, 23d. Miss L. has a larger crop of pustules. Miss H. has small pustules and great inflammation of her arms, with but one pustule likely to suppurate. After this day they gradually got well, and the pustules disappeared.

Miss H. and Miss L., two sisters, one about four years old and the other about three, were vaccinated on February 7, 1791. By the 10th, there was noticeable redness on both of their arms when viewed through a glass. On the 11th, their arms became so inflamed that it was clear that the infection had occurred. On the 12th, there was less inflammation on their arms. In the evening, Miss L. developed a rash that looked like measles. By the 12th, Miss L.'s rash was quite pronounced on her face and chest, resembling measles, along with a significant fever. It became known that measles were present in a farmhouse nearby. Miss H.'s arm was less inflamed than the previous day. On the 14th, Miss L. had a high fever, and the rash was widespread. Miss H.'s arm appeared to have healed. Miss H.'s arm was slightly redder. They were moved into separate rooms. On the 15th, Miss L.'s arms looked the same as the day before. The rash continued. Miss H.'s arms showed little change. By the 16th, Miss L.'s rashes were starting to fade, and her fever was gone. She began to have a little redness at the inoculation site on one arm. Miss H.'s arms became redder, but she showed no signs of discomfort. On the 20th, Miss L.'s arms had progressed slowly until now, and a few pustules were visible. Miss H.'s arm had shown little progress since the 16th, and she now had a fever. On the 21st, Miss L. was the same as the previous day. Miss H. had significant inflammation and an enlarged red area on one arm, about the size of a half crown, and experienced a severe fever at night, along with foul breath. By the 22nd, Miss L.'s pustules continued to advance. Miss H.'s arm inflammation and red area increased. A few red spots appeared in different areas, accompanied by some fever that morning, the 23rd. Miss L. had a larger number of pustules. Miss H. had small pustules and significant arm inflammation, with only one pustule seeming likely to burst. After this day, they gradually recovered, and the pustules disappeared.

In one of these cases the measles went through their common course with milder symptoms than usual, and in the other the measly contagion seemed just sufficient to stop the progress of variolous contagion, but without itself throwing the constitution into any disorder. At the same time both the measles and small-pox seem to have been rendered milder. Does not this give an idea, that if they were both inoculated at the same time, that neither of them might affect the patient?

In one of these cases, measles followed its usual course but with milder symptoms than normal, while in the other case, the measles contagion seemed just enough to halt the progress of smallpox contagion, without causing any disruption to the person's health. At the same time, both measles and smallpox appeared to be less severe. Doesn't this suggest that if both were inoculated simultaneously, neither might impact the patient?

From these cases I contend, that the contagious matter of these diseases does not affect the constitution by a fermentation, or chemical change of the blood, because then they must have proceeded together, and have produced a third something, not exactly similar to either of them: but that they produce new motions of the cutaneous terminations of the blood-vessels, which for a time proceed daily with increasing activity, like some paroxysms of fever, till they at length secrete or form a similar poison by these unnatural actions.

From these cases, I argue that the contagious matter of these diseases doesn’t impact the body by causing a fermentation or chemical change in the blood, because then they would have developed simultaneously and created something that isn’t exactly like either of them. Instead, they trigger new movements in the skin endings of the blood vessels, which for a time increase daily in activity, similar to episodes of fever, until they eventually produce or create a similar poison through these unnatural actions.

Now as in the measles one kind of unnatural motion takes place, and in the small-pox another kind, it is easy to conceive, that these different kinds of morbid motions cannot exist together; and therefore, that that which has first begun will continue till the system becomes habituated to the stimulus which occasions it, and has ceased to be thrown into action by it; and then the other kind of stimulus will in its turn produce fever, and new kinds of motions peculiar to itself.

Now, just like with measles where one type of abnormal movement happens, and with smallpox where another kind occurs, it's easy to understand that these different types of unhealthy movements can't happen at the same time. So, whatever starts first will keep going until the body gets used to the trigger causing it and stops reacting to it. After that, a different trigger will cause fever and new types of movements specific to itself.

10. On further considering the action of contagious matter, since the former part of this work was sent to the press; where I have asserted, in Sect. XXII. 3. 3. that it is probable, that the variolous matter is diffused through the blood; I prevailed on my friend Mr. Power, surgeon at Bosworth in Leicestershire to try, whether the small-pox could be inoculated by using the blood of a variolous patient instead of the matter from the pustules; as I thought such an experiment might throw some light at least on this interesting subject. The following is an extract from his letter:—

10. On further considering the action of contagious matter, since the former part of this work was sent to the press; where I have asserted, in Sect. XXII. 3. 3. that it is probable, that the variolous matter is diffused through the blood; I prevailed on my friend Mr. Power, surgeon at Bosworth in Leicestershire to try, whether the small-pox could be inoculated by using the blood of a variolous patient instead of the matter from the pustules; as I thought such an experiment might throw some light at least on this interesting subject. The following is an extract from his letter:—

"March 11, 1793. I inoculated two children, who had not had the small-pox, with blood; which was taken from a patient on the second day after the eruption commenced, and before it was completed. And at the same time I inoculated myself with blood from the same person, in order to compare the appearances, which might arise in a person liable to receive the infection, and in one not liable to receive it. On the same day I inoculated four other children liable to receive the infection with blood taken from another person on the fourth day after the commencement of the eruption. The patients from whom the blood was taken had the disease mildly, but had the most pustules of any I could select from twenty inoculated patients; and as much of the blood was insinuated under the cuticle as I could introduce by elevating the skin without drawing blood; and three or four such punctures were made in each of their arms, and the blood was used in its fluid state.

"March 11, 1793. I inoculated two children who had not had smallpox with blood taken from a patient the day after the rash started and before it fully developed. At the same time, I inoculated myself with blood from the same person to compare the reactions in someone who can catch the infection and someone who can’t. On the same day, I also inoculated four other children who were at risk of catching the infection with blood taken from another person four days after the rash began. The patients from whom the blood was taken had a mild case of the disease but had the most pustules of any I could find among twenty inoculated patients. I introduced as much of the blood under the skin as I could without drawing blood by lifting the skin, making three or four such punctures in each of their arms, using the blood in its liquid form."

"As the appearances in all these patients, as well as in myself, were similar, I shall only mention them in general terms. March 13. A slight subcuticular discoloration, with rather a livid appearance, without soreness or pain, was visible in them all, as well as in my own hand. 15. The discoloration somewhat less, without pain or soreness. Some patients inoculated on the same day with variolous matter have considerable inflammation. 17. The discoloration is quite gone in them all, and from my own hand, a dry mark only remaining. And they were all inoculated on the 18th, with variolous matter, which produced the disease in them all."

"As the appearances in all these patients, as well as in myself, were similar, I will only mention them generally. March 13. There was a slight discoloration under the skin, with a somewhat bluish look, but no soreness or pain was noticeable in any of them, including my own hand. On the 15th, the discoloration was somewhat less, with no pain or soreness. Some patients who were inoculated on the same day with variolous matter experienced considerable inflammation. By the 17th, the discoloration had completely disappeared in all of them, leaving only a dry mark on my own hand. They were all inoculated on the 18th with variolous matter, which caused the disease in all of them."

Mr. Power afterwards observes, that, as the patients from whom the blood was taken had the disease mildly, it may be supposed, that though the contagious matter might be mixed with the blood, it might still be in too dilute a state to convey the infection; but adds at the same time, that he has diluted recent matter with at least five times its quantity of water, and which has still given the infection; though he has sometimes diluted it so far as to fail.

Mr. Power later notes that since the patients from whom the blood was taken had a mild case of the disease, it can be assumed that even if the contagious material was mixed with the blood, it might still be diluted enough that it wouldn't spread the infection. However, he also points out that he has diluted fresh material with at least five times its volume in water, and it still managed to spread the infection, although there were times when he diluted it so much that it didn’t work.

The following experiments were instituted at my request by my friend Mr. Hadley, surgeon in Derby, to ascertain whether the blood of a person in the small-pox be capable of communicating the disease. "Experiment 1st. October 18th, 1793. I took some blood from a vein in the arm of a person who had the small-pox, on the second day of the eruption, and introduced a small quantity of it immediately with the point of a lancet between the scars and true skin of the right arm of a boy nine years old in two or three different places; the other arm was inoculated with variolous matter at the same time.

The following experiments were carried out at my request by my friend Mr. Hadley, a surgeon in Derby, to determine whether the blood of someone with smallpox can transmit the disease. "Experiment 1st. October 18th, 1793. I took some blood from a vein in the arm of a person with smallpox, on the second day of the rash, and introduced a small amount of it immediately with the point of a lancet between the scars and the true skin of the right arm of a nine-year-old boy in two or three different places; the other arm was inoculated with variolous matter at the same time.

"19th. The punctured parts of the right arm were surrounded with some degree of subcuticular inflammation. 20th. The inflammation more considerable, with a slight degree of itching, but no pain upon pressure. 21st. Upon examining the arm this day with a lens I found the inflammation less extensive, and the redness changing to a deep yellow or orange-colour, 22d. Inflammation nearly gone. 23d. Nothing remained, except a slight discoloration and a little scurfy appearance on the punctures. At the same time the inflammation of the arm inoculated with variolous matter was increasing fast, and he had the disease mildly at the usual time.

"19th. The punctured areas on the right arm were showing some level of inflammation just under the skin. 20th. The inflammation got a bit worse, accompanied by slight itching, but there was no pain when pressure was applied. 21st. When I examined the arm today with a magnifying glass, I noticed the inflammation was less widespread, and the redness was turning a deep yellow or orange color. 22nd. The inflammation was almost completely gone. 23rd. There was nothing left except a slight discoloration and a bit of flaky skin where the punctures were. Meanwhile, the inflammation in the arm that was inoculated with the variolous matter was rapidly increasing, and he developed the disease mildly at the usual time."

"Experiment 2d. I inoculated another child at the same time and in the same manner, with blood taken on the first day of the eruption; but as the appearance and effects were similar to those in the preceding experiment, I shall not relate them minutely.

"Experiment 2d. I inoculated another child at the same time and in the same way, using blood taken on the first day of the outbreak; however, since the appearance and effects were similar to those in the previous experiment, I won't go into detail about them."

"Experiment 3d. October 20th. Blood was taken from a person who had the small-pox, on the third day of the eruption, and on the sixth from the commencement of the eruptive fever. I introduced some of it in its fluid state into both arms of a boy seven years old.

"Experiment 3d. October 20th. Blood was taken from a person who had smallpox on the third day of the rash and on the sixth day from the start of the fever. I injected some of it in liquid form into both arms of a seven-year-old boy."

21st. There appeared to be some inflammation under the cuticle, where the punctures were made. 22d. Inflammation more considerable. 23d. On this day the inflammation was somewhat greater, and the cuticle rather elevated.

21st. There seemed to be some swelling under the skin where the punctures were made. 22nd. The swelling was more significant. 23rd. On this day, the swelling was somewhat greater, and the skin was slightly raised.

"24th. Inflammation much less, and only a brown or orange-colour remained. 25th. Scarcely any discoloration left. On this day he was inoculated with variolous matter, the progress of the infection went on in the usual way, and he had the small-pox very favourably.

"24th. The inflammation had reduced significantly, with only a brown or orange color remaining. 25th. There was barely any discoloration left. On this day, he was inoculated with smallpox material, the infection progressed normally, and he had a mild case of smallpox."

"At this time I was requested to inoculate a young person, who was thought to have had the small-pox, but his parents were not quite certain; in one arm I introduced variolous matter, and in the other blood, taken as in experiment 3d. On the second day after the operation, the punctured parts were inflamed, though I think the arm in which I had inserted variolous matter was rather more so than the other. On the third the inflammation was increased, and looked much the same as in the preceding experiment. 4th. The inflammation was much diminished, and on the 5th almost gone. He was exposed at the same time to the natural infection, but has continued perfectly well.

"At that time, I was asked to vaccinate a young person who was thought to have had smallpox, but his parents weren't entirely sure. I introduced variolous material into one arm and blood, taken as in experiment 3, into the other. On the second day after the procedure, the punctured areas were inflamed, though I believe the arm where I had injected the variolous material was a bit more inflamed than the other. By the third day, the inflammation increased and looked quite similar to what I saw in the previous experiment. By the fourth day, the inflammation had decreased significantly, and by the fifth, it was nearly gone. He was exposed to the natural infection at the same time but remained completely healthy."

"I have frequently observed (and believe most practitioners have done the same), that if variolous matter be inserted in the arm of a person who has previously had the small-pox, that the inflammation on the second or third days is much greater, than if they had not had the disease, but on the fourth or fifth it disappears.

"I have often noticed (and I believe most practitioners have too) that if variolous matter is injected into the arm of someone who has previously had smallpox, the inflammation on the second or third day is much greater than if they hadn’t had the disease, but by the fourth or fifth day, it goes away."

"On the 23d I introduced blood into the arms of three more children, taken on the third and fourth days of the eruption. The appearances were much the same as mentioned in experiments first and third. They were afterwards inoculated with variolous matter, and had the disease in the regular way.

"On the 23rd, I injected blood into the arms of three more children, taken on the third and fourth days of the eruption. The results were very similar to what was observed in the first and third experiments. They were later inoculated with smallpox material and contracted the disease in the usual way."

"The above experiments were made with blood taken from a small vein in the hand or foot of three or four different patients, whom I had at that time under inoculation. They were selected from 160, as having the greatest number of pustules. The part was washed with warm water before the blood was taken, to prevent the possibility of any matter being mixed with it from the surface."

"The above experiments were conducted using blood drawn from a small vein in the hand or foot of three or four different patients who were undergoing inoculation at that time. They were chosen from 160 patients because they had the highest number of pustules. The area was cleaned with warm water before the blood was drawn to avoid any contamination from the surface."

Shall we conclude from hence, that the variolous matter never enters the blood-vessels? but that the morbid motions of the vessels of the skin around the insertion of it continue to increase in a larger and larger circle for six or seven days; that then their quantity of morbid action becomes great enough to produce a fever-fit, and to affect the stomach by association of motions? and finally, that a second association of motions is produced between the stomach and the other parts of the skin, inducing them into morbid actions similar to those of the circle round the insertion of the variolous matter? Many more experiments and observations are required before this important question can be satisfactorily answered.

Should we conclude from this that the variolous matter never enters the blood vessels? Instead, the abnormal activity of the skin’s vessels around the injection site keeps spreading in a wider circle for about six or seven days. Then, the level of this abnormal activity becomes high enough to trigger a fever and affect the stomach due to related movements. Finally, a second set of related movements occurs between the stomach and other parts of the skin, leading them to develop abnormal activities similar to those around the injection site of the variolous matter. We need many more experiments and observations before we can satisfactorily answer this important question.

It may be adduced, that as the matter inserted into the skin of the arm frequently swells the lymphatic in the axilla, that in that circumstance it seems to be there arrested in its progress, and cannot be imagined to enter the blood by that lymphatic gland till the swelling of it subsides. Some other phænomena of the disease are more easily reconcileable to this theory of sympathetic motions than to that of absorption; as the time taken up between the insertion of the matter, and the operation of it on the system, as mentioned above. For the circle around the insertion is seen to increase, and to inflame; and I believe, undergoes a kind of diurnal paroxysm of torpor and paleness with a succeeding increase of action and colour, like a topical fever-fit. Whereas if the matter is conceived to circulate for six or seven days with the blood, without producing disorder, it ought to be rendered milder, or the blood-vessels more familiarized to its acrimony.

It can be argued that when the substance is injected into the skin of the arm, it often causes the lymphatic gland in the armpit to swell. In this case, it seems to get stuck there and can't be considered to enter the bloodstream through that lymphatic gland until the swelling goes down. Some other symptoms of the disease are more easily explained by this theory of sympathetic reactions than by that of absorption. This includes the time taken from the injection to the effect on the body, as noted earlier. The area around the injection site is seen to enlarge and become inflamed, and I believe it goes through a sort of daily cycle of numbness and paleness followed by increased activity and redness, similar to a local fever. On the other hand, if the substance is thought to circulate with the blood for six or seven days without causing any issues, it should become less harsh, or the blood vessels should become more accustomed to its irritating nature.

It is much easier to conceive from this doctrine of associated or sympathetic motions of distant parts of the system, how it happens, that the variolous infection can be received but once, as before explained; than by supposing, that a change is effected in the mass of blood by any kind of fermentative process.

It’s much easier to understand from this idea of connected or sympathetic movements of distant parts of the system how it is possible to catch the smallpox infection only once, as explained earlier, than by assuming that some kind of fermentative process changes the blood mass.

The curious circumstance of the two contagions of small-pox and measles not acting at the same time, but one of them resting or suspending its action till that of the other ceases, may be much easier explained from sympathetic or associated actions of the infected part with other parts of the system, than it can from supposing the two contagions to enter the circulation.

The interesting fact that smallpox and measles don’t occur at the same time, with one pausing or holding off until the other is over, can be better understood through the sympathetic or related reactions of the infected area with other parts of the body, rather than thinking that both contagions enter the bloodstream at the same time.

The skin of the face is subject to more frequent vicissitudes of heat and cold, from its exposure to the open air, and is in consequence more liable to sensitive association with the stomach than any other part of the surface of the body, because their actions have been more frequently thus associated. Thus in a surfeit from drinking cold water, when a person is very hot and fatigued, an eruption is liable to appear on the face in consequence of this sympathy. In the same manner the rosy eruption on the faces of drunkards more probably arises from the sympathy of the face with the stomach, rather than between the face and the liver, as is generally supposed.

The skin on the face is exposed to more changes in temperature from being outdoors, making it more sensitive and connected to the stomach than any other part of the body. This is because their functions are often linked. For example, if someone drinks cold water while feeling very hot and tired, they might develop a rash on their face due to this connection. Similarly, the rosy complexion often seen on drunk people likely comes from the connection between the face and the stomach, not the face and the liver, as people usually think.

This sympathy between the stomach and the skin of the face is apparent in the eruption of the small-pox; since, where the disease is in considerable quantity, the eruption on the face first succeeds the sickness of the stomach. In the natural disease the stomach seems to be frequently primarily affected, either alone or along with the tonsils, as the matter seems to be only diffused in the air, and by being mixed with the saliva, or mucus of the tonsils, to be swallowed into the stomach.

This connection between the stomach and the skin of the face is clear in the outbreak of smallpox; when the disease is severe, the rash on the face usually appears after the stomach issues. In the natural course of the disease, the stomach often seems to be affected first, either alone or along with the tonsils, as the infection seems to spread through the air and is mixed with saliva or mucus from the tonsils, which is then swallowed into the stomach.

After some days the irritative circles of motions become disordered by this new stimulus, which acts upon the mucus lining of the stomach; and sickness, vertigo, and a diurnal fever succeed. These disordered irritative motions become daily increased for two or three days, and then by their increased action certain sensitive motions, or inflammation, is produced, and at the next cold fit of fever, when the stomach recovers from its torpor, an inflammation of the external skin is formed in points (which afterwards suppurate), by sensitive association, in the same manner as a cough is produced in consequence of exposing the feet to cold, as described in Sect. XXV. 17. and Class IV. 2. I. 7. If the inoculated skin of the arm, as far as it appears inflamed, was to be cut out, or destroyed by caustic, before the fever commenced, as suppose on the fourth day after inoculation, would this prevent the disease? as it is supposed to prevent the hydrophobia.

After some days the irritative circles of motions become disordered by this new stimulus, which acts upon the mucus lining of the stomach; and sickness, vertigo, and a diurnal fever succeed. These disordered irritative motions become daily increased for two or three days, and then by their increased action certain sensitive motions, or inflammation, is produced, and at the next cold fit of fever, when the stomach recovers from its torpor, an inflammation of the external skin is formed in points (which afterwards suppurate), by sensitive association, in the same manner as a cough is produced in consequence of exposing the feet to cold, as described in Sect. XXV. 17. and Class IV. 2. I. 7. If the inoculated skin of the arm, as far as it appears inflamed, was to be cut out, or destroyed by caustic, before the fever commenced, as suppose on the fourth day after inoculation, would this prevent the disease? as it is supposed to prevent the hydrophobia.

III. 1. Where the new vessels, and enlarged old ones, which constitute inflammation, are not so hastily distended as to burst, and form a new kind of gland for the secretion of matter, as above mentioned; if such circumstances happen as diminish the painful sensation, the tendency to growth ceases, and by and by an absorption commences, not only of the superabundant quantity of fluids deposited in the inflamed part, but of the solids likewise, and this even of the hardest kind.

III. 1. Where the new blood vessels and enlarged old ones that cause inflammation are not stretched quickly enough to burst and form a new type of gland for releasing substances, as mentioned above; if conditions arise that lessen the painful sensation, the growth trend stops, and eventually absorption begins, not just of the excess fluids in the inflamed area, but also of the solid tissue, even the hardest types.

Thus during the growth of the second set of teeth in children, the roots of the first set are totally absorbed, till at length nothing of them remains but the crown; though a few weeks before, if they are drawn immaturely, their roots are found complete. Similar to this Mr. Hunter has observed, that where a dead piece of bone is to exfoliate, or to separate from a living one, that the dead part does not putrify, but remains perfectly sound, while the surface of the living part of the bone, which is in contact with the dead part, becomes absorbed, and thus effects its separation. Med. Comment. Edinb. V. 1. 425. In the same manner the calcareous matter of gouty concretions, the coagulable lymph deposited on inflamed membranes in rheumatism and extravasated blood become absorbed; which are all as solid and as indissoluble materials as the new vessels produced in inflammation.

During the growth of the second set of teeth in children, the roots of the first set are completely absorbed, leaving only the crowns behind; however, a few weeks earlier, if they are prematurely extracted, the roots are fully intact. Similarly, Mr. Hunter has noted that when a dead piece of bone is ready to come off or separate from a living one, the dead part doesn’t decay but stays perfectly sound, while the surface of the living bone in contact with the dead part gets absorbed, facilitating the separation. Med. Comment. Edinb. V. 1. 425. In the same way, the calcium deposits in gouty lumps, the coagulable lymph found on inflamed membranes in rheumatism, and extravasated blood are also absorbed; all of these are as solid and as durable as the new blood vessels formed during inflammation.

This absorption of the new vessels and deposited fluids of inflamed parts is called resolution: it is produced by first using such internal means as decrease the pain of the part, and in consequence its new motions, as repeated bleeding, cathartics, diluent potations, and warm bath.

This process of absorbing the new vessels and fluids in inflamed areas is called resolution. It happens by first using internal methods that reduce the pain in that area, which in turn lessens its new movements, such as repeated bloodletting, laxatives, soothing drinks, and warm baths.

After the vessels are thus emptied, and the absorption of the new vessels and deposited fluids is evidently begun, it is much promoted by stimulating the part externally by solutions of lead, or other metals, and internally by the bark, and small doses of opium. Hence when an ophthalmy begins to become paler, any acrid eye-water, as a solution of six grains of white vitriol in an ounce of water, hastens the absorption, and clears the eye in a very short time. But the same application used a few days sooner would have increased the inflammation. Hence after evacuation opium in small doses may contribute to promote the absorption of fluids deposited on the brain, as observed by Mr. Bromfield in his treatise of surgery.

After the vessels are emptied and the absorption of the new vessels and fluids is clearly starting, this process is enhanced by applying solutions of lead or other metals to the outside, and using bark and small doses of opium internally. So, when an eye infection starts to fade, using an irritating eye wash, like a solution of six grains of white vitriol in an ounce of water, speeds up the absorption and clears the eye quickly. However, using the same treatment a few days earlier would have worsened the inflammation. Therefore, after draining, small doses of opium may help promote the absorption of fluids that have built up on the brain, as noted by Mr. Bromfield in his surgery treatise.

2. Where an abscess is formed by the rupture of these new vessels, the violence of inflammation ceases, and a new gland separates a material called pus: at the same time a less degree of inflammation produces new vessels called vulgarly proud flesh; which, if no bandage confines its growth, nor any other circumstance promotes absorption in the wound, would rise to a great height above the usual size of the part.

2. When an abscess forms due to the breaking of these new blood vessels, the intense inflammation stops, and a new gland produces a substance known as pus. Meanwhile, a milder form of inflammation leads to the creation of new vessels commonly referred to as proud flesh; if no bandage restricts its growth and no other factors encourage absorption in the wound, it could grow significantly larger than the normal size of the area.

Hence the art of healing ulcers consists in producing a tendency to absorption in the wound greater than the deposition. Thus when an ill-conditioned ulcer separates a copious and thin discharge, by the use of any stimulus, as of salts of lead, or mercury, or copper externally applied, the discharge becomes diminished in quantity, and becomes thicker, as the thinner parts are first absorbed.

Hence, the art of treating ulcers is about encouraging the wound to absorb more than it creates. So, when a poorly healed ulcer produces a lot of thin discharge, using any stimulating substance like lead, mercury, or copper applied on the outside can reduce the amount of discharge and make it thicker, as the thinner parts are absorbed first.

But nothing so much contributes to increase the absorption in a wound as covering the whole limb above the sore with a bandage, which should be spread with some plaster, as with emplastrum de minio, to prevent it from slipping. By this artificial tightness of the skin, the arterial pulsations act with double their usual power in promoting the ascending current of the fluid in the valvular lymphatics.

But nothing helps increase the absorption in a wound as much as covering the entire limb above the sore with a bandage, which should be coated with some ointment, like emplastrum de minio, to keep it from slipping. This artificial tightness of the skin allows the arterial pulsations to work twice as effectively in promoting the upward flow of fluid in the valvular lymphatics.

Internally the absorption from ulcers should be promoted first by evacuation, then by opium, bark, mercury, steel.

Internally, the absorption from ulcers should first be encouraged by draining them, followed by the use of opium, bark, mercury, and steel.

3. Where the inflammation proceeds with greater violence or rapidity, that is, when by the painful sensation a more inordinate activity of the organ is produced, and by this great activity an additional quantity of painful sensation follows in an increasing ratio, till the whole of the sensorial power, or spirit of animation, in the part becomes exhausted, a mortification ensues, as in a carbuncle, in inflammations of the bowels, in the extremities of old people, or in the limbs of those who are brought near a fire after having been much benumbed with cold. And from hence it appears, why weak people are more subject to mortification than strong ones, and why in weak persons less pain will produce mortification, namely, because the sensorial power is sooner exhausted by any excess of activity. I remember seeing a gentleman who had the preceding day travelled two stages in a chaise with what he termed a bearable pain in his bowels; which when I saw him had ceased rather suddenly, and without a passage through him; his pulse was then weak, though not very quick; but as nothing which he swallowed would continue in his stomach many minutes, I concluded that the bowel was mortified; he died on the next day. It is usual for patients sinking under the small-pox with mortified pustules, and with purple spots intermixed, to complain of no pain, but to say they are pretty well to the last moment.

3. When inflammation escalates rapidly or violently, causing intense pain, it creates excessive activity in the affected area. This increased activity results in even more pain, which intensifies until the sensory power, or life force, in that part becomes depleted, leading to tissue death, as seen in conditions like carbuncles, bowel inflammations, in the limbs of elderly individuals, or in those whose limbs become numb from cold and are then exposed to heat. This explains why weaker individuals are more prone to tissue death than stronger ones, and why even minor pain can cause it in them—because their sensory power is exhausted more quickly due to the heightened activity. I recall seeing a man who had traveled a couple of stages in a carriage the day before, experiencing what he called a manageable pain in his abdomen. When I saw him, the pain had suddenly stopped without any relief; his pulse was weak but not very fast. However, anything he swallowed didn’t stay in his stomach for long, leading me to conclude that his bowel was dead; he passed away the next day. It’s common for patients suffering from smallpox with dead pustules and purple spots to report no pain and to claim they feel fine right up to the end.

Recapitulation.

Summary.

IV. When the motions of any part of the system, in consequence of previous torpor, are performed with more energy than in the irritative fevers, a disagreeable sensation is produced, and new actions of some part of the system commence in consequence of this sensation conjointly with the irritation: which motions constitute inflammation. If the fever be attended with a strong pulse, as in pleurisy, or rheumatism, it is termed synocha sensitiva, or sensitive fever with strong pulse; which is usually termed inflammatory fever. If it be attended with weak pulse, it is termed typhus sensitivus, or sensitive fever with weak pulse, or typhus gravior, or putrid malignant fever.

IV. When any part of the system moves with more energy due to previous inactivity, it creates an unpleasant sensation, and new actions in some part of the system begin as a result of this sensation along with the irritation. These movements are what we call inflammation. If the fever is accompanied by a strong pulse, like in pleurisy or rheumatism, it’s referred to as synocha sensitiva, or sensitive fever with a strong pulse; this is commonly known as inflammatory fever. If it comes with a weak pulse, it’s called typhus sensitivus, or sensitive fever with a weak pulse, or typhus gravior, or putrid malignant fever.

The synocha sensitiva, or sensitive fever with strong pulse, is generally attended with some topical inflammation, as in peripneumony, hepatitis, and is accompanied with much coagulable lymph, or size; which rises to the surface of the blood, when taken into a bason, as it cools; and which is believed to be the increased mucous secretion from the coats of the arteries, inspissated by a greater absorption of its aqueous and saline part, and perhaps changed by its delay in the circulation.

The synocha sensitiva, or sensitive fever with a rapid pulse, usually comes with some local inflammation, like in pneumonia or hepatitis, and is associated with a lot of coagulable lymph, or serum; this rises to the surface of the blood when poured into a basin as it cools. It’s thought to be an increased mucous secretion from the artery walls, thickened by greater absorption of its watery and salty components, and possibly altered by being slowed down in circulation.

The typhus sensitivus, or sensitive fever with weak pulse, is frequently attended with delirium, which is caused by the deficiency of the quantity of sensorial power, and with variety of cutaneous eruptions.

The typhus sensitivus, or sensitive fever with a weak pulse, often comes with delirium, which is caused by a lack of sensorial power, and a variety of skin rashes.

Inflammation is caused by the pains occasioned by excess of action, and not by those pains which are occasioned by defect of action. These morbid actions, which are thus produced by two sensorial powers, viz. by irritation and sensation, secrete new living fibres, which elongate the old vessels, or form new ones, and at the same time much heat is evolved from these combinations. By the rupture of these vessels, or by a new construction of their apertures, purulent matters are secreted of various kinds; which are infectious the first time they are applied to the skin beneath the cuticle, or swallowed with the saliva into the stomach. This contagion acts not by its being absorbed into the circulation, but by the sympathies, or associated actions, between the part first stimulated by the contagious matter and the other parts of the system. Thus in the natural small-pox the contagion is swallowed with the saliva, and by its stimulus inflames the stomach; this variolous inflammation of the stomach increases every day, like the circle round the puncture of an inoculated arm, till it becomes great enough to disorder the circles of irritative and sensitive motions, and thus produces fever-fits, with sickness and vomiting. Lastly, after the cold paroxysm, or fit of torpor, of the stomach has increased for two or three successive days, an inflammation of the skin commences in points; which generally first appear upon the face, as the associated actions between the skin of the face and that of the stomach have been more frequently exerted together than those of any other parts of the external surface.

Inflammation is caused by pain from too much activity, rather than pain from too little activity. These harmful reactions arise from two sensory functions, namely irritation and sensation, which create new living fibers that stretch existing blood vessels or form new ones, while generating a lot of heat in the process. When these vessels rupture or their openings change, they produce various types of pus that can be infectious the first time they come into contact with the skin beneath the outer layer or are swallowed with saliva into the stomach. This contagion does not spread by being absorbed into the bloodstream, but through the connections and reactions between the initially affected area and the rest of the body. For instance, with natural smallpox, the contagion is taken in with saliva and triggers inflammation in the stomach; this variolous inflammation in the stomach grows daily, much like the expanding circle around an inoculated arm, until it disrupts both the irritative and sensitive motions, resulting in fever, nausea, and vomiting. Finally, after the cold spells or periods of stillness in the stomach have lasted for two or three days, inflammation of the skin begins in small spots, usually first appearing on the face, since the interactions between the skin of the face and that of the stomach have been more frequently activated together than those of any other parts of the body's surface.

Contagious matters, as those of the measles and small-pox, do not act upon the system at the same time; but the progress of that which was last received is delayed, till the action of the former infection ceases. All kinds of matter, even that from common ulcers, are probably contagious the first time they are inserted beneath the cuticle or swallowed into the stomach; that is, as they were formed by certain morbid actions of the extremities of the vessels, they have the power to excite similar morbid actions in the extremities of other vessels, to which they are applied; and these by sympathy, or associations of motion, excite similar morbid actions in distant parts of the system, without entering the circulation; and hence the blood of a patient in the small-pox will not give that disease by inoculation to others.

Contagious diseases like measles and smallpox don't affect the body simultaneously; instead, the progress of the most recent infection is slowed down until the effects of the earlier one are over. All types of matter, even that from common sores, are likely contagious the first time they come into contact with the skin or are swallowed. This is because they were created by specific abnormal activities in the blood vessels' outer areas, and they can trigger similar abnormal reactions in the outer areas of other vessels they come into contact with. These reactions can then cause similar abnormal activities in other parts of the body through connections or patterns of movement, without entering the bloodstream. This is why a person with smallpox cannot transmit the disease to others through inoculation.

When the new fibres or vessels become again absorbed into the circulation, the inflammation ceases; which is promoted, after sufficient evacuations, by external stimulants and bandages: but where the action of the vessels is very great, a mortification of the part is liable to ensue, owing to the exhaustion of sensorial power; which however occurs in weak people without much pain, and without very violent previous inflammation; and, like partial paralysis, may be esteemed one mode of natural death of old people, a part dying before the whole.

When the new fibers or vessels are reabsorbed into the bloodstream, the inflammation stops. This is aided by external stimulants and bandages after sufficient drainage. However, if the activity of the vessels is too intense, it can lead to the tissue dying due to depletion of nerve energy. This can happen in frail individuals without much pain and without severe prior inflammation. Similar to partial paralysis, it can be seen as a form of natural death in older people, where a part of the body dies before the rest.



SECT. XXXIV.

DISEASES OF VOLITION.

Willingness Disorders.

I. 1. Volition defined. Motions termed involuntary are caused by volition. Desires opposed to each other. Deliberation. Ass between two hay-cocks. Saliva swallowed against one's desire. Voluntary motions distinguished from those associated with sensitive motions. 2. Pains from excess, and from defect of motion. No pain is felt during vehement voluntary exertion; as in cold fits of ague, labour-pains, strangury, tenesmus, vomiting, restlessness in fevers, convulsion of a wounded muscle. 3. Of holding the breath and screaming in pain; why swine and dogs cry out in pain, and not sheep and horses. Of grinning and biting in pain; why mad animals bite others. 4. Epileptic convulsions explained, why the fits begin with quivering of the under jaw, biting the tongue, and setting the teeth; why the convulsive motions are alternately relaxed. The phenomenon of laughter explained. Why children cannot tickle themselves. How some have died from immoderate laughter. 5. Of cataleptic spasms, of the locked jaw, of painful cramps. 6. Syncope explained. Why no external objects are perceived in syncope. 7. Of palsy and apoplexy from violent exertions. Case of Mrs. Scot. From dancing, scating, swimming. Case of Mr. Nairn. Why palsies are not always immediately preceded by violent exertions. Palsy and epilepsy from diseased livers. Why the right arm more frequently paralytic than the left. How paralytic limbs regain their motions. II. Diseases of the sensual motions from excess or defect of voluntary exertion. 1. Madness. 2. Distinguished from delirium. 3. Why mankind more liable to insanity than brutes. 4. Suspicion. Want of shame, and of cleanliness. 5. They bear cold, hunger, and fatigue. Charles XII. of Sweden. 6. Pleasureable delirium, and insanity. Child riding on a stick. Pains of martyrdom not felt. 7. Dropsy. 8. Inflammation cured by insanity. III. 1. Pain relieved by reverie. Reverie is an exertion of voluntary and sensitive motions. 2. Case of reverie. 3. Lady supposed to have two souls. 4. Methods of relieving pain.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Defining volition. Movements labeled as involuntary arise from volition. Conflicting desires. Thought process. A dilemma between two haystacks. Swallowing saliva against one's will. Distinguishing voluntary movements from those related to sensations. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Pains from overexertion and from lack of movement. No pain is experienced during intense voluntary activity; such as during severe chills from fever, labor pains, urinary discomfort, bowel straining, vomiting, and restlessness during fever, or muscle spasms from an injury. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Discussion on breath holding and screaming in pain; why pigs and dogs cry out in pain, but sheep and horses do not. About grimacing and biting in response to pain; why mad animals may attack others. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Explaining epileptic seizures, including why they start with jaw trembling, tongue biting, and teeth clenching; why the convulsive motions relax alternately. Explanation of laughter. Why children can't tickle themselves. Cases where individuals have died from excessive laughter. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. On cataleptic spasms, locked jaw, and painful cramps. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. The phenomenon of syncope explained. Why no external stimuli are registered during syncope. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Discussing paralysis and strokes resulting from intense physical activity. Case of Mrs. Scot. Related to dancing, skating, and swimming. Case of Mr. Nairn. Reasons why paralysis isn't always preceded by vigorous exertion. Paralysis and epilepsy linked to liver diseases. Reasons why the right arm is more often paralyzed than the left. How paralyzed limbs regain movement. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Conditions affecting sensory movements due to excessive or insufficient voluntary activity. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Insanity. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Differentiated from delirium. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. Reasons why humans are more prone to insanity than animals. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. Paranoia. Lack of shame and hygiene. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. How they endure cold, hunger, and fatigue. Charles XII of Sweden. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. Pleasurable delirium and insanity. A child playing on a stick. The pains of martyrdom are not felt. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. Edema. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. Inflammation treated through insanity. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_18__. Pain alleviated through daydreaming. Daydreaming is a combination of voluntary and sensory movements. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_19__. A case study on daydreaming. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_20__. A woman believed to possess two souls. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_21__. Ways to alleviate pain.

I. 1. Before we commence this Section on Diseased Voluntary Motions, it may be necessary to premise, that the word volition is not used in this work exactly in its common acceptation. Volition is said in Section V. to bear the same analogy to desire and aversion, which sensation does to pleasure and pain. And hence that, when desire or aversion produces any action of the muscular fibres, or of the organs of sense, they are termed volition; and the actions produced in consequence are termed voluntary actions. Whence it appears, that motions of our muscles or ideas may be produced in consequence of desire or aversion without our having the power to prevent them, and yet these motions may be termed voluntary, according to our definition of the word; though in common language they would be called involuntary.

I. 1. Before we commence this Section on Diseased Voluntary Motions, it may be necessary to premise, that the word volition is not used in this work exactly in its common acceptation. Volition is said in Section V. to bear the same analogy to desire and aversion, which sensation does to pleasure and pain. And hence that, when desire or aversion produces any action of the muscular fibres, or of the organs of sense, they are termed volition; and the actions produced in consequence are termed voluntary actions. Whence it appears, that motions of our muscles or ideas may be produced in consequence of desire or aversion without our having the power to prevent them, and yet these motions may be termed voluntary, according to our definition of the word; though in common language they would be called involuntary.

The objects of desire and aversion are generally at a distance, whereas those of pleasure and pain are immediately acting upon our organs. Hence, before desire or aversion are exerted, so as to cause any actions, there is generally time for deliberation; which consists in discovering the means to obtain the object of desire, or to avoid the object of aversion; or in examining the good or bad consequences, which may result from them. In this case it is evident, that we have a power to delay the proposed action, or to perform it; and this power of choosing, whether we shall act or not, is in common language expressed by the word volition, or will. Whereas in this work the word volition means simply the active state of the sensorial faculty in producing motion in consequence of desire or aversion: whether we have the power of restraining that action, or not; that is, whether we exert any actions in consequence of opposite desires or aversions, or not.

The things we desire or dislike are usually far away, while the things that bring us pleasure or pain are directly affecting us. So, before we act on our desires or aversions, we usually have time to think it over; this involves figuring out how to achieve what we want or avoid what we don’t want, or considering the good or bad outcomes that might come from them. In this situation, it's clear that we have the ability to either postpone or carry out the action, and this ability to choose whether to act or not is typically referred to as volition, or will, in everyday language. However, in this context, volition simply refers to the active state of our sensory faculties that generates movement due to desire or aversion, regardless of whether we can hold back that action; that is, whether we act on conflicting desires or aversions or not.

For if the objects of desire or aversion are present, there is no necessity to investigate or compare the means of obtaining them, nor do we always deliberate about their consequences; that is, no deliberation necessarily intervenes, and in consequence the power of choosing to act or not is not exerted. It is probable, that this twofold use of the word volition in all languages has confounded the metaphysicians, who have disputed about free will and necessity. Whereas from the above analysis it would appear, that during our sleep, we use no voluntary exertions at all; and in our waking hours, that they are the consequence of desire or aversion.

For when the things we want or dislike are right in front of us, we don’t need to figure out how to get them, nor do we always think about what might happen as a result; that is, we don’t always pause to consider our options, which means we don’t really use our ability to choose whether to act or not. It’s likely that this dual meaning of the word "volition" in all languages has confused philosophers who debate free will and necessity. However, from what we've analyzed, it seems that when we’re asleep, we don’t make any conscious choices at all; and when we’re awake, our choices stem from our desires or aversions.

To will is to act in consequence of desire; but to desire means to desire something, even if that something be only to become free from the pain, which causes the desire; for to desire nothing is not to desire; the word desire, therefore, includes both the action and the object or motive; for the object and motive of desire are the same thing. Hence to desire without an object, that is, without a motive, is a solecism in language. As if one should ask, if you could eat without food, or breathe without air.

To will is to act based on desire; however, to desire means wishing for something, even if that something is just to be free from the pain that creates the desire. Wanting nothing means you aren’t really desiring anything; thus, the term desire includes both the action and the object or reason behind it. The object and reason for desire are essentially the same. Therefore, to desire without an object, meaning without a reason, is a contradiction in terms. It’s like asking if you could eat without food or breathe without air.

From this account of volition it appears, that convulsions of the muscles, as in epileptic fits, may in the common sense of that word be termed involuntary; because no deliberation is interposed between the desire or aversion and the consequent action; but in the sense of the word, as above defined, they belong to the class of voluntary motions, as delivered in Vol. II. Class III. If this use of the word be discordant to the ear of the reader, the term morbid voluntary motions, or motions in consequence of aversion, may be substituted in its stead.

From this account of will, it seems that muscle convulsions, like in epileptic seizures, can be called involuntary in the everyday sense because there's no thought process between the desire or dislike and the resulting action. However, as defined earlier, they fit into the category of voluntary movements, as explained in Vol. II, Class III. If this usage of the term sounds off to the reader, the phrase morbid voluntary movements, or movements resulting from aversion, can be used instead.

If a person has a desire to be cured of the ague, and has at the same time an aversion (or contrary desire) to swallowing an ounce of Peruvian bark; he balances desire against desire, or aversion against aversion; and thus he acquires the power of choosing, which is the common acceptation of the word willing. But in the cold fit of ague, after having discovered that the act of shuddering, or exerting the subcutaneous muscles, relieves the pain of cold; he immediately exerts this act of volition, and shudders, as soon as the pain and consequent aversion return, without any deliberation intervening; yet is this act, as well as that of swallowing an ounce of the bark, caused by volition; and that even though he endeavours in vain to prevent it by a weaker contrary volition. This recalls to our minds the story of the hungry ass between two hay-stacks, where the two desires are supposed so exactly to counteract each other, that he goes to neither of the stacks, but perishes by want. Now as two equal and opposite desires are thus supposed to balance each other, and prevent all action, it follows, that if one of these hay-stacks was suddenly removed, that the ass would irresistibly be hurried to the other, which in the common use of the word might be called an involuntary act; but which, in our acceptation of it, would be classed amongst voluntary actions, as above explained.

If someone wants to get rid of the fever but also really doesn't want to take an ounce of Peruvian bark, they weigh one desire against the other—or one aversion against another. This gives them the ability to choose, which is what we generally mean by the word willing. However, during the cold stage of the fever, after realizing that shivering or using the muscles under the skin helps relieve the cold pain, they instinctively shiver as soon as the pain and the accompanying dislike return, without thinking about it. Yet this action, just like taking the bark, is driven by their will, even if they struggle unsuccessfully to stop it with a weaker opposing desire. This reminds us of the story of the hungry donkey caught between two haystacks, where the two desires are so evenly matched that the donkey ends up going to neither stack and dies of hunger. Since two equal and opposing desires are said to cancel each other out and halt all action, it follows that if one of those haystacks were suddenly taken away, the donkey would uncontrollably rush to the other, which might typically be seen as an involuntary act. However, in our understanding, it would be classified as a voluntary action, as explained above.

Hence to deliberate is to compare opposing desires or aversions, and that which is the most interesting at length prevails, and produces action. Similar to this, where two pains oppose each other, the stronger or more interesting one produces action; as in pleurisy the pain from suffocation would produce expansion of the lungs, but the pain occasioned by extending the inflamed membrane, which lines the chest, opposes this expansion, and one or the other alternately prevails.

Hence, to deliberate is to weigh conflicting desires or dislikes, and the one that captures our interest most ultimately wins out and leads to action. Similarly, when two pains counter each other, the stronger or more compelling one causes action; for instance, in pleurisy, the pain from suffocation encourages lung expansion, while the pain from stretching the inflamed membrane lining the chest resists this expansion, and either one or the other takes precedence at different times.

When any one moves his hand quickly near another person's eyes, the eye-lids instantly close; this act in common language is termed involuntary, as we have not time to deliberate or to exert any contrary desire or aversion, but in this work it would be termed a voluntary act, because it is caused by the faculty of volition, and after a few trials the nictitation can be prevented by a contrary or opposing volition.

When someone quickly moves their hand close to another person's eyes, the eyelids immediately shut; this reaction is usually called involuntary, since there's no time to think or to feel any opposing desire or dislike. However, in this context, it would be considered a voluntary act because it’s triggered by the ability to make a choice, and after a few attempts, the blinking can be stopped by a conscious decision.

The power of opposing volitions is best exemplified in the story of Mutius Scævola, who is said to have thrust his hand into the fire before Porcenna, and to have suffered it to be consumed for having failed him in his attempt on the life of that general. Here the aversion for the loss of same, or the unsatisfied desire to serve his country, the two prevalent enthusiasms at that time, were more powerful than the desire of withdrawing his hand, which must be occasioned by the pain of combustion; of these opposing volitions

The struggle between conflicting desires is best illustrated by the story of Mutius Scævola, who reportedly stuck his hand in the fire before Porcenna and let it burn as punishment for failing to assassinate that general. In this case, his strong dislike for experiencing failure and his deep desire to serve his country—two dominant passions at the time—were stronger than his instinct to pull his hand back due to the pain of the flames. These conflicting desires.

Vincit amor patriæ, laudumque immensa cupido.

Vincit amor patriæ, laudumque immensa cupido.

If any one is told not to swallow his saliva for a minute, he soon swallows it contrary to his will, in the common sense of that word; but this also is a voluntary action, as it is performed by the faculty of volition, and is thus to be understood. When the power of volition is exerted on any of our senses, they become more acute, as in our attempts to hear small noises in the night. As explained in Section XIX. 6. Hence by our attention to the fauces from our desire not to swallow our saliva; the fauces become more sensible; and the stimulus of the saliva is followed by greater sensation, and consequent desire of swallowing it. So that the desire or volition in consequence of the increased sensation of the saliva is more powerful, than the previous desire not to swallow it. See Vol. II. Deglutitio invita. In the same manner if a modest man wishes not to want to make water, when he is confined with ladies in a coach or an assembly-room; that very act of volition induces the circumstance, which he wishes to avoid, as above explained; insomuch that I once saw a partial insanity, which might be called a voluntary diabetes, which was occasioned by the fear (and consequent aversion) of not being able to make water at all.

If any one is told not to swallow his saliva for a minute, he soon swallows it contrary to his will, in the common sense of that word; but this also is a voluntary action, as it is performed by the faculty of volition, and is thus to be understood. When the power of volition is exerted on any of our senses, they become more acute, as in our attempts to hear small noises in the night. As explained in Section XIX. 6. Hence by our attention to the fauces from our desire not to swallow our saliva; the fauces become more sensible; and the stimulus of the saliva is followed by greater sensation, and consequent desire of swallowing it. So that the desire or volition in consequence of the increased sensation of the saliva is more powerful, than the previous desire not to swallow it. See Vol. II. Deglutitio invita. In the same manner if a modest man wishes not to want to make water, when he is confined with ladies in a coach or an assembly-room; that very act of volition induces the circumstance, which he wishes to avoid, as above explained; insomuch that I once saw a partial insanity, which might be called a voluntary diabetes, which was occasioned by the fear (and consequent aversion) of not being able to make water at all.

It is further necessary to observe here, to prevent any confusion of voluntary, with sensitive, or associate motions, that in all the instances of violent efforts to relieve pain, those efforts are at first voluntary exertions; but after they have been frequently repeated for the purpose of relieving certain pains, they become associated with those pains, and cease at those times to be subservient to the will; as in coughing, sneezing, and strangury. Of these motions those which contribute to remove or dislodge the offending cause, as the actions of the abdominal muscles in parturition or in vomiting, though they were originally excited by volition, are in this work termed sensitive motions; but those actions of the muscles or organs of sense, which do not contribute to remove the offending cause, as in general convulsions or in madness, are in this work termed voluntary motions, or motions in consequence of aversion, though in common language they are called involuntary ones. Those sensitive unrestrainable actions, which contribute to remove the cause of pain are uniformly and invariably exerted, as in coughing or sneezing; but those motions which are exerted in consequence of aversion without contributing to remove the painful cause, but only to prevent the sensation of it, as in epileptic, or cataleptic fits, are not uniformly and invariably exerted, but change from one set of muscles to another, as will be further explained; and may by this criterion also be distinguished from the former.

It’s also important to note here, to avoid any confusion between voluntary, sensitive, or associated motions, that in all cases of intense efforts to relieve pain, those efforts initially start as voluntary actions. However, after they’ve been repeated multiple times to relieve specific pains, they become linked to those pains and no longer follow the will. This is seen in actions like coughing, sneezing, and painful urination. Among these motions, those that help remove or dislodge the source of discomfort, such as the movements of the abdominal muscles during childbirth or vomiting, are referred to in this work as sensitive motions, even though they originally began as voluntary. On the other hand, actions of the muscles or sensory organs that do not help eliminate the source of pain, like in general convulsions or madness, are termed voluntary motions or aversive motions in this work, even though they are typically called involuntary in everyday language. Those sensitive, uncontrollable actions that help alleviate pain are consistently performed, as seen in coughing or sneezing; while the motions that happen as a result of aversion but don’t help remove the painful source, like during epileptic or cataleptic episodes, aren’t consistently performed and can shift from one muscle group to another, as will be explained further. These can also be distinguished from the former based on this criterion.

At the same time those motions, which are excited by perpetual stimulus, or by association with each other, or immediately by pleasureable or painful sensation, may properly be termed involuntary motions, as those of the heart and arteries; as the faculty of volition seldom affects those, except when it exists in unnatural quantity, as in maniacal people.

At the same time, those movements that are triggered by constant stimulation, by association with one another, or directly by pleasurable or painful sensations can be correctly called involuntary movements, like those of the heart and arteries; the faculty of willpower rarely influences these, except when it occurs in unnatural amounts, as seen in people with mania.

2. It was observed in Section XIV. on the Production of Ideas, that those parts of the system, which are usually termed the organs of sense, are liable to be excited into pain by the excess of the stimulus of those objects, which are by nature adapted to affect them; as of too great light, sound, or pressure. But that these organs receive no pain from the defect or absence of these stimuli, as in darkness or silence. But that our other organs of perception, which have generally been called appetites, as of hunger, thirst, want of heat, want of fresh air, are liable to be affected with pain by the defect, as well as by the excess of their appropriated stimuli.

2. It was observed in Section XIV. on the Production of Ideas, that those parts of the system, which are usually termed the organs of sense, are liable to be excited into pain by the excess of the stimulus of those objects, which are by nature adapted to affect them; as of too great light, sound, or pressure. But that these organs receive no pain from the defect or absence of these stimuli, as in darkness or silence. But that our other organs of perception, which have generally been called appetites, as of hunger, thirst, want of heat, want of fresh air, are liable to be affected with pain by the defect, as well as by the excess of their appropriated stimuli.

This excess or defect of stimulus is however to be considered only as the remote cause of the pain, the immediate cause being the excess or defect of the natural action of the affected part, according to Sect. IV. 5. Hence all the pains of the body may be divided into those from excess of motion, and those from defect of motion; which distinction is of great importance in the knowledge and the cure of many diseases. For as the pains from excess of motion either gradually subside, or are in general succeeded by inflammation; so those from defect of motion either gradually subside, or are in general succeeded by convulsion, or madness. These pains are easily distinguishable from each other by this circumstance, that the former are attended with heat of the pained part, or of the whole body; whereas the latter exists without increase of heat in the pained part, and is generally attended with coldness of the extremities of the body; which is the true criterion of what have been called nervous pains.

This excess or defect of stimulus is however to be considered only as the remote cause of the pain, the immediate cause being the excess or defect of the natural action of the affected part, according to Sect. IV. 5. Hence all the pains of the body may be divided into those from excess of motion, and those from defect of motion; which distinction is of great importance in the knowledge and the cure of many diseases. For as the pains from excess of motion either gradually subside, or are in general succeeded by inflammation; so those from defect of motion either gradually subside, or are in general succeeded by convulsion, or madness. These pains are easily distinguishable from each other by this circumstance, that the former are attended with heat of the pained part, or of the whole body; whereas the latter exists without increase of heat in the pained part, and is generally attended with coldness of the extremities of the body; which is the true criterion of what have been called nervous pains.

Thus when any acrid material, as snuff or lime, falls into the eye, pain and inflammation and heat are produced from the excess of stimulus; but violent hunger, hemicrania, or the clavus hystericus, are attended with coldness of the extremities, and defect of circulation. When we are exposed to great cold, the pain we experience from the deficiency of heat is attended with a quiescence of the motions of the vascular system; so that no inflammation is produced, but a great desire of heat, and a tremulous motion of the subcutaneous muscles, which is properly a convulsion in consequence of this pain from defect of the stimulus of heat.

Thus, when any irritating substance, like snuff or lime, gets into the eye, it causes pain, inflammation, and heat from the excess stimulation. However, severe hunger, headaches, or hysterical symptoms come with coldness in the extremities and poor circulation. When we are exposed to extreme cold, the pain we feel from the lack of heat is accompanied by a stillness in the blood flow; as a result, no inflammation occurs, but there is a strong desire for warmth and a slight trembling of the underlying muscles, which is actually a convulsion triggered by this pain from the lack of heat stimulus.

It was before mentioned, that as sensation consists in certain movements of the sensorium, beginning at some of the extremities of it, and propagated to the central parts of it; so volition consists of certain other movements of the sensorium, commencing in the central parts of it, and propagated to some of its extremities. This idea of these two great powers of motion in the animal machine is confirmed from observing, that they never exist in a great degree or universally at the same time; for while we strongly exert our voluntary motions, we cease to feel the pains or uneasinesses, which occasioned us to exert them.

It was mentioned earlier that sensation happens through specific movements in the sensory system, starting at the outer edges and moving toward the central parts. In contrast, volition involves different movements that begin in the central areas and spread out to the edges. This understanding of these two fundamental forces of movement in the body is supported by the observation that they rarely occur at a high degree or universally at the same time; when we strongly engage our voluntary movements, we stop feeling the pains or discomfort that prompted us to move in the first place.

Hence during the time of fighting with fists or swords no pain is felt by the combatants, till they cease to exert themselves. Thus in the beginning of ague-fits the painful sensation of cold is diminished, while the patient exerts himself in the shivering and gnashing of his teeth. He then ceases to exert himself, and the pain of cold returns; and he is thus perpetually induced to reiterate these exertions, from which he experiences a temporary relief. The same occurs in labour-pains, the exertion of the parturient woman relieves the violence of the pains for a time, which recur again soon after she has ceased to use those exertions. The same is true in many other painful diseases, as in the strangury, tenesmus, and the efforts of vomiting; all these disagreeable sensations are diminished or removed for a time by the various exertions they occasion, and recur alternately with those exertions.

Therefore, during the time of fighting with fists or swords, the fighters don't feel pain until they stop pushing themselves. Similarly, at the start of chills, the painful feeling of cold lessens while the patient struggles with shivering and grinding their teeth. Once they stop exerting themselves, the cold pain comes back; this leads them to keep trying to exert themselves again, which gives them temporary relief. The same happens during labor pains, where the effort of the woman in childbirth temporarily eases the intensity of her pains, which return shortly after she stops exerting herself. This is also true for many other painful conditions, such as painful urination, straining, and the act of vomiting; all these unpleasant feelings are lessened or relieved for a time by the physical efforts they cause, only to return again when those efforts cease.

The restlessness in some fevers is an almost perpetual exertion of this kind, excited to relieve some disagreeable sensations; the reciprocal opposite exertions of a wounded worm, the alternate emprosthotonos and opisthotonos of some spasmodic diseases, and the intervals of all convulsions, from whatever cause, seem to be owing to this circumstance of the laws of animation; that great or universal exertion cannot exist at the same time with great or universal sensation, though they can exist reciprocally; which is probably resolvable into the more general law, that the whole sensorial power being expended in one mode of exertion, there is none to spare for any other. Whence syncope, or temporary apoplexy, succeeds to epileptic convulsions.

The restlessness in some fevers is a nearly constant form of activity, triggered by the need to relieve unpleasant feelings; it’s like the opposing movements of an injured worm, the alternating tension and relaxation in some spasmodic conditions, and the pauses in all types of convulsions, regardless of their cause, seem to stem from the principle that significant or widespread activity cannot happen alongside intense or widespread sensation, although they can occur alternately. This probably relates to the broader principle that when all sensory energy is used in one type of activity, there isn’t any left for another. Hence, fainting, or temporary loss of consciousness, follows epileptic seizures.

3. Hence when any violent pain afflicts us, of which we can neither avoid nor remove the cause, we soon learn to endeavour to alleviate it, by exerting some violent voluntary effort, as mentioned above; and are naturally induced to use those muscles for this purpose, which have been in the early periods of our lives most frequently or most powerfully exerted.

3. Therefore, when we experience any intense pain that we cannot avoid or eliminate the cause of, we quickly learn to try to lessen it by making some intense voluntary effort, as mentioned earlier; and we are naturally inclined to use those muscles that have been most often or most strongly used during the earlier stages of our lives for this purpose.

Now the first muscles, which infants use most frequently, are those of respiration; and on this account we gain a habit of holding our breath, at the same time that we use great efforts to exclude it, for this purpose of alleviating unavoidable pain; or we press out our breath through a small aperture of the larynx, and scream violently, when the pain is greater than is relievable by the former mode of exertion. Thus children scream to relieve any pain either of body or mind, as from anger, or fear of being beaten.

Now, the first muscles that babies use the most are the ones for breathing. Because of this, we get into the habit of holding our breath while making strong efforts to keep it in, in order to ease unavoidable pain. Alternatively, we might push our breath out through a small opening in the larynx and scream loudly when the pain is more than we can handle by just holding our breath. In this way, children scream to relieve any pain, whether physical or emotional, like from anger or fear of being punished.

Hence it is curious to observe, that those animals, who have more frequently exerted their muscles of respiration violently, as in talking, barking, or grunting, as children, dogs, hogs, scream much more, when they are in pain, than those other animals, who use little or no language in their common modes of life; as horses, sheep, and cows.

It’s interesting to note that animals that often use their respiratory muscles intensely, like when they talk, bark, or grunt—such as children, dogs, and pigs—tend to scream much louder when they’re in pain compared to other animals that don’t use much or any vocalization in their everyday lives, like horses, sheep, and cows.

The next most frequent or most powerful efforts, which infants are first tempted to produce, are those with the muscles in biting hard substances; indeed the exertion of these muscles is very powerful in common mastication, as appears from the pain we receive, if a bit of bone is unexpectedly found amongst our softer food; and further appears from their acting to so great mechanical disadvantage, particularly when we bite with the incisores, or canine teeth; which are first formed, and thence are first used to violent exertion.

The next most common and strongest actions that babies are initially encouraged to perform involve using their muscles to bite down on hard substances. In fact, these muscles are very strong during regular chewing, as we can feel when we bite into a piece of bone hidden in softer food. This also shows how they work to a significant mechanical disadvantage, especially when we bite with our front teeth or canine teeth, which are the first to develop and thus the first to be used for intense effort.

Hence when a person is in great pain, the cause of which he cannot remove, he sets his teeth firmly together, or bites some substance between them with great vehemence, as another mode of violent exertion to produce a temporary relief. Thus we have a proverb where no help can be had in pain, "to grin and abide;" and the tortures of hell are said to be attended with "gnashing of teeth."

Hence, when someone is in intense pain that they can't escape, they often clench their teeth tight or bite down on something hard with a lot of force as another way to try to find temporary relief. That's why we have the saying that when there's no help for pain, you just have to "grin and bear it," and it's said that the torments of hell involve "gnashing of teeth."

Hence in violent spasmodic pains I have seen people bite not only their tongues, but their arms or fingers, or those of the attendants, or any object which was near them; and also strike, pinch, or tear, others or themselves, particularly the part of their own body, which is painful at the time. Soldiers, who die of painful wounds in battle, are said in Homer to bite the ground. Thus also in the bellon, or colica saturnina, the patients are said to bite their own flesh, and dogs in this disease to bite up the ground they lie upon. It is probable that the great endeavours to bite in mad dogs, and the violence of other mad animals, is owing to the same cause.

Hence in violent, spasmodic pain, I've seen people bite not only their tongues but also their arms or fingers, or those of the people around them, or any nearby objects; they also strike, pinch, or tear at others or themselves, especially the part of their own body that hurts at the moment. Soldiers who die from painful wounds in battle are said by Homer to bite the ground. Similarly, in conditions like bellon or colica saturnina, patients are said to bite their own flesh, and dogs with this illness tend to bite the ground they are lying on. It's likely that the intense urge to bite in rabid dogs and the aggression of other mad animals is due to the same cause.

4. If the efforts of our voluntary motions are exerted with still greater energy for the relief of some disagreeable sensation, convulsions are produced; as the various kinds of epilepsy, and in some hysteric paroxysms. In all these diseases a pain, or disagreeable sensation is produced, frequently by worms, or acidity in the bowels, or by a diseased nerve in the side, or head, or by the pain of a diseased liver.

4. When we push ourselves harder to get rid of an uncomfortable feeling, it can lead to convulsions, like in different types of epilepsy and some hysterical episodes. In all these conditions, a pain or unpleasant sensation can be caused by worms, acidity in the intestines, a damaged nerve in the side or head, or by pain from a sick liver.

In some constitutions a more intolerable degree of pain is produced in some part at a distance from the cause by sensitive association, as before explained; these pains in such constitutions arise to so great a degree, that I verily believe no artificial tortures could equal some, which I have witnessed; and am confident life would not have long been preserved, unless they had been soon diminished or removed by the universal convulsion of the voluntary motions, or by temporary madness.

In some conditions, a more unbearable level of pain occurs in a part of the body far from the source due to sensitive association, as explained earlier; these pains in such conditions can become so intense that I genuinely believe no man-made tortures could match some that I've seen. I'm convinced that life wouldn't have survived for long if these pains hadn't been quickly lessened or eliminated by the body's involuntary responses or by a temporary state of madness.

In some of the unfortunate patients I have observed, the pain has risen to an inexpressible degree, as above described, before the convulsions have supervened; and which were preceded by screaming, and grinning; in others, as in the common epilepsy, the convulsion has immediately succeeded the commencement of the disagreeable sensations; and as a stupor frequently succeeds the convulsions, they only seemed to remember that a pain at the stomach preceded the fit, or some other uneasy feel; or more frequently retained no memory at all of the immediate cause of the paroxysm. But even in this kind of epilepsy, where the patient does not recollect any preceding pain, the paroxysms generally are preceded by a quivering motion of the under jaw, with a biting of the tongue; the teeth afterwards become pressed together with vehemence, and the eyes are then convulsed, before the commencement of the universal convulsion; which are all efforts to relieve pain.

In some of the unfortunate patients I've seen, the pain has become unbearable, as I described earlier, before the convulsions started. These were often preceded by screaming and grinning; in other cases, similar to typical epilepsy, the convulsions immediately followed the onset of uncomfortable sensations. After the convulsions, the patients would often be in a stupor, and they seemed to only recall that they had felt pain in their stomach or some other discomfort before the episode, or more often, they didn't remember anything about what triggered it. However, even in this type of epilepsy, where the patient doesn't remember any preceding pain, the episodes typically begin with a trembling motion in the lower jaw and biting of the tongue; then the teeth clench tightly, and the eyes convulse before the whole body goes into convulsions, which are all attempts to relieve pain.

The reason why these convulsive motions are alternately exerted and remitted was mentioned above, and in Sect. XII. 1. 3. when the exertions are such as give a temporary relief to the pain, which excites them, they cease for a time, till the pain is again perceived; and then new exertions are produced for its relief. We see daily examples of this in the loud reiterated laughter of some people; the pleasureable sensation, which excites this laughter, arises for a time so high as to change its name and become painful: the convulsive motions of the respiratory muscles relieve the pain for a time; we are, however, unwilling to lose the pleasure, and presently put a stop to this exertion, and immediately the pleasure recurs, and again as instantly rises into pain. All of us have felt the pain of immoderate laughter; children have been tickled into convulsions of the whole body; and others have died in the act of laughing; probably from a paralysis succeeding the long continued actions of the muscles of respiration.

The reason why these convulsive motions are alternately exerted and remitted was mentioned above, and in Sect. XII. 1. 3. when the exertions are such as give a temporary relief to the pain, which excites them, they cease for a time, till the pain is again perceived; and then new exertions are produced for its relief. We see daily examples of this in the loud reiterated laughter of some people; the pleasureable sensation, which excites this laughter, arises for a time so high as to change its name and become painful: the convulsive motions of the respiratory muscles relieve the pain for a time; we are, however, unwilling to lose the pleasure, and presently put a stop to this exertion, and immediately the pleasure recurs, and again as instantly rises into pain. All of us have felt the pain of immoderate laughter; children have been tickled into convulsions of the whole body; and others have died in the act of laughing; probably from a paralysis succeeding the long continued actions of the muscles of respiration.

Hence we learn the reason, why children, who are so easily excited to laugh by the tickling of other people's fingers, cannot tickle themselves into laughter. The exertion of their hands in the endeavour to tickle themselves prevents the necessity of any exertion of the respiratory muscles to relieve the excess of pleasurable affection. See Sect. XVII. 3. 5.

Hence we learn the reason, why children, who are so easily excited to laugh by the tickling of other people's fingers, cannot tickle themselves into laughter. The exertion of their hands in the endeavour to tickle themselves prevents the necessity of any exertion of the respiratory muscles to relieve the excess of pleasurable affection. See Sect. XVII. 3. 5.

Chrysippus is recorded to have died laughing, when an ass was invited to sup with him. The same is related of one of the popes, who, when he was ill, saw a tame monkey at his bedside put on the holy thiara. Hall. Phys. T. III. p. 306.

Chrysippus is said to have died laughing when a donkey was invited to dinner with him. The same thing is mentioned about one of the popes, who, while he was sick, saw a pet monkey at his bedside wearing the holy tiara. Hall. Phys. T. III. p. 306.

There are instances of epilepsy being produced by laughing recorded by Van Swieten, T. III. 402 and 308. And it is well known, that many people have died instantaneously from the painful excess of joy, which probably might have been prevented by the exertions of laughter.

There are cases of epilepsy triggered by laughter documented by Van Swieten, T. III. 402 and 308. It's also well known that many people have died suddenly from overwhelming joy, which might have been avoided by the act of laughing.

Every combination of ideas, which we attend to, occasions pain or pleasure; those which occasion pleasure, furnish either social or selfish pleasure, either malicious or friendly, or lascivious, or sublime pleasure; that is, they give us pleasure mixed with other emotions, or they give us unmixed pleasure, without occasioning any other emotions or exertions at the same time. This unmixed pleasure, if it be great, becomes painful, like all other animal motions from stimuli of every kind; and if no other exertions are occasioned at the same time, we use the exertion of laughter to relieve this pain. Hence laughter is occasioned by such wit as excites simple pleasure without any other emotion, such as pity, love, reverence. For sublime ideas are mixed with admiration, beautiful ones with love, new ones with surprise; and these exertions of our ideas prevent the action of laughter from being necessary to relieve the painful pleasure above described. Whence laughable wit consists of frivolous ideas, without connections of any consequence, such as puns on words, or on phrases, incongruous junctions of ideas; on which account laughter is so frequent in children.

Every combination of ideas we focus on brings either pain or pleasure; those that bring pleasure provide either social or selfish enjoyment, either malicious or friendly, or sensual, or elevated pleasure. In other words, they give us pleasure mixed with other emotions, or they give us pure pleasure without triggering any other feelings or efforts at the same time. This pure pleasure, if it's intense, can turn painful, just like any other physical reactions to stimuli of various kinds; and if no other efforts are triggered simultaneously, we use laughter to relieve this discomfort. Therefore, laughter comes from wit that triggers simple pleasure without any other emotions, like pity, love, or reverence. Because elevated ideas are mixed with admiration, beautiful ones with love, and new ones with surprise, these emotional responses prevent laughter from being necessary to ease the painful pleasure mentioned earlier. That's why amusing wit consists of light ideas without significant connections, like puns or strange combinations of ideas; this is also why children laugh so readily.

Unmixed pleasure less than that, which causes laughter, causes sleep, as in singing children to sleep, or in slight intoxication from wine or food. See Sect. XVIII. 12.

Unmixed pleasure less than that, which causes laughter, causes sleep, as in singing children to sleep, or in slight intoxication from wine or food. See Sect. XVIII. 12.

5. If the pains, or disagreeable sensations, above described do not obtain a temporary relief from these convulsive exertions of the muscles, those convulsive exertions continue without remission, and one kind of catalepsy is produced. Thus when a nerve or tendon produces great pain by its being inflamed or wounded, the patient sets his teeth firmly together, and grins violently, to diminish the pain; and if the pain is not relieved by this exertion, no relaxation of the maxillary muscles takes place, as in the convulsions above described, but the jaws remain firmly fixed together. This locked jaw is the most frequent instance of cataleptic spasm, because we are more inclined to exert the muscles subservient to mastication from their early obedience to violent efforts of volition.

5. If the pains or uncomfortable sensations mentioned earlier don't get temporary relief from these muscle contractions, those contractions will keep going without a break, leading to a type of catalepsy. For example, when a nerve or tendon becomes very painful due to inflammation or injury, the patient clenches their teeth tightly and grits their teeth to lessen the pain. If this effort doesn’t relieve the pain, instead of the jaw muscles relaxing as seen in the earlier mentioned convulsions, the jaws stay locked together. This locked jaw is the most common form of cataleptic spasm, as we are more likely to use the muscles involved in chewing because they have always responded quickly to strong willful actions.

But in the case related in Sect. XIX. on Reverie, the cataleptic lady had pain in her upper teeth; and pressing one of her hands vehemently against her cheek-bone to diminish this pain, it remained in that attitude for about half an hour twice a day, till the painful paroxysm was over.

But in the case related in Sect. XIX. on Reverie, the cataleptic lady had pain in her upper teeth; and pressing one of her hands vehemently against her cheek-bone to diminish this pain, it remained in that attitude for about half an hour twice a day, till the painful paroxysm was over.

I have this very day seen a young lady in this disease, (with which she has frequently been afflicted,) she began to-day with violent pain shooting from one side of the forehead to the occiput, and after various struggles lay on the bed with her fingers and wrists bent and stiff for about two hours; in other respects she seemed in a syncope with a natural pulse. She then had intervals of pain and of spasm, and took three grains of opium every hour till she had taken nine grains, before the pains and spasm ceased.

I saw a young woman today who has been suffering from this illness multiple times. She started having intense pain that shot from one side of her forehead to the back of her head. After struggling for a while, she lay on the bed with her fingers and wrists bent and stiff for about two hours; otherwise, she appeared to be in a faint but with a normal pulse. Then she had episodes of pain and spasms, taking three grains of opium every hour until she had a total of nine grains before the pain and spasms stopped.

There is, however, another species of fixed spasm, which differs from the former, as the pain exists in the contracted muscle, and would seem rather to be the consequence than the cause of the contraction, as in the cramp in the calf of the leg, and in many other parts of the body.

There is, however, another type of fixed spasm that is different from the previous one, as the pain occurs in the contracted muscle and seems to be more of a result than a cause of the contraction, like in a cramp in the calf of the leg and in many other areas of the body.

In these spasms it should seem, that the muscle itself is first thrown into contraction by some disagreeable sensation, as of cold; and that then the violent pain is produced by the great contraction of the muscular fibres extending its own tendons, which are said to be sensible to extension only; and is further explained in Sect. XVIII. 15.

In these spasms it should seem, that the muscle itself is first thrown into contraction by some disagreeable sensation, as of cold; and that then the violent pain is produced by the great contraction of the muscular fibres extending its own tendons, which are said to be sensible to extension only; and is further explained in Sect. XVIII. 15.

6. Many instances have been given in this work, where after violent motions excited by irritation, the organ has become quiescent to less, and even to the great irritation, which induced it into violent motion; as after looking long at the sun or any bright colour, they cease to be seen; and after removing from bright day-light into a gloomy room, the eye cannot at first perceive the objects, which stimulate it less. Similar to this is the syncope, which succeeds after the violent exertions of our voluntary motions, as after epileptic fits, for the power of volition acts in this case as the stimulus in the other. This syncope is a temporary palsy, or apoplexy, which ceases after a time, the muscles recovering their power of being excited into action by the efforts of volition; as the eye in the circumstance above mentioned recovers in a little time its power of seeing objects in a gloomy room; which were invisible immediately after coming out of a stronger light. This is owing to an accumulation of sensorial power during the inaction of those fibres, which were before accustomed to perpetual exertions, as explained in Sect. XII. 7. 1. A slighter degree of this disease is experienced by every one after great fatigue, when the muscles gain such inability to further action, that we are obliged to rest them for a while, or to summon a greater power of volition to continue their motions.

6. Many instances have been given in this work, where after violent motions excited by irritation, the organ has become quiescent to less, and even to the great irritation, which induced it into violent motion; as after looking long at the sun or any bright colour, they cease to be seen; and after removing from bright day-light into a gloomy room, the eye cannot at first perceive the objects, which stimulate it less. Similar to this is the syncope, which succeeds after the violent exertions of our voluntary motions, as after epileptic fits, for the power of volition acts in this case as the stimulus in the other. This syncope is a temporary palsy, or apoplexy, which ceases after a time, the muscles recovering their power of being excited into action by the efforts of volition; as the eye in the circumstance above mentioned recovers in a little time its power of seeing objects in a gloomy room; which were invisible immediately after coming out of a stronger light. This is owing to an accumulation of sensorial power during the inaction of those fibres, which were before accustomed to perpetual exertions, as explained in Sect. XII. 7. 1. A slighter degree of this disease is experienced by every one after great fatigue, when the muscles gain such inability to further action, that we are obliged to rest them for a while, or to summon a greater power of volition to continue their motions.

In all the syncopes, which I have seen induced after convulsive fits, the pulse has continued natural, though the organs of sense, as well as the locomotive muscles, have ceased to perform their functions; for it is necessary for the perception of objects, that the external organs of sense should be properly excited by the voluntary power, as the eye-lids must be open, and perhaps the muscles of the eye put into action to distend, and thence give greater pellucidity to the cornea, which in syncope, as in death, appears flat and less transparent.

In all the fainting spells I've observed following convulsive fits, the pulse has remained normal, even though the sensory organs and movement muscles have stopped working. It's essential for us to perceive objects that the external sensory organs are stimulated by our voluntary control. For example, the eyelids need to be open, and possibly the eye muscles have to work to stretch and make the cornea clearer. During syncope, much like in death, the cornea looks flat and less transparent.

The tympanum of the ear also seems to require a voluntary exertion of its muscles, to gain its due tension, and it is probable the other external organs of sense require a similar voluntary exertion to adapt them to the distinct perception of objects. Hence in syncope as in sleep, as the power of volition is suspended, no external objects are perceived. See Sect. XVIII. 5. During the time which the patient lies in a fainting fit, the spirit of animation becomes accumulated; and hence the muscles in a while become irritable by their usual stimulation, and the fainting fit ceases. See Sect. XII. 7. 1.

The tympanum of the ear also seems to require a voluntary exertion of its muscles, to gain its due tension, and it is probable the other external organs of sense require a similar voluntary exertion to adapt them to the distinct perception of objects. Hence in syncope as in sleep, as the power of volition is suspended, no external objects are perceived. See Sect. XVIII. 5. During the time which the patient lies in a fainting fit, the spirit of animation becomes accumulated; and hence the muscles in a while become irritable by their usual stimulation, and the fainting fit ceases. See Sect. XII. 7. 1.

7. If the exertion of the voluntary motions has been still more energetic, the quiescence, which succeeds, is so complete, that they cannot again be excited into action by the efforts of the will. In this manner the palsy, and apoplexy (which is an universal palsy) are frequently produced after convulsions, or other violent exertions; of this I shall add a few instances.

7. If the effort of voluntary movements has been even more intense, the stillness that follows is so total that they can't be reactivated by willpower alone. This is how paralysis and apoplexy (which is a widespread form of paralysis) often arise after convulsions or other violent activities; I'll provide a few examples of this.

Platernus mentions some, who have died apoplectic from violent exertions in dancing; and Dr. Mead, in his Essay on Poisons, records a patient in the hydrophobia, who at one effort broke the cords which bound him, and at the same instant expired. And it is probable, that those, who have expired from immoderate laughter, have died from this paralysis consequent to violent exertion. Mrs. Scott of Stafford was walking in her garden in perfect health with her neighbour Mrs. ——; the latter accidentally fell into a muddy rivulet, and tried in vain to disengage herself by the assistance of Mrs. Scott's hand. Mrs. Scott exerted her utmost power for many minutes, first to assist her friend, and next to prevent herself from being pulled into the morass, as her distressed companion would not disengage her hand. After other assistance was procured by their united screams, Mrs. Scott walked to a chair about twenty yards from the brook, and was seized with an apoplectic stroke: which continued many days, and terminated in a total loss of her right arm, and her speech; neither of which she ever after perfectly recovered.

Platernus mentions some people who have died from heart attacks due to extreme exertion while dancing; and Dr. Mead, in his Essay on Poisons, notes a patient with hydrophobia who broke free from the restraints binding him and died at the same moment. It’s likely that those who have died from excessive laughter have succumbed to a paralysis caused by intense exertion. Mrs. Scott from Stafford was walking in her garden in perfect health with her neighbor, Mrs. —; the latter accidentally fell into a muddy stream and struggled unsuccessfully to free herself with Mrs. Scott's help. Mrs. Scott used all her strength for several minutes, first to help her friend and then to keep herself from being pulled into the mire, as her distressed companion wouldn't let go of her hand. After additional help was called for by their combined screams, Mrs. Scott walked to a chair about twenty yards from the brook and was suddenly struck by a stroke that lasted many days, resulting in a complete loss of use of her right arm and her speech; neither of which she ever fully regained.

It is said, that many people in Holland have died after skating too long or too violently on their frozen canals; it is probable the death of these, and of others, who have died suddenly in swimming, has been owing to this great quiescence or paralysis; which has succeeded very violent exertions, added to the concomitant cold, which has had greater effect after the sufferers had been heated and exhausted by previous exercise.

Many people in Holland are said to have died after skating too long or too vigorously on their frozen canals. It's likely that the deaths of these individuals, along with others who have suddenly died while swimming, are due to this extreme relaxation or paralysis that follows intense exertion, compounded by the cold, which affects people more severely after they've been warmed up and worn out from earlier activity.

I remember a young man of the name of Nairne at Cambridge, who walking on the edge of a barge fell into the river. His cousin and fellow-student of the same name, knowing the other could not swim, plunged into the water after him, caught him by his clothes, and approaching the bank by a vehement exertion propelled him safe to the land, but that instant, seized, as was supposed, by the cramp, or paralysis, sunk to rise no more. The reason why the cramp of the muscles, which compose the calf of the leg, is so liable to affect swimmers, is, because these muscles have very weak antagonists, and are in walking generally elongated again after their contraction by the weight of the body on the ball of the toe, which is very much greater than the resistance of the water in swimming. See Section XVIII. 15.

I remember a young man of the name of Nairne at Cambridge, who walking on the edge of a barge fell into the river. His cousin and fellow-student of the same name, knowing the other could not swim, plunged into the water after him, caught him by his clothes, and approaching the bank by a vehement exertion propelled him safe to the land, but that instant, seized, as was supposed, by the cramp, or paralysis, sunk to rise no more. The reason why the cramp of the muscles, which compose the calf of the leg, is so liable to affect swimmers, is, because these muscles have very weak antagonists, and are in walking generally elongated again after their contraction by the weight of the body on the ball of the toe, which is very much greater than the resistance of the water in swimming. See Section XVIII. 15.

It does not follow that every apoplectic or paralytic attack is immediately preceded by vehement exertion; the quiescence, which succeeds exertion, and which is not so great as to be termed paralysis, frequently recurs afterwards at certain periods; and by other causes of quiescence, occurring with those periods, as was explained in treating of the paroxysms of intermitting fevers; the quiescence at length, becomes so great as to be incapable of again being removed by the efforts of volition, and complete paralysis is formed. See Section XXXII. 3. 2.

It does not follow that every apoplectic or paralytic attack is immediately preceded by vehement exertion; the quiescence, which succeeds exertion, and which is not so great as to be termed paralysis, frequently recurs afterwards at certain periods; and by other causes of quiescence, occurring with those periods, as was explained in treating of the paroxysms of intermitting fevers; the quiescence at length, becomes so great as to be incapable of again being removed by the efforts of volition, and complete paralysis is formed. See Section XXXII. 3. 2.

Many of the paralytic patients, whom I have seen, have evidently had diseased livers from the too frequent potation of spirituous liquors; some of them have had the gutta rosea on their faces and breasts; which has in some degree receded either spontaneously, or by the use of external remedies, and the paralytic stroke has succeeded; and as in several persons, who have drank much vinous spirits, I have observed epileptic fits to commence at about forty or fifty years of age, without any hereditary taint, from the stimulus, as I believed, of a diseased liver; I was induced to ascribe many paralytic cases to the same source; which were not evidently the effect of age, or of unacquired debility. And the account given before of dropsies, which very frequently are owing to a paralysis of the absorbent system, and are generally attendant on free drinkers of spirituous liquors, confirmed me in this opinion.

Many of the paralytic patients I've seen clearly had unhealthy livers from drinking too much alcohol. Some of them had rosacea on their faces and chests, which has in some cases improved either on its own or with external treatments, followed by a stroke. I've also noticed that in several people who heavily consumed wine, epileptic seizures began around the ages of forty to fifty, without any family history, which I believed was triggered by liver damage. This led me to think that many cases of paralysis could be linked to the same issue, rather than just being due to age or general weakness. The earlier account of dropsy, which often results from a paralysis of the lymphatic system and usually occurs in people who drink a lot of alcohol, supported my view.

The disagreeable irritation of a diseased liver produces exertions and consequent quiescence; these by the accidental concurrence of other causes of quiescence, as cold, solar or lunar periods, inanition, the want of their usual portion of spirit of wine, at length produces paralysis.

The unpleasant irritation of a sick liver causes periods of activity followed by rest. These periods of rest, combined with other factors like cold weather, solar or lunar cycles, lack of food, or not having their usual amount of alcohol, eventually lead to paralysis.

This is further confirmed by observing, that the muscles, we most frequently, or most powerfully exert, are most liable to palsy; as those of the voice and of articulation, and of those paralytics which I have seen, a much greater proportion have lost the use of their right arm; which is so much more generally exerted than the left.

This is further confirmed by observing that the muscles we use most often or with the most strength are the ones most likely to become paralyzed, like those used for speaking and articulating. Among the paralysis cases I've seen, a significantly larger number have lost the use of their right arm, which is used much more frequently than the left.

I cannot dismiss this subject without observing, that after a paralytic stroke, if the vital powers are not much injured, that the patient has all the movements of the affected limb to learn over again, just as in early infancy; the limb is first moved by the irritation of its muscles, as in stretching, (of which a case was related in Section VII. 1. 3.) or by the electric concussion; afterwards it becomes obedient to sensation, as in violent danger or fear; and lastly, the muscles become again associated with volition, and gradually acquire their usual habits of acting together.

I cannot dismiss this subject without observing, that after a paralytic stroke, if the vital powers are not much injured, that the patient has all the movements of the affected limb to learn over again, just as in early infancy; the limb is first moved by the irritation of its muscles, as in stretching, (of which a case was related in Section VII. 1. 3.) or by the electric concussion; afterwards it becomes obedient to sensation, as in violent danger or fear; and lastly, the muscles become again associated with volition, and gradually acquire their usual habits of acting together.

Another phænomenon in palsies is, that when the limbs of one side are disabled, those of the other are in perpetual motion. This can only be explained from conceiving that the power of motion, whatever it is, or wherever it resides, and which is capable of being exhausted by fatigue, and accumulated in rest, is now less expended, whilst one half of the body is capable of receiving its usual proportion of it, and is hence derived with greater ease or in greater abundance into the limbs, which remain unaffected.

Another phenomenon in paralysis is that when the limbs on one side are disabled, those on the other side are constantly in motion. This can only be explained by understanding that the ability to move, whatever it is or wherever it comes from, can be depleted by fatigue and built up during rest. With one half of the body unable to move, the power of motion is less used, allowing the other half to receive its usual share more easily and in greater abundance, which keeps those unaffected limbs in action.

II. 1. The excess or defect of voluntary exertion produces similar effects upon the sensual motions, or ideas of the mind, as those already mentioned upon the muscular fibres. Thus when any violent pain, arising from the defect of some peculiar stimulus, exists either in the muscular or sensual systems of fibres, and which cannot be removed by acquiring the defective stimulus; as in some constitutions convulsions of the muscles are produced to procure a temporary relief, so in other constitutions vehement voluntary exertions of the ideas of the mind are produced for the same purpose; for during this exertion, like that of the muscles, the pain either vanishes or is diminished: this violent exertion constitutes madness; and in many cases I have seen the madness take place, and the convulsions cease, and reciprocally the madness cease, and the convulsions supervene. See Section III. 5. 8.

II. 1. The excess or defect of voluntary exertion produces similar effects upon the sensual motions, or ideas of the mind, as those already mentioned upon the muscular fibres. Thus when any violent pain, arising from the defect of some peculiar stimulus, exists either in the muscular or sensual systems of fibres, and which cannot be removed by acquiring the defective stimulus; as in some constitutions convulsions of the muscles are produced to procure a temporary relief, so in other constitutions vehement voluntary exertions of the ideas of the mind are produced for the same purpose; for during this exertion, like that of the muscles, the pain either vanishes or is diminished: this violent exertion constitutes madness; and in many cases I have seen the madness take place, and the convulsions cease, and reciprocally the madness cease, and the convulsions supervene. See Section III. 5. 8.

2. Madness is distinguishable from delirium, as in the latter the patient knows not the place where he resides, nor the persons of his friends or attendants, nor is conscious of any external objects, except when spoken to with a louder voice, or stimulated with unusual force, and even then he soon relapses into a state of inattention to every thing about him. Whilst in the former he is perfectly sensible to every thing external, but has the voluntary powers of his mind intensely exerted on some particular object of his desire or aversion, he harbours in his thoughts a suspicion of all mankind, lest they should counteract his designs; and while he keeps his intentions, and the motives of his actions profoundly secret; he is perpetually studying the means of acquiring the object of his wish, or of preventing or revenging the injuries he suspects.

2. Madness is different from delirium. In delirium, the person doesn’t know where they are, who their friends or caregivers are, and they aren’t aware of external things unless someone speaks loudly or forces their attention in some way. Even then, they quickly slip back into a state of unawareness about everything around them. In contrast, someone who is mad is fully aware of everything outside but is intensely focused on a specific desire or fear. They have a deep suspicion of everyone, fearing they might interfere with their plans. While keeping their intentions and motivations hidden, they are constantly figuring out how to acquire what they want or how to prevent or take revenge for perceived wrongs.

3. A late French philosopher, Mr. Helvetius, has deduced almost all our actions from this principle of their relieving us from the ennui or tædium vitæ; and true it is, that our desires or aversions are the motives of all our voluntary actions; and human nature seems to excel other animals in the more facil use of this voluntary power, and on that account is more liable to insanity than other animals. But in mania this violent exertion of volition is expended on mistaken objects, and would not be relieved, though we were to gain or escape the objects, that excite it. Thus I have seen two instances of madmen, who conceived that they had the itch, and several have believed they had the venereal infection, without in reality having a symptom of either of them. They have been perpetually thinking upon this subject, and some of them were in vain salivated with design of convincing them to the contrary.

3. A late French philosopher, Mr. Helvetius, concluded that almost all our actions stem from the need to relieve ourselves from boredom or the weariness of life; and it’s true that our desires and dislikes drive all our voluntary actions. Human nature seems to be better at exercising this voluntary power compared to other animals, which is why we are more prone to insanity than they are. In cases of mania, this intense exercise of will is misdirected towards incorrect objects and wouldn’t be alleviated even if we were to obtain or escape those things that trigger it. For example, I have seen two cases of individuals who believed they had an itch, and several others thought they had a sexually transmitted infection, even though they showed no actual symptoms of either. They were constantly fixated on this idea, and some were treated with salivation in a futile attempt to convince them otherwise.

4. In the minds of mad people those volitions alone exist, which are unmixed with sensation; immoderate suspicion is generally the first symptom, and want of shame, and want of delicacy about cleanliness. Suspicion is a voluntary exertion of the mind arising from the pain of fear, which it is exerted to relieve: shame is the name of a peculiar disagreeable sensation, see Fable of the Bees, and delicacy about cleanliness arises from another disagreeable sensation. And therefore are not found in the minds of maniacs, which are employed solely in voluntary exertions. Hence the most modest women in this disease walk naked amongst men without any kind of concern, use obscene discourse, and have no delicacy about their natural evacuations.

4. In the minds of insane people, only those intentions exist that are not mixed with feelings; extreme suspicion is usually the first sign, along with a lack of shame and a disregard for cleanliness. Suspicion is a conscious effort of the mind that arises to alleviate the pain of fear; shame is a specific unpleasant feeling, see Fable of the Bees, and the lack of concern for cleanliness stems from another uncomfortable sensation. Therefore, these feelings are absent in the minds of the insane, which focus only on voluntary actions. As a result, even the most modest women dealing with this issue walk around naked among men without any concern, engage in vulgar talk, and show no sensitivity regarding their bodily functions.

5. Nor are maniacal people more attentive to their natural appetites, or to the irritations which surround them, except as far as may respect their suspicions or designs; for the violent and perpetual exertions of their voluntary powers of mind prevents their perception of almost every other object, either of irritation or of sensation. Hence it is that they bear cold, hunger, and fatigue, with much greater pertinacity than in their sober hours, and are less injured by them in respect to their general health. Thus it is asserted by historians, that Charles the Twelfth of Sweden slept on the snow, wrapped only in his cloak, at the siege of Frederickstad, and bore extremes of cold and hunger, and fatigue, under which numbers of his soldiers perished; because the king was insane with ambition, but the soldier had no such powerful stimulus to preserve his system from debility and death.

5. Likewise, people who are fanatical aren’t any more aware of their basic desires or the annoyances around them, except when it comes to their suspicions or goals; because the intense and constant exertions of their mental willpower prevent them from noticing nearly all other things, whether irritating or sensory. This is why they endure cold, hunger, and exhaustion with much more determination than when they are in their right mind, and are less affected by these conditions regarding their overall health. Historians claim that Charles the Twelfth of Sweden slept on the snow, wrapped only in his cloak during the siege of Frederickstad, and withstood extreme cold, hunger, and fatigue that caused many of his soldiers to die; because the king was driven mad by ambition, while the soldiers lacked such a strong motivator to keep them from weakness and death.

6. Besides the insanities arising from exertions in consequence of pain, there is also a pleasurable insanity, as well as a pleasurable delirium; as the insanity of personal vanity, and that of religious fanaticism. When agreeable ideas excite into motion the sensorial power of sensation, and this again causes other trains of agreeable ideas, a constant stream of pleasurable ideas succeeds, and produces pleasurable delirium. So when the sensorial power of volition excites agreeable ideas, and the pleasure thus produced excites more volition in its turn, a constant flow of agreeable voluntary ideas succeeds; which when thus exerted in the extreme constitutes insanity.

6. In addition to the craziness that comes from dealing with pain, there's also a kind of pleasurable madness and enjoyable delirium, like the madness of personal vanity and the fervor of religious zeal. When pleasant thoughts trigger the sensory response, leading to more pleasant thoughts, it creates a continuous flow of enjoyable ideas that results in pleasurable delirium. Similarly, when the sensory ability to make choices sparks pleasant thoughts, the resulting pleasure can lead to even more choices, creating an ongoing stream of enjoyable voluntary ideas; when pushed to the limit, this can lead to insanity.

Thus when our muscular actions are excited by our sensations of pleasure, it is termed play; when they are excited by our volition, it is termed work; and the former of these is attended with less fatigue, because the muscular actions in play produce in their turn more pleasurable sensation; which again has the property of producing more muscular action. An agreeable instance of this I saw this morning. A little boy, who was tired with walking, begged of his papa to carry him. "Here," says the reverend doctor, "ride upon my gold-headed cane;" and the pleased child, putting it between his legs, gallopped away with delight, and complained no more of his fatigue. Here the aid of another sensorial power, that of pleasurable sensation, superadded vigour to the exertion of exhausted volition. Which could otherwise only have been excited by additional pain, as by the lash of slavery. On this account where the whole sensorial power has been exerted on the contemplation of the promised joys of heaven, the saints of all persecuted religions have borne the tortures of martyrdom with otherwise unaccountable firmness.

So when our muscles are engaged by feelings of pleasure, we call it play; when they're engaged by our will, we call it work. Play usually involves less fatigue because the muscle movements in play create more pleasurable feelings, which in turn encourage more muscle movement. I saw a great example of this this morning. A little boy, tired from walking, asked his dad to carry him. “Here,” said the reverend doctor, “ride on my gold-headed cane.” Happy, the child straddled it and happily galloped away, no longer complaining about being tired. Here, the added boost from pleasurable feelings helped him overcome his tiredness. Otherwise, he might have needed more pain to motivate him, like the whip of slavery. This is why those who have fully focused on the promised joys of heaven have endured the tortures of martyrdom with an otherwise inexplicable strength.

7. There are some diseases, which obtain at least a temporary relief from the exertions of insanity; many instances of dropsies being thus for a time cured are recorded. An elderly woman labouring with ascites I twice saw relieved for some weeks by insanity, the dropsy ceased for several weeks, and recurred again alternating with the insanity. A man afflicted with difficult respiration on lying down, with very irregular pulse, and œdematous legs, whom I saw this day, has for above a week been much relieved in respect to all those symptoms by the accession of insanity, which is shewn by inordinate suspicion, and great anger.

7. Some diseases can experience at least temporary relief from the effects of insanity; there are many recorded cases of dropsy being cured for a while in this way. I saw an elderly woman suffering from ascites who was relieved for several weeks by insanity; her dropsy went away for a time but returned again, alternating with the insanity. A man I saw today, who has difficulty breathing when lying down, an irregular pulse, and swollen legs, has experienced significant relief from all these symptoms for over a week due to the onset of insanity, which is evident through excessive suspicion and intense anger.

In cases of common temporary anger the increased action of the arterial system is seen by the red skin, and increased pulse, with the immediate increase of muscular activity. A friend of mine, when he was painfully fatigued by riding on horseback, was accustomed to call up ideas into his mind, which used to excite his anger or indignation, and thus for a time at least relieved the pain of fatigue. By this temporary insanity, the effect of the voluntary power upon the whole of his system was increased; as in the cases of dropsy above mentioned, it would appear, that the increased action of the voluntary faculty of the sensorium affected the absorbent system, as well as the secerning one.

In instances of normal short-term anger, you can notice the increased activity of the arterial system through red skin and a faster pulse, along with an immediate boost in muscle activity. A friend of mine, when he was really tired from riding horseback, would often think of things that made him feel angry or indignant, which temporarily eased his fatigue. This momentary madness seemed to enhance the effect of his willpower on his entire system; as mentioned in earlier cases of dropsy, it seems that the heightened functioning of his voluntary mind also impacted both his absorption system and his secretion system.

8. In respect to relieving inflammatory pains, and removing fever, I have seen many instances, as mentioned in Sect. XII. 2. 4. One lady, whom I attended, had twice at some years interval a locked jaw, which relieved a pain on her sternum with peripneumony. Two other ladies I saw, who towards the end of violent peripneumony, in which they frequently lost blood, were at length cured by insanity supervening. In the former the increased voluntary exertion of the muscles of the jaw, in the latter that of the organs of sense, removed the disease; that is, the disagreeable sensation, which had produced the inflammation, now excited the voluntary power, and these new voluntary exertions employed or expended the superabundant sensorial power, which had previously been exerted on the arterial system, and caused inflammation.

8. In respect to relieving inflammatory pains, and removing fever, I have seen many instances, as mentioned in Sect. XII. 2. 4. One lady, whom I attended, had twice at some years interval a locked jaw, which relieved a pain on her sternum with peripneumony. Two other ladies I saw, who towards the end of violent peripneumony, in which they frequently lost blood, were at length cured by insanity supervening. In the former the increased voluntary exertion of the muscles of the jaw, in the latter that of the organs of sense, removed the disease; that is, the disagreeable sensation, which had produced the inflammation, now excited the voluntary power, and these new voluntary exertions employed or expended the superabundant sensorial power, which had previously been exerted on the arterial system, and caused inflammation.

Another case, which I think worth relating, was of a young man about twenty; he had laboured under an irritative fever with debility for three or four weeks, with very quick and very feeble pulse, and other usual symptoms of that species of typhus, but at this time complained much and frequently of pain of his legs and feet. When those who attended him were nearly in despair of his recovery, I observed with pleasure an insanity of mind supervene: which was totally different from delirium, as he knew his friends, calling them by their names, and the room in which he lay, but became violently suspicious of his attendants, and calumniated with vehement oaths his tender mother, who sat weeping by his bed. On this his pulse became slower and firmer, but the quickness did not for some time intirely cease, and he gradually recovered. In this case the introduction of an increased quantity of the power of volition gave vigour to those movements of the system, which are generally only actuated by the power of irritation, and of association.

Another case I think is worth sharing involved a young man around twenty. He had been suffering from a debilitating fever for three or four weeks, with a very rapid and weak pulse, along with other typical symptoms of that type of typhus. At that time, he often complained about pain in his legs and feet. When those caring for him were nearly hopeless about his recovery, I noticed with relief that he developed a form of insanity that was completely different from delirium; he recognized his friends, called them by name, and knew the room he was in, but he became extremely suspicious of his caregivers and accused his distressed mother, who was crying by his bedside, with harsh claims. As this happened, his pulse became slower and stronger, though the rapidity didn’t completely subside for a while, and he gradually improved. In this case, the increase in his willpower seemed to energize those bodily functions that are usually driven only by irritation and association.

Another case I recollect of a young man, about twenty-five, who had the scarlet-fever, with very quick pulse, and an universal eruption on his skin, and was not without reason esteemed to be in great danger of his life. After a few days an insanity supervened, which his friends mistook for delirium, and he gradually recovered, and the cuticle peeled off. From these and a few other cases I have always esteemed insanity to be a favourable sign in fevers, and have cautiously distinguished it from delirium.

Another case I remember is of a young man, around twenty-five, who had scarlet fever, a very fast pulse, and a widespread rash on his skin, and it was reasonable to think he was in serious danger of dying. After a few days, insanity set in, which his friends mistook for delirium, and he slowly recovered, with his skin peeling off. From these and a few other cases, I have always considered insanity a positive sign during fevers and have carefully distinguished it from delirium.

III. Another mode of mental exertion to relieve pain, is by producing a train of ideas not only by the efforts of volition, as in insanity; but by those of sensation likewise, as in delirium and sleep. This mental effort is termed reverie, or somnambulation, and is described more at large in Sect. XIX. on that subject. But I shall here relate another case of that wonderful disease, which fell yesterday under my eye, and to which I have seen many analogous alienations of mind, though not exactly similar in all circumstances. But as all of them either began or terminated with pain or convulsion, there can be no doubt but that they are of epileptic origin, and constitute another mode of mental exertion to relieve some painful sensation.

III. Another mode of mental exertion to relieve pain, is by producing a train of ideas not only by the efforts of volition, as in insanity; but by those of sensation likewise, as in delirium and sleep. This mental effort is termed reverie, or somnambulation, and is described more at large in Sect. XIX. on that subject. But I shall here relate another case of that wonderful disease, which fell yesterday under my eye, and to which I have seen many analogous alienations of mind, though not exactly similar in all circumstances. But as all of them either began or terminated with pain or convulsion, there can be no doubt but that they are of epileptic origin, and constitute another mode of mental exertion to relieve some painful sensation.

1. Master A. about nine years old, had been seized at seven every morning for ten days with uncommon fits, and had had slight returns in the afternoon. They were supposed to originate from worms, and had been in vain attempted to be removed by vermifuge purges. As his fit was expected at seven yesterday morning, I saw him before that hour; he was asleep, seemed free from pain, and his pulse natural. About seven he began to complain of pain about his navel, or more to the left side, and in a few minutes had exertions of his arms and legs like swimming. He then for half an hour hunted a pack of hounds; as appeared by his hallooing, and calling the dogs by their names, and discoursing with the attendants of the chase, describing exactly a day of hunting, which (I was informed) he had witnessed a year before, going through all the most minute circumstances of it; calling to people, who were then present, and lamenting the absence of others, who were then also absent. After this scene he imitated, as he lay in bed, some of the plays of boys, as swimming and jumping. He then sung an English and then an Italian song; part of which with his eyes open, and part with them closed, but could not be awakened or excited by any violence, which it was proper to use.

1. Master A, who was about nine years old, had been experiencing unusual seizures every morning at seven for ten days, along with mild episodes in the afternoon. They were thought to be caused by worms, and attempts to treat them with worm medicine had failed. As his seizure was expected yesterday morning at seven, I went to see him beforehand; he was asleep, seemed to be in no pain, and had a normal pulse. Around seven, he started complaining of pain near his navel, more towards the left side, and within a few minutes, he began moving his arms and legs as if swimming. For half an hour, he acted like he was hunting with a pack of hounds; I could hear him calling the dogs by name and talking to his companions about the hunt, describing in detail an event he had seen a year prior, covering all the little details, calling out to people who were there and expressing sadness over those who weren't. After this episode, he lay in bed imitating some children's games like swimming and jumping. He then sang an English song followed by an Italian one, doing part of it with his eyes open and part with them closed, but he couldn’t be awakened or roused by any force that was appropriate to use.

After about an hour he came suddenly to himself with apparent surprise, and seemed quite ignorant of any part of what had passed, and after being apparently well for half an hour, he suddenly fell into a great stupor, with slower pulse than natural, and a slow moaning respiration, in which he continued about another half hour, and then recovered.

After about an hour, he suddenly became aware of his surroundings with obvious surprise and seemed totally unaware of what had happened. After seeming fine for another half hour, he suddenly fell into a deep stupor, with a slower than normal pulse and slow, moaning breaths. He stayed like that for about half an hour before he finally recovered.

The sequel of this disease was favourable; he was directed one grain of opium at six every morning, and then to rise out of bed; at half past six he was directed fifteen drops of laudanum in a glass of wine and water. The first day the paroxysm became shorter, and less violent. The dose of opium was increased to one-half more, and in three or four days the fits left him. The bark and filings of iron were also exhibited twice a day; and I believe the complaint returned no more.

The follow-up to this illness was positive; he was instructed to take one grain of opium every morning at six and then get out of bed. At six-thirty, he was told to take fifteen drops of laudanum in a glass of wine and water. On the first day, the episodes became shorter and less intense. The opium dosage was increased by half, and in about three or four days, the fits stopped. The bark and iron filings were also given twice a day, and I believe the issue did not come back.

2. In this paroxysm it must be observed, that he began with pain, and ended with stupor, in both circumstances resembling a fit of epilepsy. And that therefore the exertions both of mind and body, both the voluntary ones, and those immediately excited by pleasurable sensation, were exertions to relieve pain.

2. In this outburst, it's important to note that he started with pain and ended in a daze, resembling an epileptic seizure in both cases. Therefore, the efforts of both mind and body, whether voluntary or triggered by pleasure, were attempts to alleviate pain.

The hunting scene appeared to be rather an act of memory than of imagination, and was therefore rather a voluntary exertion, though attended with the pleasurable eagerness, which was the consequence of those ideas recalled by recollection, and not the cause of them.

The hunting scene seemed more like a recall from memory than an act of imagination, so it felt like a conscious effort, although it was accompanied by the enjoyable anticipation that came from those thoughts triggered by memory, rather than being the reason for them.

These ideas thus voluntarily recollected were succeeded by sensations of pleasure, though his senses were unaffected by the stimuli of visible or audible objects; or so weakly excited by them as not to produce sensation or attention. And the pleasure thus excited by volition produced other ideas and other motions in consequence of the sensorial power of sensation. Whence the mixed catenations of voluntary and sensitive ideas and muscular motions in reverie; which, like every other kind of vehement exertion, contribute to relieve pain, by expending a large quantity of sensorial power.

These ideas, which he recalled willingly, were followed by feelings of pleasure, even though his senses weren't impacted by what he could see or hear, or only weakly stimulated by them to the point of not producing sensation or attention. The pleasure triggered by his will created more ideas and movements due to the sensory power of feeling. This led to a mix of voluntary and sensory ideas and muscle movements during daydreaming; similar to any intense effort, this helps relieve pain by using up a significant amount of sensory power.

Those fits generally commence during sleep, from whence I suppose they have been thought to have some connection with sleep, and have thence been termed Somnambulism; but their commencement during sleep is owing to our increased excitability by internal sensations at that time, as explained in Sect. XVIII. 14. and 15., and not to any similitude between reverie and sleep.

Those fits generally commence during sleep, from whence I suppose they have been thought to have some connection with sleep, and have thence been termed Somnambulism; but their commencement during sleep is owing to our increased excitability by internal sensations at that time, as explained in Sect. XVIII. 14. and 15., and not to any similitude between reverie and sleep.

3. I was once concerned for a very elegant and ingenious young lady, who had a reverie on alternate days, which continued nearly the whole day; and as in her days of disease she took up the same kind of ideas, which she had conversed about on the alternate day before, and could recollect nothing of them on her well-day; she appeared to her friends to possess two minds. This case also was of epileptic kind, and was cured, with some relapses, by opium administered before the commencement of the paroxysm.

3. I was once worried about a very elegant and clever young woman who daydreamed every other day, which lasted nearly all day long. On her days of illness, she would dwell on the same ideas that she had discussed the previous healthy day, but she couldn't remember any of them on her well days. It seemed to her friends that she had two different minds. This case was also related to epilepsy, and was treated, with some setbacks, using opium given before the onset of the episodes.

4. Whence it appears, that the methods of relieving inflammatory pains, is by removing all stimulus, as by venesection, cool air, mucilaginous diet, aqueous potation, silence, darkness.

4. It seems that the ways to relieve inflammatory pain are by eliminating all stimulation, such as through bloodletting, cool air, a slimy diet, drinking water, silence, and darkness.

The methods of relieving pains from defect of stimulus is by supplying the peculiar stimulus required, as of food, or warmth.

The ways to alleviate pain from a lack of stimulation involve providing the specific type of stimulation needed, like food or warmth.

And the general method of relieving pain is by exciting into action some great part of the system for the purpose of expending a part of the sensorial power. This is done either by exertion of the voluntary ideas and muscles, as in insanity and convulsion; or by exerting both voluntary and sensitive motions, as in reverie; or by exciting the irritative motions by wine or opium internally, and by the warm bath or blisters externally; or lastly, by exciting the sensitive ideas by good news, affecting stories, or agreeable passions.

And the common way to relieve pain is by stimulating a significant part of the system to use up some of the sensory energy. This can be achieved either through the use of voluntary thoughts and muscles, like in cases of madness or convulsions; or by engaging both voluntary and sensory movements, as seen in daydreaming; or by stimulating the body's reactions with substances like alcohol or opium taken internally, and with methods like warm baths or blisters applied externally; or finally, by provoking positive feelings through good news, touching stories, or enjoyable emotions.



SECT. XXXV.

DISEASES OF ASSOCIATION.

Co-occurring conditions.

I. 1. Sympathy or consent of parts. Primary and secondary parts of an associated train of motions reciprocally affect each other. Parts of irritative trains of motion affect each other in four ways. Sympathies of the skin and stomach. Flushing of the face after a meal. Eruption of the small-pox on the face. Chilness after a meal. 2. Vertigo from intoxication. 3. Absorption from the lungs and pericardium by emetics. In vomiting the actions of the stomach are decreased, not increased. Digestion strengthened after an emetic. Vomiting from deficiency of sensorial power. 4. Dyspnœa from cold bathing. Slow pulse from digitalis. Death from gout in the stomach. II. 1. Primary and secondary parts of sensitive associations affect each other. Pain from gall-stone, from urinary stone, Hemicrania. Painful epilepsy. 2. Gout and red face from inflamed liver. Shingles from inflamed kidney. 3. Coryza from cold applied to the feet. Pleurisy. Hepatitis. 4. Pain of shoulders from inflamed liver. III. Diseases from the associations of ideas.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Sympathy or agreement among parts. Primary and secondary components of a connected series of movements influence one another. Parts of irritative series of motions impact each other in four ways. Connections between the skin and stomach. Blushing after a meal. Outbreak of smallpox on the face. Chills after eating. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Dizziness from intoxication. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Absorption in the lungs and pericardium by emetics. During vomiting, the actions of the stomach are reduced, not increased. Digestion improves after an emetic. Vomiting due to lack of sensory power. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Shortness of breath from cold bathing. Slow pulse from digitalis. Death from gout in the stomach. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Primary and secondary components of sensitive associations influence each other. Pain from gallstones, from urinary stones, Hemicrania. Painful epilepsy. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Gout and flushed face from inflamed liver. Shingles from inflamed kidney. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Cold symptoms from cold applied to the feet. Pleurisy. Hepatitis. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Shoulder pain from inflamed liver. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Diseases resulting from associations of ideas.

I. 1. Many synchronous and successive motions of our muscular fibres, and of our organs of sense, or ideas, become associated so as to form indissoluble tribes or trains of action, as shewn in Section X. on Associate Motions. Some constitutions more easily establish these associations, whether by voluntary, sensitive, or irritative repetitions, and some more easily lose them again, as shewn in Section XXXI. on Temperaments.

I. 1. Many synchronous and successive motions of our muscular fibres, and of our organs of sense, or ideas, become associated so as to form indissoluble tribes or trains of action, as shewn in Section X. on Associate Motions. Some constitutions more easily establish these associations, whether by voluntary, sensitive, or irritative repetitions, and some more easily lose them again, as shewn in Section XXXI. on Temperaments.

When the beginning of such a train of actions becomes by any means disordered, the succeeding part is liable to become disturbed in consequence, and this is commonly termed sympathy or consent of parts by the writers of medicine. For the more clear understanding of these sympathies we must consider a tribe or train of actions as divided into two parts, and call one of them the primary or original motions, and the other the secondary or sympathetic ones.

When the start of any series of actions gets interrupted in any way, the following actions are likely to get disturbed as a result. This is often referred to as sympathy or consent of parts by medical writers. To understand these sympathies more clearly, we should think of a series of actions as divided into two parts, calling one the primary or original motions, and the other the secondary or sympathetic ones.

The primary and secondary parts of a train of irritative actions may reciprocally affect each other in four different manners. 1. They may both be exerted with greater energy than natural. 2. The former may act with greater, and the latter with less energy. 3. The former may act with less, and the latter with greater energy. 4. They may both act with less energy than natural. I shall now give an example of each kind of these modes of action, and endeavour to shew, that though the primary and secondary parts of these trains or tribes of motion are connected by irritative association, or their previous habits of acting together, as described in Sect. XX. on Vertigo. Yet that their acting with similar or dissimilar degrees of energy, depends on the greater or less quantity of sensorial power, which the primary part of the train expends in its exertions.

The primary and secondary parts of a train of irritative actions may reciprocally affect each other in four different manners. 1. They may both be exerted with greater energy than natural. 2. The former may act with greater, and the latter with less energy. 3. The former may act with less, and the latter with greater energy. 4. They may both act with less energy than natural. I shall now give an example of each kind of these modes of action, and endeavour to shew, that though the primary and secondary parts of these trains or tribes of motion are connected by irritative association, or their previous habits of acting together, as described in Sect. XX. on Vertigo. Yet that their acting with similar or dissimilar degrees of energy, depends on the greater or less quantity of sensorial power, which the primary part of the train expends in its exertions.

The actions of the stomach constitute so important a part of the associations of both irritative and sensitive motions, that it is said to sympathize with almost every part of the body; the first example, which I shall adduce to shew that both the primary and secondary parts of a train of irritative associations of motion act with increased energy, is taken from the consent of the skin with this organ. When the action of the fibres of the stomach is increased, as by the stimulus of a full meal, the exertions of the cutaneous arteries of the face become increased by their irritative associations with those of the stomach, and a glow or flushing of the face succeeds. For the small vessels of the skin of the face having been more accustomed to the varieties of action, from their frequent exposure to various degrees of cold and heat become more easily excited into increased action, than those of the covered parts of our bodies, and thus act with more energy from their irritative or sensitive associations with the stomach. On this account in small-pox the eruption in consequence of the previous affection of the stomach breaks out a day sooner on the face than on the hands, and two days sooner than on the trunk, and recedes in similar times after maturation.

The stomach plays a crucial role in the connections between both irritative and sensitive movements, so much so that it's said to connect with nearly every part of the body. The first example I’ll use to illustrate how both the primary and secondary parts of a series of irritative associations of movement operate with increased intensity comes from the skin's reaction to this organ. When the stomach's muscles are activated, like when you eat a large meal, the blood flow in the facial arteries increases due to their connection with the stomach, resulting in a flushed face. The small blood vessels in the facial skin, having adapted to varying actions due to frequent exposure to different temperatures, become more easily triggered into heightened activity compared to those in the covered areas of our bodies. Therefore, they respond more vigorously because of their irritative or sensitive connections with the stomach. This is why, during smallpox, the rash appears on the face a day earlier than on the hands, and two days earlier than on the torso, and it also recedes at similar times after maturation.

But secondly, in weaker constitutions, that is, in those who possess less sensorial power, so much of it is expended in the increased actions of the fibres of the stomach excited by the stimulus of a meal, that a sense of chilness succeeds instead of the universal glow above mentioned; and thus the secondary part of the associated train of motions is diminished in energy, in consequence of the increased activity of the primary part of it.

But secondly, in weaker individuals, meaning those who have less sensory power, a lot of it is used up by the increased activity of the stomach's fibers triggered by the stimulus of a meal. As a result, a feeling of chilliness replaces the overall warmth mentioned earlier; thus, the secondary part of the associated series of actions becomes less energetic due to the heightened activity of the primary part.

2. Another instance of a similar kind, where the secondary part of the train acts with less energy in consequence of the greater exertions of the primary part, is the vertigo attending intoxication; in this circumstance so much sensorial power is expended on the stomach, and on its nearest or more strongly associated motions, as those of the subcutaneous vessels, and probably of the membranes of some internal viscera, that the irritative motions of the retina become imperfectly exerted from deficiency of sensorial power, as explained in Sect. XX. and XXI. 3. on Vertigo and on Drunkenness, and hence the staggering inebriate cannot completely balance himself by such indistinct vision.

2. Another instance of a similar kind, where the secondary part of the train acts with less energy in consequence of the greater exertions of the primary part, is the vertigo attending intoxication; in this circumstance so much sensorial power is expended on the stomach, and on its nearest or more strongly associated motions, as those of the subcutaneous vessels, and probably of the membranes of some internal viscera, that the irritative motions of the retina become imperfectly exerted from deficiency of sensorial power, as explained in Sect. XX. and XXI. 3. on Vertigo and on Drunkenness, and hence the staggering inebriate cannot completely balance himself by such indistinct vision.

3. An instance of the third circumstance, where the primary part of a train of irritative motions acts with less, and the secondary part with greater energy, may be observed by making the following experiment. If a person lies with his arms and shoulders out of bed, till they become cold, a temporary coryza or catarrh is produced; so that the passage of the nostrils becomes totally obstructed; at least this happens to many people; and then on covering the arms and shoulders, till they become warm, the passage of the nostrils ceases again to be obstructed, and a quantity of mucus is discharged from them. In this case the quiescence of the vessels of the skin of the arms and shoulders, occasioned by exposure to cold air, produces by irritative association an increased action of the vessels of the membrane of the nostrils; and the accumulation of sensorial power during the torpor of the arms and shoulders is thus expended in producing a temporary coryza or catarrh.

3. An example of the third situation, where the main part of a series of irritating motions works less actively while the secondary part works more intensely, can be seen in the following experiment. If someone lies with their arms and shoulders hanging out of bed until they get cold, they can temporarily develop a runny nose or nasal congestion; this happens to many people. Once they cover their arms and shoulders until they warm up, the nasal passage becomes unblocked again, and mucus is released. Here, the inactivity of the blood vessels in the skin of the arms and shoulders, caused by exposure to cold air, leads to an increased reaction of the blood vessels in the nasal membranes due to irritative association. Consequently, the build-up of nervous energy during the numbness of the arms and shoulders is used up in causing a temporary runny nose or nasal congestion.

Another instance may be adduced from the sympathy or consent of the motions of the stomach with other more distant links of the very extensive tribes or trains of irritative motions associated with them, described in Sect. XX. on Vertigo. When the actions of the fibres of the stomach are diminished or inverted, the actions of the absorbent vessels, which take up the mucus from the lungs, pericardium, and other cells of the body, become increased, and absorb the fluids accumulated in them with greater avidity, as appears from the exhibition of foxglove, antimony, or other emetics in cases of anasarca, attended with unequal pulse and difficult respiration.

Another instance may be adduced from the sympathy or consent of the motions of the stomach with other more distant links of the very extensive tribes or trains of irritative motions associated with them, described in Sect. XX. on Vertigo. When the actions of the fibres of the stomach are diminished or inverted, the actions of the absorbent vessels, which take up the mucus from the lungs, pericardium, and other cells of the body, become increased, and absorb the fluids accumulated in them with greater avidity, as appears from the exhibition of foxglove, antimony, or other emetics in cases of anasarca, attended with unequal pulse and difficult respiration.

That the act of nausea and vomiting is a decreased exertion of the fibres of the stomach may be thus deduced; when an emetic medicine is administered, it produces the pain of sickness, as a disagreeable taste in the mouth produces the pain of nausea; these pains, like that of hunger, or of cold, or like those, which are usually termed nervous, as the head-ach or hemicrania, do not excite the organ into greater action; but in this case I imagine the pains of sickness or of nausea counteract or destroy the pleasurable sensation, which seems necessary to digestion, as shewn in Sect. XXXIII. 1. 1. The peristaltic motions of the fibres of the stomach become enfeebled by the want of this stimulus of pleasurable sensation, and in consequence stop for a time, and then become inverted; for they cannot become inverted without being previously stopped. Now that this inversion of the trains of motion of the fibres of the stomach is owing to the deficiency of pleasurable sensation is evinced from this circumstance, that a nauseous idea excited by words will produce vomiting as effectually us a nauseous drug.

That the act of nausea and vomiting is a decreased exertion of the fibres of the stomach may be thus deduced; when an emetic medicine is administered, it produces the pain of sickness, as a disagreeable taste in the mouth produces the pain of nausea; these pains, like that of hunger, or of cold, or like those, which are usually termed nervous, as the head-ach or hemicrania, do not excite the organ into greater action; but in this case I imagine the pains of sickness or of nausea counteract or destroy the pleasurable sensation, which seems necessary to digestion, as shewn in Sect. XXXIII. 1. 1. The peristaltic motions of the fibres of the stomach become enfeebled by the want of this stimulus of pleasurable sensation, and in consequence stop for a time, and then become inverted; for they cannot become inverted without being previously stopped. Now that this inversion of the trains of motion of the fibres of the stomach is owing to the deficiency of pleasurable sensation is evinced from this circumstance, that a nauseous idea excited by words will produce vomiting as effectually us a nauseous drug.

Hence it appears, that the act of nausea or vomiting expends less sensorial power than the usual peristaltic motions of the stomach in the digestion of our aliment; and that hence there is a greater quantity of sensorial power becomes accumulated in the fibres of the stomach, and more of it in consequence to spare for the action of those parts of the system, which are thus associated with the stomach, as of the whole absorbent series of vessels, and which are at the same time excited by their usual stimuli.

It seems that the process of nausea or vomiting uses less sensory power than the normal peristaltic movements of the stomach during digestion. As a result, more sensory power builds up in the stomach's fibers, providing additional power for the functioning of those parts of the system that are connected to the stomach, like the entire network of absorbent vessels, which are also stimulated by their typical triggers.

From this we can understand, how after the operation of an emetic the stomach becomes more irritable and sensible to the stimulus, and the pleasure of food; since as the sensorial power becomes accumulated during the nausea and vomiting, the digestive power is afterwards exerted more forceably for a time. It should, however, be here remarked, that though vomiting is in general produced by the defect of this stimulus of pleasurable sensation, as when a nauseous drug is administered; yet in long continued vomiting, as in sea-sickness, or from habitual dram-drinking, it arises from deficiency of sensorial power, which in the former case is exhausted by the increased exertion of the irritative ideas of vision, and in the latter by the frequent application of an unnatural stimulus.

From this, we can understand how after the effects of an emetic, the stomach becomes more sensitive and responsive to the pleasure of food. As the sensory power builds up during nausea and vomiting, the digestive power is afterward exerted more strongly for a while. However, it should be noted that while vomiting is generally caused by a lack of this pleasurable sensation—like when a nauseating drug is taken—in cases of prolonged vomiting, such as with seasickness or from excessive drinking, it results from a deficiency in sensory power. In the former situation, this power is drained by the heightened stimulation of visual irritations, and in the latter, by the repeated exposure to an unnatural stimulus.

4. An example of the fourth circumstance above mentioned, where both the primary and secondary parts of a train of motions proceed with energy less than natural, may be observed in the dyspnœa, which occurs in going into a very cold bath, and which has been described and explained in Sect. XXXII. 3. 2.

4. An example of the fourth circumstance above mentioned, where both the primary and secondary parts of a train of motions proceed with energy less than natural, may be observed in the dyspnœa, which occurs in going into a very cold bath, and which has been described and explained in Sect. XXXII. 3. 2.

And by the increased debility of the pulsations of the heart and arteries during the operation of an emetic. Secondly, from the slowness and intermission of the pulsations of the heart from the incessant efforts to vomit occasioned by an overdose of digitalis. And thirdly, from the total stoppage of the motions of the heart, or death, in consequence of the torpor of the stomach, when affected with the commencement or cold paroxysm of the gout. See Sect. XXV. 17.

And by the increased debility of the pulsations of the heart and arteries during the operation of an emetic. Secondly, from the slowness and intermission of the pulsations of the heart from the incessant efforts to vomit occasioned by an overdose of digitalis. And thirdly, from the total stoppage of the motions of the heart, or death, in consequence of the torpor of the stomach, when affected with the commencement or cold paroxysm of the gout. See Sect. XXV. 17.

II. 1. The primary and secondary parts of the trains of sensitive association reciprocally affect each other in different manners. 1. The increased sensation of the primary part may cease, when that of the secondary part commences. 2. The increased action of the primary part may cease, when that of the secondary part commences. 3. The primary part may have increased sensation, and the secondary part increased action. 4. The primary part may have increased action, and the secondary part increased sensation.

II. 1. The primary and secondary parts of the sensitive association trains influence each other in various ways. 1. The heightened sensation of the primary part may stop when the secondary part starts. 2. The increased activity of the primary part may stop when the secondary part starts. 3. The primary part may have heightened sensation, while the secondary part has increased activity. 4. The primary part may have increased activity, while the secondary part has heightened sensation.

Examples of the first mode, where the increased sensation of the primary part of a train of sensitive association ceases, when that of the secondary part commences, are not unfrequent; as this is the general origin of those pains, which continue some time without being attended with inflammation, such as the pain at the pit of the stomach from a stone at the neck of the gall-bladder, and the pain of strangury in the glans penis from a stone at the neck of the urinary bladder. In both these cases the part, which is affected secondarily, is believed to be much more sensible than the part primarily affected, as described in the catalogue of diseases, Class II. 1. 1. 11. and IV. 2. 2. 2. and IV. 2. 2. 4.

Examples of the first mode, where the heightened sensation of the main part of a chain of sensitive connections stops when that of the secondary part starts, are quite common. This is the usual cause of those pains that last for a while without being accompanied by inflammation, such as the pain in the pit of the stomach from a stone at the neck of the gallbladder, and the pain of strangury in the glans penis from a stone at the neck of the urinary bladder. In both cases, the secondary affected area is believed to be much more sensitive than the initially affected area, as outlined in the catalog of diseases, Class II. 1. 1. 11. and IV. 2. 2. 2. and IV. 2. 2. 4.

The hemicrania, or nervous headach, as it is called, when it originates from a decaying tooth, is another disease of this kind; as the pain of the carious tooth always ceases, when the pain over one eye and temple commences. And it is probable, that the violent pains, which induce convulsions in painful epilepsies, are produced in the same manner, from a more sensible part sympathizing with a diseased one of less sensibility. See Catalogue of Diseases, Class IV. 2. 2. 8. and III. 1. 1. 6.

The hemicrania, or nervous headache, as it's called, occurs when it comes from a decaying tooth; it's another type of this condition. The pain from the decayed tooth always stops when the pain starts over one eye and in the temple. It's likely that the intense pain that triggers convulsions in severe epileptic episodes is caused in a similar way, with a more sensitive area reacting to a less sensitive, diseased one. See Catalogue of Diseases, Class IV. 2. 2. 8. and III. 1. 1. 6.

The last tooth, or dens sapientiæ, of the upper jaw most frequently decays first, and is liable to produce pain over the eye and temple of that side. The last tooth of the under-jaw is also liable to produce a similar hemicrania, when it begins to decay. When a tooth in the upper-jaw is the cause of the headach, a slighter pain is sometimes perceived on the cheek-bone. And when a tooth in the lower-jaw is the cause of headach, a pain sometimes affects the tendons of the muscles of the neck, which are attached near the jaws. But the clavus hystericus, or pain about the middle of the parietal bone on one side of the head, I have seen produced by the second of the molares, or grinders, of the under-jaw; of which I shall relate the following case. See Class IV. 2. 2. 8.

The last tooth, or wisdom tooth, in the upper jaw usually decays first and can cause pain around the eye and temple on that side. The last tooth in the lower jaw can also cause similar headaches when it starts to decay. If a tooth in the upper jaw is causing a headache, a milder pain may sometimes be felt in the cheekbone. And if a tooth in the lower jaw is the source of the headache, it can occasionally cause pain in the neck muscles near the jaw. However, I have observed that pain around the middle of the parietal bone on one side of the head, known as hysterical headache, can be caused by the second molar in the lower jaw; I will recount the following case. See Class IV. 2. 2. 8.

Mrs. ——, about 30 years of age, was seized with great pain about the middle of the right parietal bone, which had continued a whole day before I saw her, and was so violent as to threaten to occasion convulsions. Not being able to detect a decaying tooth, or a tender one, by examination with my eye, or by striking them with a tea-spoon, and fearing bad consequences from her tendency to convulsion, I advised her to extract the last tooth of the under-jaw on the affected side; which was done without any good effect. She was then directed to lose blood, and to take a brisk cathartic; and after that had operated, about 60 drops of laudanum were given her, with large doses of bark; by which the pain was removed. In about a fortnight she took a cathartic medicine by ill advice, and the pain returned with greater violence in the same place; and, before I could arrive, as she lived 30 miles from me, she suffered a paralytic stroke; which affected her limbs and her face on one side, and relieved the pain of her head.

Mrs. ——, around 30 years old, was hit with severe pain in the middle of her right parietal bone that lasted an entire day before I examined her, and it was intense enough to risk causing convulsions. I couldn't find any decayed or tender teeth through visual inspection or by tapping them with a teaspoon, and worried about potential complications from her convulsive tendencies, I recommended extracting the last tooth on the lower jaw on the affected side. This was done, but it didn't help. She was then advised to have some blood drawn and to take a strong laxative; after that worked, she was given about 60 drops of laudanum along with high doses of bark, which relieved the pain. About two weeks later, she took a laxative against advice, and the pain came back even more intensely in the same spot. Before I could reach her, since she lived 30 miles away, she suffered a stroke that affected one side of her limbs and face, which relieved her head pain.

About a year afterwards I was again called to her on account of a pain as violent as before exactly on the same part of the other parietal bone. On examining her mouth I found the second molaris of the under-jaw on the side before affected was now decayed, and concluded, that this tooth had occasioned the stroke of the palsy by the pain and consequent exertion it had caused. On this account I earnestly entreated her to allow the sound molaris of the same jaw opposite to the decayed one to be extracted; which was forthwith done, and the pain of her head immediately ceased, to the astonishment of her attendants.

About a year later, I was called to see her again because she was experiencing a pain as intense as before, exactly in the same spot on the other parietal bone. When I examined her mouth, I found that the second molar on the lower jaw on the side that had been affected was now decayed. I concluded that this tooth had caused the stroke of paralysis due to the pain and the strain it had caused. For this reason, I strongly urged her to let me extract the healthy molar on the same jaw, opposite the decayed one. This was done immediately, and her headache stopped right away, to the surprise of those around her.

In the cases above related of the pain existing in a part distant from the seat of the disease, the pain is owing to defect of the usual motions of the painful part. This appears from the coldness, paleness, and emptiness of the affected vessels, or of the extremities of the body in general, and from there being no tendency to inflammation. The increased action of the primary part of these associated motions, as of the hepatic termination of the bile-duct; from the stimulus of a gall-stone, or of the interior termination of the urethra from the stimulus of a stone in the bladder, or lastly, of a decaying tooth in hemicrania, deprives the secondary part of these associated motions, namely, the exterior terminations of the bile-duct or urethra, or the pained membranes of the head in hemicrania, of their natural share of sensorial power: and hence the secondary parts of these sensitive trains of association become pained from the deficiency of their usual motions, which is accompanied with deficiency of secretions and of heat. See Sect. IV. 5. XII. 5. 3. XXXIV. 1.

In the cases above related of the pain existing in a part distant from the seat of the disease, the pain is owing to defect of the usual motions of the painful part. This appears from the coldness, paleness, and emptiness of the affected vessels, or of the extremities of the body in general, and from there being no tendency to inflammation. The increased action of the primary part of these associated motions, as of the hepatic termination of the bile-duct; from the stimulus of a gall-stone, or of the interior termination of the urethra from the stimulus of a stone in the bladder, or lastly, of a decaying tooth in hemicrania, deprives the secondary part of these associated motions, namely, the exterior terminations of the bile-duct or urethra, or the pained membranes of the head in hemicrania, of their natural share of sensorial power: and hence the secondary parts of these sensitive trains of association become pained from the deficiency of their usual motions, which is accompanied with deficiency of secretions and of heat. See Sect. IV. 5. XII. 5. 3. XXXIV. 1.

Why does the pain of the primary part of the association cease, when that of the secondary part commences? This is a question of intricacy, but perhaps not inexplicable. The pain of the primary part of these associated trains of motion was owing to too great stimulus, as of the stone at the neck of the bladder, and was consequently caused by too great action of the pained part. This greater action than natural of the primary part of these associated motions, by employing or expending the sensorial power of irritation belonging to the whole associated train of motions, occasioned torpor, and consequent pain in the secondary part of the associated train; which was possessed of greater sensibility than the primary part of it. Now the great pain of the secondary part of the train, as soon as it commences, employs or expends the sensorial power of sensation belonging to the whole associated train of motions; and in consequence the motions of the primary part, though increased by the stimulus of an extraneous body, cease to be accompanied with pain or sensation.

Why does the pain in the primary part of the association stop when the pain in the secondary part starts? This is a complex question, but perhaps it can be explained. The pain in the primary part of these connected movements was due to excessive stimulation, like a stone in the bladder, which resulted from too much action in the affected area. This increased activity in the primary part drained the sensory energy linked to the entire set of motions, leading to numbness and subsequent pain in the secondary part, which was more sensitive than the primary part. Now, when the severe pain in the secondary part begins, it uses up the sensory energy associated with the whole set of movements, and as a result, the actions in the primary part, even though intensified by the stimulus of an external object, stop being painful or sensitive.

If this mode of reasoning be just it explains a curious fact, why when two parts of the body are strongly stimulated, the pain is felt only in one of them, though it is possible by voluntary attention it may be alternately perceived in them both. In the same manner, when two new ideas are presented to us from the stimulus of external bodies, we attend to but one of them at a time. In other words, when one set of fibres, whether of the muscles or organs of sense, contract so strongly as to excite much sensation; another set of fibres contracting more weakly do not excite sensation at all, because the sensorial power of sensation is pre-occupied by the first set of fibres. So we cannot will more than one effect at once, though by associations previously formed we can move many fibres in combination.

If this way of thinking is correct, it explains an interesting fact: when two parts of the body are stimulated strongly, we only feel pain in one of them, though we can switch our focus to feel pain in both if we pay attention. Similarly, when we encounter two new ideas from external stimuli, we can only focus on one at a time. In other words, when one group of fibers, whether in our muscles or sensory organs, contracts strongly enough to create a noticeable sensation, another group of fibers that contracts more weakly doesn't generate any sensation at all because the sensation power is already taken up by the first group. Thus, we can't consciously will more than one effect at a time, but through previously formed associations, we can activate many fibers together.

Thus in the instances above related, the termination of the bile duct in the duodenum, and the exterior extremity of the urethra, are more sensible than their other terminations. When these parts are deprived of their usual motions by deficiency of sensorial power, as above explained, they become painful according to law the fifth in Section IV. and the less pain originally excited by the stimulus of concreted bile, or of a stone at their other extremities ceases to be perceived. Afterwards, however, when the concretions of bile, or the stone on the urinary bladder, become more numerous or larger, the pain from their increased stimulus becomes greater than the associated pain; and is then felt at the neck of the gall bladder or urinary bladder; and the pain of the glans penis, or at the pit of the stomach, ceases to be perceived.

Thus in the instances above related, the termination of the bile duct in the duodenum, and the exterior extremity of the urethra, are more sensible than their other terminations. When these parts are deprived of their usual motions by deficiency of sensorial power, as above explained, they become painful according to law the fifth in Section IV. and the less pain originally excited by the stimulus of concreted bile, or of a stone at their other extremities ceases to be perceived. Afterwards, however, when the concretions of bile, or the stone on the urinary bladder, become more numerous or larger, the pain from their increased stimulus becomes greater than the associated pain; and is then felt at the neck of the gall bladder or urinary bladder; and the pain of the glans penis, or at the pit of the stomach, ceases to be perceived.

2. Examples of the second mode, where the increased action of the primary part of a train of sensitive association ceases, when that of the secondary part commences, are also not unfrequent; as this is the usual manner of the translation of inflammations from internal to external parts of the system, such as when an inflammation of the liver or stomach is translated to the membranes of the foot, and forms the gout; or to the skin of the face, and forms the rosy drop; or when an inflammation of the membranes of the kidneys is translated to the skin of the loins, and forms one kind of herpes, called shingles; in these cases by whatever cause the original inflammation may have been produced, as the secondary part of the train of sensitive association is more sensible, it becomes exerted with greater violence than the first part of it; and by both its increased pain, and the increased motion of its fibres, so far diminishes or exhausts the sensorial power of sensation; that the primary part of the train being less sensible ceases both to feel pain, and to act with unnatural energy.

2. Examples of the second mode, where the heightened activity of the main part of a sensitive association stops when that of the secondary part begins, are also quite common; this is the typical way that inflammations can shift from internal to external areas of the body. For instance, when an inflammation in the liver or stomach moves to the membranes of the foot, it results in gout; or when it moves to the facial skin, causing what’s known as rosy drop; or when inflammation of the kidney membranes translates to the skin of the lower back, resulting in a type of herpes called shingles. In these cases, no matter how the original inflammation was triggered, the secondary part of the sensitive association tends to be more responsive and therefore reacts more intensely than the first part. This leads to increased pain and heightened movement in its fibers, which significantly reduces or depletes the sensorial power of sensation, causing the primary part of the association to become less responsive, ultimately resulting in it feeling no pain and reducing its unnatural activity.

3. Examples of the third mode, where the primary part of a train of sensitive association of motions may experience increased sensation, and the secondary part increased action, are likewise not unfrequent; as it is in this manner that most inflammations commence. Thus, after standing some time in snow, the feet become affected with the pain of cold, and a common coryza, or inflammation of the membrane of the nostrils, succeeds. It is probable that the internal inflammations, as pleurisies, or hepatitis, which are produced after the cold paroxysm of fever, originate in the same manner from the sympathy of those parts with some others, which were previously pained from quiescence; as happens to various parts of the system during the cold fits of fevers. In these cases it would seem, that the sensorial power of sensation becomes accumulated during the pain of cold, as the torpor of the vessels occasioned by the defect of heat contributes to the increase or accumulation of the sensorial power of irritation, and that both these become exerted on some internal part, which was not rendered torpid by the cold which affected the external parts, nor by its association with them; or which sooner recovered its sensibility. This requires further consideration.

3. Examples of the third mode, where the main part of a sensitive association of movements may feel heightened sensations, and the secondary part may experience increased activity, are not uncommon; this is how most inflammations begin. For instance, after standing in snow for a while, the feet feel the pain of cold, and then a common cold, or inflammation of the nasal membrane, follows. It's likely that internal inflammations, like pleurisies or hepatitis, which occur after the cold stage of a fever, arise in a similar way due to the connection of those areas with others that were previously affected by the cold; this happens to various parts of the body during the cold stages of fevers. In these situations, it seems that the ability to sense pain builds up during the cold pain, as the reduced blood flow caused by the lack of heat leads to an increase in the ability to feel irritation, and both of these affect some internal part that wasn’t made numb by the cold affecting the outside or was quicker to regain its sensitivity. This needs further exploration.

4. An example of the fourth mode, or where the primary part of a sensitive association of motions may have increased action, and the secondary part increased sensation, may be taken from the pain of the shoulder, which attends inflammation of the membranes of the liver, see Class IV. 2. 2. 9.; in this circumstance so much sensorial power seems to be expended in the violent actions and sensations of the inflamed membranes of the liver, that the membranes associated with them become quiescent to their usual stimuli, and painful in consequence.

4. An example of the fourth mode, where the main part of a sensitive connection of movements may have intensified action, and the secondary part heightened sensation, can be seen in the pain of the shoulder that accompanies inflammation of the liver membranes, see Class IV. 2. 2. 9.; in this situation, so much sensory energy seems to be used up in the intense actions and feelings of the inflamed liver membranes that the membranes linked to them become unresponsive to their usual triggers and painful as a result.

There may be other modes in which the primary and secondary parts of the trains of associated sensitive motions may reciprocally affect each other, as may be seen by looking over Class IV. in the catalogue of diseases; all which may probably be resolved into the plus and minus of sensorial power, but we have not yet had sufficient observations made upon them with a view to this doctrine.

There might be other ways in which the main and secondary parts of the related sensitive movements can influence each other, as you can see by reviewing Class IV. in the disease catalog; all of which could likely be explained by the positive and negative aspects of sensory power, but we haven't conducted enough observations on them to support this theory yet.

III. The associated trains of our ideas may have sympathies, and their primary and secondary parts affect each other in some manner similar to those above described; and may thus occasion various curious phenomena not yet adverted to, besides those explained in the Sections on Dreams, Reveries, Vertigo, and Drunkenness; and may thus disturb the deductions of our reasonings, as well as the streams of our imaginations; present us with false degrees of fear, attach unfounded value to trivial circumstances; give occasion to our early prejudices and antipathies; and thus embarrass the happiness of our lives. A copious and curious harvest might be reaped from this province of science, in which, however, I shall not at present wield my sickle.

III. The connected thoughts we have might resonate with each other, and their main and secondary elements influence one another in a way similar to what was described earlier; this can lead to various intriguing phenomena that haven't been addressed yet, apart from those discussed in the Sections on Dreams, Daydreams, Dizziness, and Intoxication; and can disrupt our logical thinking as well as our imagination; present us with exaggerated fears, assign unreasonable importance to trivial things; fuel our early biases and dislikes; and can therefore complicate our happiness. There's a wealth of interesting insights to be gained from this field of study, but I won’t be exploring that right now.



SECT. XXXVI.

OF THE PERIODS OF DISEASES.

DISEASE PERIODS.

I. Muscles excited by volition soon cease to contract, or by sensation, or by irritation, owing to the exhaustion of sensorial power. Muscles subjected to less stimulus have their sensorial power accumulated. Hence the periods of some fevers. Want of irritability after intoxication. II. 1. Natural actions catenated with daily habits of life. 2. With solar periods. Periods of sleep. Of evacuating the bowels. 3. Natural actions catenated with lunar periods. Menstruation. Venereal orgasm of animals. Barrenness. III. Periods of diseased animal actions from stated returns of nocturnal cold, from solar and lunar influence. Periods of diurnal fever, hectic fever, quotidian, tertian, quartan fever. Periods of gout, pleurisy, of fevers with arterial debility, and with arterial strength, Periods of rhaphania, of nervous cough, hemicrania, arterial hæmorrhages, hæmorrhoids, hæmoptoe, epilepsy, palsy, apoplexy, madness. IV. Critical days depend on lunar periods. Lunar periods in the small pox.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Voluntary muscle contractions quickly stop, whether caused by will, sensation, or irritation, due to the depletion of sensory power. Muscles that experience less stimulation build up their sensory power. This explains some fever cycles. Lack of muscle response after intoxication. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Natural actions connected with daily life habits. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Related to solar cycles. Sleep periods. Times for bowel movements. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Natural actions linked to lunar cycles. Menstruation. Animal sexual responses. Infertility. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Periods of illness in animals arising from regular occurrences of night chills, solar and lunar influences. Types of fevers like daytime fever, hectic fever, daily, every other day, and every fourth day fevers. Periods of gout, pleurisy, fevers with weak and strong arteries, periods of nerve-related issues, coughing, migraines, arterial bleeding, hemorrhoids, coughing up blood, epilepsy, paralysis, strokes, and madness. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Critical days are influenced by lunar cycles. Lunar cycles relate to smallpox occurrences.

I. If any of our muscles be made to contract violently by the power of volition, as those of the fingers, when any one hangs by his hands on a swing, fatigue soon ensues; and the muscles cease to act owing to the temporary exhaustion of the spirit of animation; as soon as this is again accumulated in the muscles, they are ready to contract again by the efforts of volition.

I. When we force any of our muscles to tighten strongly using our will, like the muscles in our fingers when someone hangs from their hands on a swing, fatigue sets in quickly; the muscles stop working due to a temporary drain on their energy. Once this energy is restored in the muscles, they can contract again with the power of our will.

Those violent muscular actions induced by pain become in the same manner intermitted and recurrent; as in labour-pains, vomiting, tenesmus, strangury; owing likewise to the temporary exhaustion of the spirit of animation, as above mentioned.

Those intense muscle movements triggered by pain become intermittent and recurring in the same way; like labor pains, vomiting, straining, and painful urination; also due to the temporary depletion of life energy, as mentioned above.

When any stimulus continues long to act with unnatural violence, so as to produce too energetic action of any of our moving organs, those motions soon cease, though the stimulus continues to act; as in looking long on a bright object, as on an inch-square of red silk laid on white paper in the sunshine. See Plate I. in Sect. III. 1.

When any stimulus continues long to act with unnatural violence, so as to produce too energetic action of any of our moving organs, those motions soon cease, though the stimulus continues to act; as in looking long on a bright object, as on an inch-square of red silk laid on white paper in the sunshine. See Plate I. in Sect. III. 1.

On the contrary, where less of the stimulus of volition, sensation, or irritation, have been applied to a muscle than usual; there appears to be an accumulation of the spirit of animation in the moving organ; by which it is liable to act with greater energy from less quantity of stimulus, than was previously necessary to excite it into so great action; as after having been immersed in snow the cutaneous vessels of our hands are excited into stronger action by the stimulus of a less degree of heat, than would previously have produced that effect.

On the other hand, when a muscle experiences less stimulus from will, sensation, or irritation than normal, it seems to build up a sort of energy that allows it to respond more forcefully to a smaller amount of stimulus than was needed before to trigger such strong activity. For example, after being exposed to snow, the blood vessels in our hands react more strongly to a lower temperature than would have been enough to cause that reaction earlier.

From hence the periods of some fever-fits may take their origin, either simply, or by their accidental coincidence with lunar and solar periods, or with the diurnal periods of heat and cold, to be treated of below; for during the cold fit at the commencement of a fever, from whatever cause that cold fit may have been induced, it follows, 1. That the spirit of animation must become accumulated in the parts, which exert during this cold fit less than their natural quantity of action. 2. If the cause producing the cold fit does not increase, or becomes diminished; the parts before benumbed or inactive become now excitable by smaller stimulus, and are thence thrown into more violent action than is natural; that is a hot fit succeeds the cold one. 3. By the energetic action of the system during the hot fit, if it continues long, an exhaustion of the spirit of animation takes place; and another cold fit is liable to succeed, from the moving system not being excitable into action from its usual stimulus. This inirritability of the system from a too great previous stimulus, and consequent exhaustion of sensorial power, is the cause of the general debility, and sickness, and head-ach, some hours after intoxication. And hence we see one of the causes of the periods of fever-fits; which however are frequently combined with the periods of our diurnal habits, or of heat and cold, or of solar or lunar periods.

From this, the timing of some fever episodes may originate, either on its own or by coinciding with lunar and solar cycles, or with daily patterns of heat and cold, which will be discussed below. During the cold phase at the start of a fever, regardless of how that cold phase was triggered, it follows that: 1. The life force must build up in the parts that are acting less than their normal level during this cold phase. 2. If the cause of the cold phase does not increase or starts to decrease, the previously numb or inactive parts become responsive to smaller stimuli, leading to a more intense reaction than usual; in other words, a hot phase follows the cold one. 3. If the body's active response during the hot phase lasts for a long time, there will be a depletion of the life force, and another cold phase may follow, as the active system can no longer respond to its usual stimuli. This lack of responsiveness due to excessive previous stimulation and the resulting exhaustion of sensory power is why there is general weakness, nausea, and headache several hours after intoxication. Thus, we can see one of the reasons for the timing of fever episodes; however, these are often intertwined with the timing of our daily habits, or with phases of heat and cold, or with solar or lunar cycles.

When besides the tendency to quiescence occasioned by the expenditure of sensorial power during the hot fit of fever, some other cause of torpor, as the solar or lunar periods, is necessary to the introduction of a second cold fit; the fever becomes of the intermittent kind; that is, there is a space of time intervenes between the end of the hot fit, and the commencement of the next cold one. But where no exteriour cause is necessary to the introduction of the second cold fit; no such interval of health intervenes; but the second cold fit commences, as soon as the sensorial power is sufficiently exhausted by the hot fit; and the fever becomes continual.

When in addition to the tendency to calmness caused by the depletion of sensory energy during the intense heat of a fever, another factor like solar or lunar cycles is needed to trigger a second chill, the fever is classified as intermittent. This means there is a period of time between the end of the hot phase and the start of the next chill. However, when no external factor is needed for the onset of the second chill, there is no gap of health; the second chill starts as soon as the sensory energy is sufficiently drained by the hot phase, and the fever becomes continuous.

II. 1. The following are natural animal actions, which are frequently catenated with our daily habits of life, as well as excited by their natural irritations. The periods of hunger and thirst become catenated with certain portions of time, or degrees of exhaustion, or other diurnal habits of life. And if the pain of hunger be not relieved by taking food at the usual time, it is liable to cease till the next period of time or other habits recur; this is not only true in respect to our general desire of food, but the kinds of it also are governed by this periodical habit; insomuch that beer taken to breakfast will disturb the digestion of those, who have been accustomed to tea; and tea taken at dinner will disagree with those, who have been accustomed to beer. Whence it happens, that those, who have weak stomachs, will be able to digest more food, if they take their meals at regular hours; because they have both the stimulus of the aliment they take, and the periodical habit, to assist their digestion.

II. 1. The following are natural animal behaviors that are often linked to our daily routines and triggered by their natural irritations. Hunger and thirst become connected to specific times or levels of exhaustion, as well as other daily habits. If the pain of hunger isn't relieved by eating at the usual time, it may stop until the next scheduled time or other habits return. This holds true not just for our general desire for food, but also for the types of food we prefer, as these preferences are influenced by our regular habits. For instance, having beer for breakfast can upset the digestion of those used to tea, and tea at dinner can be hard to digest for those who typically drink beer. As a result, people with weak stomachs may be able to digest more food if they eat at regular times, since they benefit from both the stimulus of the food they consume and the consistency of their eating schedule to aid their digestion.

The periods of emptying the bladder are not only dependent on the acrimony or distention of the water in it, but are frequently catenated with external cold applied to the skin, as in cold bathing, or washing the hands; or with other habits of life, as many are accustomed to empty the bladder before going to bed, or into the house after a journey, and this whether it be full or not.

The times when we empty our bladder aren’t just influenced by how full or uncomfortable we feel, but are often linked to external cold on our skin, like when we take a cold bath or wash our hands. They can also be related to our daily habits; for example, many people tend to use the bathroom before going to bed or as soon as they get home after traveling, regardless of whether their bladder is full or not.

Our times of respiration are not only governed by the stimulus of the blood in the lungs, or our desire of fresh air, but also by our attention to the hourly objects before us. Hence when a person is earnestly contemplating an idea of grief, he forgets to breathe, till the sensation in his lungs becomes very urgent; and then a sigh succeeds for the purpose of more forceably pushing forwards the blood, which is accumulated in the lungs.

Our breathing isn't just controlled by how our blood feels in our lungs or our need for fresh air, but also by what we're focused on in the moment. So when someone is deeply thinking about something sad, they may forget to breathe until it hurts, and then they let out a sigh to push the blood that's built up in their lungs.

Our times of respiration are also frequently governed in part by our want of a steady support for the actions of our arms, and hands, as in threading a needle, or hewing wood, or in swimming; when we are intent upon these objects, we breathe at the intervals of the exertion of the pectoral muscles.

Our breathing patterns are often influenced by our need for a stable surface to support our arms and hands, such as when we're threading a needle, chopping wood, or swimming. When we focus on these tasks, we breathe at the pauses between using our chest muscles.

2. The following natural animal actions are influenced by solar periods. The periods of sleep and of waking depend much on the solar period, for we are inclined to sleep at a certain hour, and to awake at a certain hour, whether we have had more or less fatigue during the day, if within certain limits; and are liable to wake at a certain hour, whether we went to bed earlier or later, within certain limits. Hence it appears, that those who complain of want of sleep, will be liable to sleep better or longer, if they accustom themselves to go to rest, and to rise, at certain hours.

2. The following natural behaviors of animals are affected by the sun’s cycles. Our sleep and wake times are heavily influenced by these cycles; we tend to fall asleep at specific times and wake up at specific times, regardless of how tired we are during the day, as long as it's within certain limits. We can wake up at the same time, whether we went to bed early or late, again within certain limits. Therefore, it seems that those who struggle with insomnia may find they sleep better or longer if they train themselves to go to bed and wake up at regular times.

The periods of evacuating the bowels are generally connected with some part of the solar day, as well as with the acrimony or distention occasioned by the feces. Hence one method of correcting costiveness is by endeavouring to establish a habit of evacuation at a certain hour of the day, as recommended by Mr. Locke, which may be accomplished by using daily voluntary efforts at those times, joined with the usual stimulus of the material to be evacuated.

The times for bowel movements are usually linked to a specific part of the day, as well as to the discomfort or fullness caused by the stool. Therefore, one way to manage constipation is to try to create a routine for bowel movements at a certain time of day, as suggested by Mr. Locke. This can be achieved by making daily efforts at those times, along with the usual urge from the stool that needs to be eliminated.

3. The following natural animal actions are connected with lunar periods. 1. The periods of female menstruation are connected with lunar periods to great exactness, in some instances even to a few hours. These do not commence or terminate at the full or change, or at any other particular part of the lunation, but after they have commenced at any part of it, they continue to recur at that part with great regularity, unless disturbed by some violent circumstance, as explained in Sect. XXXII. No. 6. their return is immediately caused by deficient venous absorption, which is owing to the want of the stimulus, designed by nature, of amatorial copulation, or of the growing fetus. When the catamenia returns sooner than the period of lunation, it shows a tendency of the constitution to inirritability; that is to debility, or deficiency of sensorial power, and is to be relieved by small doses of steel and opium.

3. The following natural animal actions are connected with lunar periods. 1. The periods of female menstruation are connected with lunar periods to great exactness, in some instances even to a few hours. These do not commence or terminate at the full or change, or at any other particular part of the lunation, but after they have commenced at any part of it, they continue to recur at that part with great regularity, unless disturbed by some violent circumstance, as explained in Sect. XXXII. No. 6. their return is immediately caused by deficient venous absorption, which is owing to the want of the stimulus, designed by nature, of amatorial copulation, or of the growing fetus. When the catamenia returns sooner than the period of lunation, it shows a tendency of the constitution to inirritability; that is to debility, or deficiency of sensorial power, and is to be relieved by small doses of steel and opium.

The venereal orgasm of birds and quadrupeds seems to commence, or return about the most powerful lunations at the vernal or autumnal equinoxes; but if it be disappointed of its object, it is said to recur at monthly periods; in this respect resembling the female catamenia. Whence it is believed, that women are more liable to become pregnant at or about the time of their catamenia, than at the intermediate times; and on this account they are seldom much mistaken in their reckoning of nine lunar periods from the last menstruation; the inattention to this may sometimes have been the cause of supposed barrenness, and is therefore worth the observation of those, who wish to have children.

The sexual peak of birds and mammals seems to start or return around the strongest moon phases during the spring or fall equinoxes; however, if it doesn’t happen as expected, it supposedly recurs monthly, similar to female menstrual cycles. Because of this, it’s believed that women are more likely to get pregnant around the time of their periods than at other times. For this reason, they’re usually quite accurate in tracking nine lunar cycles from their last menstruation. Not paying attention to this can sometimes lead to misunderstandings about infertility, so it’s important for those who want to have children to keep this in mind.

III. We now come to the periods of diseased animal actions. The periods of fever-fits, which depend on the stated returns of nocturnal cold, are discussed in Sect. XXXII. 3. Those, which originate or recur at solar or lunar periods, are also explained in Section XXXII. 6. These we shall here enumerate; observing, however, that it is not more surprising, that the influence of the varying attractions of the sun and moon, should raise the ocean into mountains, than that it should affect the nice sensibilities of animal bodies; though the manner of its operation on them is difficult to be understood. It is probable however, that as this influence gradually lessens during the course of the day, or of the lunation, or of the year, some actions of our system become less and less; till at length a total quiescence of some part is induced; which is the commencement of the paroxysms of fever, of menstruation, of pain with decreased action of the affected organ, and of consequent convulsion.

III. We now come to the periods of diseased animal actions. The periods of fever-fits, which depend on the stated returns of nocturnal cold, are discussed in Sect. XXXII. 3. Those, which originate or recur at solar or lunar periods, are also explained in Section XXXII. 6. These we shall here enumerate; observing, however, that it is not more surprising, that the influence of the varying attractions of the sun and moon, should raise the ocean into mountains, than that it should affect the nice sensibilities of animal bodies; though the manner of its operation on them is difficult to be understood. It is probable however, that as this influence gradually lessens during the course of the day, or of the lunation, or of the year, some actions of our system become less and less; till at length a total quiescence of some part is induced; which is the commencement of the paroxysms of fever, of menstruation, of pain with decreased action of the affected organ, and of consequent convulsion.

1. A diurnal fever in some weak people is distinctly observed to come on towards evening, and to cease with a moist skin early in the morning, obeying the solar periods. Persons of weak constitutions are liable to get into better spirits at the access of the hot fit of this evening fever; and are thence inclined to sit up late; which by further enfeebling them increases the disease; whence they lose their strength and their colour.

1. In some people with weak health, a daily fever typically starts in the evening and goes away with a sweaty skin early in the morning, following the cycle of the sun. Individuals with fragile constitutions often feel better just before the fever hits in the evening, which leads them to stay up late; this further weakens them and worsens their illness, causing them to lose both strength and color.

2. The periods of hectic fever, supposed to arise from absorption of matter, obeys the diurnal periods like the above, having the exacerbescence towards evening, and its remission early in the morning, with sweats, or diarrhœa, or urine with white sediment.

2. The intense episodes of fever, thought to be caused by the absorption of substances, follow a daily cycle similar to the one mentioned above, peaking in intensity during the evening and easing off early in the morning, often accompanied by sweats, diarrhea, or urine that has a white sediment.

3. The periods of quotidian fever are either catenated with solar time, and return at the intervals of twenty-four hours; or with lunar time, recurring at the intervals of about twenty-five hours. There is great use in knowing with what circumstances the periodical return or new morbid motions are conjoined, as the most effectual times of exhibiting the proper medicines are thus determined. So if the torpor, which ushers in an ague fit, is catenated with the lunar day: it is known, when the bark or opium must be given, so as to exert its principal effect about the time of the expected return. Solid opium should be given about an hour before the expected cold fit; liquid opium and wine about half an hour; the bark repeatedly for six or eight hours previous to the expected return.

3. The episodes of daily fever are either linked to solar time, returning every twenty-four hours, or to lunar time, occurring roughly every twenty-five hours. It's very helpful to know which circumstances accompany these periodic returns or new illness symptoms, as this determines the best times to administer the right medications. For example, if the sluggishness that precedes a fever fit is associated with the lunar day, it’s clear when to give the bark or opium to ensure it works most effectively at the time of the anticipated return. Solid opium should be administered about an hour before the expected chill; liquid opium and wine about half an hour prior; and the bark should be given repeatedly six to eight hours before the expected return.

4. The periods of tertian fevers, reckoned from the commencement of one cold fit to the commencement of the next cold fit, recur with solar intervals of forty-eight hours, or with lunar ones of about fifty hours. When these of recurrence begin one or two hours earlier than the solar period, it shews, that the torpor or cold fit is produced by less external influence; and therefore that it is more liable to degenerate into a fever with only remissions; so when menstruation recurs sooner than the period of lunation, it shews a tendency of the habit to torpor of inirritability.

4. The episodes of tertian fevers, measured from the start of one cold episode to the start of the next, happen at intervals of forty-eight hours based on solar cycles or about fifty hours based on lunar cycles. When these recurrence times start one or two hours earlier than the solar period, it indicates that the cold episode is influenced by less external factors; therefore, it is more likely to turn into a fever with only temporary breaks. Similarly, when menstruation occurs sooner than the lunar cycle, it indicates a tendency toward torpor or lack of responsiveness.

5. The periods of quartan fevers return at solar intervals of seventy-two hours, or at lunar ones of about seventy-four hours and an half. This kind of ague appears most in moist cold autumns, and in cold countries replete with marshes. It is attended with greater debility, and its cold access more difficult to prevent. For where there is previously a deficiency of sensorial power, the constitution is liable to run into greater torpor from any further diminution of it; two ounces of bark and some steel should be given on the day before the return of the cold paroxysm, and a pint of wine by degrees a few hours before its return, and thirty drops of laudanum one hour before the expected cold fit.

5. Quartan fevers come back in cycles of seventy-two hours, following the solar calendar, or about seventy-four and a half hours based on the lunar calendar. This type of fever is most common in damp, chilly autumns and in cold areas filled with marshes. It causes more fatigue, and it’s harder to prevent the onset of chills. When there’s already a lack of sensory power, the body is more likely to experience extreme lethargy from any additional loss. Two ounces of bark and some iron should be administered the day before the chills are expected to return, along with a pint of wine taken slowly a few hours prior, and thirty drops of laudanum one hour before the expected chill.

6. The periods of the gout generally commence about an hour before sun-rise, which is usually the coldest part of the twenty-four hours. The greater periods of the gout seem also to observe the solar influence, returning about the same season of the year.

6. Gout attacks typically start about an hour before sunrise, which is usually the coldest time of day. The major bouts of gout also seem to follow the pattern of the seasons, occurring around the same time each year.

7. The periods of the pleurisy recur with exacerbation of the pain and fever about sun-set, at which time venesection is of most service. The same may be observed of the inflammatory rheumatism, and other fevers with arterial strength, which seem to obey solar periods; and those with debility seem to obey lunar ones.

7. The episodes of pleurisy come back with increased pain and fever around sunset, when bloodletting is most effective. The same can be noted for inflammatory rheumatism and other fevers with strong arteries, which seem to follow solar cycles; those with weakness appear to follow lunar cycles.

8. The periods of fevers with arterial debility seem to obey the lunar day, having their access daily nearly an hour later; and have sometimes two accesses in a day, resembling the lunar effects upon the tides.

8. The fever episodes with low blood pressure seem to follow the lunar cycle, occurring about an hour later each day; and sometimes there are two episodes in a day, similar to how the moon affects the tides.

9. The periods of rhaphania, or convulsions of the limbs from rheumatic pains, seem to be connected with solar influence, returning at nearly the same hour for weeks together, unless disturbed by the exhibition of powerful doses of opium.

9. The episodes of rhaphania, or convulsions in the limbs caused by rheumatic pain, seem to be linked to solar activity, recurring at almost the same time for weeks on end, unless interrupted by strong doses of opium.

So the periods of Tussis ferina, or violent cough with slow pulse, called nervous cough, recurs by solar periods. Five grains of opium, given at the time the cough commenced disturbed the period, from seven in the evening to eleven, at which time it regularly returned for some days, during which time the opium was gradually omitted. Then 120 drops of laudanum were given an hour before the access of the cough, and it totally ceased. The laudanum was continued a fortnight, and then gradually discontinued.

So the episodes of Tussis ferina, or intense coughing with a slow pulse, known as nervous cough, happen in cycles related to the sun. Taking five grains of opium when the cough started disrupted the schedule, shifting it from seven in the evening to eleven, when it usually came back for several days. During this time, the opium was gradually reduced. Then, 120 drops of laudanum were administered an hour before the coughing started, and it completely stopped. The laudanum treatment continued for two weeks and was then slowly tapered off.

10. The periods of hemicrania, and of painful epilepsy, are liable to obey lunar periods, both in their diurnal returns, and in their greater periods of weeks, but are also induced by other exciting causes.

10. Episodes of hemicrania and painful epilepsy tend to follow lunar cycles, both in their daily occurrences and in longer cycles of weeks, but can also be triggered by other stimulating factors.

11. The periods of arterial hæmorrhages seem to return at solar periods about the same hour of the evening or morning. Perhaps the venous hæmorrhages obey the lunar periods, as the catamenia, and hæmorrhoids.

11. The episodes of arterial bleeding seem to occur at solar intervals around the same time in the evening or morning. It’s possible that venous bleeding aligns with lunar cycles, similar to menstruation and hemorrhoids.

12. The periods of the hæmorrhoids, or piles, in some recur monthly, in others only at the greater lunar influence about the equinoxes.

12. The episodes of hemorrhoids, or piles, may happen monthly for some, while for others, they only occur during the stronger lunar effects around the equinoxes.

13. The periods of hæmoptoe sometimes obey solar influence, recurring early in the morning for several days; and sometimes lunar periods, recurring monthly; and sometimes depend on our hours of sleep. See Class I. 2. 1. 9.

13. The times of coughing up blood can sometimes be affected by the sun, happening early in the morning for several days; sometimes they follow lunar cycles, happening monthly; and at times they depend on how much sleep we get. See Class I. 2. 1. 9.

14. Many of the first periods of epileptic fits obey the monthly lunation with some degree of accuracy; others recur only at the most powerful lunations before the vernal equinox, and after the autumnal one; but when the constitution has gained a habit of relieving disagreeable sensations by this kind of exertion, the fit recurs from any slight cause.

14. Many of the initial episodes of epileptic seizures follow the lunar cycle with some consistency; others only happen during the strongest moons before the spring equinox and after the fall one; however, once the body has developed a pattern of easing unpleasant sensations through this kind of activity, the seizures can occur from even minor triggers.

15. The attack of palsy and apoplexy are known to recur with great frequency about the equinoxes.

15. The attacks of paralysis and stroke are known to happen very often around the equinoxes.

16. There are numerous instances of the effect of the lunations upon the periods of insanity, whence the name of lunatic has been given to those afflicted with this disease.

16. There are many examples of how the phases of the moon affect the times of madness, which is where the term lunatic comes from for those suffering from this condition.

IV. The critical days, in which fevers are supposed to terminate, have employed the attention of medical philosophers from the days of Hippocrates to the present time. In whatever part of a lunation a fever commences, which owes either its whole cause to solar and lunar influence, or to this in conjunction with other causes; it would seem, that the effect would be the greatest at the full and new moon, as the tides rise highest at those times, and would be the least at the quadratures; thus if a fever-fit should commence at the new or full moon, occasioned by the solar and lunar attraction diminishing some chemical affinity of the particles of blood, and thence decreasing their stimulus on our sanguiferous system, as mentioned in Sect. XXXII. 6. this effect will daily decrease for the first seven days, and will then increase till about the fourteenth day, and will again decrease till about the twenty-first day, and increase again till the end of the lunation. If a fever-fit from the above cause should commence on the seventh day after either lunation, the reverse of the above circumstances would happen. Now it is probable, that those fevers, whose crisis or terminations are influenced by lunations, may begin at one or other of the above times, namely at the changes or quadratures; though sufficient observations have not been made to ascertain this circumstance. Hence I conclude, that the small-pox and measles have their critical days, not governed by the times required for certain chemical changes in the blood, which affect or alter the stimulus of the contagious matter, but from the daily increasing or decreasing effect of this lunar link of catenation, as explained in Section XVII. 3. 3. And as other fevers terminate most frequently about the seventh, fourteenth, twenty-first, or about the end of four weeks, when no medical assistance has disturbed their periods, I conclude, that these crises, or terminations, are governed by periods of the lunations; though we are still ignorant of their manner of operation.

IV. The critical days, in which fevers are supposed to terminate, have employed the attention of medical philosophers from the days of Hippocrates to the present time. In whatever part of a lunation a fever commences, which owes either its whole cause to solar and lunar influence, or to this in conjunction with other causes; it would seem, that the effect would be the greatest at the full and new moon, as the tides rise highest at those times, and would be the least at the quadratures; thus if a fever-fit should commence at the new or full moon, occasioned by the solar and lunar attraction diminishing some chemical affinity of the particles of blood, and thence decreasing their stimulus on our sanguiferous system, as mentioned in Sect. XXXII. 6. this effect will daily decrease for the first seven days, and will then increase till about the fourteenth day, and will again decrease till about the twenty-first day, and increase again till the end of the lunation. If a fever-fit from the above cause should commence on the seventh day after either lunation, the reverse of the above circumstances would happen. Now it is probable, that those fevers, whose crisis or terminations are influenced by lunations, may begin at one or other of the above times, namely at the changes or quadratures; though sufficient observations have not been made to ascertain this circumstance. Hence I conclude, that the small-pox and measles have their critical days, not governed by the times required for certain chemical changes in the blood, which affect or alter the stimulus of the contagious matter, but from the daily increasing or decreasing effect of this lunar link of catenation, as explained in Section XVII. 3. 3. And as other fevers terminate most frequently about the seventh, fourteenth, twenty-first, or about the end of four weeks, when no medical assistance has disturbed their periods, I conclude, that these crises, or terminations, are governed by periods of the lunations; though we are still ignorant of their manner of operation.

In the distinct small-pox the vestiges of lunation are very apparent, after inoculation a quarter of a lunation precedes the commencement of the fever, another quarter terminates with the complete eruption, another quarter with the complete maturation, and another quarter terminates the complete absorption of a material now rendered inoffensive to the constitution.

In distinct smallpox, the effects of the lunar cycle are quite noticeable. After inoculation, a quarter of a lunar cycle passes before the fever starts, another quarter coincides with the full eruption, another quarter with the complete maturation, and the final quarter marks the complete absorption of a substance that is now harmless to the body.



SECT. XXXVII.

OF DIGESTION, SECRETION, NUTRITION.

DIGESTION, SECRETION, NUTRITION.

I. Crystals increase by the greater attraction of their sides. Accretion by chemical precipitations, by welding, by pressure, by agglutination. II. Hunger, digestion, why it cannot be imitated out of the body. Lacteals absorb by animal selection or appetency. III. The glands and pores absorb nutritious particles by animal selection. Organic particles of Buffon. Nutrition applied at the time of elongation of fibres. Like inflammation. IV. It seems easier to have preserved animals than to reproduce them. Old age and death from inirritability. Three causes of this. Original fibres of the organs of sense and muscles unchanged. V. Art of producing long life.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Crystals grow due to the stronger attraction of their sides. They accumulate through chemical precipitation, welding, pressure, and agglutination. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Hunger, digestion, and why they can’t be recreated outside the body. Lacteals absorb based on animal choice or preference. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. The glands and pores take in nutritious particles based on animal selection. Organic particles from Buffon. Nutrition applied during the elongation of fibers. Similar to inflammation. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. It seems easier to preserve animals than to reproduce them. Aging and death caused by lack of stimulation. Three reasons for this. The original fibers of the sense organs and muscles remain unchanged. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. The art of promoting longevity.

I. The larger crystals of saline bodies may be conceived to arise from the combination of smaller crystals of the same form, owing to the greater attractions of their sides than of their angles. Thus if four cubes were floating in a fluid, whose friction or resistance is nothing, it is certain the sides of these cubes would attract each other stronger than their angles; and hence that these four smaller cubes would so arrange themselves as to produce one larger one.

I. The bigger crystals in salty formations can be thought of as coming together from smaller crystals of the same shape because the sides attract each other more than the angles do. So, if you had four cubes floating in a fluid with no friction or resistance, the sides of those cubes would definitely pull towards each other more strongly than their angles would; as a result, those four smaller cubes would position themselves in a way that creates one larger cube.

There are other means of chemical accretion, such as the depositions of dissolved calcareous or siliceous particles, as are seen in the formation of the stalactites of limestone in Derbyshire, or of calcedone in Cornwall. Other means of adhesion are produced by heat and pressure, as in the welding of iron-bars; and other means by simple pressure, as in forcing two pieces of caoutchou, or elastic gum, to adhere; and lastly, by the agglutination of a third substance penetrating the pores of the other two, as in the agglutination of wood by means of animal gluten. Though the ultimate particles of animal bodies are held together during life, as well as after death, by their specific attraction of cohesion, like all other matter; yet it does not appear, that their original organization was produced by chemical laws, and their production and increase must therefore only be looked for from the laws of animation.

There are other ways chemical accumulation occurs, such as the deposition of dissolved calcium or silica particles, seen in the formation of limestone stalactites in Derbyshire or chalcedony in Cornwall. Other forms of adhesion happen through heat and pressure, like welding iron bars; and some occur simply through pressure, as when two pieces of rubber or elastic gum stick together. Lastly, there’s adhesion involving a third substance penetrating the pores of the other two, like how wood can be glued together with animal glue. While the tiny particles in animal bodies are held together during life and after death by their specific attractive force of cohesion, similar to all other matter, it doesn’t seem that their original organization was created by chemical laws, so their creation and growth must be rooted in the laws of life.

II. When the pain of hunger requires relief, certain parts of the material world, which surround us, when applied to our palates, excite into action the muscles of deglutition; and the material is swallowed into the stomach. Here the new aliment becomes mixed with certain animal fluids, and undergoes a chemical process, termed digestion; which however chemistry has not yet learnt to imitate out of the bodies of living animals or vegetables. This process seems very similar to the saccharine process in the lobes of farinaceous seeds, as of barley, when it begins to germinate; except that, along with the sugar, oil and mucilage are also produced; which form the chyle of animals, which is very similar to their milk.

II. When the pain of hunger demands relief, certain things in our physical world activate the muscles involved in swallowing when they touch our taste buds; then, the food is swallowed down into the stomach. Here, the new food mixes with various bodily fluids and goes through a chemical process known as digestion, which chemistry hasn't been able to replicate outside the bodies of living animals or plants. This process is quite similar to the sugary transformation in the seed pods of grains like barley when they start to sprout; except that, along with sugar, oil and mucilage are also produced, which creates the chyle in animals, very much like their milk.

The reason, I imagine, why this chyle-making, or saccharine process, has not yet been imitated by chemical operations, is owing to the materials being in such a situation in respect to warmth, moisture, and motion; that they will immediately change into the vinous or acetous fermentation; except the new sugar be absorbed by the numerous lacteal or lymphatic vessels, as soon as it is produced; which is not easy to imitate in the laboratory.

The reason I think this chyle-making, or sugar-producing process hasn't been replicated by chemical methods is that the materials are in a specific state regarding warmth, moisture, and movement. They will quickly change into alcoholic or acetic fermentation unless the new sugar is absorbed by the many lacteal or lymphatic vessels right after it's made, which is hard to replicate in a lab.

These lacteal vessels have mouths, which are irritated into action by the stimulus of the fluid, which surrounds them; and by animal selection, or appetency, they absorb such part of the fluid as is agreeable to their palate; those parts, for instance, which are already converted into chyle, before they have time to undergo another change by a vinous or acetous fermentation. This animal absorption of fluid is almost visible to the naked eye in the action of the puncta lacrymalia; which imbibe the tears from the eye, and discharge them again into the nostrils.

These tiny vessels have openings that are triggered to work by the fluid around them; and through some kind of selective appetite, they absorb the parts of the fluid that they find pleasant, like those that have already been turned into a substance called chyle, before they have the chance to change further through fermentation. This process of absorbing fluid can almost be seen with the naked eye in the action of the tear ducts, which take in tears from the eye and release them back into the nostrils.

III. The arteries constitute another reservoir of a changeful fluid; from which, after its recent oxygenation in the lungs, a further animal selection of various fluids is absorbed by the numerous glands; these select their respective fluids from the blood, which is perpetually undergoing a chemical change; but the selection by these glands, like that of the lacteals, which open their mouths into the digesting aliment in the stomach, is from animal appetency, not from chemical affinity; secretion cannot therefore be imitated in the laboratory, as it consists in a selection of part of a fluid during the chemical change of that fluid.

III. The arteries serve as another storage for a changing fluid; from which, after it has been recently oxygenated in the lungs, various fluids are absorbed by the many glands. These glands pick their specific fluids from the blood, which is continuously undergoing chemical changes. However, unlike the lacteals that draw from the food being digested in the stomach, the selection made by these glands is driven by animal needs, not by chemical attraction. Therefore, secretion cannot be replicated in a lab, as it involves choosing a part of a fluid during the chemical transformation of that fluid.

The mouths of the lacteals, and lymphatics, and the ultimate terminations of the glands, are finer than can easily be conceived; yet it is probable, that the pores, or interstices of the parts, or coats, which constitute these ultimate vessels, may still have greater tenuity; and that these pores from the above analogy must posses a similar power of irritability, and absorb by their living energy the particles of fluid adapted to their purposes, whether to replace the parts abraded or dissolved, or to elongate and enlarge themselves. Not only every kind of gland is thus endued with its peculiar appetency, and selects the material agreeable to its taste from the blood, but every individual pore acquires by animal selection the material, which it wants; and thus nutrition seems to be performed in a manner so similar to secretion; that they only differ in the one retaining, and the other parting again with the particles, which they have selected from the blood.

The openings of the lacteals, lymphatics, and the final points of the glands are more delicate than one might easily imagine; yet, it's likely that the pores or gaps in these parts or layers, which make up these tiny vessels, could be even finer. These pores, given the analogy above, probably have a similar ability to react and absorb the fluid particles suited to their functions, whether to replace worn-out or dissolved parts, or to grow and expand themselves. Not only is each type of gland equipped with its unique preference, selecting materials it likes from the blood, but each individual pore also picks the material it needs through biological selection. Therefore, nutrition seems to happen in a way that's very similar to secretion, differing only in that one retains the particles while the other releases them after selecting from the blood.

This way of accounting for nutrition from stimulus, and the consequent animal selection of particles, is much more analogous to other phenomena of the animal microcosm, than by having recourse to the microscopic animalcula, or organic particles of Buffon, and Needham; which being already compounded must themselves require nutritive particles to continue their own existence. And must be liable to undergo a change by our digestive or secretory organs; otherwise mankind would soon resemble by their theory the animals, which they feed upon. He, who is nourished by beef or venison, would in time become horned; and he, who feeds on pork or bacon, would gain a nose proper for rooting into the earth, as well as for the perception of odours.

This way of thinking about nutrition as a response to stimuli, and the resulting selection of particles by animals, is much more similar to other phenomena in the animal world than relying on microscopic organisms or organic particles like those proposed by Buffon and Needham. Since these particles are already complex, they also need nutrients to sustain their existence. They would also be affected by our digestive or secretory systems; otherwise, according to their theory, humans would start resembling the animals they consume. Someone who eats beef or venison would eventually grow horns, and someone who eats pork or bacon would develop a nose suitable for rooting in the ground as well as for smelling things.

The whole animal system may be considered as consisting of the extremities of the nerves, or of having been produced from them; if we except perhaps the medullary part of the brain residing in the head and spine, and in the trunks of the nerves. These extremities of the nerves are either of those of locomotion, which are termed muscular fibres; or of those of sensation, which constitute the immediate organs of sense, and which have also their peculiar motions. Now as the fibres, which constitute the bones and membranes, possessed originally sensation and motion; and are liable again to possess them, when they become inflamed; it follows, that those were, when first formed, appendages to the nerves of sensation or locomotion, or were formed from them. And that hence all these solid parts of the body, as they have originally consisted of extremities of nerves, require an apposition of nutritive particles of a similar kind, contrary to the opinion of Buffon and Needham above recited.

The entire animal system can be viewed as made up of nerve endings or having originated from them, with the possible exception of the medullary part of the brain located in the head and spine, and within the nerve trunks. These nerve endings are either related to movement, known as muscular fibers, or related to sensation, which make up the immediate organs of sense and have their unique motions. Since the fibers that make up bones and membranes originally had sensation and movement, and can regain these properties when inflamed, it indicates that they were, at first, extensions of the sensory or mobility nerves or were derived from them. Therefore, all of these solid parts of the body, which were originally made up of nerve endings, need to be supplied with similar nutritive particles, contrary to what Buffon and Needham suggested.

Lastly, as all these filaments have possessed, or do possess, the power of contraction, and of consequent inertion or elongation; it seems probable, that the nutritive particles are applied during their times of elongation; when their original constituent particles are removed to a greater distance from each other. For each muscular or sensual fibre may be considered as a row or string of beads; which approach, when in contraction, and recede during its rest or elongation; and our daily experience shews us, that great action emaciates the system, and that it is repaired during rest.

Lastly, since all these filaments have had, or currently have, the ability to contract, leading to inactivity or elongation, it seems likely that the nourishing particles are applied during their times of elongation, when their original component particles are further apart from each other. Each muscle or sensory fiber can be seen as a row or string of beads, which come together when contracted and pull apart during rest or elongation. Our everyday experience shows us that intense activity weakens the body, and that it is repaired during periods of rest.

Something like this is seen out of the body; for if a hair, or a single untwisted fibre of flax or silk, be soaked in water; it becomes longer and thicker by the water, which is absorbed into its pores. Now if a hair could be supposed to be thus immersed in a solution of particles similar to those, which compose it; one may imagine, that it might be thus increased in weight and magnitude; as the particles of oak-bark increase the substance of the hides of beasts in the process of making leather. I mention these not as philosophic analogies, but as similes to facilitate our ideas, how an accretion of parts may be effected by animal appetences, or selections, in a manner somewhat similar to mechanical or chemical attractions.

Something like this can be observed outside the body; because when a hair or a single untwisted strand of flax or silk is soaked in water, it becomes longer and thicker as it absorbs the water into its pores. Now, if we think of a hair being submerged in a solution of particles similar to those that make it up, one could imagine it increasing in weight and size, just like how the particles of oak bark add substance to animal hides during the leather-making process. I bring these up not as scientific comparisons, but as examples to help us understand how the growth of parts can occur through animal desires or choices, somewhat like mechanical or chemical attractions.

If those new particles of matter, previously prepared by digestion and sanguification, only supply the places of those, which have been abraded by the actions of the system, it is properly termed nutrition. If they are applied to the extremities of the nervous fibrils, or in such quantity as to increase the length or crassitude of them, the body becomes at the same time enlarged, and its growth is increased, as well as its deficiences repaired.

If those new particles of matter, previously created through digestion and blood production, only replace the ones that have been worn away by the body's activities, it's rightly called nutrition. If they are applied to the ends of the nerve fibers, or in such amounts that they increase their length or thickness, the body will also grow larger, and its growth will be enhanced, while any deficiencies will be repaired.

In this last case something more than a simple apposition or selection of particles seems to be necessary; as many parts of the system during its growth are caused to recede from those, with which they were before in contact; as the ends of the bones, or cartilages, recede from each other, as their growth advances: this process resembles inflammation, as appears in ophthalmy, or in the production of new flesh in ulcers, where old vessels are enlarged, and new ones produced; and like that is attended with sensation. In this situation the vessels become distended with blood, and acquire greater sensibility, and may thus be compared to the erection of the penis, or of the nipples of the breasts of women; while new particles become added at the same time; as in the process of nutrition above described.

In this last case, something more than just a simple combination or selection of particles seems necessary; many parts of the system, during its development, start to move away from those they were previously in contact with, just as the ends of bones or cartilage pull apart as they grow. This process is similar to inflammation, as seen in conditions like eye inflammation or the formation of new tissue in ulcers, where existing blood vessels expand and new ones are created; and like that, it is accompanied by sensation. In this scenario, the blood vessels become engorged with blood and become more sensitive, which can be compared to the erection of the penis or the nipples of women; at the same time, new particles are added, similar to the process of nutrition mentioned earlier.

When only the natural growth of the various parts of the body are produced, a pleasurable sensation attends it, as in youth, and perhaps in those, who are in the progress of becoming fat. When an unnatural growth is the consequence, as in inflammatory diseases, a painful sensation attends the enlargement of the system.

When only the natural growth of different parts of the body happens, it feels pleasant, like in youth, and maybe for those who are starting to gain weight. But when growth happens unnaturally, like in inflammatory conditions, it causes a painful feeling as the body swells.

IV. This apposition of new parts, as the old ones disappear, selected from the aliment we take, first enlarges and strengthens our bodies for twenty years, for another twenty years it keeps us in health and vigour, and adds strength and solidity to the system; and then gradually ceases to nourish us properly, and for another twenty years we gradually sink into decay, and finally cease to act, and to exist.

IV. This combination of new components, as the old ones fade away, chosen from the food we consume, first grows and strengthens our bodies for twenty years, then maintains our health and energy for another twenty years, adding strength and stability to the system; after that, it slowly stops nourishing us properly, and for another twenty years we gradually decline, ultimately stopping to function and to exist.

On considering this subject one should have imagined at first view, that it might have been easier for nature to have supported her progeny for ever in health and life, than to have perpetually reproduced them by the wonderful and mysterious process of generation. But it seems our bodies by long habit cease to obey the stimulus of the aliment, which should support us. After we have acquired our height and solidity we make no more new parts, and the system obeys the irritations, sensations, volitions; and associations, with, less and less energy, till the whole sinks into inaction.

When thinking about this topic, one might initially imagine that it would have been easier for nature to keep her offspring healthy and alive forever rather than constantly reproducing them through the amazing and mysterious process of generation. However, it appears that our bodies, through long habits, stop responding to the stimuli from the nourishment that should sustain us. Once we reach our full height and strength, we stop creating new parts, and the system responds to irritations, sensations, intentions, and associations with less and less energy until everything eventually falls into inactivity.

Three causes may conspire to render our nerves less excitable, which have been already mentioned, 1. If a stimulus be greater than natural, it produces too great an exertion of the stimulated organ, and in consequence exhausts the spirit of animation; and the moving organ ceases to act, even though the stimulus be continued. And though rest will recruit this exhaustion, yet some degree of permanent injury remains, as is evident after exposing the eyes long to too strong a light. 2. If excitations weaker than natural be applied, so as not to excite the organ into action, (as when small doses of aloe or rhubarb are exhibited,) they may be gradually increased, without exciting the organ into action; which will thus acquire a habit of disobedience to the stimulus; thus by increasing the dose by degrees, great quantities of opium or wine may be taken without intoxication. See Sect. XII. 3. 1.

Three causes may conspire to render our nerves less excitable, which have been already mentioned, 1. If a stimulus be greater than natural, it produces too great an exertion of the stimulated organ, and in consequence exhausts the spirit of animation; and the moving organ ceases to act, even though the stimulus be continued. And though rest will recruit this exhaustion, yet some degree of permanent injury remains, as is evident after exposing the eyes long to too strong a light. 2. If excitations weaker than natural be applied, so as not to excite the organ into action, (as when small doses of aloe or rhubarb are exhibited,) they may be gradually increased, without exciting the organ into action; which will thus acquire a habit of disobedience to the stimulus; thus by increasing the dose by degrees, great quantities of opium or wine may be taken without intoxication. See Sect. XII. 3. 1.

3. Another mode, by which life is gradually undermined, is when irritative motions continue to be produced in consequence of stimulus, but are not succeeded by sensation; hence the stimulus of contagious matter is not capable of producing fever a second time, because it is not succeeded by sensation. See Sect. XII. 3. 6. And hence, owing to the want of the general pleasurable sensation, which ought to attend digestion and glandular secretion, an irksomeness of life ensues; and, where this is in greater excess, the melancholy of old age occurs, with torpor or debility.

3. Another mode, by which life is gradually undermined, is when irritative motions continue to be produced in consequence of stimulus, but are not succeeded by sensation; hence the stimulus of contagious matter is not capable of producing fever a second time, because it is not succeeded by sensation. See Sect. XII. 3. 6. And hence, owing to the want of the general pleasurable sensation, which ought to attend digestion and glandular secretion, an irksomeness of life ensues; and, where this is in greater excess, the melancholy of old age occurs, with torpor or debility.

From hence I conclude, that it is probable that the fibrillæ, or moving filaments at the extremities of the nerves of sense, and the fibres which constitute the muscles (which are perhaps the only parts of the system that are endued with contractile life) are not changed, as we advance in years, like the other parts of the body; but only enlarged or elongated with our growth; and in consequence they become less and less excitable into action. Whence, instead of gradually changing the old animal, the generation of a totally new one becomes necessary with undiminished excitability; which many years will continue to acquire new parts, or new solidity, and then losing its excitability in time, perish like its parent.

From this, I conclude that it’s likely the fibrils, or moving strands at the tips of the sensory nerves, and the fibers that make up the muscles (which might be the only parts of the system that have the ability to contract) don’t change as we age, unlike other parts of the body. Instead, they just get bigger or longer as we grow; and because of this, they become less and less responsive to action. Therefore, rather than gradually transforming the old being, it becomes necessary to generate an entirely new one that retains its responsiveness. This new being will take many years to develop new parts or new strength, and eventually, it will also lose its responsiveness over time, dying just like its predecessor.

V. From this idea the art of preserving long health and life may be deduced; which must consist in using no greater stimulus, whether of the quantity or kind of our food and drink, or of external circumstances, such as heat, and exercise, and wakefulness, than is sufficient to preserve us in vigour; and gradually, as we grow old to increase the stimulus of our aliment, as the irritability of our system increases.

V. From this idea, we can figure out how to maintain good health and longevity. This involves not using more stimulation—whether through the amount or type of food and drink we consume, or from external factors like heat, exercise, and wakefulness—than what is needed to keep us feeling strong. As we get older, we should slowly increase our intake of food and drink to match the growing sensitivity of our bodies.

The debilitating effects ascribed by the poet MARTIAL to the excessive use of warm bathing in Italy, may with equal propriety be applied to the warm rooms of England; which, with the general excessive stimulus of spirituous or fermented liquors, and in some instances of immoderate venery, contribute to shorten our lives.

The harmful effects that the poet MARTIAL attributed to the overuse of warm baths in Italy can also be applied to the heated rooms in England. These, along with the overall excessive consumption of alcoholic drinks and, in some cases, excessive indulgence in sexual activity, contribute to a shorter lifespan.

Balnea, vina, venus, corrumpunt corpora nostra,

Baths, wine, and love corrupt our bodies,

At faciunt vitam balnea, vina, venus!

They make life with baths, wine, and love!

Wine, women, warmth, against our lives combine;

Wine, women, and warmth blend into our lives;

But what is life without warmth, women, wine!

But what is life without warmth, women, and wine!



SECT. XXXVIII.

OF THE OXYGENATION OF THE BLOOD IN THE LUNGS, AND IN THE PLACENTA.

OF THE OXYGENATION OF THE BLOOD IN THE LUNGS, AND IN THE PLACENTA.

I. Blood absorbs oxygene from the air, whence phosphoric acid changes its colour, gives out heat, and some phlogistic material, and acquires an ethereal spirit, which is dissipated in fibrous motion. II. The placenta is a pulmonary organ like the gills of fish. Oxygenation of the blood from air, from water, by lungs, by gills, by the placenta; necessity of this oxygenation to quadrupeds, to fish, to the fœtus in utero. Placental vessels inserted into the arteries of the mother. Use of cotyledons in cows. Why quadrupeds have not sanguiferous lochia. Oxygenation of the chick in the egg, of feeds. III. The liquor amnii is not excrementitious. It is nutritious. It is found in the esophagus and stomach, and forms the meconium. Monstrous births without heads. Question of Dr. Harvey.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Blood takes in oxygen from the air, which causes phosphoric acid to change its color, release heat, and some flammable substance, while gaining a spiritual essence that is released during movement. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. The placenta acts like a lung, similar to fish gills. The process of oxygenating blood occurs through air, water, lungs, gills, and the placenta; this oxygenation is essential for mammals, fish, and fetuses in the womb. The placenta's vessels connect to the mother’s arteries. The role of cotyledons in cows. Why mammals do not have blood lochia. Oxygenation of a chick while in the egg, and during feeding. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. The amniotic fluid is not waste. It is nutritious. It can be found in the esophagus and stomach, and it contributes to forming meconium. Abnormal births without heads. A question posed by Dr. Harvey.

I. From the recent discoveries of many ingenious philosophers it appears, that during respiration the blood imbibes the vital part of the air, called oxygene, through the membranes of the lungs; and that hence respiration may be aptly compared to a slow combustion. As in combustion the oxygene of the atmosphere unites with some phlogistic or inflammable body, and forms an acid (as in the production of vitriolic acid from sulphur, or carbonic acid from charcoal,) giving out at the same time a quantity of the matter of heat; so in respiration the oxygene of the air unites with the phlogistic part of the blood, and probably produces phosphoric or animal acid, changing the colour of the blood from a dark to a bright red; and probably some of the matter of heat is at the same time given out according to the theory of Dr. Crawford. But as the evolution of heat attends almost all chemical combinations, it is probable, that it also attends the secretions of the various fluids from the blood; and that the constant combinations or productions of new fluids by means of the glands constitute the more general source of animal heat; this seems evinced by the universal evolution of the matter of heat in the blush of shame or of anger; in which at the same time an increased secretion of the perspirable matter occurs; and the partial evolution of it from topical inflammations, as in gout or rheumatism, in which there is a secretion of new blood-vessels.

I. Recent discoveries by many clever philosophers suggest that during breathing, the blood absorbs the essential part of the air, called oxygen, through the lung membranes; and that breathing can be well compared to a slow combustion process. Just like in combustion where the oxygen in the atmosphere joins with a combustible substance to create an acid (like how sulfur produces vitriolic acid, or charcoal produces carbonic acid), releasing heat at the same time; during breathing, the oxygen in the air combines with the combustible part of the blood, likely producing phosphoric or animal acid, which changes the blood's color from dark to bright red; and some heat is probably released too, according to Dr. Crawford's theory. Since heat is produced in almost all chemical reactions, it is likely that it also occurs during the secretion of various fluids from the blood; and that the continuous formation of new fluids via the glands is a significant source of body heat; this is supported by the consistent release of heat when experiencing shame or anger, which also results in increased secretion of sweat; and the partial release of heat from localized inflammations, like in gout or rheumatism, where new blood vessels are formed.

Some medical philosophers have ascribed the heat of animal bodies to the friction of the particles of the blood against the sides of the vessels. But no perceptible heat has ever been produced by the agitation of water, or oil, or quicksilver, or other fluids; except those fluids have undergone at the same time some chemical change, as in agitating milk or wine, till they become sour.

Some medical philosophers have attributed the warmth of animal bodies to the friction of blood particles against the walls of blood vessels. However, no noticeable heat has ever been generated by the motion of water, oil, mercury, or other fluids unless those fluids also underwent some kind of chemical change, like when milk or wine is stirred until it turns sour.

Besides the supposed production of phosphoric acid, and change of colour of the blood, and the production of carbonic acid, there would appear to be something of a more subtile nature perpetually acquired from the atmosphere; which is too fine to be long contained in animal vessels, and therefore requires perpetual renovation; and without which life cannot continue longer than a minute or two; this ethereal fluid is probably secreted from the blood by the brain, and perpetually dissipated in the actions of the muscles and organs of sense.

Besides the supposed production of phosphoric acid, the change in blood color, and the creation of carbonic acid, there seems to be something subtler that is constantly taken in from the atmosphere; it’s too delicate to remain in living beings for long, so it needs to be continuously renewed; without it, life can’t last more than a minute or two. This ethereal fluid is likely secreted from the blood by the brain and is constantly lost through the actions of the muscles and senses.

That the blood acquires something from the air, which is immediately necessary to life, appears from an experiment of Dr. Hare (Philos. Transact. abridged, Vol. III. p. 239.) who found, "that birds, mice, &c. would live as long again in a vessel, where he had crowded in double the quantity of air by a condensing engine, than they did when confined in air of the common density." Whereas if some kind of deleterious vapour only was exhaled from the blood in respiration; the air, when condensed into half its compass, could not be supposed to receive so much of it.

That blood absorbs something from the air that is essential for life, as shown in an experiment by Dr. Hare (Philos. Transact. abridged, Vol. III. p. 239). He discovered that "birds, mice, etc. lived twice as long in a container where he had compressed double the amount of air using a condensing engine than they did when confined in air of normal density." If only harmful vapors were released from the blood during respiration, then the air, when compressed to half its volume, wouldn't be expected to contain as much of those vapors.

II. Sir Edward Hulse, a physician of reputation at the beginning of the present century, was of opinion, that the placenta was a respiratory organ, like the gills of fish; and not an organ to supply nutriment to the fœtus; as mentioned in Derham's Physico-theology. Many other physicians seem to have espoused the same opinion, as noticed by Haller. Elem. Physiologiæ, T. 1. Dr. Gipson published a defence of this theory in the Medical Essays of Edinburgh, Vol. I. and II. which doctrine is there controverted at large by the late Alexander Monro; and since that time the general opinion has been, that the placenta is an organ of nutrition only, owing perhaps rather to the authority of so great a name, than to the validity of the arguments adduced in its support. The subject has lately been resumed by Dr. James Jeffray, and by Dr. Forester French, in their inaugural dissertations at Edinburgh and at Cambridge; who have defended the contrary opinion in an able and ingenious manner; and from whose Theses I have extracted many of the following remarks.

II. Sir Edward Hulse, a well-known physician at the start of this century, believed that the placenta functioned like a respiratory organ, similar to fish gills, rather than a source of nourishment for the fetus, as mentioned in Derham's Physico-theology. Many other physicians appeared to share this view, as noted by Haller in Elem. Physiologiæ, T. 1. Dr. Gipson published a defense of this theory in the Medical Essays of Edinburgh, Vol. I. and II., which was thoroughly opposed by the late Alexander Monro. Since then, the prevailing belief has been that the placenta serves only as a nutritional organ, possibly owing more to Monro's esteemed reputation than to the strength of the arguments presented in favor of that view. Recently, Dr. James Jeffray and Dr. Forester French revisited the topic in their inaugural dissertations at Edinburgh and Cambridge, respectively, defending the opposing view skillfully and creatively, and I have extracted many remarks from their Theses.

First, by the late discoveries of Dr. Priestley, M. Lavoisier, and other philosophers, it appears, that the basis of atmospherical air, called oxygene, is received by the blood through the membranes of the lungs; and that by this addition the colour of the blood is changed from a dark to a light red. Secondly, that water possesses oxygene also as a part of its composition, and contains air likewise in its pores; whence the blood of fish receives oxygene from the water, or from the air it contains, by means of their gills, in the same manner as the blood is oxygenated in the lungs of air-breathing animals; it changes its colour at the same time from a dark to a light red in the vessels of their gills, which constitute a pulmonary organ adapted to the medium in which they live. Thirdly, that the placenta consists of arteries carrying the blood to its extremities, and a vein bringing it back, resembling exactly in structure the lungs and gills above mentioned; and that the blood changes its colour from a dark to a light red in passing through these vessels.

First, according to the later discoveries of Dr. Priestley, M. Lavoisier, and other scientists, it seems that the essential component of atmospheric air, known as oxygen, is absorbed by the blood through the membranes of the lungs; and this addition changes the color of the blood from dark to light red. Secondly, water also contains oxygen as part of its makeup and holds air in its pores; thus, fish absorb oxygen from the water or the air it contains through their gills, in the same way that air-breathing animals oxygenate their blood in their lungs; in this process, the color changes from dark to light red in the vessels of their gills, which act as a pulmonary organ adapted to their environment. Thirdly, the placenta is made up of arteries carrying blood to its ends and a vein returning it, closely resembling the structure of the lungs and gills mentioned above; and the blood changes its color from dark to light red as it flows through these vessels.

This analogy between the lungs and gills of animals, and the placenta of the fetus, extends through a great variety of other circumstances; thus air-breathing creatures and fish can live but a few minutes without air or water; or when they are confined in such air or water, as has been spoiled by their own respiration; the same happens to the fetus, which, as soon as the placenta is separated from the uterus, must either expand its lungs, and receive air, or die. Hence from the structure, as well as the use of the placenta, it appears to be a respiratory organ, like the gills of fish, by which the blood in the fetus becomes oxygenated.

This comparison between the lungs and gills of animals and the placenta of the fetus applies to a wide range of other situations. Air-breathing animals and fish can only survive for a few minutes without air or water; if they’re in air or water that’s been contaminated by their own breathing, they struggle as well. The same is true for the fetus, which must either use its lungs to breathe air or face death as soon as the placenta is detached from the uterus. Therefore, based on both its structure and function, the placenta seems to act as a respiratory organ, similar to the gills of fish, allowing the fetus's blood to become oxygenated.

From the terminations of the placental vessels not being observed to bleed after being torn from the uterus, while those of the uterus effuse a great quantity of florid arterial blood, the terminations of the placental vessels would seem to be inserted into the arterial ones of the mother; and to receive oxygenation from the passing currents of her blood through their coats or membranes; which oxygenation is proved by the change of the colour of the blood from dark to light red in its passage from the placental arteries to the placental vein.

From the ends of the placental vessels not bleeding after being torn from the uterus, while the uterine vessels release a large amount of bright red arterial blood, it seems that the ends of the placental vessels are connected to the mother's arterial vessels. They appear to receive oxygen from the flow of her blood through their walls or membranes, which is evident from the change in blood color from dark to light red as it moves from the placental arteries to the placental vein.

The curious structure of the cavities or lacunæ of the placenta, demonstrated by Mr. J. Hunter, explain this circumstance. That ingenious philosopher has shewn, that there are numerous cavities of lacunæ formed on that side of the placenta, which is in contact with the uterus; those cavities or cells are filled with blood from the maternal arteries, which open into them; which blood is again taken up by the maternal veins, and is thus perpetually changed. While the terminations of the placental arteries and veins are spread in fine reticulation on the sides of these cells. And thus, as the growing fetus requires greater oxygenation, an apparatus is produced resembling exactly the air-cells of the lungs.

The interesting structure of the cavities or lacunæ of the placenta, shown by Mr. J. Hunter, explains this situation. That clever philosopher has demonstrated that there are many lacunae on the side of the placenta that touches the uterus; these cavities or cells are filled with blood from the maternal arteries that open into them. This blood is then absorbed by the maternal veins, meaning it is constantly being refreshed. Meanwhile, the ends of the placental arteries and veins are spread out in a fine network on the sides of these cells. As the growing fetus needs more oxygen, an arrangement is created that looks exactly like the air cells in the lungs.

In cows, and other ruminating animals, the internal surface of the uterus is unequal like hollow cups, which have been called cotyledons; and into these cavities the prominencies of the numerous placentas, with which the fetus of those animals is furnished, are inserted, and strictly adhere; though they may be extracted without effusion of blood. These inequalities of the uterus, and the numerous placentas in consequence, seem to be designed for the purpose of expanding a greater surface for the terminations of the placental vessels for the purpose of receiving oxygenation from the uterine ones; as the progeny of this class of animals are more completely formed before their nativity, than that of the carnivorous classes, and must thence in the latter weeks of pregnancy require greater oxygenation. Thus calves and lambs can walk about in a few minutes after their birth; while puppies and kittens remain many days without opening their eyes. And though on the separation of the cotyledons of ruminating animals no blood is effused, yet this is owing clearly to the greater power of contraction of their uterine lacunæ or alveoli. See Medical Essays, Vol. V. page 144. And from the same cause they are not liable to a sanguiferous menstruation.

In cows and other ruminant animals, the inner surface of the uterus has uneven areas like hollow cups, known as cotyledons. The numerous placentas that develop with the fetus fit into these spaces and attach firmly, but they can be removed without causing any bleeding. These irregularities in the uterus, along with the many placentas, seem to be intended to increase the surface area for the ends of the placental vessels to receive oxygen from the uterine vessels. This is important because the young of these animals are more fully developed before birth compared to carnivorous animals, and they therefore need more oxygen in the later weeks of pregnancy. As a result, calves and lambs can stand and walk shortly after they’re born, while puppies and kittens take several days before they can open their eyes. Although no blood is lost when the cotyledons are separated in ruminant animals, this is clearly due to the stronger contraction abilities of their uterine lacunae or alveoli. See Medical Essays, Vol. V. page 144. For the same reason, they don’t experience any blood menstruation.

The necessity of the oxygenation of the blood in the fetus is farther illustrated by the analogy of the chick in the egg; which appears to have its blood oxygenated at the extremities of the vessels surrounding the yolk; which are spread on the air-bag at the broad end of the egg, and may absorb oxygene through that moist membrane from the air confined behind it; and which is shewn by experiments in the exhausted receiver to be changeable though the shell.

The need for blood oxygenation in the fetus is further demonstrated by comparing it to a chick in an egg. The chick's blood seems to get oxygenated at the ends of the vessels that surround the yolk. These vessels are spread over the air sac at the wider end of the egg and can absorb oxygen through the moist membrane from the air trapped behind it. Experiments in a vacuum show that this process can occur even through the shell.

This analogy may even be extended to the growing seeds of vegetables; which were shewn by Mr. Scheele to require a renovation of the air over the water, in which they were confined. Many vegetable seeds are surrounded with air in their pods or receptacles, as peas, the fruit of staphylea, and lichnis vesicaria; but it is probable, that those seeds, after they are shed, as well as the spawn of fish, by the situation of the former on or near the moist and aerated surface of the earth, and of the latter in the ever-changing and ventilated water, may not be in need of an apparatus for the oxygenation of their first blood, before the leaves of one, and the gills of the other, are produced for this purpose.

This analogy can even be applied to the growing seeds of vegetables, which Mr. Scheele demonstrated need fresh air over the water they’re in. Many vegetable seeds are surrounded by air in their pods or containers, like peas, the fruit of staphylea, and lichnis vesicaria. However, it’s likely that these seeds, once they fall, along with fish spawn, due to their location on or near the moist and oxygenated surface of the earth, and in the constantly moving and aerated water, may not require a method for oxygenating their initial blood before the leaves of one and the gills of the other develop for that purpose.

III. 1. There are many arguments, besides the strict analogy between the liquor amnii and the albumen ovi, which shew the former to be a nutritive fluid; and that the fetus in the latter months of pregnancy takes it into its stomach; and that in consequence the placenta is produced for some other important purpose.

III. 1. There are several reasons, aside from the direct comparison between amniotic fluid and egg white, that show the former is a nourishing liquid; and that the fetus consumes it in the later months of pregnancy; and that, as a result, the placenta serves another important function.

First, that the liquor amnii is not an excrementitious fluid is evinced, because it is found in greater quantity, when the fetus is young, decreasing after a certain period till birth. Haller asserts, "that in some animals but a small quantity of this fluid remains at the birth. In the eggs of hens it is consumed on the eighteenth day, so that at the exclusion of the chick scarcely any remains. In rabbits before birth there is none." Elem. Physiol. Had this been an excrementitious fluid, the contrary would probably have occurred. Secondly, the skin of the fetus is covered with a whitish crust or pellicle, which would seem to preclude any idea of the liquor amnii being produced by any exsudation of perspirable matter. And it cannot consist of urine, because in brute animals the urachus passes from the bladder to the alantois for the express purpose of carrying off that fluid; which however in the human fetus seems to be retained in the distended bladder, as the feces are accumulated in the bowels of all animals.

First, the amniotic fluid is not a waste product, as evidenced by the fact that its volume is greater when the fetus is young and decreases after a certain point until birth. Haller states that "in some animals, only a small amount of this fluid remains at birth. In hen eggs, it's used up by the eighteenth day, so that there’s hardly any left at the hatching of the chick. In rabbits, there isn’t any before birth." Elem. Physiol. If this were a waste product, we would likely see the opposite. Secondly, the fetus's skin is covered with a whitish coating, which suggests that the amniotic fluid is not produced by any kind of bodily sweat. It also cannot be urine because, in other animals, the urachus connects the bladder to the allantois specifically to remove that fluid; however, in humans, it seems to be retained in the enlarged bladder, just as feces are stored in the intestines of all animals.

2. The nutritious quality of the liquid, which surrounds the fetus, appears from the following considerations. 1. It is coagulable by heat, by nitrous acid, and by spirit of wine, like milk, serum of blood, and other fluids, which daily experience evinces to be nutritious. 2. It has a saltish taste according to the accurate Baron Haller, not unlike the whey of milk, which it even resembles in smell. 3. The white of the egg which constitutes the food of the chick, is shewn to be nutritious by our daily experience; besides the experiment of its nutritious effects mentioned by Dr. Fordyce in his late Treatise on Digestion, p. 178; who adds, that it much resembles the essential parts of the serum of blood.

2. The nutritious quality of the liquid surrounding the fetus can be seen from the following points. 1. It can coagulate with heat, nitrous acid, and alcohol, similar to milk, blood serum, and other fluids that we know are nutritious from daily experience. 2. According to the precise Baron Haller, it has a slightly salty taste, somewhat like whey, and it even has a similar smell. 3. The egg white that provides food for the chick is shown to be nutritious by our everyday experiences; additionally, Dr. Fordyce mentions its nutritious effects in his recent Treatise on Digestion, p. 178; he notes that it closely resembles the essential parts of blood serum.

3. A fluid similar to the fluid, with which the fetus is surrounded, except what little change may be produced by a beginning digestion, is found in the stomach of the fetus; and the white of the egg is found, in the same manner in the stomach of the chick.

3. A liquid similar to the one surrounding the fetus, except for minor changes that may occur from initial digestion, is present in the stomach of the fetus; and the egg white is similarly found in the stomach of the chick.

Numerous hairs, similar to those of its skin, are perpetually found among the contents of the stomach in new-born calves; which must therefore have licked themselves before their nativity. Blasii Anatom. See Sect. XVI. 2. on Instinct.

Numerous hairs, similar to those of its skin, are perpetually found among the contents of the stomach in new-born calves; which must therefore have licked themselves before their nativity. Blasii Anatom. See Sect. XVI. 2. on Instinct.

The chick in the egg is seen gently to move in its surrounding fluid, and to open and shut its mouth alternately. The same has been observed in puppies. Haller's El. Phys. I. 8. p. 201.

The chick in the egg can be seen moving gently in the surrounding fluid and opening and closing its mouth alternately. The same has been observed in puppies. Haller's El. Phys. I. 8. p. 201.

A column of ice has been seen to reach down the œsophagus from the mouth to the stomach in a frozen fetus; and this ice was the liquor amnii frozen.

A column of ice has been observed extending down the esophagus from the mouth to the stomach in a frozen fetus; and this ice was the amniotic fluid that had frozen.

The meconium, or first fæces, in the bowels of new-born infants evince, that something has been digested; and what could this be but the liquor amnii together with the recrements of the gastric juice and gall, which were necessary for its digestion?

The meconium, or first feces, in the intestines of newborn infants shows that something has been digested; and what could this be but the amniotic fluid along with the remnants of gastric juice and bile, which were needed for its digestion?

There have been recorded some monstrous births of animals without heads, and consequently without mouths, which seem to have been delivered on doubtful authority, or from inaccurate observation. There are two of such monstrous productions however better attested; one of a human fetus, mentioned by Gipson in the Scots Medical Essays; which having the gula impervious was furnished with an aperture into the wind-pipe, which communicated below into the gullet; by means of which the liquor amnii might be taken into the stomach before nativity without danger of suffocation, while the fetus had no occasion to breathe. The other monstrous fetus is described by Vander Wiel, who asserts, that he saw a monstrous lamb, which had no mouth; but instead of it was furnished with an opening in the lower part of the neck into the stomach. Both these instances evidently favour the doctrine of the fetus being nourished by the mouth; as otherwise there had been no necessity for new or unnatural apertures into the stomach, when the natural ones were deficient?

There have been records of some bizarre births of animals without heads, and therefore without mouths, which seem to have come from unreliable sources or incorrect observations. However, there are two such unusual cases that are better documented: one of a human fetus, mentioned by Gipson in the Scots Medical Essays, which had an impassable throat but had an opening into the windpipe that connected to the esophagus below. This allowed the amniotic fluid to be taken into the stomach before birth without the risk of suffocation, since the fetus did not need to breathe. The other unusual fetus is described by Vander Wiel, who claims he saw a deformed lamb that had no mouth; instead, it had an opening in the lower part of its neck leading to the stomach. Both these examples clearly support the idea that the fetus is nourished through the mouth; otherwise, there would have been no need for new or unnatural openings into the stomach when the natural ones were missing.

From these facts and observations we may safely infer, that the fetus in the womb is nourished by the fluid which surrounds it; which during the first period of gestation is absorbed by the naked lacteals; and is afterwards swallowed into the stomach and bowels, when these organs are perfected; and lastly that the placenta is an organ for the purpose of giving due oxygenation to the blood of the fetus; which is more necessary, or at least more frequently necessary, than even the supply of food.

From these facts and observations, we can confidently conclude that the fetus in the womb is nourished by the fluid surrounding it. During the early stages of pregnancy, this fluid is absorbed by the exposed lacteals, and then it is swallowed into the stomach and intestines once these organs are developed. Finally, the placenta serves as an organ to ensure the fetus's blood receives proper oxygenation, which is often more crucial than, or at least more regularly required than, the supply of food.

The question of the great Harvey becomes thus easily answered. "Why is not the fetus in the womb suffocated for want of air, when it remains there even to the tenth month without respiration: yet if it be born in the seventh or eighth month, and has once respired, it becomes immediately suffocated for want of air, if its respiration be obstructed?"

The question about the great Harvey can be easily answered: "Why isn't the fetus in the womb suffocated due to lack of air, when it can stay there for up to ten months without breathing? Yet, if it's born in the seventh or eighth month and has breathed even once, it immediately suffocates due to lack of air if its breathing is blocked?"

For further information on this subject, the reader is referred to the Tentamen Medicum of Dr. Jeffray, printed at Edinburgh in 1786. And it is hoped that Dr. French will some time give his theses on this subject to the public.

For more information on this topic, the reader is directed to the Tentamen Medicum by Dr. Jeffray, published in Edinburgh in 1786. It is also hoped that Dr. French will eventually share his theses on this topic with the public.



SECT. XXXIX.

OF GENERATION.

OF GENERATION.

Felix, qui causas altà caligine mersas

Felix, who was immersed in deep fog

Pandit, et evolvit tenuissima vincula rerum.

Pandit, and it developed the thinnest connections between things.

I. Habits of acting and feeling of individuals attend the soul into a future life, and attend the new embryon at the time of its production. The new speck of entity absorbs nutriment, and receives oxygene. Spreads the terminations of its vessels on cells, which communicate with the arteries of the uterus; sometimes with those of the peritoneum. Afterwards it swallows the liquor amnii, which it produces by its irritation from the uterus, or peritoneum. Like insects in the heads of calves and sheep. Why the white of egg is of two consistencies. Why nothing is found in quadrupeds similar to the yolk, nor in most vegetable seeds. II. 1. Eggs of frogs and fish impregnated out of their bodies. Eggs of fowls which are not fecundated, contain only the nutriment for the embryon. The embryon is produced by the male, and the nutriment by the female. Animalcula in semine. Profusion of nature's births. 2. Vegetables viviparous. Buds and bulbs have each a father but no mother. Vessels of the leaf and bud inosculate. The paternal offspring exactly resembles the parent. 3. Insects impregnated for six generations. Polypus branches like buds. Creeping roots. Viviparous flowers. Tænia, volvox. Eve from Adam's rib. Semen not a stimulus to the egg. III. 1. Embryons not originally created within other embryons. Organized matter is not so minute. 2. All the parts of the embryon are not formed in the male parent. Crabs produce their legs, worms produce their heads and tails. In wens, cancers, and inflammations, new vessels are formed. Mules partake of the forms of both parents. Hair and nails grow by elongation, not by distention. 3. Organic particles of Buffon. IV. 1. Rudiment of the embryon a simple living filament, becomes a living ring, and then a living tube. 2. It acquires irritabilities, and sensibilities with new organizations, as in wounded snails, polypi, moths, gnats, tadpoles. Hence new parts are acquired by addition not by distention. 3. All parts of the body grow if not confined. 4. Fetuses deficient at their extremities, or have a duplicature of parts. Monstrous births. Double parts of vegetables. 5. Mules cannot be formed by distention of the seminal ens. 6. Families of animals from a mixture of their orders. Mules imperfect. 7. Animal appetency like chemical affinity. Vis fabricatrix and medicatrix of nature. 8. The changes of animals before and after nativity. Similarity of their structure. Changes in them from lust, hunger, and danger. All warm-blooded animals derived from one living filament. Cold-blooded animals, insects, worms, vegetables, derived also from one living filament. Male animals have teats. Male pigeon gives milk. The world itself generated. The cause of causes. A state of probation and responsibility. V. 1. Efficient cause of the colours of birds eggs, and of hair and feathers, which become white in snowy countries. Imagination of the female colours the egg. Ideas or motions of the retina imitated by the extremities of the nerves of touch, or rete mucosum. 2. Nutriment supplied by the female of three kinds. Her imagination can only affect the first kind. Mules how produced, and mulattoes. Organs of reproduction why deficient in mules. Eggs with double yolks. VI. 1. Various secretions produced by the extremities of the vessels, as in the glands. Contagious matter. Many glands affected by pleasurable ideas, as those which secrete the semen. 2. Snails and worms are hermaphrodite, yet cannot impregnate themselves. Final cause of this. 3. The imagination of the male forms the sex. Ideas, or motions of the nerves of vision or of touch, are imitated by the ultimate extremities of the glands of the testes, which mark the sex. This effect of the imagination belongs only to the male. The sex of the embryon is not owing to accident. 4. Causes of the changes in animals from imagination as in monsters. From the male. From the female. 5. Miscarriages from fear. 6. Power of the imagination of the male over the colour, form, and sex of the progeny. An instance of. 7. Act of generation accompanied with ideas of the male or female form. Art of begetting beautiful children of either sex. VII. Recapitulation. VIII. Conclusion. Of cause and effect. The atomic philosophy leads to a first cause.

__A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. People's habits and feelings influence their soul in the afterlife and affect the new embryo at its creation. The tiny entity takes in nutrients and oxygen, spreading its vessels to connect with the cells that communicate with the uterine arteries, sometimes with those of the peritoneum. It then swallows amniotic fluid, which it generates through irritation from the uterus or peritoneum, much like insects found in the heads of calves and sheep. This explains why egg whites have two different consistencies and why quadrupeds lack a yolk-like substance, unlike most plant seeds. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Frog and fish eggs are fertilized outside of their bodies. Unfertilized chicken eggs contain only nutrients for the embryo. The male produces the embryo, while the female provides the nutrients. There are tiny organisms found in semen, showcasing nature's abundant creations. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Some plants reproduce live offspring. Buds and bulbs have a father but no mother. The vessels in leaves and buds connect. The offspring looks just like the parent. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Insects can reproduce for six generations. Polyps branch like buds. Creeping roots. Some plants have living flowers that produce live offspring. Tænia, volvox. Eve was created from Adam's rib. Semen does not stimulate the egg. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Embryos are not created inside other embryos. Organized matter isn't that small. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Not all parts of the embryo come from the male parent. Crabs develop their legs, worms develop their heads and tails. New vessels form in conditions like wens, cancers, and inflammations. Mules exhibit features from both parents. Hair and nails grow by elongation rather than distension. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Organic particles according to Buffon. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. The embryo starts as a simple living filament, then develops into a living ring, and finally becomes a living tube. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. It gains irritability and sensitivity with new structures, similar to wounded snails, polyps, moths, gnats, and tadpoles. Thus, new parts arise through addition, not through distension. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. All body parts grow if they are not restricted. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. Fetuses may lack extremities or have duplicate parts. Abnormal births. Double parts in plants. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. Mules cannot be produced by simply distending the male seed. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_15__. Animal families arise from mixing their types. Mules are not fully formed. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_16__. Animal appetites resemble chemical affinity. Nature has a creative and healing force. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_17__. Changes in animals occur before and after birth. Their structures show similarities and undergo changes due to lust, hunger, and danger. All warm-blooded animals come from one living filament. Cold-blooded animals, insects, worms, and plants also emerge from one living filament. Male animals have mammary glands. Male pigeons can produce a form of milk. The world itself is generated, a cause of all causes. It’s a state of trial and responsibility. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_18__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_19__. The source of the colors in bird eggs and in hair and feathers, which turn white in snowy regions. The female's imagination colors the egg. Ideas or motions from the retina are mimicked by the nerve endings of touch or rete mucosum. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_20__. Three types of nutrition are provided by the female. Her imagination can only influence the first type. Here's how mules and mulattoes are created. The organs of reproduction are deficient in mules. Eggs can have double yolks. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_21__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_22__. Various secretions arise from the ends of the vessels, like in the glands. Contagious substances exist. Many glands are affected by pleasurable thoughts, including those that secrete semen. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_23__. Snails and worms are hermaphroditic but cannot self-fertilize. This serves a purpose. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_24__. The male's imagination determines the sex. Ideas or movements of the visual or touch nerves are replicated by the glands at the testes, which indicate sex. This imaginative effect is unique to males. The sex of the embryo isn’t random. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_25__. Factors causing changes in animals due to imagination can lead to monstrosities. Influences from the male. Influences from the female. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_26__. Miscarriages can occur from fear. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_27__. The male's imagination has power over the color, form, and sex of the offspring. Here's an example. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_28__. The act of reproduction is accompanied by thoughts of the male or female form. Skills in creating beautiful children of either sex. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_29__. Summary. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_30__. Conclusion. About cause and effect. Atomic philosophy leads to a first cause.

I. The ingenious Dr. Hartley in his work on man, and some other philosophers, have been of opinion, that our immortal part acquires during this life certain habits of action or of sentiment, which become for ever indissoluble, continuing after death in a future state of existence; and add, that if these habits are of the malevolent kind, they must render the possessor miserable even in heaven. I would apply this ingenious idea to the generation or production of the embryon, or new animal, which partakes so much of the form and propensities of the parent.

I. The clever Dr. Hartley, along with some other philosophers, believes that our immortal soul adopts certain habits of behavior or feelings during this life that become permanent and persist after death in a future existence. They argue that if these habits are negative, they will make the individual miserable even in heaven. I want to connect this interesting idea to the creation of the embryo or new animal, which closely resembles the form and tendencies of its parent.

Owing to the imperfection of language the offspring is termed a new animal, but is in truth a branch or elongation of the parent; since a part of the embryon-animal is, or was, a part of the parent; and therefore in strict language it cannot be said to be entirely new at the time of its production; and therefore it may retain some of the habits of the parent-system.

Due to the limitations of language, the offspring is called a new animal, but in reality, it's just an extension or branch of the parent; since part of the embryonic animal is, or was, part of the parent. So, technically, it can't be considered completely new at the time it comes into being, and it might keep some of the habits of the parent system.

At the earliest period of its existence the embryon, as secreted from the blood of the male, would seem to consist of a living filament with certain capabilities of irritation, sensation, volition, and association; and also with some acquired habits or propensities peculiar to the parent: the former of these are in common with other animals; the latter seem to distinguish or produce the kind of animal, whether man or quadruped, with the similarity of feature or form to the parent. It is difficult to be conceived, that a living entity can be separated or produced from the blood by the action of a gland; and which shall afterwards become an animal similar to that in whose vessels it is formed; even though we should suppose with some modern theorists, that the blood is alive; yet every other hypothesis concerning generation rests on principles still more difficult to our comprehension.

At the earliest stage of its existence, the embryo, as secreted from the male's blood, seems to consist of a living filament with certain abilities for irritation, sensation, will, and connection; as well as some acquired habits or tendencies unique to the parent. The former traits are shared with other animals, while the latter appear to define or create the kind of animal it will become, whether human or quadruped, resembling the parent in features or form. It’s hard to imagine that a living being can be separated or created from blood by the action of a gland and then develop into an animal similar to the one it originated from. Even if we suppose, as some modern theorists do, that blood is alive, every other theory about generation is based on principles that are even trickier for us to understand.

At the time of procreation this speck of entity is received into an appropriated nidus, in which it must acquire two circumstances necessary to its life and growth; one of these is food or sustenance, which is to be received by the absorbent mouths of its vessels; and the other is that part of atmospherical air, or of water, which by the new chemistry is termed oxygene, and which affects the blood by passing through the coats of the vessels which contain it. The fluid surrounding the embryon in its new habitation, which is called liquor amnii, supplies it with nourishment; and as some air cannot but be introduced into the uterus along with a new embryon, it would seem that this same fluid would for a short time, suppose for a few hours, supply likewise a sufficient quantity of the oxygene for its immediate existence.

At the time of conception, this tiny entity is placed into a designated space where it needs to obtain two essential things for life and growth. One is food or nourishment, which is taken in through the absorbent openings of its vessels. The other is that part of atmospheric air, or water, that new chemistry calls oxygen, which impacts the blood by passing through the walls of the vessels holding it. The fluid surrounding the embryo in its new environment, known as amniotic fluid, provides it with nourishment. Since some air inevitably enters the uterus along with the new embryo, it seems this fluid would, for a short time, maybe a few hours, also supply enough oxygen for its immediate survival.

On this account the vegetable impregnation of aquatic plants is performed in the air; and it is probable that the honey-cup or nectary of vegetables requires to be open to the air, that the anthers and stigmas of the flower may have food of a more oxygenated kind than the common vegetable sap-juice.

On this basis, the fertilization of aquatic plants happens in the air; and it's likely that the nectar or honey-producing parts of plants need to be exposed to the air so that the pollen and flower stigmas can access a more oxygen-rich type of food than the typical plant sap.

On the introduction of this primordium of entity into the uterus the irritation of the liquor amnii, which surrounds it, excites the absorbent mouths of the new vessels into action; they drink up a part of it, and a pleasurable sensation accompanies this new action; at the same time the chemical affinity of the oxygene acts through the vessels of the rubescent blood; and a previous want, or disagreeable sensation, is relieved by this process.

On the introduction of this initial form of life into the uterus, the irritation of the amniotic fluid surrounding it stimulates the absorbent openings of the new blood vessels into action; they absorb some of it, and a pleasurable feeling comes with this new activity. At the same time, the chemical attraction of oxygen acts through the vessels of the red blood, relieving a prior need or uncomfortable sensation through this process.

As the want of this oxygenation of the blood is perpetual, (as appears from the incessant necessity of breathing by lungs or gills,) the vessels become extended by the efforts of pain or desire to seek this necessary object of oxygenation, and to remove the disagreeable sensation, which that want occasions. At the same time new particles of matter are absorbed, or applied to these extended vessels, and they become permanently elongated, as the fluid in contact with them soon loses the oxygenous part, which it at first possessed, which was owing to the introduction of air along with the embryon. These new blood-vessels approach the sides of the uterus, and penetrate with their fine terminations into the vessels of the mother; or adhere to them, acquiring oxygene through their coats from the passing currents of the arterial blood of the mother. See Sect. XXXVIII. 2.

As the want of this oxygenation of the blood is perpetual, (as appears from the incessant necessity of breathing by lungs or gills,) the vessels become extended by the efforts of pain or desire to seek this necessary object of oxygenation, and to remove the disagreeable sensation, which that want occasions. At the same time new particles of matter are absorbed, or applied to these extended vessels, and they become permanently elongated, as the fluid in contact with them soon loses the oxygenous part, which it at first possessed, which was owing to the introduction of air along with the embryon. These new blood-vessels approach the sides of the uterus, and penetrate with their fine terminations into the vessels of the mother; or adhere to them, acquiring oxygene through their coats from the passing currents of the arterial blood of the mother. See Sect. XXXVIII. 2.

This attachment of the placental vessels to the internal side of the uterus by their own proper efforts appears further illustrated by the many instances of extra-uterine fetuses, which have thus attached or inserted their vessels into the peritoneum; or on the viscera, exactly in the same manner as they naturally insert or attach them to the uterus.

This connection of the placental vessels to the inner side of the uterus by their own efforts is further shown by many cases of ectopic pregnancies, where these fetuses have attached or inserted their vessels into the peritoneum or onto the organs, just like they would naturally do with the uterus.

The absorbent vessels of the embryon continue to drink up nourishment from the fluid in which they swim, or liquor amnii; and which at first needs no previous digestive preparation; but which, when the whole apparatus of digestion becomes complete, is swallowed by the mouth into the stomach, and being mixed with saliva, gastric juice, bile, pancreatic juice, and mucus of the intestines, becomes digested, and leaves a recrement, which produces the first feces of the infant, called meconium.

The absorbent vessels of the embryo keep soaking up nutrients from the fluid they're in, known as amniotic fluid, which initially doesn't require any digestion. However, once the entire digestive system is fully developed, it’s taken in through the mouth into the stomach. There, it mixes with saliva, gastric juice, bile, pancreatic juice, and intestinal mucus to be digested, resulting in waste that forms the infant's first stool, called meconium.

The liquor amnii is secreted into the uterus, as the fetus requires it, and may probably be produced by the irritation of the fetus as an extraneous body; since a similar fluid is acquired from the peritoneum in cases of extra-uterine gestation. The young caterpillars of the gadfly placed in the skins of cows, and the young of the ichneumon-fly placed in the backs of the caterpillars on cabbages, seem to produce their nourishment by their irritating the sides of their nidus. A vegetable secretion and concretion is thus produced on oak-leaves by the gall-insect, and by the cynips in the bedeguar of the rose; and by the young grasshopper on many plants, by which the animal surrounds itself with froth. But in no circumstance is extra-uterine gestation so exactly resembled as by the eggs of a fly, which are deposited in the frontal sinus of sheep and calves. These eggs float in some ounces of fluid collected in a thin pellicle or hydatide. This bag of fluid compresses the optic nerve on one side, by which the vision being less distinct in that eye, the animal turns in perpetual circles towards the side affected, in order to get a more accurate view of objects; for the same reason as in squinting the affected eye is turned away from the object contemplated. Sheep in the warm months keep their noses close to the ground to prevent this fly from so readily getting into their nostrils.

The amniotic fluid is secreted into the uterus as needed by the fetus, likely due to the fetus irritating the area as if it were a foreign body; this is similar to the fluid that forms from the peritoneum in cases of ectopic pregnancy. Young gadfly caterpillars placed in cow skin, and the larvae of the ichneumon fly inserted into caterpillars on cabbage, seem to obtain their nourishment by causing irritation to the walls of their host. A plant secretion and formation occurs on oak leaves caused by the gall-insect and cynips in the bedeguar of roses; and young grasshoppers create a frothy environment around themselves on various plants. However, no case of ectopic pregnancy resembles this more closely than fly eggs laid in the nasal cavities of sheep and calves. These eggs float in a few ounces of fluid collected in a thin membrane or hydatid. This fluid bag compresses the optic nerve on one side, causing vision to be less clear in that eye, which makes the animal turn in endless circles towards the affected side to get a better view of objects—similar to how a squinting person turns the affected eye away from what they are looking at. In warm weather, sheep keep their noses close to the ground to make it harder for flies to enter their nostrils.

The liquor amnii is secreted into the womb as it is required, not only in respect to quantity, but, as the digestive powers of the fetus become formed, this fluid becomes of a different consistence and quality, till it is exchanged for milk after nativity. Haller. Physiol. V. 1. In the egg the white part, which is analogous to the liquor amnii of quadrupeds, consists of two distinct parts; one of which is more viscid, and probably more difficult of digestion, and more nutritive than the other; and this latter is used in the last week of incubation. The yolk of the egg is a still stronger or more nutritive fluid, which is drawn up into the bowels of the chick just at its exclusion from the shell, and serves it for nourishment for a day or two, till it is able to digest, and has learnt to choose the harder seeds or grains, which are to afford it sustenance. Nothing analogous to this yolk is found in the fetus of lactiferous animals, as the milk is another nutritive fluid ready prepared for the young progeny.

The amniotic fluid is released into the womb as needed, not just in terms of amount, but also as the fetus's digestive capabilities develop; this fluid changes in consistency and quality until it is replaced by milk after birth. Haller. Physiol. V. 1. In the egg, the white part, which is similar to the amniotic fluid of mammals, has two distinct components; one is thicker and likely harder to digest and more nutritious than the other, which is used in the final week of incubation. The yolk of the egg is an even richer or more nutritious fluid that is absorbed into the chick's intestines just as it breaks free from the shell, providing nourishment for a day or two until it can digest and learns to pick harder seeds or grains for food. There’s nothing similar to this yolk in the fetus of milk-producing animals, as milk is another ready-made nutritious fluid for the young offspring.

The yolk therefore is not necessary to the spawn of fish, the eggs of insects, or for the seeds of vegetables; as their embryons have probably their food presented to them as soon as they are excluded from their shells, or have extended their roots. Whence it happens that some insects produce a living progeny in the spring and summer, and eggs in the autumn; and some vegetables have living roots or buds produced in the place of seeds, as the polygonum viviparum, and magical onions. See Botanic Garden, p. 11. art. anthoxanthum.

The yolk is not necessary for the growth of fish spawn, insect eggs, or vegetable seeds; their embryos likely receive nourishment as soon as they hatch from their shells or establish their roots. This is why some insects give birth to live young in the spring and summer but lay eggs in the autumn; and why some plants produce living roots or buds instead of seeds, like the polygonum viviparum and magical onions. See Botanic Garden, p. 11. art. anthoxanthum.

There seems however to be a reservoir of nutriment prepared for some seeds besides their cotyledons or seed-leaves, which may be supposed in some measure analogous to the yolk of the egg. Such are the saccharine juices of apples, grapes and other fruits, which supply nutrition to the seeds after they fall on the ground. And such is the milky juice in the centre of the cocoa-nut, and part of the kernel of it; the same I suppose of all other monocotyledon seeds, as of the palms, grasses, and lilies.

There seems to be a reservoir of nutrients available for some seeds beyond their cotyledons or seed leaves, which could be somewhat similar to the yolk of an egg. These include the sugary juices of apples, grapes, and other fruits that provide nourishment to the seeds after they drop to the ground. The milky juice inside a coconut, along with part of its kernel, is another example. I assume this is true for all other monocot seeds, like those from palms, grasses, and lilies.

II. 1. The process of generation is still involved in impenetrable obscurity, conjectures may nevertheless be formed concerning some of its circumstances. First, the eggs of fish and frogs are impregnated, after they leave the body of the female; because they are deposited in a fluid, and are not therefore covered with a hard shell. It is however remarkable, that neither frogs nor fish will part with their spawn without the presence of the male; on which account female carp and gold-fish in small ponds, where there are no males, frequently die from the distention of their growing spawn. 2. The eggs of fowls, which are laid without being impregnated, are seen to contain only the yolk and white, which are evidently the food or sustenance for the future chick. 3. As the cicatricula of these eggs is given by the cock, and is evidently the rudiment of the new animal; we may conclude, that the embryon is produced by the male, and the proper food and nidus by the female. For if the female be supposed to form an equal part of the embryon, why should she form the whole of the apparatus for nutriment and for oxygenation? the male in many animals is larger, stronger, and digests more food than the female, and therefore should contribute as much or more towards the reproduction of the species; but if he contributes only half the embryon and none of the apparatus for sustenance and oxygenation, the division is unequal; the strength of the male, and his consumption of food are too great for the effect, compared with that of the female, which is contrary to the usual course of nature.

II. 1. The process of generation remains quite unclear, but we can still make some guesses about certain aspects of it. First, fish and frog eggs get fertilized after they leave the female's body since they are laid in water and aren’t protected by a hard shell. It’s interesting to note that neither frogs nor fish will release their eggs without a male present; as a result, female carp and goldfish in small ponds without males often die from the buildup of their developing eggs. 2. The eggs of birds, which are laid unfertilized, contain only the yolk and egg white, which clearly serve as food for the future chick. 3. Since the male chicken provides the cicatricula, which is clearly the beginning of the new animal, we can infer that the embryo is formed by the male, while the female provides the necessary food and environment. If we assume the female also contributes equally to the embryo, why then does she provide all the nutrients and oxygen supply? The male is often larger, stronger, and consumes more food than the female, so he should contribute equally or more to species reproduction. If he only contributes half of the embryo and none of the nourishing environment, the division is not balanced; the male's strength and food intake are too significant for the outcome when compared to the female, which goes against the natural order.

In objection to this theory of generation it may be said, if the animalcula in femine, as seen by the microscope, be all of them rudiments of homunculi, when but one of them can find a nidus, what a waste nature has made of her productions? I do not assert that these moving particles, visible by the microscope, are homunciones; perhaps they may be the creatures of stagnation or putridity, or perhaps no creatures at all; but if they are supposed to be rudiments of homunculi, or embryons, such a profusion of them corresponds with the general efforts of nature to provide for the continuance of her species of animals. Every individual tree produces innumerable seeds, and every individual fish innumerable spawn, in such inconceivable abundance as would in a short space of time crowd the earth and ocean with inhabitants; and these are much more perfect animals than the animalcula in femine can be supposed to be, and perish in uncounted millions. This argument only shews, that the productions of nature are governed by general laws; and that by a wise superfluity of provision she has ensured their continuance.

In response to this theory of reproduction, one might argue that if the microscopic animalcules in females are all potential homunculi, then how wasteful is nature to create so many when only one of them can actually develop? I’m not claiming that these moving particles, visible under the microscope, are homunculi; they might just be stagnant or decaying matter, or maybe not even living things at all. However, if we consider them to be the beginnings of homunculi or embryos, the sheer number of them reflects nature’s overall effort to sustain various animal species. Just like individual trees produce countless seeds and individual fish release vast amounts of eggs, in quantities so immense that they could quickly fill the earth and oceans with life; and these are far more developed animals than the microscopic animalcules could ever be, yet they die in unmeasurable numbers. This argument merely demonstrates that nature's productions follow general laws, and that through an abundance of resources, she has guaranteed their survival.

2. That the embryon is secreted or produced by the male, and not by the conjunction of fluids from both male and female, appears from the analogy of vegetable seeds. In the large flowers, as the tulip, there is no similarity of apparatus between the anthers and the stigma: the seed is produced according to the observations of Spallanzani long before the flowers open, and in consequence long before it can be impregnated, like the egg in the pullet. And after the prolific dust is shed on the stigma, the seed becomes coagulated in one point first, like the cicatricula of the impregnated egg. See Botanic Garden, Part I. additional note 38. Now in these simple products of nature, if the female contributed to produce the new embryon equally with the male, there would probably have been some visible similarity of parts for this purpose, besides those necessary for the nidus and sustenance of the new progeny. Besides in many flowers the males are more numerous than the females, or than the separate uterine cells in their germs, which would shew, that the office of the male was at least as important as that of the female; whereas if the female, besides producing the egg or seed, was to produce an equal part of the embryon, the office of reproduction would be unequally divided between them.

2. It seems that the embryo is created by the male, not by the mixing of fluids from both the male and female, similar to how plant seeds work. In big flowers, like tulips, the structures of the anthers and stigma are quite different. The seed forms, as observed by Spallanzani, well before the flowers bloom, and therefore long before it can be fertilized, similar to the egg in a hen. After the pollen is deposited on the stigma, the seed starts to develop in one spot first, like the scar on a fertilized egg. See Botanic Garden, Part I. additional note 38. Now, in these straightforward products of nature, if the female played an equal role in creating the new embryo alongside the male, we would likely see some visible similarities in their structures for this purpose, in addition to what is needed for the nurturing and support of the new offspring. Also, in many flowers, there are more males than females or than the separate uterine chambers in their seeds, suggesting that the male's role is at least as significant as the female's. However, if the female not only produces the egg or seed but also contributes equally to the embryo's creation, the reproductive roles would not be equally shared between them.

Add to this, that in the most simple kind of vegetable reproduction, I mean the buds of trees, which are their viviparous offspring, the leaf is evidently the parent of the bud, which rises in its bosom, according to the observation of Linnaeus. This leaf consists of absorbent vessels, and pulmonary ones, to obtain its nutriment, and to impregnate it with oxygene. This simple piece of living organization is also furnished with a power of reproduction; and as the new offspring is thus supported adhering to its father, it needs no mother to supply it with a nidus, and nutriment, and oxygenation; and hence no female leaf has existence.

Additionally, in the simplest form of plant reproduction, specifically the buds of trees, which are their live offspring, it's clear that the leaf is the source of the bud that grows from it, as noted by Linnaeus. This leaf contains absorbent vessels and air chambers that allow it to gather nutrients and enrich itself with oxygen. This basic unit of living tissue also has the ability to reproduce; since the new offspring is sustained while attached to its parent, it doesn’t require a mother to provide a home, food, or oxygen; thus, no female leaf exists.

I conceive that the vessels between the bud and the leaf communicate or inosculate; and that the bud is thus served with vegetable blood, that is, with both nutriment and oxygenation, till the death of the parent-leaf in autumn. And in this respect it differs from the fetus of viviparous animals. Secondly, that then the bark-vessels belonging to the dead-leaf, and in which I suppose a kind of manna to have been deposited, become now the placental vessels, if they may be so called, of the new bud. From the vernal sap thus produced of one sugar-maple-tree in New-York and in Pennsylvania, five or six pounds of good sugar may be made annually without destroying the tree. Account of maple-sugar by B. Rushes. London, Phillips. (See Botanic Garden, Part I. additional note on vegetable placentation.)

I believe that the vessels between the bud and the leaf connect or merge, and that the bud receives nourishment and oxygen until the parent leaf dies in the autumn. In this way, it’s different from the fetus of live-bearing animals. Secondly, once the leaf is dead, the bark vessels connected to it, which I think have stored a kind of manna, become the placental vessels for the new bud, if we can call them that. From the sap produced in one sugar maple tree in New York and Pennsylvania, we can make five or six pounds of good sugar each year without harming the tree. Account of maple-sugar by B. Rushes. London, Phillips. (See Botanic Garden, Part I. additional note on vegetable placentation.)

These vessels, when the warmth of the vernal sun hatches the young bud, serve it with a saccharine nutriment, till it acquires leaves of its own, and shoots a new system of absorbents down the bark and root of the tree, just as the farinaceous or oily matter in seeds, and the saccharine matter in fruits, serve their embryons with nutriment, till they acquire leaves and roots. This analogy is as forceable in so obscure a subject, as it is curious, and may in large buds, as of the horse-chesnut, be almost seen by the naked eye; if with a penknife the remaining rudiment of the last year's leaf, and of the new bud in its bosom, be cut away slice by slice. The seven ribs of the last year's leaf will be seen to have arisen from the pith in seven distinct points making a curve; and the new bud to have been produced in their centre, and to have pierced the alburnum and cortex, and grown without the assistance of a mother. A similar process may be seen on dissecting a tulip-root in winter; the leaves, which inclosed the last year's flower-stalk, were not necessary for the flower; but each of these was the father of a new bud, which may be now found at its base; and which, as it adheres to the parent, required no mother.

These vessels, when the warmth of the spring sun awakens the young bud, provide it with sweet nutrition until it develops its own leaves and sends a new set of absorbent roots down through the tree's bark and roots, similar to how the starchy or oily substances in seeds and the sugary content in fruits nourish their embryos until they grow leaves and roots. This comparison is just as compelling in a complex subject as it is interesting, and it can often be observed with the naked eye in large buds, such as those of the horse chestnut, by carefully slicing away the remnants of last year's leaf and the new bud within it. The seven veins of the previous year's leaf will be shown to emerge from the pith at seven distinct points in a curve, with the new bud forming in the center and breaking through the cambium and bark, growing independently of a mother. A similar process can be observed when dissecting a tulip bulb in winter; the leaves that surrounded last year's flower stem were not necessary for the flower, but each of these leaves gave rise to a new bud, which can now be found at its base, and since it remains attached to the parent, it doesn't require a mother.

This paternal offspring of vegetables, I mean their buds and bulbs, is attended with a very curious circumstance; and that is, that they exactly resemble their parents, as is observable in grafting fruit-trees, and in propagating flower-roots; whereas the seminal offspring of plants, being supplied with nutriment by the mother, is liable to perpetual variation. Thus also in the vegetable class dioicia, where the male flowers are produced on one tree, and the female ones on another; the buds of the male trees uniformly produce either male flowers, or other buds similar to themselves; and the buds of the female trees produce either female flowers, or other buds similar to themselves; whereas the seeds of these trees produce either male or female plants. From this analogy of the production of vegetable buds without a mother, I contend that the mother does not contribute to the formation of the living ens in animal generation, but is necessary only for supplying its nutriment and oxygenation.

This plant offspring of vegetables, meaning their buds and bulbs, has a really interesting aspect: they look exactly like their parents, similar to how grafting works with fruit trees and propagating flower roots. On the other hand, the seed-based offspring of plants, which get their nutrition from the mother, tends to show constant variation. In the case of dioecious plants, where male flowers grow on one tree and female flowers on another, the buds from male trees consistently develop either male flowers or other similar buds, while the buds from female trees produce either female flowers or similar buds. However, the seeds from these trees can result in either male or female plants. From this comparison of how plant buds form without a mother, I argue that the mother doesn't play a role in creating the living being in animal reproduction; she is only needed to provide nutrition and oxygen.

There is another vegetable fact published by M. Koelreuter, which he calls "a complete metamorphosis of one natural species of plants into another," which shews, that in seeds as well as in buds, the embryon proceeds from the male parent, though the form of the subsequent mature plant is in part dependant on the female. M. Koelreuter impregnated a stigma of the nicotiana rustica with the farina of the nicotiana paniculata, and obtained prolific seeds from it. With the plants which sprung from these seeds, he repeated the experiment, impregnating them with the farina of the nicotiana paniculata. As the mule plants which he thus produced were prolific, he continued to impregnate them for many generations with the farina of the nicotiana paniculata, and they became more and more like the male parent, till he at length obtained six plants in every respect perfectly similar to the nicotiana paniculata; and in no respect resembling their female parent the nicotiana rustica. Blumenbach on Generation.

There’s another vegetable fact shared by M. Koelreuter, which he refers to as "a complete metamorphosis of one natural species of plants into another." This shows that in seeds as well as in buds, the embryo comes from the male parent, even though the form of the mature plant depends partially on the female. M. Koelreuter pollinated a stigma of the nicotiana rustica with the pollen of the nicotiana paniculata and harvested productive seeds from it. With the plants that grew from these seeds, he repeated the experiment, pollinating them with the pollen of the nicotiana paniculata. Since the mule plants he produced were prolific, he continued to pollinate them for many generations with the pollen of the nicotiana paniculata, and they began to resemble the male parent more and more, until he eventually obtained six plants that were entirely identical to the nicotiana paniculata and bore no resemblance to their female parent, the nicotiana rustica. Blumenbach on Generation.

3. It is probable that the insects, which are said to require but one impregnation for six generations, as the aphis (see Amenit. Academ.) produce their progeny in the manner above described, that is, without a mother, and not without a father; and thus experience a lucina sine concubitu. Those who have attended to the habits of the polypus, which is found in the stagnant water of our ditches in July, affirm, that the young ones branch out from the side of the parent like the buds of trees, and after a time separate themselves from them. This is so analogous to the manner in which the buds of trees appear to be produced, that these polypi may be considered as all male animals, producing embryons, which require no mother to supply them with a nidus, or with nutriment, and oxygenation.

3. It's likely that insects, which are believed to need just one fertilization for six generations, like the aphis (see Amenit. Academ.), reproduce as described above, meaning they do so without a mother but not without a father; thus, they undergo a form of birth without mating. Those who have observed the habits of the polyp, found in the stagnant water of our ditches in July, say that the young ones grow out from the side of the parent like tree buds and eventually separate from them. This process is so similar to how tree buds appear to grow that these polyps can be seen as all male creatures, producing embryos that don’t require a mother for a place to develop, nourishment, or oxygen.

This lateral or lineal generation of plants, not only obtains in the buds of trees, which continue to adhere to them, but is beautifully seen in the wires of knot-grass, polygonum aviculare, and in those of strawberries, fragaria vesca. In these an elongated creeping bud is protruded, and, where it touches the ground, takes root, and produces a new plant derived from its father, from which it acquires both nutriment and oxygenation; and in consequence needs no maternal apparatus for these purposes. In viviparous flowers, as those of allium magicum, and polygonum viviparum, the anthers and the stigmas become effete and perish; and the lateral or paternal offspring succeeds instead of seeds, which adhere till they are sufficiently mature, and then fall upon the ground, and take root like other bulbs.

This lateral or linear generation of plants occurs not only in the buds of trees that remain attached to them, but is also clearly seen in the runners of knot-grass (Polygonum aviculare) and strawberries (Fragaria vesca). In these, an elongated creeping bud grows out, and where it touches the ground, it takes root and produces a new plant that comes from its parent, gaining both nutrients and oxygen from it; as a result, it doesn’t require any maternal structures for these needs. In viviparous flowers, like those of Allium magicum and Polygonum viviparum, the anthers and stigmas lose their effectiveness and die; instead, the lateral or paternal offspring takes the place of seeds, which remain attached until they are mature enough, after which they fall to the ground and take root like other bulbs.

The lateral production of plants by wires, while each new plant is thus chained to its parent, and continues to put forth another and another, as the wire creeps onward on the ground, is exactly resembled by the tape-worm, or tænia, so often found in the bowels, stretching itself in a chain quite from the stomach to the rectum. Linnæus asserts, "that it grows old at one extremity, while it continues to generate young ones at the other, proceeding ad infinitum, like a root of grass. The separate joints are called gourd-worms, and propagate new joints like the parent without end, each joint being furnished with its proper mouth, and organs of digestion." Systema naturæ. Vermes tenia. In this animal there evidently appears a power of reproduction without any maternal apparatus for the purpose of supplying nutriment and oxygenation to the embryon, as it remains attached to its father till its maturity. The volvox globator, which is a transparent animal, is said by Linnæus to bear within it sons and grand-sons to the fifth generation. These are probably living fetuses, produced by the father, of different degrees of maturity, to be detruded at different periods of time, like the unimpregnated eggs of various sizes, which are found in poultry; and as they are produced without any known copulation, contribute to evince, that the living embryon in other orders of animals is formed by the male-parent, and not by the mother, as one parent has the power to produce it.

The way plants grow sideways using wires, with each new plant connected to its parent and continuing to produce more as the wire stretches across the ground, is similar to a tapeworm, or tænia, which often lives in the intestines, stretching in a chain from the stomach to the rectum. Linnæus claims, "it ages at one end while generating new ones at the other, going on forever, like a grass root. The individual segments are called gourd-worms, and they create new segments like the parent endlessly, each segment equipped with its own mouth and digestive organs." Systema naturæ. Vermes tenia. In this creature, there is clearly a reproductive ability without any maternal system providing nourishment and oxygen to the embryo, as it stays attached to its parent until fully developed. The volvox globator, a transparent organism, is said by Linnæus to carry within it sons and grandsons up to the fifth generation. These are likely living embryos produced by the father at varying stages of development, released over time, similar to the different-sized unfertilized eggs found in birds; and since they are produced without any known mating, it suggests that the living embryo in other animal groups is formed by the male parent, rather than the female, highlighting that one parent can create it.

This idea of the reproduction of animals from a single living filament of their fathers, appears to have been shadowed or allegorized in the curious account in sacred writ of the formation of Eve from a rib of Adam.

This concept of animals reproducing from a single living strand of their fathers seems to be hinted at or symbolized in the intriguing story in religious texts about the creation of Eve from one of Adam's ribs.

From all these analogies I conclude, that the embryon is produced solely by the male, and that the female supplies it with a proper nidus, with sustenance, and with oxygenation; and that the idea of the semen of the male constituting only a stimulus to the egg of the female, exciting it into life, (as held by some philosophers) has no support from experiment or analogy.

From all these comparisons, I conclude that the embryo is created solely by the male, while the female provides a suitable place, nutrition, and oxygen; and that the idea of the male's semen merely stimulating the female's egg to start life, as some thinkers suggest, has no backing from experiments or comparisons.

III. 1. Many ingenious philosophers have found so great difficulty in conceiving the manner of the reproduction of animals, that they have supposed all the numerous progeny, to have existed in miniature in the animal originally created; and that these infinitely minute forms are only evolved or distended, as the embryon increases in the womb. This idea, besides its being unsupported by any analogy we are acquainted with, ascribes a greater tenuity to organized matter, than we can readily admit; as these included embryons are supposed each of them to consist of the various and complicate parts of animal bodies: they must possess a much greater degree of minuteness, than that which was ascribed to the devils that tempted St. Anthony; of whom 20,000 were said to have been able to dance a saraband on the point of the finest needle without incommoding each other.

III. 1. Many clever philosophers have struggled to understand how animals reproduce, leading them to believe that all the many offspring existed in tiny form in the first animal created. They think these minuscule forms simply grow or stretch as the embryo develops in the womb. This idea, apart from lacking any supporting analogy we know of, assumes a level of fineness in organized matter that we can hardly accept. These supposed tiny embryos are thought to contain the complex parts of animal bodies, so they must be much smaller than what was attributed to the devils that tempted St. Anthony, of whom it was said that 20,000 could dance a saraband on the tip of the finest needle without bothering one another.

2. Others have supposed, that all the parts of the embryon are formed in the male, previous to its being deposited in the egg or uterus; and that it is then only to have its parts evolved or distended as mentioned above; but this is only to get rid of one difficulty by proposing another equally incomprehensible: they found it difficult to conceive, how the embryon could be formed in the uterus or egg, and therefore wished it to be formed before it came thither. In answer to both these doctrines it may be observed, 1st, that some animals, as the crab-fish, can reproduce a whole limb, as a leg which has been broken off; others, as worms and snails, can reproduce a head, or a tail, when either of them has been cut away; and that hence in these animals at least a part can be formed anew, which cannot be supposed to have existed previously in miniature.

2. Some people believe that all parts of the embryo are created in the male before it is deposited in the egg or uterus, and that its parts only then grow or expand as mentioned above. However, this just replaces one challenge with another equally hard to understand: they struggle to imagine how the embryo could develop in the uterus or egg, so they prefer to think it was formed beforehand. In response to both of these theories, it's worth noting that some animals, like crabs, can regenerate an entire limb, such as a leg that has been lost; others, like worms and snails, can regenerate a head or tail if either one is cut off. This shows that at least in these creatures, a part can be formed anew, which couldn't have existed before in a smaller version.

Secondly, there are new parts or new vessels produced in many diseases, as on the cornea of the eye in ophthalmy, in wens and cancers, which cannot be supposed to have had a prototype or original miniature in the embryon.

Secondly, new tissues or vessels are formed in many diseases, like on the cornea of the eye in inflammation, in cysts, and cancers, which can’t be thought to have had a prototype or original model in the embryo.

Thirdly, how could mule-animals be produced, which partake of the forms of both the parents, if the original embryon was a miniature existing in the semen of the male parent? if an embryon of the male ass was only expanded, no resemblance to the mare could exist in the mule.

Thirdly, how could mules be produced that have traits from both parents if the original embryo was just a tiny version found in the male parent's sperm? If the embryo of the male donkey was simply enlarged, there would be no similarities to the female horse in the mule.

This mistaken idea of the extension of parts seems to have had its rise from the mature man resembling the general form of the fetus; and from thence it was believed, that the parts of the fetus were distended into the man; whereas they have increased 100 times in weight, as well as 100 times in size; now no one will call the additional 99 parts a distention of the original one part in respect to weight. Thus the uterus during pregnancy is greatly enlarged in thickness and solidity as well as in capacity, and hence must have acquired this additional size by accretion of new parts, not by an extension of the old ones; the familiar act of blowing up the bladder of an animal recently slaughtered has led our imaginations to apply this idea of distention to the increase of size from natural growth; which however must be owing to the apposition of new parts; as it is evinced from the increase of weight along with the increase of dimension; and is even visible to our eyes in the elongation of our hair from the colour of its ends; or when it has been dyed on the head; and in the growth of our nails from the specks sometimes observable on them; and in the increase of the white crescent at their roots, and in the growth of new flesh in wounds, which consists of new nerves as well as of new blood-vessels.

This incorrect idea about how parts extend seems to come from the mature man looking like the general shape of a fetus; people then believed that the parts of the fetus stretched into the man. However, they have increased 100 times in weight as well as 100 times in size; no one would call the extra 99 parts just a stretching of the original part in terms of weight. Similarly, the uterus during pregnancy becomes much thicker and more solid, as well as larger in capacity, so it must gain this extra size by adding new parts rather than stretching the old ones. The common act of inflating a recently slaughtered animal's bladder has led us to think of stretching as a way to understand size increase from natural growth, which should actually come from the addition of new parts. This is evident in the increase of weight along with the increase in size; we can even see it in the lengthening of our hair from the color at its ends, or when it's dyed on our heads, and in the growth of our nails from the sometimes visible specks, and in the increase of the white crescent at their roots, as well as in the new flesh that grows in wounds, which includes new nerves and new blood vessels.

3. Lastly, Mr. Buffon has with great ingenuity imagined the existence of certain organic particles, which are supposed to be partly alive, and partly mechanic springs. The latter of these were discovered by Mr. Needham in the milt or male organ of a species of cuttle fish, called calmar; the former, or living animalcula, are found in both male and female secretions, in the infusions of seeds, as of pepper, in the jelly of roasted veal, and in all other animal and vegetable substances. These organic particles he supposes to exist in the spermatic fluids of both sexes, and that they are derived thither from every part of the body, and must therefore resemble, as he supposes, the parts from whence they are derived. These organic particles he believes to be in constant activity, till they become mixed in the womb, and then they instantly join and produce an embryon or fetus similar to the two parents.

3. Lastly, Mr. Buffon has cleverly proposed the idea of certain organic particles that are thought to be both partly alive and partly mechanical springs. The latter were discovered by Mr. Needham in the milt or male organ of a type of cuttlefish called calmar; the former, or living microorganisms, are found in both male and female secretions, in infusions of seeds like pepper, in the jelly of roasted veal, and in all other animal and plant substances. He believes these organic particles exist in the reproductive fluids of both sexes and come from every part of the body, so they should resemble the parts they come from. He thinks these organic particles are always active until they mix in the womb, at which point they immediately combine to create an embryo or fetus that resembles both parents.

Many objections might be adduced to this fanciful theory, I shall only mention two. First, that it is analogous to no known animal laws. And secondly, that as these fluids, replete with organic particles derived both from the male and female organs, are supposed to be similar; there is no reason why the mother should not produce a female embryon without the assistance of the male, and realize the lucina sine concubitu.

Many objections could be raised against this fanciful theory, but I will only mention two. First, it doesn't resemble any known animal laws. Second, since these fluids, full of organic particles from both the male and female organs, are thought to be similar, there's no reason why the mother couldn't produce a female embryo without the help of the male, achieving conception without intercourse.

IV. 1. I conceive the primordium, or rudiment of the embryon, as secreted from the blood of the parent, to consist of a simple living filament as a muscular fibre; which I suppose to be an extremity of a nerve of loco-motion, as a fibre of the retina is an extremity of a nerve of sensation; as for instance one of the fibrils, which compose the mouth of an absorbent vessel; I suppose this living filament, of whatever form it may be, whether sphere, cube, or cylinder, to be endued with the capability of being excited into action by certain kinds of stimulus. By the stimulus of the surrounding fluid, in which it is received from the male, it may bend into a ring; and thus form the beginning of a tube. Such moving filaments, and such rings, are described by those, who have attended to microscopic animalcula. This living ring may now embrace or absorb a nutritive particle of the fluid, in which it swims; and by drawing it into its pores, or joining it by compression to its extremities, may increase its own length or crassitude; and by degrees the living ring may become a living tube.

IV. 1. I see the origin, or basic form of the embryo, as being created from the parent's blood, made up of a simple living strand like a muscle fiber. I believe this to be the end of a movement nerve, similar to how a fiber in the retina is the end of a sensory nerve. For example, one of the fibers that make up the mouth of an absorbent vessel; I think this living strand, no matter what shape it is—be it a sphere, cube, or cylinder—has the ability to be activated by certain types of stimuli. With the stimulus from the surrounding fluid it receives from the male, it might bend into a ring and thus start to form a tube. Such moving strands and rings have been noted by those who have studied microscopic creatures. This living ring can now surround or absorb a nutrient particle from the fluid it’s in; by pulling it into its pores or compressing it to its ends, it can grow longer or thicker; and gradually, the living ring can turn into a living tube.

2. With this new organization, or accretion of parts, new kinds of irritability may commence; for so long as there was but one living organ, it could only be supposed to possess irritability; since sensibility may be conceived to be an extension of the effect of irritability over the rest of the system. These new kinds of irritability and of sensibility in consequence of new organization, appear from variety of facts in the more mature animal; thus the formation of the testes, and consequent secretion of the semen, occasion the passion of lust; the lungs must be previously formed before their exertions to obtain fresh air can exist; the throat or œsophagus must be formed previous to the sensation or appetites of hunger and thirst; one of which seems to reside at the upper end, and the other at the lower end of that canal.

2. With this new structure, or addition of parts, new types of irritability may begin; as long as there was only one living organ, we could assume it only had irritability since sensitivity can be seen as an extension of irritability's effects throughout the rest of the system. These new types of irritability and sensitivity arise from this new organization, as seen in various cases in more developed animals; for example, the formation of the testes and the subsequent production of semen lead to feelings of lust; the lungs must be fully formed before they can function to take in fresh air; the throat or esophagus needs to be developed before we can experience hunger and thirst, which seem to be associated with the upper and lower ends of that canal, respectively.

Thus also the glans penis, when it is distended with blood, acquires a new sensibility, and a new appetency. The same occurs to the nipples of the breasts of female animals, when they are distended with blood, they acquire the new appetency of giving milk. So inflamed tendons and membranes, and even bones, acquire new sensations; and the parts of mutilated animals, as of wounded snails, and polypi, and crabs, are reproduced; and at the same time acquire sensations adapted to their situations. Thus when the head of a snail is reproduced after decollation with a sharp rasor, those curious telescopic eyes are also reproduced, and acquire their sensibility to light, as well as their adapted muscles for retraction on the approach of injury.

So, when the glans penis fills with blood, it gains heightened sensitivity and a new desire. The same happens with the nipples of female animals; when they fill with blood, they develop the new desire to produce milk. Inflamed tendons, membranes, and even bones also gain new sensations. Additionally, the parts of mutilated animals, like wounded snails, octopuses, and crabs, regenerate and develop sensations suited to their circumstances. For instance, when a snail's head regenerates after being cleanly cut off, those fascinating telescopic eyes are also regrown and gain sensitivity to light along with the muscle adaptations needed to retract in response to threats.

With every new change, therefore, of organic form, or addition of organic parts, I suppose a new kind of irritability or of sensibility to be produced; such varieties of irritability or of sensibility exist in our adult state in the glands; every one of which is furnished with an irritability, or a taste, or appetency, and a consequent mode of action peculiar to itself.

With every new change in organic form or the addition of organic parts, I believe a new type of irritability or sensitivity arises. Different kinds of irritability or sensitivity are present in our adult state in the glands; each one has its own specific irritability, taste, or desire, along with a distinct way of acting that is unique to itself.

In this manner I conceive the vessels of the jaws to produce those of the teeth, those of the fingers to produce the nails, those of the skin to produce the hair; in the same manner as afterwards about the age of puberty the beard and other great changes in the form of the body, and disposition of the mind, are produced in consequence of the new secretion of semen; for if the animal is deprived of this secretion those changes do not take place. These changes I conceive to be formed not by elongation or distention of primeval stamina, but by apposition of parts; as the mature crab-fish, when deprived of a limb, in a certain space of time has power to regenerate it; and the tadpole puts forth its feet long after its exclusion from the spawn; and the caterpillar in changing into a butterfly acquires a new form, with new powers, new sensations, and new desires.

In this way, I believe that the vessels in the jaws create the teeth, those in the fingers create the nails, and those in the skin create the hair; just like later, around puberty, the beard and other significant changes in the body's shape and mental disposition occur due to the new production of semen. If the animal lacks this secretion, those changes don’t happen. I think these changes are formed not by the stretching or expanding of original materials but by the addition of parts; for example, the mature crab can regenerate a limb within a certain amount of time if it loses one, and the tadpole grows its legs long after it leaves the spawn; similarly, the caterpillar transforms into a butterfly, gaining a new shape, new abilities, new sensations, and new desires.

The natural history of butterflies, and moths, and beetles, and gnats, is full of curiosity; some of them pass many months, and others even years, in their caterpillar or grub state; they then rest many weeks without food, suspended in the air, buried in the earth, or submersed in water; and change themselves during this time into an animal apparently of a different nature; the stomachs of some of them, which before digested vegetable leaves or roots, now only digest honey; they have acquired wings for the purpose of seeking this new food, and a long proboscis to collect it from flowers, and I suppose a sense of smell to detect the secret places in flowers, where it is formed. The moths, which fly by night, have a much longer proboscis rolled up under their chins like a watch spring; which they extend to collect the honey from flowers in their sleeping state; when they are closed, and the nectaries in consequence more difficult to be plundered. The beetle kind are furnished with an external covering of a hard material to their wings, that they may occasionally again make holes in the earth, in which they passed the former state of their existence.

The natural history of butterflies, moths, beetles, and gnats is full of intrigue. Some of them spend many months, and others even years, in their caterpillar or larval stage. Then they rest for weeks without food, either suspended in the air, buried in the ground, or submerged in water, transforming into an animal that seems completely different. The stomachs of some of them, which used to digest plant leaves or roots, now only digest nectar; they have developed wings to seek this new food and a long proboscis to collect it from flowers. I assume they also have a sense of smell to find the hidden spots in flowers where nectar is formed. Night-flying moths have a much longer proboscis that is curled up under their chins like a watch spring; they extend it to gather nectar from flowers while they are resting, making it harder to reach the nectaries. Beetles are equipped with a hard outer covering over their wings so they can occasionally dig holes in the ground, where they spent the previous stage of their lives.

But what most of all distinguishes these new animals is, that they are new furnished with the powers of reproduction; and that they now differ from each other in sex, which does not appear in their caterpillar or grub state. In some of them the change from a caterpillar into a butterfly or moth seems to be accomplished for the sole purpose of their propagation; since they immediately die after this is finished, and take no food in the interim, as the silk-worm in this climate; though it is possible, it might take honey as food, if it was presented to it. For in general it would seem, that food of a more stimulating kind, the honey of vegetables instead of their leaves, was necessary for the purpose of the seminal reproduction of these animals, exactly similar to what happens in vegetables; in these the juices of the earth are sufficient for their purpose of reproduction by buds or bulbs; in which the new plant seems to be formed by irritative motions, like the growth of their other parts, as their leaves or roots; but for the purpose of seminal or amatorial reproduction, where sensation is required, a more stimulating food becomes necessary for the anther, and stigma; and this food is honey; as explained in Sect. XIII. on Vegetable Animation.

But what most of all distinguishes these new animals is, that they are new furnished with the powers of reproduction; and that they now differ from each other in sex, which does not appear in their caterpillar or grub state. In some of them the change from a caterpillar into a butterfly or moth seems to be accomplished for the sole purpose of their propagation; since they immediately die after this is finished, and take no food in the interim, as the silk-worm in this climate; though it is possible, it might take honey as food, if it was presented to it. For in general it would seem, that food of a more stimulating kind, the honey of vegetables instead of their leaves, was necessary for the purpose of the seminal reproduction of these animals, exactly similar to what happens in vegetables; in these the juices of the earth are sufficient for their purpose of reproduction by buds or bulbs; in which the new plant seems to be formed by irritative motions, like the growth of their other parts, as their leaves or roots; but for the purpose of seminal or amatorial reproduction, where sensation is required, a more stimulating food becomes necessary for the anther, and stigma; and this food is honey; as explained in Sect. XIII. on Vegetable Animation.

The gnat and the tadpole resemble each other in their change from natant animals with gills into aerial animals with lungs; and in their change of the element in which they live; and probably of the food, with which they are supported; and lastly, with their acquiring in their new state the difference of sex, and the organs of seminal or amatorial reproduction. While the polypus, who is their companion in their former state of life, not being allowed to change his form and element, can only propagate like vegetable buds by the same kind of irritative motions, which produces the growth of his own body, without the seminal or amatorial propagation, which requires sensation; and which in gnats and tadpoles seems to require a change both of food and of respiration.

The gnat and the tadpole are similar in how they transform from swimming creatures with gills to land-dwelling ones with lungs; they also change the environment they live in, likely their diet, and eventually develop sexual differences along with reproductive organs. Meanwhile, the polyp, which shares their earlier way of life, cannot change its form or environment. It can only reproduce like plant buds through the same kinds of movements that promote its own growth, without the sexual reproduction that needs sensation, which in gnats and tadpoles appears to necessitate a shift in both diet and breathing.

From hence I conclude, that with the acquisition of new parts, new sensations, and new desires, as well as new powers, are produced; and this by accretion to the old ones, and not by distention of them. And finally, that the most essential parts of the system, as the brain for the purpose of distributing the power of life, and the placenta for the purpose of oxygenating the blood, and the additional absorbent vessels for the purpose of acquiring aliment, are first formed by the irritations above mentioned, and by the pleasurable sensations attending those irritations, and by the exertions in consequence of painful sensations, similar to those of hunger and suffocation. After these an apparatus of limbs for future uses, or for the purpose of moving the body in its present natant state, and of lungs for future respiration, and of testes for future reproduction, are formed by the irritations and sensations, and consequent exertions of the parts previously existing, and to which the new parts are to be attached.

From this, I conclude that when new parts are acquired, new sensations, desires, and abilities emerge; this happens by adding to the existing ones rather than stretching them. Ultimately, the most crucial parts of the system—like the brain for distributing life energy, the placenta for oxygenating the blood, and the extra absorbent vessels for obtaining nourishment—are formed first through the irritations mentioned above, the pleasurable sensations that accompany those irritations, and the efforts resulting from painful sensations like hunger and suffocation. Following this, a system of limbs for future uses, lungs for future breathing, and testes for future reproduction are developed through the irritations and sensations, as well as the resultant efforts of the already existing parts to which the new parts will connect.

3. In confirmation of these ideas it may be observed, that all the parts of the body endeavour to grow, or to make additional parts to themselves throughout our lives; but are restrained by the parts immediately containing them; thus, if the skin be taken away, the fleshy parts beneath soon shoot out new granulations, called by the vulgar proud flesh. If the periosteum be removed, a similar growth commences from the bone. Now in the case of the imperfect embryon, the containing or confining parts are not yet supposed to be formed, and hence there is nothing to restrain its growth.

3. To support these ideas, it can be observed that all parts of the body strive to grow or to develop additional parts throughout our lives; however, they are restricted by the surrounding tissues. For example, if the skin is removed, the underlying flesh quickly produces new granulations commonly known as proud flesh. Similarly, if the periosteum is taken away, a similar growth starts from the bone. In the case of an imperfect embryo, the surrounding or confining structures are not yet thought to be formed, so there is nothing to limit its growth.

4. By the parts of the embryon being thus produced by new apportions, many phenomena both of animal and vegetable productions receive an easier explanation; such as that many fetuses are deficient at the extremities, as in a finger or a toe, or in the end of the tongue, or in what is called a hare-lip with deficiency of the palate. For if there should be a deficiency in the quantity of the first nutritive particles laid up in the egg for the reception of the first living filament, the extreme parts, as being last formed, must shew this deficiency by their being imperfect.

4. By the parts of the embryo being produced through new contributions, many phenomena related to both animal and plant development can be more easily explained. For instance, many fetuses are lacking in certain extremities, like a finger or a toe, or at the tip of the tongue, or have a condition known as a hare-lip where the palate is also missing. If there is a shortage in the amount of the initial nutrients stored in the egg for the formation of the first living thread, the extremities, being the last to develop, will show this deficiency by being underdeveloped.

This idea of the growth of the embryon accords also with the production of some monstrous births, which consist of a duplicature of the limbs, as chickens with four legs; which could not occur, if the fetus was formed by the distention of an original stamen, or miniature. For if there should be a superfluity of the first nutritive particles laid up in the egg for the first living filament; it is easy to conceive, that a duplicature of some parts may be formed. And that such superfluous nourishment sometimes exists, is evinced by the double yolks in some eggs, which I suppose were thus formed previous to their impregnation by the exuberant nutriment of the hen.

This idea about the growth of the embryo also connects with the occurrence of some strange births, like chickens with four legs. This could not happen if the fetus developed from an original stamen or miniature. If there were an excess of the first nourishing particles stored in the egg for the initial living filament, it's easy to imagine that a duplication of some parts could form. The existence of such extra nourishment is shown by double yolks in some eggs, which I believe were formed before they were fertilized due to the hen's abundant nutrients.

This idea is confirmed by the analogy of the monsters in the vegetable world also; in which a duplicate or triplicate production of various parts of the flower is observable, as a triple nectary in some columbines, and a triple petal in some primroses; and which are supposed to be produced by abundant nourishment.

This idea is supported by the similarity of monsters in the plant world as well; where you can see duplicate or triplicate growths of different parts of the flower, like a triple nectary in some columbines and a triple petal in some primroses. These are thought to result from ample nourishment.

5. If the embryon be received into a fluid, whose stimulus is different in some degree from the natural, as in the production of mule-animals, the new irritabilities or sensibilities acquired by the increasing or growing organized parts may differ, and thence produce parts not similar to the father, but of a kind belonging in part to the mother; and thus, though the original stamen or living ens was derived totally from the father, yet new irritabilities or sensibilities being excited, a change of form corresponding with them will be produced. Nor could the production of mules exist, if the stamen or miniature of all the parts of the embryon is previously formed in the male semen, and is only distended by nourishment in the female uterus. Whereas this difficulty ceases, if the embryon be supposed to consist of a living filament, which acquires or makes new parts with new irritabilities, as it advances in its growth.

5. If an embryo is placed in a fluid that has a different kind of stimulation than what is natural, like in the case of mules, the new sensitivities or responsiveness that develop as the organism grows may vary. This can result in parts that are not exactly like the father’s but share some characteristics with the mother’s. So, even though the original seed or living essence comes entirely from the father, the activation of new sensitivities will lead to a change in form that matches those differences. The existence of mules wouldn't be possible if the seed or miniature of all the embryo's parts was completely formed in the male sperm and merely expanded through nourishment in the female womb. This problem disappears if we think of the embryo as a living thread that creates or develops new parts with new sensitivities as it grows.

The form, solidity, and colour, of the particles of nutriment laid up for the reception of the first living filament, as well as their peculiar kind of stimulus, may contribute to produce a difference in the form, solidity, and colour of the fetus, so as to resemble the mother, as it advances in life. This also may especially happen during the first state of the existence of the embryon, before it has acquired organs, which can change these first nutritive particles, as explained in No. 5. 2. of this Section. And as these nutritive particles are supposed to be similar to those, which are formed for her own nutrition, it follows that the fetus should so far resemble the mother.

The form, solidity, and colour, of the particles of nutriment laid up for the reception of the first living filament, as well as their peculiar kind of stimulus, may contribute to produce a difference in the form, solidity, and colour of the fetus, so as to resemble the mother, as it advances in life. This also may especially happen during the first state of the existence of the embryon, before it has acquired organs, which can change these first nutritive particles, as explained in No. 5. 2. of this Section. And as these nutritive particles are supposed to be similar to those, which are formed for her own nutrition, it follows that the fetus should so far resemble the mother.

This explains, why hereditary diseases may be derived either from the male or female parent, as well as the peculiar form of either of their bodies. Some of these hereditary diseases are simply owing to a deficient activity of a part of the system, as of the absorbent vessels, which open into the cells or cavities of the body, and thus occasion dropsies. Others are at the same time owing to an increase of sensation, as in scrophula and consumption; in these the obstruction of the fluids is first caused by the inirritability of the vessels, and the inflammation and ulcers which succeed, are caused by the consequent increase of sensation in the obstructed part. Other hereditary diseases, as the epilepsy, and other convulsions, consist in too great voluntary exertions in consequence of disagreeable sensation in some particular diseased part. Now as the pains, which occasion these convulsions, are owing to defect of the action of the diseased part, as shewn in Sect. XXXIV. it is plain, that all these hereditary diseases may have their origin either from defective irritability derived from the father, or from deficiency of the stimulus of the nutriment derived from the mother. In either case the effect would be similar; as a scrophulous race is frequently produced among the poor from the deficient stimulus of bad diet, or of hunger; and among the rich, by a deficient irritability from their having been long accustomed to too great stimulus, as of vinous spirit.

This explains, why hereditary diseases may be derived either from the male or female parent, as well as the peculiar form of either of their bodies. Some of these hereditary diseases are simply owing to a deficient activity of a part of the system, as of the absorbent vessels, which open into the cells or cavities of the body, and thus occasion dropsies. Others are at the same time owing to an increase of sensation, as in scrophula and consumption; in these the obstruction of the fluids is first caused by the inirritability of the vessels, and the inflammation and ulcers which succeed, are caused by the consequent increase of sensation in the obstructed part. Other hereditary diseases, as the epilepsy, and other convulsions, consist in too great voluntary exertions in consequence of disagreeable sensation in some particular diseased part. Now as the pains, which occasion these convulsions, are owing to defect of the action of the diseased part, as shewn in Sect. XXXIV. it is plain, that all these hereditary diseases may have their origin either from defective irritability derived from the father, or from deficiency of the stimulus of the nutriment derived from the mother. In either case the effect would be similar; as a scrophulous race is frequently produced among the poor from the deficient stimulus of bad diet, or of hunger; and among the rich, by a deficient irritability from their having been long accustomed to too great stimulus, as of vinous spirit.

6. From this account of reproduction it appears, that all animals have a similar origin, viz. from a single living filament; and that the difference of their forms and qualities has arisen only from the different irritabilities and sensibilities, or voluntarities, or associabilities, of this original living filament; and perhaps in some degree from the different forms of the particles of the fluids, by which it has been at first stimulated into activity. And that from hence, as Linnæus has conjectured in respect to the vegetable world, it is not impossible, but the great variety of species of animals, which now tenant the earth, may have had their origin from the mixture of a few natural orders. And that those animal and vegetable mules, which could continue their species, have done so, and constitute the numerous families of animals and vegetables which now exist; and that those mules, which were produced with imperfect organs of generation, perished without reproduction, according to the observation of Aristotle; and are the animals, which we now call mules. See Botanic Garden, Part II. Note on Dianthus.

6. From this description of reproduction, it seems that all animals share a common origin, specifically from a single living filament. The differences in their shapes and qualities have arisen solely from varying levels of irritability, sensitivity, willfulness, or ability to associate in this original living filament. Additionally, these differences might be influenced to some extent by the distinct structures of the particles in the fluids that initially triggered its activity. Thus, as Linnæus has proposed regarding the plant kingdom, it’s possible that the great diversity of animal species that inhabit the earth today originated from a combination of a few natural groups. Furthermore, those animal and plant hybrids that could reproduce have done so and make up the many families of animals and plants we see today, while those hybrids with imperfect reproductive organs did not survive to reproduce, as Aristotle noted; these are the animals we now refer to as mules. See Botanic Garden, Part II. Note on Dianthus.

Such a promiscuous intercourse of animals is said to exist at this day in New South Wales by Captain Hunter. And that not only amongst the quadrupeds and birds of different kinds, but even amongst the fish, and, as he believes, amongst the vegetables. He speaks of an animal between the opossum and the kangaroo, from the size of a sheep to that of a rat. Many fish seemed to partake of the shark; some with a shark's head and shoulders, and the hind part of a shark; others with a shark's head and the body of a mullet; and some with a shark's head and the flat body of a sting-ray. Many birds partake of the parrot; some have the head, neck, and bill of a parrot, with long straight feet and legs; others with legs and feet of a parrot, with head and neck of a sea gull. Voyage to South Wales by Captain John Hunter, p. 68.

Such a mix of animals is said to exist today in New South Wales, according to Captain Hunter. This is true not just among various mammals and birds, but even among fish, and, as he believes, among plants as well. He describes an animal that is a mix between an opossum and a kangaroo, ranging in size from a sheep to a rat. Many fish appear to share traits with sharks; some have the head and shoulders of a shark but the back end of a shark, while others have a shark's head and the body of a mullet, and some have a shark's head with the flat body of a stingray. Many birds resemble parrots; some have the head, neck, and beak of a parrot but with long straight feet and legs, while others have the legs and feet of a parrot with the head and neck of a seagull. Voyage to South Wales by Captain John Hunter, p. 68.

7. All animals therefore, I contend, have a similar cause of their organization, originating from a single living filament, endued indeed with different kinds of irritabilities and sensibilities, or of animal appetencies; which exist in every gland, and in every moving organ of the body, and are as essential to living organization as chemical affinities are to certain combinations of inanimate matter.

7. I believe that all animals have a common source for their structure, stemming from a single living thread, which, while having various kinds of sensitivities and responses, or instinctual drives, is present in every gland and in every muscle of the body, and is as crucial to living organisms as chemical bonds are to specific combinations of non-living matter.

If I might be indulged to make a simile in a philosophical work, I should say, that the animal appetencies are not only perhaps less numerous originally than the chemical affinities; but that like these latter, they change with every new combination; thus vital air and azote, when combined, produce nitrous acid; which now acquires the property of dissolving silver; so with every new additional part to the embryon, as of the throat or lungs, I suppose a new animal appetency to be produced.

If I'm allowed to make a comparison in a philosophical piece, I would say that animal desires are probably fewer in number from the start than chemical affinities; but like the latter, they change with each new combination. For instance, when vital air and nitrogen are combined, they create nitrous acid, which then gains the ability to dissolve silver. Similarly, with every new part added to the embryo, like the throat or lungs, I believe a new animal desire is generated.

In this early formation of the embryon from the irritabilities, sensibilities, and associabilities, and consequent appetencies, the faculty of volition can scarcely be supposed to have had its birth. For about what can the fetus deliberate, when it has no choice of objects? But in the more advanced state of the fetus, it evidently possesses volition; as it frequently changes its attitude, though it seems to sleep the greatest part of its time; and afterwards the power of volition contributes to change or alter many parts of the body during its growth to manhood, by our early modes of exertion in the various departments of life. All these faculties then constitute the vis fabricatrix, and the vis conservatrix, as well as the vis medicatrix of nature, so much spoken of, but so little understood by philosophers.

In the early development of the embryo, with its sensitivities, responses, and connections, along with resulting desires, the ability to make choices likely hasn’t developed yet. After all, what can the fetus contemplate when it has no options to choose from? However, in the later stages of fetal development, it clearly has the ability to make choices; it often changes its position, even though it seems to sleep most of the time. Later, this ability to choose plays a significant role in altering various parts of the body as it grows into adulthood, shaped by our initial efforts in different areas of life. Together, all these abilities make up the natural forces of building, preserving, and healing that are often discussed but not well understood by philosophers.

8. When we revolve in our minds, first, the great changes, which we see naturally produced in animals after their nativity, as in the production of the butterfly with painted wings from the crawling caterpillar; or of the respiring frog from the subnatant tadpole; from the feminine boy to the bearded man, and from the infant girl to the lactescent woman; both which changes may be prevented by certain mutilations of the glands necessary to reproduction.

8. When we think about the big changes we naturally see in animals after they're born, like the transformation of a caterpillar into a butterfly with colorful wings, or a tadpole turning into a breathing frog, or a boy becoming a man with facial hair, and a young girl growing into a mature woman, we realize that these transformations can be stopped by certain removals of glands that are essential for reproduction.

Secondly, when we think over the great changes introduced into various animals by artificial or accidental cultivation, as in horses, which we have exercised for the different purposes of strength or swiftness, in carrying burthens or in running races; or in dogs, which have been cultivated for strength and courage, as the bull-dog; or for acuteness of his sense or smell, as the hound and spaniel; or for the swiftness of his foot, as the greyhound; or for his swimming in the water, or for drawing snow-sledges, as the rough-haired dogs of the north; or lastly, as a play-dog for children, as the lap-dog; with the changes of the forms of the cattle, which have been domesticated from the greatest antiquity, as camels, and sheep; which have undergone so total a transformation, that we are now ignorant from what species of wild animals they had their origin. Add to these the great changes of shape and colour, which we daily see produced in smaller animals from our domestication of them, as rabbits, or pigeons; or from the difference of climates and even of seasons; thus the sheep of warm climates are covered with hair instead of wool; and the hares and partridges of the latitudes, which are long buried in snow, become white during the winter months; add to these the various changes produced in the forms of mankind, by their early modes of exertion; or by the diseases occasioned by their habits of life; both of which became hereditary, and that through many generations. Those who labour at the anvil, the oar, or the loom, as well as those who carry sedan-chairs, or who have been educated to dance upon the rope, are distinguishable by the shape of their limbs; and the diseases occasioned by intoxication deform the countenance with leprous eruptions, or the body with tumid viscera, or the joints with knots and distortions.

Secondly, when we consider the significant changes made to various animals through artificial or accidental breeding, like horses, which we have trained for different purposes such as strength or speed, whether for carrying loads or competing in races; or dogs, which have been bred for traits like strength and bravery, as seen in bulldogs; or for their keen sense of smell, like hounds and spaniels; or for their speed, as in greyhounds; or for their swimming ability, or for pulling sleds in the snow, like northern breeds; or finally, as companion animals for kids, like lap dogs; along with the transformations in domesticated livestock, such as camels and sheep, which have changed so drastically that we now have no idea what wild species they originated from. Additionally, we see major changes in shape and color in smaller domesticated animals like rabbits and pigeons, or due to varying climates and seasons; for example, sheep in warmer climates have hair instead of wool, and hares and partridges in snowy regions turn white in winter. We must also consider the various changes in human forms due to their early physical activities or diseases resulting from their lifestyle, both of which can be passed down through many generations. People who work at an anvil, row, or weave, as well as those who carry sedan chairs or have been trained to walk on tightropes, can be recognized by the shape of their limbs; and diseases caused by excessive drinking can disfigure the face with sores, swell the body with bloated organs, or cause knots and deformities in the joints.

Thirdly, when we enumerate the great changes produced in the species of animals before their nativity; these are such as resemble the form or colour of their parents, which have been altered by the cultivation or accidents above related, and are thus continued to their posterity. Or they are changes produced by the mixture of species as in mules; or changes produced probably by the exuberance of nourishment supplied to the fetus, as in monstrous births with additional limbs; many of these enormities of shape are propagated, and continued as a variety at least, if not as a new species of animal. I have seen a breed of cats with an additional claw on every foot; of poultry also with an additional claw, and with wings to their feet; and of others without rumps. Mr. Buffon mentions a breed of dogs without tails, which are common at Rome and at Naples, which he supposes to have been produced by a custom long established of cutting their tails close off. There are many kinds of pigeons, admired for their peculiarities, which are monsters thus produced and propagated. And to these must be added, the changes produced by the imagination of the male parent, as will be treated of more at large in No. VI. of this Section.

Thirdly, when we enumerate the great changes produced in the species of animals before their nativity; these are such as resemble the form or colour of their parents, which have been altered by the cultivation or accidents above related, and are thus continued to their posterity. Or they are changes produced by the mixture of species as in mules; or changes produced probably by the exuberance of nourishment supplied to the fetus, as in monstrous births with additional limbs; many of these enormities of shape are propagated, and continued as a variety at least, if not as a new species of animal. I have seen a breed of cats with an additional claw on every foot; of poultry also with an additional claw, and with wings to their feet; and of others without rumps. Mr. Buffon mentions a breed of dogs without tails, which are common at Rome and at Naples, which he supposes to have been produced by a custom long established of cutting their tails close off. There are many kinds of pigeons, admired for their peculiarities, which are monsters thus produced and propagated. And to these must be added, the changes produced by the imagination of the male parent, as will be treated of more at large in No. VI. of this Section.

When we consider all these changes of animal form, and innumerable others, which may be collected from the books of natural history; we cannot but be convinced, that the fetus or embryon is formed by apposition of new parts, and not by the distention of a primordial nest of germs, included one within another, like the cups of a conjurer.

When we think about all these changes in animal forms, along with countless others found in natural history books, we can't help but believe that a fetus or embryo is created by adding new parts, not by expanding a basic nest of germs layered inside each other like a magician’s cups.

Fourthly, when we revolve in our minds the great similarity of structure, which obtains in all the warm-blooded animals, as well quadrupeds, birds, and amphibious animals, as in mankind; from the mouse and bat to the elephant and whale; one is led to conclude, that they have alike been produced from a similar living filament. In some this filament in its advance to maturity has acquired hands and fingers, with a fine sense of touch, as in mankind. In others it has acquired claws or talons, as in tygers and eagles. In others, toes with an intervening web, or membrane, as in seals and geese. In others it has acquired cloven hoofs, as in cows and swine; and whole hoofs in others, as in the horse. While in the bird kind this original living filament has put forth wings instead of arms or legs, and feathers instead of hair. In some it has protruded horns on the forehead instead of teeth in the fore part of the upper jaw; in others tushes instead of horns; and in others beaks instead of either. And all this exactly as is daily seen in the transmutations of the tadpole, which acquires legs and lungs, when he wants them; and loses his tail, when it is no longer of service to him.

Fourthly, when we think about the great similarity in structure that exists among all warm-blooded animals, whether they are four-legged, birds, or amphibians, as well as humans; from mice and bats to elephants and whales; we are led to conclude that they have all come from a similar living thread. In some, this thread has developed into hands and fingers, with a delicate sense of touch, like in humans. In others, it has turned into claws or talons, like in tigers and eagles. In others, it has formed webbed toes or membranes, like in seals and geese. In others, it has developed split hooves, like in cows and pigs, and solid hooves in others, like in horses. Meanwhile, in birds, this original living thread has grown wings instead of arms or legs, and feathers instead of fur. In some, it has produced horns on the forehead instead of teeth in the upper jaw; in others, tusks instead of horns; and in others, beaks instead of both. And all of this happens just as we see every day in the transformations of a tadpole, which develops legs and lungs when it needs them and loses its tail when it's no longer useful.

Fifthly, from their first rudiment, or primordium, to the termination of their lives, all animals undergo perpetual transformations; which are in part produced by their own exertions in consequence of their desires and aversions, of their pleasures and their pains, or of irritations, or of associations; and many of these acquired forms or propensities are transmitted to their posterity. See Sect. XXXI. 1.

Fifthly, from their first rudiment, or primordium, to the termination of their lives, all animals undergo perpetual transformations; which are in part produced by their own exertions in consequence of their desires and aversions, of their pleasures and their pains, or of irritations, or of associations; and many of these acquired forms or propensities are transmitted to their posterity. See Sect. XXXI. 1.

As air and water are supplied to animals in sufficient profusion, the three great objects of desire, which have changed the forms of many animals by their exertions to gratify them, are those of lust, hunger, and security. A great want of one part of the animal world has consisted in the desire of the exclusive possession of the females; and these have acquired weapons to combat each other for this purpose, as the very thick, shield-like, horny skin on the shoulder of the boar is a defence only against animals of his own species, who strike obliquely upwards, nor are his tushes for other purposes, except to defend himself, as he is not naturally a carnivorous animal. So the horns of the stag are sharp to offend his adversary, but are branched for the purpose of parrying or receiving the thrusts of horns similar to his own, and have therefore been formed for the purpose of combating other stags for the exclusive possession of the females; who are observed, like the ladies in the times of chivalry, to attend the car of the victor.

As animals have enough air and water, the three main desires that have driven many animals to change their forms to fulfill them are lust, hunger, and security. A significant need in the animal kingdom is the desire for exclusive access to females, which has led these animals to develop weapons to fight each other for this. For example, the thick, shield-like, tough skin on a boar's shoulder serves as protection against other boars that attack from above, and his tusks exist mainly for self-defense since he isn't naturally a carnivorous animal. Similarly, the stag's horns are sharp to ward off competition but are also branched to deflect and absorb the impact of other stags' horns, meaning they are designed for battling other stags for exclusive access to the females, who, much like ladies during chivalric times, follow the winner.

The birds, which do not carry food to their young, and do not therefore marry, are armed with spurs for the purpose of fighting for the exclusive possession of the females, as cocks and quails. It is certain that these weapons are not provided for their defence against other adversaries, because the females of these species are without this armour. The final cause of this contest amongst the males seems to be, that the strongest and most active animal should propagate the species, which should thence become improved.

The birds that don’t bring food to their young and therefore don’t form pair bonds are equipped with spurs to fight for exclusive access to females, just like roosters and quails. It’s clear that these weapons aren’t meant for defense against other threats, since the females of these species lack this protection. The ultimate purpose of this competition among the males seems to be that the strongest and most active individuals should pass on their genes, leading to an improved species over time.

Another great want consists in the means of procuring food, which has diversified the forms of all species of animals. Thus the nose of the swine has become hard for the purpose of turning up the soil in search of insects and of roots. The trunk of the elephant is an elongation of the nose for the purpose of pulling down the branches of trees for his food, and for taking up water without bending his knees. Beasts of prey have acquired strong jaws or talons. Cattle have acquired a rough tongue and a rough palate to pull off the blades of grass, as cows and sheep. Some birds have acquired harder beaks to crack nuts, as the parrot. Others have acquired beaks adapted to break the harder seeds, as sparrows. Others for the softer seeds of flowers, or the buds of trees, as the finches. Other birds have acquired long beaks to penetrate the moister soils in search of insects or roots, as woodcocks; and others broad ones to filtrate the water of lakes, and to retain aquatic insects. All which seem to have been gradually produced during many generations by the perpetual endeavour of the creatures to supply the want of food, and to have been delivered to their posterity with constant improvement of them for the purposes required.

Another major need is finding food, which has led to different adaptations in all animal species. For example, the pig's snout has become tough for digging up soil to find insects and roots. The elephant's trunk is an extended version of its nose, allowing it to reach tree branches for food and drink water without bending its knees. Predatory animals have developed strong jaws or claws. Cattle have rough tongues and palates to graze on grass, like cows and sheep. Some birds have evolved stronger beaks to crack nuts, like parrots, while others have beaks designed to break tougher seeds, like sparrows. Some birds have beaks suited for softer seeds or flower buds, like finches. Other birds possess long beaks to dig into moist soil for insects or roots, like woodcocks, and some have broad beaks to filter water in lakes and catch aquatic insects. All these adaptations appear to have developed gradually over many generations as animals continuously worked to meet their need for food, passing these improvements down to their offspring.

The third great want amongst animals is that of security, which seems much to have diversified the forms of their bodies and the colour of them; these consist in the means of escaping other animals more powerful than themselves. Hence some animals have acquired wings instead of legs, as the smaller birds, for the purpose of escape. Others great length of fin, or of membrane, as the flying fish, and the bat. Others great swiftness of foot, as the hare. Others have acquired hard or armed shells, as the tortoise and the echinus marinus.

The third major need among animals is security, which has greatly influenced the shapes and colors of their bodies. This is all about finding ways to escape from stronger predators. For instance, some animals have developed wings instead of legs, like smaller birds, to help them flee. Others, like flying fish and bats, have long fins or membranes. Some have become really fast on their feet, like the hare. And others have evolved tough or armored shells, like the tortoise and the sea urchin.

Mr. Osbeck, a pupil of Linnæus, mentions the American frog fish, Lophius Histrio, which inhabits the large floating islands of sea-weed about the Cape of Good Hope, and has fulcra resembling leaves, that the fishes of prey may mistake it for the sea-weed, which it inhabits. Voyage to China, p. 113.

Mr. Osbeck, a student of Linnæus, talks about the American frog fish, Lophius Histrio, which lives in the large floating islands of seaweed near the Cape of Good Hope. It has leaf-like structures that help predatory fish confuse it for the seaweed it resides in. Voyage to China, p. 113.

The contrivances for the purposes of security extend even to vegetables, as is seen in the wonderful and various means of their concealing or defending their honey from insects, and their seeds from birds. On the other hand swiftness of wing has been acquired by hawks and swallows to pursue their prey; and a proboscis of admirable structure has been acquired by the bee, the moth, and the humming bird, for the purpose of plundering the nectaries of flowers. All which seem to have been formed by the original living filament, excited into action by the necessities of the creatures, which possess them, and on which their existence depends.

The methods of protection even reach vegetables, as shown in the amazing and various ways they hide or safeguard their nectar from insects and their seeds from birds. On the flip side, hawks and swallows have developed swift wings to chase their prey; and bees, moths, and hummingbirds have evolved a remarkable proboscis to extract nectar from flowers. All of this appears to have been shaped by a fundamental living thread, prompted into action by the needs of the creatures that have these adaptations, which are essential for their survival.

From thus meditating on the great similarity of the structure of the warm-blooded animals, and at the same time of the great changes they undergo both before and after their nativity; and by considering in how minute a portion of time many of the changes of animals above described have been produced; would it be too bold to imagine, that in the great length of time, since the earth began to exist, perhaps millions of ages before the commencement of the history of mankind, would it be too bold to imagine, that all warm-blooded animals have arisen from one living filament, which THE GREAT FIRST CAUSE endued with animality, with the power of acquiring new parts, attended with new propensities, directed by irritations, sensations, volitions, and associations; and thus possessing the faculty of continuing to improve by its own inherent activity, and of delivering down those improvements by generation to its posterity, world without end!

From reflecting on the strong similarities in the structure of warm-blooded animals, along with the significant transformations they experience both before and after they are born; and by considering how many of the described changes in animals occur in a very short span of time; would it be too daring to think that over the vast stretch of time since the Earth began, perhaps millions of years before human history started, it might not be too bold to propose that all warm-blooded animals have evolved from a single living thread, which THE BEST FFIRST CAUSE endowed with life, the ability to develop new traits, influenced by stimuli, feelings, choices, and connections; and thus possessing the capacity to keep improving through its own active nature, passing those improvements down through reproduction to its descendants, forever!

Sixthly, The cold-blooded animals, as the fish-tribes, which are furnished with but one ventricle of the heart, and with gills instead of lungs, and with fins instead of feet or wings, bear a great similarity to each other; but they differ, nevertheless, so much in their general structure from the warm-blooded animals, that it may not seem probable at first view, that the same living filament could have given origin to this kingdom of animals, as to the former. Yet are there some creatures, which unite or partake of both these orders of animation, as the whales and seals; and more particularly the frog, who changes from an aquatic animal furnished with gills to an aerial one furnished with lungs.

Sixthly, cold-blooded animals, like fish, have only one ventricle in their hearts, gills instead of lungs, and fins instead of feet or wings. They are quite similar to each other; however, they differ significantly in their overall structure from warm-blooded animals. At first glance, it might not seem likely that the same life form could have given rise to both groups of animals. Yet, there are some creatures that combine traits from both types of animals, like whales and seals, and especially frogs, which transform from aquatic animals with gills to land animals with lungs.

The numerous tribes of insects without wings, from the spider to the scorpion, from the flea to the lobster; or with wings, from the gnat and the ant to the wasp and the dragon-fly, differ so totally from each other, and from the red-blooded classes above described, both in the forms of their bodies, and their modes of life; besides the organ of sense, which they seem to possess in their antennæ or horns, to which it has been thought by some naturalists, that other creatures have nothing similar; that it can scarcely be supposed that this nation of animals could have been produced by the same kind of living filament, as the red-blooded classes above mentioned. And yet the changes which many of them undergo in their early state to that of their maturity, are as different, as one animal can be from another. As those of the gnat, which passes his early state in water, and then stretching out his new wings, and expanding his new lungs, rises in the air; as of the caterpillar, and bee-nymph, which feed on vegetable leaves or farina, and at length bursting from their self-formed graves, become beautiful winged inhabitants of the skies, journeying from flower to flower, and nourished by the ambrosial food of honey.

The many tribes of wingless insects, from spiders to scorpions, and fleas to lobsters; or winged ones, like gnats, ants, wasps, and dragonflies, are so completely different from each other, and from the red-blooded animals mentioned earlier, in both their body shapes and ways of living. They also have a sensory organ in their antennae or horns, which some naturalists believe is unique compared to other creatures. It’s hard to believe that this group of animals could have come from the same type of life form as the red-blooded classes mentioned above. Yet, the transformations many of them go through from their early stages to adulthood are as distinct as one animal can be from another. Take the gnat, for example, which lives in water during its early stage, and then, after developing its wings and lungs, takes to the air. Then there are caterpillars and bee larvae, which consume plant leaves or pollen, and eventually emerge from their self-made cocoons, becoming beautiful winged creatures that roam the skies, traveling from flower to flower and feeding on the sweet nectar of honey.

There is still another class of animals, which are termed vermes by Linnæus, which are without feet, or brain, and are hermaphrodites, as worms, leeches, snails, shell-fish, coralline insects, and sponges; which possess the simplest structure of all animals, and appear totally different from those already described. The simplicity of their structure, however, can afford no argument against their having been produced from a living filament as above contended.

There’s yet another group of animals, called vermes by Linnaeus, that have no feet or brain and are hermaphrodites, like worms, leeches, snails, shellfish, coralline insects, and sponges. They have the most basic structure of all animals and seem completely different from the ones already described. However, the simplicity of their structure doesn’t prove that they weren’t formed from a living filament, as previously argued.

Last of all the various tribes of vegetables are to be enumerated amongst the inferior orders of animals. Of these the anthers and stigmas have already been shewn to possess some organs of sense, to be nourished by honey, and to have the power of generation like insects, and have thence been announced amongst the animal kingdom in Sect. XIII. and to these must be added the buds and bulbs which constitute the viviparous offspring of vegetation. The former I suppose to be beholden to a single living filament for their seminal or amatorial procreation; and the latter to the same cause for their lateral or branching generation, which they possess in common with the polypus, tænia, and volvox; and the simplicity of which is an argument in favour of the similarity of its cause.

Last of all the various tribes of vegetables are to be enumerated amongst the inferior orders of animals. Of these the anthers and stigmas have already been shewn to possess some organs of sense, to be nourished by honey, and to have the power of generation like insects, and have thence been announced amongst the animal kingdom in Sect. XIII. and to these must be added the buds and bulbs which constitute the viviparous offspring of vegetation. The former I suppose to be beholden to a single living filament for their seminal or amatorial procreation; and the latter to the same cause for their lateral or branching generation, which they possess in common with the polypus, tænia, and volvox; and the simplicity of which is an argument in favour of the similarity of its cause.

Linnæus supposes, in the Introduction to his Natural Orders, that very few vegetables were at first created, and that their numbers were increased by their intermarriages, and adds, suadent hæc Creatoris leges a simplicibus ad composita. Many other changes seem to have arisen in them by their perpetual contest for light and air above ground, and for food or moisture beneath the soil. As noted in Botanic Garden, Part II. Note on Cuscuta. Other changes of vegetables from climate, or other causes, are remarked in the Note on Curcuma in the same work. From these one might be led to imagine, that each plant at first consisted of a single bulb or flower to each root, as the gentianella and daisy; and that in the contest for air and light new buds grew on the old decaying flower stem, shooting down their elongated roots to the ground, and that in process of ages tall trees were thus formed, and an individual bulb became a swarm of vegetables. Other plants, which in this contest for light and air were too slender to rise by their own strength, learned by degrees to adhere to their neighbours, either by putting forth roots like the ivy, or by tendrils like the vine, or by spiral contortions like the honeysuckle; or by growing upon them like the misleto, and taking nourishment from their barks; or by only lodging or adhering on them, and deriving nourishment from the air, as tillandsia.

Linnaeus suggests in the Introduction to his Natural Orders that very few plants were originally created, and their numbers grew through crossbreeding. He adds that this follows the Creator's laws from simple to complex. Many other changes seem to have emerged from their constant struggle for light and air above ground, and for nutrients or moisture below the soil, as noted in the Botanic Garden, Part II, Note on Cuscuta. Other changes in plants due to climate or other factors are mentioned in the Note on Curcuma in the same work. From this, one might think that each plant initially consisted of a single bulb or flower for each root, like the gentianella and daisy. In their struggle for air and light, new buds developed on the old, dying flower stems, extending their roots down to the ground, leading over time to the formation of tall trees, turning a single bulb into a multitude of plants. Other plants, which were too weak to rise on their own in this contest for light and air, gradually learned to cling to their neighbors, either by sending out roots like ivy, or tendrils like vines, or twisting spirally like honeysuckle; or by growing on them like mistletoe and drawing nourishment from their bark; or by simply lodging or adhering to them and getting sustenance from the air, as seen in tillandsia.

Shall we then say that the vegetable living filament was originally different from that of each tribe of animals above described? And that the productive living filament of each of those tribes was different originally from the other? Or, as the earth and ocean were probably peopled with vegetable productions long before the existence of animals; and many families of these animals long before other families of them, shall we conjecture that one and the same kind of living filaments is and has been the cause of all organic life?

Shall we then say that the plant life form was originally different from that of each group of animals mentioned earlier? And that the life form that produced each of those groups was initially different from the others? Or, since the earth and ocean were likely filled with plant life long before animals existed, and many groups of these animals appeared long before others, should we speculate that one and the same type of living forms has been the source of all organic life?

This idea of the gradual formation and improvement of the animal world accords with the observations of some modern philosophers, who have supposed that the continent of America has been raised out of the ocean at a later period of time than the other three quarters of the globe, which they deduce from the greater comparative heights of its mountains, and the consequent greater coldness of its respective climates, and from the less size and strength of its animals, as the tygers and allegators compared with those of Asia or Africa. And lastly, from the less progress in the improvements of the mind of its inhabitants in respect to voluntary exertions.

This idea of the gradual formation and improvement of the animal world aligns with the observations of some modern philosophers, who suggest that the continent of America emerged from the ocean later than the other three-quarters of the globe. They base this on the greater heights of its mountains, which lead to colder climates, as well as the smaller size and strength of its animals, like tigers and alligators, compared to those in Asia or Africa. Finally, they note the slower progress in the intellectual development of its inhabitants in terms of their voluntary efforts.

This idea of the gradual formation and improvement of the animal world seems not to have been unknown to the ancient philosophers. Plato having probably observed the reciprocal generation of inferior animals, as snails and worms, was of opinion, that mankind with all other animals were originally hermaphrodites during the infancy of the world, and were in process of time separated into male and female. The breasts and teats of all male quadrupeds, to which no use can be now assigned, adds perhaps some shadow of probability to this opinion. Linnæus excepts the horse from the male quadrupeds, who have teats; which might have shewn the earlier origin of his exigence; but Mr. J. Hunter asserts, that he has discovered the vestiges of them on his sheath, and has at the same time enriched natural history with a very curious fact concerning the male pigeon; at the time of hatching the eggs both the male and female pigeon undergo a great change in their crops; which thicken and become corrugated, and secrete a kind of milky fluid, which coagulates, and with which alone they for a few days feed their young, and afterwards feed them with this coagulated fluid mixed with other food. How this resembles the breasts of female quadrupeds after the production of their young! and how extraordinary, that the male should at this time give milk as well as the female! See Botanic Garden, Part II. Note on Curcuma.

This idea of the gradual development and improvement of the animal kingdom seems to have been recognized by ancient philosophers. Plato likely observed the mutual reproduction of lesser animals, like snails and worms, and believed that humans, along with other animals, were originally hermaphrodites in the early stages of the world, later evolving into distinct male and female forms. The existence of breasts and nipples in male quadrupeds, which currently serve no purpose, adds some credibility to this view. Linnæus excludes the horse from the group of male quadrupeds that have nipples, which might suggest a more primitive origin for its existence. However, Mr. J. Hunter claims he has found remnants of them on the horse’s sheath, and at the same time has contributed a fascinating fact to natural history regarding male pigeons; during egg incubation, both male and female pigeons experience significant changes in their crops, which thicken and become textured, allowing them to produce a milky fluid. This fluid solidifies and is used exclusively to feed their young for a few days, after which they mix it with other food. This is reminiscent of the way female quadrupeds produce milk after giving birth! It's remarkable that the male also produces milk just like the female at this time! See Botanic Garden, Part II. Note on Curcuma.

The late Mr. David Hume, in his posthumous works, places the powers of generation much above those of our boasted reason; and adds, that reason can only make a machine, as a clock or a ship, but the power of generation makes the maker of the machine; and probably from having observed, that the greatest part of the earth has been formed out of organic recrements; as the immense beds of limestone, chalk, marble, from the shells of fish; and the extensive provinces of clay, sandstone, ironstone, coals, from decomposed vegetables; all which have been first produced by generation, or by the secretions of organic life; he concludes that the world itself might have been generated, rather than created; that is, it might have been gradually produced from very small beginnings, increasing by the activity of its inherent principles, rather than by a sudden evolution of the whole by the Almighty fire.—What a magnificent idea of the infinite power of THE GREAT ARCHITECT! THE CAUSE OF CAUSES! PARENT OF PARENTS! ENS ENTIUM!

The late Mr. David Hume, in his posthumous works, argues that the powers of generation are far superior to our celebrated reason. He mentions that reason can only create a machine, like a clock or a ship, while the power of generation creates the maker of the machine. He likely observed that most of the earth has formed from organic remnants, such as the massive deposits of limestone, chalk, and marble from fish shells, and the extensive areas of clay, sandstone, ironstone, and coal from decomposed plants. All of these were initially produced through generation or organic life processes. He concludes that the world itself could have been generated rather than created, meaning it may have gradually emerged from very small beginnings, growing through the activity of its inherent principles, instead of a sudden creation by a supreme force. — What a magnificent idea of the infinite power of THE GREAT AARCHITECT! THE CAUSE OF CAUSES! PARE NOT OF PPARENTS! ENS ENTIUM!

For if we may compare infinities, it would seem to require a greater infinity of power to cause the causes of effects, than to cause the effects themselves. This idea is analogous to the improving excellence observable in every part of the creation; such as in the progressive increase of the solid or habitable parts of the earth from water; and in the progressive increase of the wisdom and happiness of its inhabitants; and is consonant to the idea of our present situation being a state of probation, which by our exertions we may improve, and are consequently responsible for our actions.

For if we can compare infinities, it seems like it takes a greater infinity of power to create the causes of effects than to create the effects themselves. This idea is similar to the observable improvement in every part of creation, like the gradual increase of solid land or habitable areas from water, and the progressive growth of wisdom and happiness among its inhabitants. It aligns with the notion that our current situation is a test, which we can improve through our efforts, and for which we are therefore responsible for our actions.

V. 1. The efficient cause of the various colours of the eggs of birds, and of the air and feathers of animals, is a subject so curious, that I shall beg to introduce it in this place. The colours of many animals seem adapted to their purposes of concealing themselves either to avoid danger, or to spring upon their prey. Thus the snake and wild cat, and leopard, are so coloured as to resemble dark leaves and their lighter interstices; birds resemble the colour of the brown ground, or the green hedges, which they frequent; and moths and butterflies are coloured like the flowers which they rob of their honey. Many instances are mentioned of this kind in Botanic Garden, p. 2. Note on Rubia.

V. 1. The reason behind the different colors of bird eggs, as well as the fur and feathers of animals, is such an interesting topic that I’d like to bring it up here. The colors of many animals appear to be perfect for helping them hide, either to escape predators or to sneak up on their prey. For example, snakes, wildcats, and leopards have colors that mimic dark leaves and their lighter gaps; birds blend in with the brown soil or the green bushes they inhabit; and moths and butterflies match the colors of the flowers they feed on. There are numerous examples of this type found in Botanic Garden, p. 2. Note on Rubia.

These colours have, however, in some instances another use, as the black diverging area from the eyes of the swan; which, as his eyes are placed less prominent than those of other birds, for the convenience of putting down his head under water, prevents the rays of light from being reflected into his eye, and thus dazzling his sight, both in air and beneath the water; which must have happened, if that surface had been white like the rest of his feathers.

These colors have, in some cases, a different purpose, like the black area around the swan's eyes. Since its eyes are positioned less prominently than those of other birds—allowing it to dip its head underwater—the black area helps prevent light from reflecting into its eyes, which could otherwise blind it both in the air and underwater. This would happen if that area were white like the rest of its feathers.

There is a still more wonderful thing concerning these colours adapted to the purpose of concealment; which is, that the eggs of birds are so coloured as to resemble the colour of the adjacent objects and their interfaces. The eggs of hedge-birds are greenish with dark spots; those of crows and magpies, which are seen from beneath through wicker nests, are white with dark spots; and those of larks and partridges are russet or brown, like their nests or situations.

There’s an even more amazing aspect about these colors used for camouflage: bird eggs are colored to blend in with their surroundings and the objects around them. Hedgebird eggs are greenish with dark spots; those of crows and magpies, which can be seen from below through woven nests, are white with dark spots; and lark and partridge eggs are rusty or brown, matching their nests or environment.

A thing still more astonishing is, that many animals in countries covered with snow become white in winter, and are said to change their colour again in the warmer months, as bears, hares, and partridges. Our domesticated animals lose their natural colours, and break into great variety, as horses, dogs, pigeons. The final cause of these colours is easily understood, as they serve some purposes of the animal, but the efficient cause would seem almost beyond conjecture.

An even more astonishing thing is that many animals in snowy regions turn white in the winter and are said to change their color again in the warmer months, like bears, rabbits, and partridges. Our domesticated animals lose their natural colors and show a wide variety, such as horses, dogs, and pigeons. The reason for these colors is easy to understand since they serve certain purposes for the animals, but the cause behind them seems almost impossible to guess.

First, the choroid coat of the eye, on which the semitransparent retina is expanded, is of different colour in different animals; in those which feed on grass it is green; from hence there would appear some connexion between the colour of the choroid coat and of that constantly painted on the retina by the green grass. Now, when the ground becomes covered with snow, it would seem, that that action of the retina, which is called whiteness, being constantly excited in the eye, may be gradually imitated by the extremities of the nerves of touch, or rete mucosum of the skin. And if it be supposed, that the action of the retina in producing the perception of any colour consists in so disposing its own fibres or surface, as to reflect those coloured rays only, and transmit the others like soap-bubbles; then that part of the retina, which gives us the perception of snow, must at that time be white; and that which gives us the perception of grass, must be green.

First, the choroid layer of the eye, which the semitransparent retina covers, varies in color among different animals; in those that eat grass, it’s green. This suggests a connection between the color of the choroid layer and the constant green of the grass on the retina. Now, when the ground is covered with snow, it appears that the action of the retina, known as whiteness, being constantly stimulated in the eye, may gradually be mimicked by the ends of the touch nerves or the rete mucosum of the skin. If we assume that the retina's action in creating the perception of any color involves arranging its fibers or surface to only reflect those colored rays while allowing others to pass through like soap bubbles, then the part of the retina responsible for our perception of snow must be white at that time, while the part responsible for our perception of grass must be green.

Then if by the laws of imitation, as explained in Section XII. 3. 3. and XXXIX. 6. the extremities of the nerves of touch in the rete mucosum be induced into similar action, the skin or feathers, or hair, may in like manner so dispose their extreme fibres, as to reflect white; for it is evident, that all these parts were originally obedient to irritative motions during their growth, and probably continue to be so; that those irritative motions are not liable in a healthy state to be succeeded by sensation; which however is no uncommon thing in their diseased state, or in their infant state, as in plica polonica, and in very young pen-feathers, which are still full of blood.

Then if by the laws of imitation, as explained in Section XII. 3. 3. and XXXIX. 6. the extremities of the nerves of touch in the rete mucosum be induced into similar action, the skin or feathers, or hair, may in like manner so dispose their extreme fibres, as to reflect white; for it is evident, that all these parts were originally obedient to irritative motions during their growth, and probably continue to be so; that those irritative motions are not liable in a healthy state to be succeeded by sensation; which however is no uncommon thing in their diseased state, or in their infant state, as in plica polonica, and in very young pen-feathers, which are still full of blood.

It was shewn in Section XV. on the Production of Ideas, that the moving organ of sense in some circumstances resembled the object which produced that motion. Hence it may be conceived, that the rete mucosum, which is the extremity of the nerves of touch, may by imitating the motions of the retina become coloured. And thus, like the fable of the camelion, all animals may possess a tendency to be coloured somewhat like the colours they most frequently inspect, and finally, that colours may be thus given to the egg-shell by the imagination of the female parent; which shell is previously a mucous membrane, indued with irritability, without which it could not circulate its fluids, and increase in its bulk. Nor is this more wonderful than that a single idea of imagination mould in an instant colour the whole surface of the body of a bright scarlet, as in the blush of shame, though by a very different process. In this intricate subject nothing but loose analogical conjectures can be had, which may however lead to future discoveries; but certain it is that both the change of the colour of animals to white in the winters of snowy countries, and the spots on birds eggs, must have some efficient cause; since the uniformity of their production shews it cannot arise from a fortuitous concurrence of circumstances; and how is this efficient cause to be detected, or explained, but from its analogy to other animal facts?

It was shewn in Section XV. on the Production of Ideas, that the moving organ of sense in some circumstances resembled the object which produced that motion. Hence it may be conceived, that the rete mucosum, which is the extremity of the nerves of touch, may by imitating the motions of the retina become coloured. And thus, like the fable of the camelion, all animals may possess a tendency to be coloured somewhat like the colours they most frequently inspect, and finally, that colours may be thus given to the egg-shell by the imagination of the female parent; which shell is previously a mucous membrane, indued with irritability, without which it could not circulate its fluids, and increase in its bulk. Nor is this more wonderful than that a single idea of imagination mould in an instant colour the whole surface of the body of a bright scarlet, as in the blush of shame, though by a very different process. In this intricate subject nothing but loose analogical conjectures can be had, which may however lead to future discoveries; but certain it is that both the change of the colour of animals to white in the winters of snowy countries, and the spots on birds eggs, must have some efficient cause; since the uniformity of their production shews it cannot arise from a fortuitous concurrence of circumstances; and how is this efficient cause to be detected, or explained, but from its analogy to other animal facts?

2. The nutriment supplied by the female parent in viviparous animals to their young progeny may be divided into three kinds, corresponding with the age of the new creature. 1. The nutriment contained in the ovum as previously prepared for the embryon in the ovary. 2. The liquor amnii prepared for the fetus in the uterus, and in which it swims; and lastly, the milk prepared in the pectoral glands for the new born-child. There is reason to conclude that variety of changes may be produced in the new animal from all these sources of nutriment, but particularly from the first of them..

2. The nutrients provided by the female parent to their young in live-bearing animals can be categorized into three types, depending on the age of the offspring. 1. The nutrients found in the egg as previously formed for the embryo in the ovary. 2. The amniotic fluid prepared for the fetus in the uterus, in which it floats; and finally, the milk produced in the mammary glands for the newborn. There is reason to believe that various changes can occur in the new animal from all these sources of nourishment, especially from the first one.

The organs of digestion and of sanguification in adults, and afterwards those of secretion, prepare or separate the particles proper for nourishment from other combinations of matter, or recombine them into new kinds of matter, proper to excite into action the filaments, which absorb or attract them by animal appetency. In this process we must attend not only to the action of the living filament which receives a nutritive particle to its bosom, but also to the kind of particle, in respect to form, or size, or colour, or hardness, which is thus previously prepared for it by digestion, sanguification, and secretion. Now as the first filament of entity cannot be furnished with the preparative organs above mentioned, the nutritive particles, which are at first to be received by it, are prepared by the mother; and deposited in the ovum ready for its reception. These nutritive particles must be supposed to differ in some respects, when thus prepared by different animals. They may differ in size, solidity, colour, and form; and yet may be sufficiently congenial to the living filament, to which they are applied, as to excite its activity by their stimulus, and its animal appetency to receive them, and to combine them with itself into organization.

The digestive and blood-related organs in adults, and later those involved in secretion, prepare or separate the particles needed for nourishment from other substances, or rearrange them into new types of matter that can stimulate the tissues which absorb or attract them due to biological hunger. In this process, we must consider not only the action of the living tissue that takes in a nutritious particle but also the type of particle in terms of shape, size, color, or hardness that has been processed for it through digestion, blood formation, and secretion. Since the initial living tissue cannot have the organs mentioned earlier, the nutritious particles it will first receive are prepared by the mother and placed in the egg ready for acceptance. These nutritious particles are likely to vary in certain ways when prepared by different animals. They may differ in size, density, color, and shape, yet still be compatible enough with the living tissue they are meant for, enabling them to stimulate its activity and encourage its instinct to absorb and integrate them into its structure.

By this first nutriment thus prepared for the embryon is not meant the liquor amnii, which is produced afterwards, nor the larger exterior parts of the white of the egg; but the fluid prepared, I suppose, in the ovary of viviparous animals, and that which immediately surrounds the cicatricula of an impregnated egg, and is visible to the eye in a boiled one.

By this first nourishment prepared for the embryo, I don’t mean the amniotic fluid that forms later, nor the larger outer parts of the egg white; instead, I'm referring to the fluid created, I believe, in the ovaries of live-bearing animals, and the fluid that directly surrounds the scar of a fertilized egg, which can be seen in a boiled egg.

Now these ultimate particles of animal matter prepared by the glands of the mother may be supposed to resemble the similar ultimate particles, which were prepared for her own nourishment; that is, to the ultimate particles of which her own organization consists. And that hence when these become combined with a new embryon, which in its early state is not furnished with stomach, or glands, to alter them; that new embryon will bear some resemblance to the mother.

Now, these fundamental particles of animal matter created by the mother’s glands can be thought of as similar to the particles that were prepared for her own nourishment; in other words, the basic particles that make up her own body. Therefore, when these particles combine with a new embryo, which in its early stages lacks a stomach or glands to modify them, that new embryo will share some resemblance to the mother.

This seems to be the origin of the compound forms of mules, which evidently partake of both parents, but principally of the male parent. In this production of chimeras the antients seem to have indulged their fancies, whence the sphinxes, griffins, dragons, centaurs, and minotaurs, which are vanished from modern credulity.

This appears to be the source of the mixed traits in mules, which clearly inherit features from both parents, but mainly from the male. In creating these mythical creatures, ancient people seem to have embraced their imaginations, leading to the sphinxes, griffins, dragons, centaurs, and minotaurs, all of which are no longer believed in today.

It would seem, that in these unnatural conjunctions, when the nutriment deposited by the female was so ill adapted to stimulate the living filament derived from the male into action, and to be received; or embraced by it, and combined with it into organization, as not to produce the organs necessary to life, as the brain, or heart, or stomach, that no mule was produced. Where all the parts necessary to life in these compound animals were formed sufficiently perfect, except the parts of generation, those animals were produced which are now called mules.

It seems that in these unnatural pairings, when the nourishment provided by the female was so poorly suited to activate the living element from the male and to be accepted by it, or to blend with it into a functioning organization, it didn’t lead to the development of essential organs for life, like the brain, heart, or stomach, resulting in no mules being born. When all the necessary parts for life in these hybrid animals were formed adequately except for the reproductive organs, the animals we now refer to as mules were produced.

The formation of the organs of sexual generation, in contradistinction to that by lateral buds, in vegetables, and in some animals, as the polypus, the tænia, and the volvox, seems the chef d'œuvre, the master-piece of nature; as appears from many flying insects, as in moths and butterflies, who seem to undergo a general change of their forms solely for the purpose of sexual reproduction, and in all other animals this organ is not complete till the maturity of the creature. Whence it happens that, in the copulation of animals of different species, the parts necessary to life are frequently completely formed; but those for the purpose of generation are defective, as requiring a nicer organization; or more exact coincidence of the particles of nutriment to the irritabilities or appetencies of the original living filament. Whereas those mules, where all the parts could be perfectly formed, may have been produced in early periods of time, and may have added to the numbers of our various species of animals, as before observed.

The development of reproductive organs, unlike that of lateral buds in plants and some animals like polyps, tapeworms, and volvox, seems to be the masterpiece of nature. This is evident in many flying insects, such as moths and butterflies, which appear to undergo significant transformations solely for reproduction. In all other animals, these reproductive organs don’t fully develop until the organism reaches maturity. This is why, in mating between different species, the necessary biological structures often develop fully, while those meant for reproduction often do not, as they require a more precise organization or a better match between nutrients and the sensitivities of the original living cell. In contrast, mules, where all the reproductive parts can form properly, may have been produced in earlier times and could have contributed to the diversity of our various animal species, as mentioned before.

As this production of mules is a constant effect from the conjunction of different species of animals, those between the horse and the female ass always resembling the horse more than the ass; and those, on the contrary, between the male ass and the mare, always resembling the ass more than the mare; it cannot be ascribed to the imagination of the male animal which cannot be supposed to operate so uniformly; but to the form of the first nutritive particles, and to their peculiar stimulus exciting the living filament to select and combine them with itself. There is a similar uniformity of effect in respect to the colour of the progeny produced between a white man, and a black woman, which, if I am well informed, is always of the mulatto kind, or a mixture of the two; which may perhaps be imputed to the peculiar form of the particles of nutriment supplied to the embryon by the mother at the early period of its existence, and their peculiar stimulus; as this effect, like that of the mule progeny above treated of, is uniform and consistent, and cannot therefore be ascribed to the imagination of either of the parents.

As the production of mules consistently results from the combination of different animal species, those resulting from the horse and female donkey tend to look more like the horse, while those from the male donkey and mare typically resemble the donkey more than the mare. This cannot be attributed to the imagination of the male animal, which doesn't operate so consistently, but instead to the nature of the initial nutritive particles and their specific stimulus that encourages the living filament to select and combine them with itself. A similar consistency can be seen in the color of offspring produced between a white man and a black woman, which, as far as I know, always results in mulatto children, or a blend of the two. This may be due to the unique structure of the nutrients supplied by the mother to the embryo during its early development and their specific stimulus; as this effect, like that of the mule offspring mentioned earlier, is uniform and reliable, and cannot therefore be attributed to the imagination of either parent.

Dr. Thunberg observes, in his Journey to the Cape of Good Hope, that there are some families, which have descended from blacks in the female line for three generations. The first generation proceeding from an European, who married a tawny slave, remains tawny, but approaches to a white complexion; but the children of the third generation, mixed with Europeans, become quite white, and are often remarkably beautiful. V. i. p. 112.

Dr. Thunberg notes in his Journey to the Cape of Good Hope that there are families that have ancestors of black descent through their mothers for three generations. The first generation, coming from a European who married a light-skinned slave, stays light-skinned but is getting closer to a white complexion. However, the children of the third generation, mixed with Europeans, become completely white and are often strikingly beautiful. V. i. p. 112.

When the embryon has produced a placenta, and furnished itself with vessels for selection of nutritious particles, and for oxygenation of them, no great change in its form or colour is likely to be produced by the particles of sustenance it now takes from the fluid, in which it is immersed; because it has now acquired organs to alter or new combine them. Hence it continues to grow, whether this fluid, in which it swims, be formed by the uterus or by any other cavity of the body, as in extra-uterine gestation; and which would seem to be produced by the stimulus of the fetus on the sides of the cavity, where it is found, as mentioned before. And thirdly, there is still less reason to expect any unnatural change to happen to the child after its birth from the difference of the milk it now takes; because it has acquired a stomach, and lungs, and glands, of sufficient power to decompose and recombine the milk; and thus to prepare from it the various kinds of nutritious particles, which the appetencies of the various fibrils or nerves may require.

Once the embryo has developed a placenta and formed blood vessels to filter nutrients and oxygen, there won't be a significant change in its shape or color from the nutrients it absorbs from the surrounding fluid. This is because it has obtained the organs necessary to modify or reorganize them. As a result, it continues to grow, whether this fluid comes from the uterus or another part of the body, as in cases of ectopic pregnancy. This appears to be stimulated by the fetus pressing against the walls of the cavity it's in, as mentioned earlier. Lastly, there's even less reason to expect any abnormal changes in the child after birth due to the difference in the milk it consumes. That's because it now has a stomach, lungs, and glands that are capable of breaking down and recombining milk, effectively preparing various nutrients that the different nerve fibers may require.

From all this reasoning I would conclude, that though the imagination of the female may be supposed to affect the embryon by producing a difference in its early nutriment; yet that no such power can affect it after it has obtained a placenta, and other organs; which may select or change the food, which is presented to it either in the liquor amnii, or in the milk. Now as the eggs in pullets, like the seeds in vegetables, are produced gradually, long before they are impregnated, it does not appear how any sudden effect of imagination of the mother at the time of impregnation can produce any considerable change in the nutriment already thus laid up for the expected or desired embryon. And that hence any changes of the embryon, except those uniform ones in the production of mules and mulattoes, more probably depend on the imagination of the male parent. At the same time it seems manifest, that those monstrous births, which consist in some deficiencies only, or some redundancies of parts, originate from the deficiency or redundance of the first nutriment prepared in the ovary, or in the part of the egg immediately surrounding the cicatricula, as described above; and which continues some time to excite the first living filament into action, after the simple animal is completed; or ceases to excite it, before the complete form is accomplished. The former of these circumstances is evinced by the eggs with double yolks, which frequently happen to our domesticated poultry, and which, I believe, are so formed before impregnation, but which would be well worth attending to, both before and after impregnation; as it is probable, something valuable on this subject might be learnt from them. The latter circumstance, or that of deficiency of original nutriment, may be deduced from reverse analogy.

From all this reasoning, I would conclude that while a woman's imagination might be thought to influence her unborn child by affecting its early nourishment, this power no longer applies once the embryo has developed a placenta and other organs that can select or change the nutrients it receives, whether from amniotic fluid or breast milk. Since the eggs in hens, like seeds in plants, are produced gradually long before fertilization, it seems unlikely that a sudden influence from the mother's imagination at the time of conception could cause any significant change in the nourishment already stored for the expected embryo. Therefore, any changes in the embryo, aside from the consistent ones seen in mules and mixed-race offspring, are probably more influenced by the imagination of the male parent. At the same time, it's clear that monstrous births—characterized by either missing or extra body parts—likely come from a lack or excess of the initial nourishment prepared in the ovary or in the portion of the egg surrounding the embryo, which stimulates the first living cell into action after the simple animal is formed, or stops stimulating it before the complete form is achieved. The first situation is demonstrated by eggs with double yolks, which often occur in domesticated birds and I believe are formed before fertilization. This phenomenon deserves further study both before and after fertilization, as it is likely that valuable insights could be gained from it. The second situation, concerning a lack of original nourishment, can be inferred from reverse reasoning.

There are, however, other kinds of monstrous births, which neither depend on deficiency of parts, or supernumerary ones; nor are owing to the conjunction of animals of different species; but which appear to be new conformations, or new dispositions of parts in respect to each other, and which, like the variation of colours and forms of our domesticated animals, and probably the sexual parts of all animals, may depend on the imagination of the male parent, which we now come to consider.

There are, however, other types of monstrous births that don't rely on missing or extra body parts, nor are they caused by the mating of different species. Instead, they seem to be new shapes or arrangements of parts in relation to one another. This is similar to the variation in colors and forms of our domesticated animals, and likely the reproductive organs of all animals, which may be influenced by the imagination of the male parent, a topic we will now explore.

VI. 1. The nice actions of the extremities of our various glands are exhibited in their various productions, which are believed to be made by the gland, and not previously to exist as such in the blood.

VI. 1. The positive functions of our different glands are shown through the various substances they produce, which are thought to be created by the gland itself and not to have existed in the blood beforehand.

Thus the glands, which constitute the liver, make bile; those of the stomach make gastric acid; those beneath the jaw, saliva; those of the ears, ear-wax; and the like. Every kind of gland must possess a peculiar irritability, and probably a sensibility, at the early state of its existence; and must be furnished with a nerve of sense, or of motion, to perceive, and to select, and to combine the particles, which compose the fluid it secretes. And this nerve of sense which perceives the different articles which compose the blood, must at least be conceived to be as fine and subtile an organ, as the optic or auditory nerve, which perceive light or sound. See Sect. XIV. 9.

Thus the glands, which constitute the liver, make bile; those of the stomach make gastric acid; those beneath the jaw, saliva; those of the ears, ear-wax; and the like. Every kind of gland must possess a peculiar irritability, and probably a sensibility, at the early state of its existence; and must be furnished with a nerve of sense, or of motion, to perceive, and to select, and to combine the particles, which compose the fluid it secretes. And this nerve of sense which perceives the different articles which compose the blood, must at least be conceived to be as fine and subtile an organ, as the optic or auditory nerve, which perceive light or sound. See Sect. XIV. 9.

But in nothing is this nice action of the extremities of the blood-vessels so wonderful, as in the production of contagious matter. A small drop of variolous contagion diffused in the blood, or perhaps only by being inserted beneath the cuticle, after a time, (as about a quarter of a lunation,) excites the extreme vessels of the skin into certain motions, which produce a similar contagious material, filling with it a thousand pustules. So that by irritation, or by sensation in consequence of irritation, or by association of motions, a material is formed by the extremities of certain cutaneous vessels, exactly similar to the stimulating material, which caused the irritation, or consequent sensation, or association.

But nothing showcases the remarkable action of the tiny blood vessels quite like the creation of contagious matter. A tiny drop of variola virus introduced into the blood, or even just placed under the skin, after a while (about a quarter of a lunar month), triggers the small vessels in the skin to perform specific movements that generate similar contagious material, filling thousands of pustules with it. So, through irritation, or sensation stemming from that irritation, or the coordination of movements, a substance is produced by the ends of certain skin vessels that is identical to the original stimulating material that caused the irritation, sensation, or movement.

Many glands of the body have their motions, and in consequence their secreted fluids, affected by pleasurable or painful ideas, since they are in many instances influenced by sensitive associations, as well as by the irritations of the particles of the passing blood. Thus the idea of meat, excited in the minds of hungry dogs, by their sense of vision, or of smell, increases the discharge of saliva, both in quantity and viscidity; as is seen in its hanging down in threads from their mouths, as they stand round a dinner-table. The sensations of pleasure, or of pain, of peculiar kinds, excite in the same manner a great discharge of tears; which appear also to be more saline at the time of their secretion, from their inflaming the eyes and eye-lids. The paleness from fear, and the blush of shame, and of joy, are other instances of the effects of painful, or pleasurable sensations, on the extremities of the arterial system.

Many glands in the body react to thoughts and feelings, causing changes in the fluids they produce. These reactions are often influenced by emotional connections and the stimulation of blood particles. For example, when hungry dogs think about meat, whether through sight or smell, it leads to increased saliva production, making it drip in threads from their mouths as they gather around a dinner table. Similarly, strong feelings of pleasure or pain can trigger a significant release of tears, which tend to be saltier when they come out, irritating the eyes and eyelids. The pale look from fear and the blush from shame or joy are other examples of how emotional experiences impact the function of the blood vessels at the body's surface.

It is probable, that the pleasurable sensation excited in the stomach by food, as well as its irritation, contributes to excite into action the gastric glands, and to produce a greater secretion of their fluids. The same probably occurs in the secretion of bile; that is, that the pleasurable sensation excited in the stomach, affects this secretion by sensitive association, as well as by irritative association.

It’s likely that the enjoyable feeling triggered in the stomach by food, along with its irritation, helps activate the gastric glands and leads to a greater release of their fluids. The same probably happens with bile secretion; that is, the enjoyable sensation in the stomach influences this secretion through both sensory and irritative connections.

And lastly it would seem, that all the glands in the body have their secreted fluids affected, in quantity and quality, by the pleasurable or painful sensations, which produce or accompany those secretions. And that the pleasurable sensations arising from these secretions may constitute the unnamed pleasure of exigence, which is contrary to what is meant by tedium vitæ, or ennui; and by which we sometimes feel ourselves happy, without being able to ascribe it to any mental cause, as after an agreeable meal, or in the beginning of intoxication.

And finally, it seems that all the glands in the body have their secreted fluids influenced, both in amount and quality, by pleasurable or painful sensations that produce or go along with those secretions. The pleasurable sensations from these secretions might represent a kind of unnamed pleasure that contrasts with what we mean by tedium vitæ, or boredom; and through which we sometimes feel happy without being able to pinpoint any mental reason for it, like after a nice meal or at the beginning of getting drunk.

Now it would appear, that no secretion or excretion of fluid is attended with so much agreeable sensation, as that of the semen; and it would thence follow, that the glands, which perform this secretion, are more likely to be much affected by their catenations with pleasurable sensations. This circumstance is certain, that much more of this fluid is produced in a given time, when the object of its exclusion is agreeable to the mind.

Now it seems that no secretion or excretion of fluid comes with as much pleasant sensation as that of semen; therefore, it follows that the glands responsible for this secretion are likely to be significantly influenced by their connections to pleasurable feelings. One thing is sure: a lot more of this fluid is produced in a certain amount of time when the reason for its release is enjoyable to the mind.

2. A forceable argument, which shews the necessity of pleasurable sensation to copulation, is, that the act cannot be performed without it; it is easily interrupted by the pain of fear or bashfulness; and no efforts of volition or of irritation can effect this process, except such as induce pleasurable ideas or sensations. See Sect. XXXIII. 1. 1.

2. A forceable argument, which shews the necessity of pleasurable sensation to copulation, is, that the act cannot be performed without it; it is easily interrupted by the pain of fear or bashfulness; and no efforts of volition or of irritation can effect this process, except such as induce pleasurable ideas or sensations. See Sect. XXXIII. 1. 1.

A curious analogical circumstance attending hermaphrodite insects, as snails and worms, still further illustrates this theory; if the snail or worm could have impregnated itself, there might have been a saving of a large male apparatus; but as this is not so ordered by nature, but each snail and worm reciprocally receives and gives impregnation, it appears, that a pleasurable excitation seems also to have been required.

A curious comparison involving hermaphrodite insects, like snails and worms, further illustrates this theory; if a snail or worm could self-fertilize, it could have saved a large male reproductive system. However, since nature doesn't work that way and each snail and worm both receives and provides fertilization, it seems that some kind of pleasurable stimulation is also necessary.

This wonderful circumstance of many insects being hermaphrodites, and at the same time not having power to impregnate themselves, is attended to by Dr. Lister, in his Exercitationes Anatom. de Limacibus, p. 145; who, amongst many other final causes, which he adduces to account for it, adds, ut tam tristibus et frigidis animalibus majori cum voluptate perficiatur venus.

This fascinating situation where many insects are hermaphrodites but cannot fertilize themselves is discussed by Dr. Lister in his Exercitationes Anatom. de Limacibus, p. 145. Among many other reasons he offers to explain this, he adds that it allows for a more enjoyable mating experience for these cold and gloomy creatures.

There is, however, another final cause, to which this circumstance may be imputed: it was observed above, that vegetable buds and bulbs, which are produced without a mother, are always exact resemblances of their parent; as appears in grafting fruit-trees, and in the flower-buds of the dioiceous plants, which are always of the same sex on the same tree; hence those hermaphrodite insects, if they could have produced young without a mother, would not have been, capable of that change or improvement, which is seen in all other animals, and in those vegetables, which are procreated by the male embryon received and nourished by the female. And it is hence probable, that if vegetables could only have been produced by buds and bulbs, and not by sexual generation, that there would not at this time have existed one thousandth part of their present number of species; which have probably been originally mule-productions; nor could any kind of improvement or change have happened to them, except by the difference of soil or climate.

There is, however, another ultimate reason that can be attributed to this situation: as mentioned earlier, plant buds and bulbs that grow without a parent are always exact replicas of their original source. This is evident in the grafting of fruit trees and in the flower buds of dioecious plants, which are always of the same sex on the same tree. Therefore, those hermaphrodite insects, if they could have produced offspring without a mother, wouldn’t have been capable of the changes or improvements that we see in other animals and in those plants that are created by the male embryo being received and nurtured by the female. It's likely that if plants could only be produced by buds and bulbs and not through sexual reproduction, there would currently be only a tiny fraction—one thousandth—of the number of species we have today, which probably originated as hybrids; nor would any significant improvement or change have occurred, except due to variations in soil or climate.

3. I conclude, that the imagination of the male at the time of copulation, or at the time of the secretion of the semen, may so affect this secretion by irritative or sensitive association, as described in No. 5. 1. of this section, as to cause the production of similarity of form and of features, with the distinction of sex; as the motions of the chissel of the turner imitate or correspond with those of the ideas of the artist. It is not here to be understood, that the first living fibre, which is to form an animal, is produced with any similarity of form to the future animal; but with propensities, or appetences, which shall produce by accretion of parts the similarity of form, feature, or sex, corresponding to the imagination of the father.

3. I conclude, that the imagination of the male at the time of copulation, or at the time of the secretion of the semen, may so affect this secretion by irritative or sensitive association, as described in No. 5. 1. of this section, as to cause the production of similarity of form and of features, with the distinction of sex; as the motions of the chissel of the turner imitate or correspond with those of the ideas of the artist. It is not here to be understood, that the first living fibre, which is to form an animal, is produced with any similarity of form to the future animal; but with propensities, or appetences, which shall produce by accretion of parts the similarity of form, feature, or sex, corresponding to the imagination of the father.

Our ideas are movements of the nerves of sense, as of the optic nerve in recollecting visible ideas, suppose of a triangular piece of ivory. The fine moving fibres of the retina act in a manner to which I give the name of white; and this action is confined to a defined part of it; to which figure I give the name of triangle. And it is a preceding pleasurable sensation existing in my mind, which occasions me to produce this particular motion of the retina, when no triangle is present. Now it is probable, that the acting fibres of the ultimate terminations of the secreting apertures of the vessels of the testes, are as fine as those of the retina; and that they are liable to be thrown into that peculiar action, which marks the sex of the secreted embryon, by sympathy with the pleasurable motions of the nerves of vision or of touch; that is, with certain ideas of imagination. From hence it would appear, that the world has long been mistaken in ascribing great power to the imagination of the female, whereas from this account of it, the real power of imagination, in the act of generation, belongs solely to the male. See Sect. XII. 3. 3.

Our ideas are movements of the nerves of sense, as of the optic nerve in recollecting visible ideas, suppose of a triangular piece of ivory. The fine moving fibres of the retina act in a manner to which I give the name of white; and this action is confined to a defined part of it; to which figure I give the name of triangle. And it is a preceding pleasurable sensation existing in my mind, which occasions me to produce this particular motion of the retina, when no triangle is present. Now it is probable, that the acting fibres of the ultimate terminations of the secreting apertures of the vessels of the testes, are as fine as those of the retina; and that they are liable to be thrown into that peculiar action, which marks the sex of the secreted embryon, by sympathy with the pleasurable motions of the nerves of vision or of touch; that is, with certain ideas of imagination. From hence it would appear, that the world has long been mistaken in ascribing great power to the imagination of the female, whereas from this account of it, the real power of imagination, in the act of generation, belongs solely to the male. See Sect. XII. 3. 3.

It may be objected to this theory, that a man may be supposed to have in his mind, the idea of the form and features of the female, rather than his own, and therefore there should be a greater number of female births. On the contrary, the general idea of our own form occurs to every one almost perpetually, and is termed consciousness of our existence, and thus may effect, that the number of males surpasses that of females. See Sect. XV. 3. 4. and XVIII. 13. And what further confirms this idea is, that the male children most frequently resemble the father in form, or feature, as well as in sex; and the female most frequently resemble the mother, in feature, and form, as well as in sex.

It may be objected to this theory, that a man may be supposed to have in his mind, the idea of the form and features of the female, rather than his own, and therefore there should be a greater number of female births. On the contrary, the general idea of our own form occurs to every one almost perpetually, and is termed consciousness of our existence, and thus may effect, that the number of males surpasses that of females. See Sect. XV. 3. 4. and XVIII. 13. And what further confirms this idea is, that the male children most frequently resemble the father in form, or feature, as well as in sex; and the female most frequently resemble the mother, in feature, and form, as well as in sex.

It may again be objected, if a female child sometimes resembles the father, and a male child the mother, the ideas of the father, at the time of procreation, must suddenly change from himself to the mother, at the very instant, when the embryon is secreted or formed. This difficulty ceases when we consider, that it is as easy to form an idea of feminine features with male organs of reproduction, or of male features with female ones, as the contrary; as we conceive the idea of a sphinx or mermaid as easily and as distinctly as of a woman. Add to this, that at the time of procreation the idea of the male organs, and of the female features, are often both excited at the same time, by contact, or by vision.

It might be argued again that if a girl sometimes resembles her father and a boy resembles his mother, then the father's thoughts at the moment of conception must suddenly shift from his own characteristics to those of the mother, precisely when the embryo is created. This concern disappears when we realize that it’s just as easy to imagine feminine traits with male reproductive organs, or male traits with female ones, as it is the other way around; we can think of a sphinx or a mermaid as easily and clearly as we do of a woman. Furthermore, at the time of conception, the ideas of male reproductive organs and female features are often both stimulated at the same moment, through touch or sight.

I ask, in my turn, is the sex of the embryon produced by accident? Certainly whatever is produced has a cause; but when this cause is too minute for our comprehension, the effect is said in common language to happen by chance, as in throwing a certain number on dice. Now what cause can occasionally produce the male or female character of the embryon, but the peculiar actions of those glands, which form the embryon? And what can influence or govern these actions of the gland, but its associations or catenations with other sensitive motions? Nor is this more extraordinary, than that the catenations of irritative motions with the apparent vibrations of objects at sea should produce sickness of the stomach; or that a nauseous story should occasion vomiting.

I ask, in my turn, is the sex of the embryo produced by accident? Certainly, whatever is produced has a cause; but when this cause is too small for us to understand, we often say it happens by chance, like when rolling a certain number on dice. Now, what cause could occasionally determine whether the embryo is male or female, if not the specific actions of the glands that form the embryo? And what could influence or control these gland actions, except their connections with other sensitive movements? This isn't more unusual than how the connections of irritating movements with the visible vibrations of objects at sea can cause nausea, or how a disgusting story can lead to vomiting.

4. An argument, which evinces the effect of imagination on the first rudiment of the embryon, may be deduced from the production of some peculiar monsters. Such, for instance, as those which have two heads joined to one body, and those which have two bodies joined to one head; of which frequent examples occur amongst our domesticated quadrupeds, and poultry. It is absurd to suppose, that such forms could exist in primordial germs, as explained in No. IV. 4. of this section. Nor is it possible, that such deformities could be produced by the growth of two embryons, or living filaments; which should afterwards adhere together; as the head and tail part of different polypi are said to do (Blumenbach on Generation, Cadel, London); since in that case one embryon, or living filament, must have begun to form one part first, and the other another part first. But such monstrous conformations become less difficult to comprehend, when they are considered as an effect of the imagination, as before explained, on the living filament at the time of its secretion; and that such duplicature of limbs were produced by accretion of new parts, in consequence of propensities, or animal appetencies thus acquired from the male parent.

4. An argument, which evinces the effect of imagination on the first rudiment of the embryon, may be deduced from the production of some peculiar monsters. Such, for instance, as those which have two heads joined to one body, and those which have two bodies joined to one head; of which frequent examples occur amongst our domesticated quadrupeds, and poultry. It is absurd to suppose, that such forms could exist in primordial germs, as explained in No. IV. 4. of this section. Nor is it possible, that such deformities could be produced by the growth of two embryons, or living filaments; which should afterwards adhere together; as the head and tail part of different polypi are said to do (Blumenbach on Generation, Cadel, London); since in that case one embryon, or living filament, must have begun to form one part first, and the other another part first. But such monstrous conformations become less difficult to comprehend, when they are considered as an effect of the imagination, as before explained, on the living filament at the time of its secretion; and that such duplicature of limbs were produced by accretion of new parts, in consequence of propensities, or animal appetencies thus acquired from the male parent.

For instance, I can conceive, if a turkey-cock should behold a rabbit, or a frog, at the time of procreation, that it might happen, that a forcible or even a pleasurable idea of the form of a quadruped might so occupy his imagination, as to cause a tendency in the nascent filament to resemble such a form, by the apposition of a duplicature of limbs. Experiments on the production of mules and monsters would be worthy the attention of a Spallanzani, and might throw much light upon this subject, which at present must be explained by conjectural analogies.

For example, I can imagine that if a male turkey saw a rabbit or a frog during mating, it might lead to a strong or even enjoyable image of a four-legged animal taking over its mind, causing its offspring to have similar features, like extra limbs. Experiments on creating mules and strange creatures would be worthy of attention from someone like Spallanzani and could provide valuable insights into this topic, which currently has to be explained through speculative comparisons.

The wonderful effect of imagination, both in the male and female parent, is shewn in the production of a kind of milk in the crops both of the male and female pigeons after the birth of their young, as observed by Mr. Hunter, and mentioned before. To this should be added, that there are some instances of men having had milk secreted in their breasts, and who have given suck to children, as recorded by Mr. Buffon. This effect of imagination, of both the male and female parent, seems to have been attended to in very early times; Jacob is said not only to have placed rods of trees, in part stripped of their bark, so as to appear spotted, but also to have placed spotted lambs before the flocks, at the time of their copulation. Genesis, chap. xxx. verse 40.

The amazing power of imagination, in both male and female parents, is demonstrated in the production of a type of milk in the crops of both male and female pigeons after their young are born, as noted by Mr. Hunter and mentioned earlier. Additionally, there are some cases of men having milk produced in their breasts, allowing them to nurse children, as recorded by Mr. Buffon. This imaginative effect in both male and female parents seems to have been recognized since ancient times; Jacob is said to have not only placed rods from trees that were partially stripped of their bark to look spotted but also to have set spotted lambs before the flocks during mating. Genesis, chap. xxx. verse 40.

5. In respect to the imagination of the mother, it is difficult to comprehend, how this can produce any alteration in the fetus, except by affecting the nutriment laid up for its first reception, as described in No. V. 2. of this section, or by affecting the nourishment or oxygenation with which she supplies it afterwards. Perpetual anxiety may probably affect the secretion of the liquor amnii into the uterus, as it enfeebles the whole system; and sudden fear is a frequent cause of miscarriage; for fear, contrary to joy, decreases for a time the action of the extremities of the arterial system; hence sudden paleness succeeds, and a shrinking or contraction of the vessels of the skin, and other membranes. By this circumstance, I imagine, the terminations of the placental vessels are detached from their adhesions, or insertions, into the membrane of the uterus; and the death of the child succeeds, and consequent miscarriage.

5. In respect to the imagination of the mother, it is difficult to comprehend, how this can produce any alteration in the fetus, except by affecting the nutriment laid up for its first reception, as described in No. V. 2. of this section, or by affecting the nourishment or oxygenation with which she supplies it afterwards. Perpetual anxiety may probably affect the secretion of the liquor amnii into the uterus, as it enfeebles the whole system; and sudden fear is a frequent cause of miscarriage; for fear, contrary to joy, decreases for a time the action of the extremities of the arterial system; hence sudden paleness succeeds, and a shrinking or contraction of the vessels of the skin, and other membranes. By this circumstance, I imagine, the terminations of the placental vessels are detached from their adhesions, or insertions, into the membrane of the uterus; and the death of the child succeeds, and consequent miscarriage.

Of this I recollect a remarkable instance, which could be ascribed to no other cause, and which I shall therefore relate in few words. A healthy young woman, about twenty years of age, had been about five months pregnant, and going down into her cellar to draw some beer, was frighted by a servant boy starting up from behind the barrel, where he had concealed himself with design to alarm the maid-servant, for whom he mistook his mistress. She came with difficulty up stairs, began to flood immediately, and miscarried in a few hours. She has since borne several children, nor ever had any tendency to miscarry of any of them.

Of this, I remember a striking example, which can be attributed to no other cause, so I will share it briefly. A healthy young woman, around twenty years old, was about five months pregnant. While she was going down to her cellar to get some beer, she was startled by a servant boy who jumped up from behind the barrel, having hidden there to scare the maid, thinking she was his mistress. She struggled to come back upstairs, started bleeding immediately, and miscarried a few hours later. Since then, she has had several children and has never had any issues with miscarrying with any of them.

6. In respect to the power of the imagination of the male over the form, colour, and sex of the progeny, the following instances have fallen under my observation, and may perhaps be found not very unfrequent, if they were more attended to. I am acquainted with a gentleman, who has one child with dark hair and eyes, though his lady and himself have light hair and eyes; and their other four children are like their parents. On observing this dissimilarity of one child to the others he assured me, that he believed it was his own imagination, that produced the difference; and related to me the following story. He said, that when his lady lay in of her third child, he became attached to a daughter of one of his inferior tenants, and offered her a bribe for her favours in vain; and afterwards a greater bribe, and was equally unsuccessful; that the form of this girl dwelt much in his mind for some weeks, and that the next child, which was the dark-ey'd young lady above mentioned, was exceedingly like, in both features and colour, to the young woman who refused his addresses.

6. Regarding the influence of a man's imagination on the appearance, color, and gender of his offspring, I've noticed some cases that might not be as rare as one would think, if they were given more attention. I know a man who has one child with dark hair and eyes, while both he and his wife have light hair and eyes; their other four children all resemble their parents. Noticing this difference in one child compared to the others, he told me he believed it was his own imagination that caused the variation, and he shared the following story. He said that when his wife was pregnant with their third child, he became infatuated with a daughter of one of his tenants and tried bribing her for her affection unsuccessfully; he offered a larger bribe later and had no better luck. He mentioned that the image of that girl occupied his thoughts for several weeks, and that the next child, who was the dark-eyed young lady mentioned earlier, bore a striking resemblance in both features and color to the young woman who had turned down his advances.

To this instance I must add, that I have known two families, in which, on account of an intailed estate in expectation, a male heir was most eagerly desired by the father; and on the contrary, girls were produced to the seventh in one, and to the ninth in another; and then they had each of them a son. I conclude, that the great desire of a male heir by the father produced rather a disagreeable than an agreeable sensation; and that his ideas dwelt more on the fear of generating a female, than on the pleasurable sensations or ideas of his own male form or organs at the time of copulation, or of the secretion of the semen; and that hence the idea of the female character was more present to his mind than that of the male one; till at length in despair of generating a male these ideas ceased, and those of the male character presided at the genial hour.

To this situation, I should mention that I've known two families where a male heir was desperately wanted by the father due to the expectation of an inherited estate. However, in one family, they had daughters up to the seventh child, and in another, up to the ninth, before finally having a son. I conclude that the father's strong desire for a male heir led to more frustration than satisfaction; his thoughts were more focused on the fear of having a daughter rather than the enjoyment or thoughts related to the male body during intimacy or the ejaculation process. As a result, the idea of having a daughter was more prominent in his mind than that of having a son, until eventually, when he lost hope of having a boy, thoughts of a son took over at the moment of conception.

7. Hence I conclude, that the act of generation cannot exist without being accompanied with ideas, and that a man must have at that time either a general idea of his own male form, or of the form of his male organs; or an idea of the female form, or of her organs; and that this marks the sex, and the peculiar resemblances of the child to either parent. From whence it would appear, that the phalli, which were hung round the necks of the Roman ladies, or worn in their hair, might have effect in producing a greater proportion of male children; and that the calipædia, or art of begetting beautiful children, and of procreating either males or females, may be taught by affecting the imagination of the male-parent; that is, by the fine extremities of the seminal glands, imitating the actions of the organs of sense either of sight or touch. But the manner of accomplishing this cannot be unfolded with sufficient delicacy for the public eye; but may be worth the attention of those, who are seriously interested in the procreation of a male or female child.

7. Therefore, I conclude that the process of creation can't happen without being accompanied by ideas, and that a man must either have a general idea of his own male form, or the form of his male organs; or an idea of the female form, or her organs; and that this defines the sex and the distinct similarities of the child to either parent. It seems that the phalli worn around the necks of Roman women, or styled in their hair, may have influenced a higher proportion of male children being born; and that calipædia, or the art of producing beautiful children and choosing between males or females, could be taught by stimulating the imagination of the male parent; that is, through the fine details of the seminal glands, mimicking the functions of the senses of sight or touch. However, the method of achieving this can't be explained in a way that's delicate enough for public exposure; but it might be worth the attention of those who are genuinely interested in having a male or female child.

Recapitulation.

Summary.

VII. 1. A certain quantity of nutritive particles are produced by the female parent before impregnation, which require no further digestion, secretion, or oxygenation. Such are seen in the unimpregnated eggs of birds, and in the unimpregnated seed-vessels of vegetables.

VII. 1. A certain amount of nourishing particles is created by the female parent before fertilization, which don't need any additional digestion, secretion, or oxygenation. This can be observed in the unfertilized eggs of birds and in the unfertilized seed pods of plants.

2. A living filament is produced by the male, which being inserted amidst these first nutritive particles, is stimulated into action by them; and in consequence of this action, some of the nutritive particles are embraced, and added to the original living filament; in the same manner as common nutrition is performed in the adult animal.

2. A living thread is created by the male, which, when inserted among these initial nutrient particles, is activated by them; and as a result of this activation, some of the nutrient particles are taken in and added to the original living thread, similar to how regular nutrition occurs in an adult animal.

3. Then this new organization, or additional part, becomes stimulated by the nutritive particles in its vicinity, and sensation is now superadded to irritation; and other particles are in consequence embraced, and added to the living filament; as is seen in the new granulations of flesh in ulcers.

3. Then this new organization, or additional part, gets energized by the nutrients around it, and sensation is added to irritation; as a result, other particles are incorporated and added to the living thread, just like in the new growths of tissue in wounds.

By the power of association, or by irritation, the parts already produced continue their motions, and new ones are added by sensation, as above mentioned; and lastly by volition, which last sensorial power is proved to exist in the fetus in its maturer age, because it has evidently periods of activity and of sleeping; which last is another word for a temporary suspension of volition.

By the power of connection, or through irritation, the already produced parts keep moving, and new ones are added through sensation, as mentioned earlier; finally, through willpower, which has been shown to exist in the fetus as it matures, since it clearly has periods of activity and sleep; the latter being another way of saying a temporary pause in willpower.

The original living filament may be conceived to possess a power of repulsing the particles applied to certain parts of it, as well as of embracing others, which stimulate other parts of it; as these powers exist in different parts of the mature animal; thus the mouth of every gland embraces the particles or fluid, which suits its appetency; and its excretory duct repulses those particles, which are disagreeable to it.

The original living filament is thought to have the ability to repel certain particles while attracting others that stimulate different areas of it. These abilities are found in various parts of a mature animal. For example, the mouth of each gland attracts the particles or fluid that it needs, while its excretory duct repels those particles that it finds unpleasant.

4. Thus the outline or miniature of the new animal is produced gradually, but in no great length of time; because the original nutritive particles require no previous preparation by digestion, secretion, and oxygenation: but require simply the selection and apposition, which is performed by the living filament. Mr. Blumenbach says, that he possesses a human fetus of only five weeks old, which is the size of a common bee, and has all the features of the face, every finger, and every toe, complete; and in which the organs of generation are distinctly seen. P. 76. In another fetus, whose head was not larger than a pea, the whole of the basis of the skull with all its depressions, apertures, and processes, were marked in the most sharp and distinct manner, though without any ossification. Ib.

4. So, the outline or tiny version of the new animal develops gradually, but it doesn’t take a long time; because the original nutrient particles don’t need prior processing through digestion, secretion, and oxygenation: they just need to be selected and positioned, which is done by the living filament. Mr. Blumenbach mentions that he has a human fetus only five weeks old, which is the size of a common bee, and already has all the facial features, every finger, and every toe fully formed; and the reproductive organs are clearly visible. P. 76. In another fetus, with a head no bigger than a pea, the entire base of the skull with all its indentations, openings, and structures was sharply and clearly defined, even though it wasn't yet ossified. Ib.

5. In some cases by the nutriment originally deposited by the mother the filament acquires parts not exactly similar to those of the father, as in the production of mules and mulattoes. In other cases, the deficiency of this original nutriment causes deficiencies of the extreme parts of the fetus, which are last formed, as the fingers, toes, lips. In other cases, a duplicature of limbs are caused by the superabundance of this original nutritive fluid, as in the double yolks of eggs, and the chickens from them with four legs and four wings. But the production of other monsters, as those with two heads, or with parts placed in wrong situations, seems to arise from the imagination of the father being in some manner imitated by the extreme vessels of the seminal glands; as the colours of the spots on eggs, and the change of the colour of the hair and feathers of animals by domestication, may be caused in the same manner by the imagination of the mother.

5. In some instances, the nutrients initially provided by the mother can lead to offspring that don’t look exactly like the father, such as in the case of mules and mulattoes. In other cases, if there’s a lack of this original nutrition, it can result in underdeveloped parts of the fetus, like fingers, toes, and lips, since those are formed last. Sometimes, an excess of this original nourishing fluid can cause duplication of limbs, similar to double-yolked eggs that produce chicks with four legs and four wings. However, the development of more unusual creatures, like those with two heads or limbs in the wrong places, seems to happen when the father's imagination somehow influences the outer vessels of the seminal glands; similarly, the variations in the colors of egg spots and the changes in the color of animals' hair and feathers from domestication may be influenced by the mother's imagination.

6. The living filament is a part of the father, and has therefore certain propensities, or appetencies, which belong to him; which may have been gradually acquired during a million of generations, even from the infancy of the habitable earth; and which now possesses such properties, as would render, by the apposition of nutritious particles, the new fetus exactly similar to the father; as occurs in the buds and bulbs of vegetables, and in the polypus, and tænia or tape-worm. But as the first nutriment is supplied by the mother, and therefore resembles such nutritive particles, as have been used for her own nutriment or growth, the progeny takes in part of the likeness of the mother.

6. The living filament is a part of the father and has certain tendencies or appetites that come from him; these may have developed gradually over a million generations, dating back to the early days of the habitable earth. This filament now has properties that would cause the new fetus, with the addition of nutritious particles, to be exactly like the father, similar to how buds and bulbs of plants, as well as polyps and tape-worms, function. However, since the first nourishment comes from the mother, which resembles the nutrients she used for her own nourishment or growth, the offspring also takes on some of the mother's likeness.

Other similarity of the excitability, or of the form of the male parent, such as the broad or narrow shoulders, or such as constitute certain hereditary diseases, as scrophula, epilepsy, insanity, have their origin produced in one or perhaps two generations; as in the progeny of those who drink much vinous spirits; and those hereditary propensities cease again, as I have observed, if one or two sober generations succeed; otherwise the family becomes extinct.

Other similarities in excitability or the appearance of the male parent, like broad or narrow shoulders, or certain hereditary diseases like scrofula, epilepsy, and mental illness, often originate in one or maybe two generations. This can happen in the offspring of people who consume a lot of alcohol. I've noticed that these hereditary traits can disappear if one or two sober generations follow; otherwise, the family line dies out.

This living filament from the father is also liable to have its propensities, or appetencies, altered at the time of its production by the imagination of the male parent; the extremities of the seminal glands imitating the motions of the organs of sense; and thus the sex of the embryon is produced; which may be thus made a male or a female by affecting the imagination of the father at the time of impregnation. See Sect. XXXIX. 6. 3. and 7.

This living filament from the father is also liable to have its propensities, or appetencies, altered at the time of its production by the imagination of the male parent; the extremities of the seminal glands imitating the motions of the organs of sense; and thus the sex of the embryon is produced; which may be thus made a male or a female by affecting the imagination of the father at the time of impregnation. See Sect. XXXIX. 6. 3. and 7.

7. After the fetus is thus completely formed together with its umbilical vessels and placenta, it is now supplied with a different kind of food, as appears by the difference of consistency of the different parts of the white of the egg, and of the liquor amnii, for it has now acquired organs for digestion or secretion, and for oxygenation, though they are as yet feeble; which can in some degree change, as well as select, the nutritive particles, which are now presented to it. But may yet be affected by the deficiency of the quantity of nutrition supplied by the mother, or by the degree of oxygenation supplied to its placenta by the maternal blood.

7. After the fetus is fully formed along with its umbilical vessels and placenta, it begins receiving a different type of nourishment, as indicated by the varying consistency of the different parts of the egg white and the amniotic fluid. At this stage, it has developed organs for digestion, secretion, and oxygenation, although these organs are still quite weak; they are capable of somewhat altering and selecting the nutrients that are now available to it. However, the fetus can still be affected by the amount of nutrition provided by the mother and the level of oxygenation delivered to its placenta through the maternal blood.

The augmentation of the complete fetus by additional particles of nutriment is not accomplished by distention only, but by apposition to every part both external and internal; each of which acquires by animal appetencies the new addition of the particles which it wants. And hence the enlarged parts are kept similar to their prototypes, and may be said to be extended; but their extension must be conceived only as a necessary consequence of the enlargement of all their parts by apposition of new particles.

The growth of the entire fetus by additional nutrients isn't just due to stretching, but also by the addition of particles to every part, both external and internal. Each part gathers the necessary new particles through biological cravings. Therefore, the larger parts remain similar to their original forms and can be considered extended; however, this extension should be understood as a natural result of all parts growing by the addition of new particles.

Hence the new apposition of parts is not produced by capillary attraction, because the whole is extended; whereas capillary attraction would rather tend to bring the sides of flexible tubes together, and not to distend them. Nor is it produced by chemical affinities, for then a solution of continuity would succeed, as when sugar is dissolved in water; but it is produced by an animal process, which is the consequence of irritation, or sensation; and which may be termed animal appetency.

Hence the new arrangement of parts is not caused by capillary attraction, because the whole is spread out; while capillary attraction would actually pull the sides of flexible tubes together, not stretch them apart. It’s also not caused by chemical affinities, since that would lead to a break in continuity, like when sugar dissolves in water; rather, it’s produced by a biological process that results from irritation or sensation, which can be referred to as animal appetency.

This is further evinced from experiments, which have been instituted to shew, that a living muscle of an animal body requires greater force to break it, than a similar muscle of a dead body. Which evinces, that besides the attraction of cohesion, which all matter possesses, and besides the chemical attractions of affinities, which hold many bodies together, there is an animal adhesion, which adds vigour to these common laws of the inanimate world.

This is further demonstrated by experiments that have been conducted to show that a living muscle in an animal body needs more force to break it than a similar muscle in a dead body. This indicates that, in addition to the attraction of cohesion that all matter has and the chemical attractions of affinities that bind many substances together, there is an animal adhesion that adds strength to these basic laws of the inanimate world.

8. At the nativity of the child it deposits the placenta or gills, and by expanding its lungs acquires more plentiful oxygenation from the currents of air, which it must now continue perpetually to respire to the end of its life; as it now quits the liquid element, in which it was produced, and like the tadpole, when it changes into a frog, becomes an aerial animal.

8. At the birth of the child, it sheds the placenta or gills, and by expanding its lungs, it takes in more oxygen from the air currents, which it must now continuously breathe for the rest of its life; as it leaves the liquid environment where it was formed and, like a tadpole transforming into a frog, becomes a creature of the air.

9. As the habitable parts of the earth have been, and continue to be, perpetually increasing by the production of sea-shells and corallines, and by the recrements of other animals, and vegetables; so from the beginning of the existence of this terraqueous globe, the animals, which inhabit it, have constantly improved, and are still in a state of progressive improvement.

9. As the livable areas of the Earth have been, and continue to be, constantly expanding due to the accumulation of seashells, coral, and the remains of other animals and plants; so from the very start of this world’s existence, the animals that live here have continually evolved and are still undergoing constant improvement.

This idea of the gradual generation of all things seems to have been as familiar to the ancient philosophers as to the modern ones; and to have given rise to the beautiful hieroglyphic figure of the προτον ωον, or first great egg, produced by NIGHT, that is, whose origin is involved in obscurity, and animated by ερος, that is, by DIVINE LOVE; from whence proceeded all things which exist.

This idea of the gradual creation of everything appears to have been just as familiar to ancient philosophers as it is to modern ones; it inspired the beautiful hieroglyphic representation of the προτον ωον, or first great egg, produced by NIGHT, which means its origin is shrouded in mystery, and brought to life by ερος, or DDIVINE LOVE; from which everything that exists emerged.

Conclusion.

Conclusion.

VIII. 1. Cause and effect may be considered as the progression, or successive motions, of the parts of the great system of Nature. The state of things at this moment is the effect of the state of things, which existed in the preceding moment; and the cause of the state of things, which shall exist in the next moment.

VIII. 1. Cause and effect can be seen as the flow or ongoing movements of the components in the vast system of Nature. The condition of things right now is the result of what was happening just before this moment, and it influences what will happen in the next moment.

These causes and effects may be more easily comprehended, if motion be considered as a change of the figure of a group of bodies, as proposed in Sect. XIV. 2. 2. inasmuch as our ideas of visible or tangible objects are more distinct, than our abstracted ideas of their motions. Now the change of the configuration of the system of nature at this moment must be an effect of the preceding configuration, for a change of configuration cannot exist without a previous configuration; and the proximate cause of every effect must immediately precede that effect. For example, a moving ivory ball could not proceed onwards, unless it had previously began to proceed; or unless an impulse had been previously given it; which previous motion or impulse constitutes a part of the last situation of things.

These causes and effects may be more easily comprehended, if motion be considered as a change of the figure of a group of bodies, as proposed in Sect. XIV. 2. 2. inasmuch as our ideas of visible or tangible objects are more distinct, than our abstracted ideas of their motions. Now the change of the configuration of the system of nature at this moment must be an effect of the preceding configuration, for a change of configuration cannot exist without a previous configuration; and the proximate cause of every effect must immediately precede that effect. For example, a moving ivory ball could not proceed onwards, unless it had previously began to proceed; or unless an impulse had been previously given it; which previous motion or impulse constitutes a part of the last situation of things.

As the effects produced in this moment of time become causes in the next, we may consider the progressive motions of objects as a chain of causes only; whose first link proceeded from the great Creator, and which have existed from the beginning of the created universe, and are perpetually proceeding.

As the effects created in this moment become the causes in the next, we can view the movements of objects as a chain of causes only; the first link originated from the great Creator and has existed since the beginning of the created universe, continually unfolding.

2. These causes may be conveniently divided into two kinds, efficient and inert causes, according with the two kinds of entity supposed to exist in the natural world, which may be termed matter and spirit, as proposed in Sect. I. and further treated of in Sect. XIV. The efficient causes of motion, or new configuration, consist either of the principle of general gravitation, which actuates the sun and planets; or of the principle of particular gravitation, as in electricity, magnetism, heat; or of the principle of chemical affinity, as in combustion, fermentation, combination; or of the principle of organic life, as in the contraction of vegetable and animal fibres. The inert causes of motion, or new configuration, consist of the parts of matter, which are introduced within the spheres of activity of the principles above described. Thus, when an apple falls on the ground, the principle of gravitation is the efficient cause, and the matter of the apple the inert cause. If a bar of iron be approximated to a magnet, it may be termed the inert cause of the motion, which brings these two bodies into contact; while the magnetic principle may be termed the efficient cause. In the same manner the fibres, which constitute the retina, may be called the inert cause of the motions of that organ in vision, while the sensorial power may be termed the efficient cause.

2. These causes may be conveniently divided into two kinds, efficient and inert causes, according with the two kinds of entity supposed to exist in the natural world, which may be termed matter and spirit, as proposed in Sect. I. and further treated of in Sect. XIV. The efficient causes of motion, or new configuration, consist either of the principle of general gravitation, which actuates the sun and planets; or of the principle of particular gravitation, as in electricity, magnetism, heat; or of the principle of chemical affinity, as in combustion, fermentation, combination; or of the principle of organic life, as in the contraction of vegetable and animal fibres. The inert causes of motion, or new configuration, consist of the parts of matter, which are introduced within the spheres of activity of the principles above described. Thus, when an apple falls on the ground, the principle of gravitation is the efficient cause, and the matter of the apple the inert cause. If a bar of iron be approximated to a magnet, it may be termed the inert cause of the motion, which brings these two bodies into contact; while the magnetic principle may be termed the efficient cause. In the same manner the fibres, which constitute the retina, may be called the inert cause of the motions of that organ in vision, while the sensorial power may be termed the efficient cause.

3. Another more common distribution of the perpetual chain of causes and effects, which constitute the motions, or changing configurations, of the natural world, is into active and passive. Thus, if a ball in motion impinges against another ball at rest, and communicates its motion to it, the former ball is said to act, and the latter to be acted upon. In this sense of the words a magnet is said to attract iron; and the prick of a spur to stimulate a horse into exertion; so that in this view of the works of nature all things may be said either simply to exist, or to exist as causes, or to exist as effects; that is, to exist either in an active or passive state.

3. Another common way to look at the endless chain of causes and effects that drive the movements and changing forms of the natural world is by categorizing them as active and passive. For example, when a moving ball hits a stationary ball and transfers its motion, the first ball is considered to be acting, while the second ball is seen as being acted upon. In this context, we say that a magnet attracts iron, and that a spur motivates a horse to move; thus, from this perspective on nature's mechanisms, everything can be described as simply existing, existing as causes, or existing as effects—meaning they exist either in an active or passive state.

This distribution of objects, and their motions, or changes of position, has been found so convenient for the purposes of common life, that on this foundation rests the whole construction or theory of language. The names of the things themselves are termed by grammarians Nouns, and their modes of existence are termed Verbs. The nouns are divided into substantives, which denote the principal things spoken of; and into adjectives, which denote some circumstances, or less kinds of things, belonging to the former. The verbs are divided into three kinds, such as denote the existence of things simply, as, to be; or their existence in an active state, as, to eat; or their existence in a passive state, as, to be eaten. Whence it appears, that all languages consist only of nouns and verbs, with their abbreviations for the greater expedition of communicating our thoughts; as explained in the ingenious work of Mr. Horne Tooke, who has unfolded by a single flash of light the whole theory of language, which had so long lain buried beneath the learned lumber of the schools. Diversions of Purley. Johnson. London.

This distribution of objects and their movements or changes in position has proven to be really useful in everyday life, and the entire structure or theory of language is built on this foundation. The names of these objects are called Nouns by grammarians, and their states of being are called Verbs. Nouns are divided into two categories: substantives, which refer to the main things being discussed; and adjectives, which describe certain aspects or lesser types of things related to the substantives. Verbs are classified into three types: those that simply indicate the existence of things, like “to be”; those that show an active state of existence, like “to eat”; and those that indicate a passive state of existence, like “to be eaten.” Therefore, it appears that all languages consist solely of nouns and verbs, along with their abbreviated forms for quicker communication of our thoughts, as explained in the clever work of Mr. Horne Tooke, who illuminated the entire theory of language that had long been hidden under the scholarly clutter of educational institutions. Diversions of Purley. Johnson. London.

4. A third division of causes has been into proximate and remote; these have been much spoken of by the writers on medical subjects, but without sufficient precision. If to proximate and remote causes we add proximate and remote effects, we shall include four links of the perpetual chain of causation; which will be more convenient for the discussion of many philosophical subjects.

4. Another way to categorize causes is by dividing them into immediate and distant causes; this distinction has been widely discussed by medical writers, although not always clearly. If we consider immediate and distant effects alongside immediate and distant causes, we will encompass four connections in the ongoing chain of causation, which will be more helpful for discussing various philosophical topics.

Thus if a particle of chyle be applied to the mouth of a lacteal vessel, it may be termed the remote cause of the motions of the fibres, which compose the mouth of that lacteal vessel; the sensorial power is the proximate cause; the contraction of the fibres of the mouth of the vessel is the proximate effect; and their embracing the particle of chyle is the remote effect; and these four links of causation constitute absorption.

Thus, if a particle of chyle is placed at the opening of a lacteal vessel, it can be called the distant cause of the movement of the fibers that make up the opening of that lacteal vessel; the sensory power is the immediate cause; the contraction of the fibers at the opening of the vessel is the immediate effect; and their surrounding the particle of chyle is the distant effect; these four links of causation make up absorption.

Thus when we attend to the rising sun, first the yellow rays of light stimulate the sensorial power residing in the extremities of the optic nerve, this is the remote cause. 2. The sensorial power is excited into a state of activity, this is the proximate cause. 3. The fibrous extremities of the optic nerve are contracted, this is the proximate effect. 4. A pleasurable or painful sensation is produced in consequence of the contraction of these fibres of the optic nerve, this is the remote effect; and these four links of the chain of causation constitute the sensitive idea, or what is commonly termed the sensation of the rising sun.

Thus, when we notice the rising sun, first the yellow rays of light stimulate the sensory power located in the ends of the optic nerve; this is the remote cause. 2. The sensory power becomes active; this is the proximate cause. 3. The fibrous ends of the optic nerve contract; this is the proximate effect. 4. A pleasurable or painful sensation arises from the contraction of these nerve fibers; this is the remote effect. These four links in the chain of causation make up the sensitive idea, or what we usually call the sensation of the rising sun.

5. Other causes have been announced by medical writers under the names of causa procatarctica, and causa proegumina, and causa sine quâ non. All which are links more or less distant of the chain of remote causes.

5. Other causes have been discussed by medical writers with terms like causa procatarctica, causa proegumina, and causa sine quâ non. These are all more or less distant links in the chain of remote causes.

To these must be added the final cause, so called by many authors, which means the motive, for the accomplishment of which the preceding chain of causes was put into action. The idea of a final cause, therefore, includes that of a rational mind, which employs means to effect its purposes; thus the desire of preserving himself from the pain of cold, which he has frequently experienced, induces the savage to construct his hut; the fixing stakes into the ground for walls, branches of trees for rafters, and turf for a cover, are a series of successive voluntary exertions; which are so many means to produce a certain effect. This effect of preserving himself from cold, is termed the final cause; the construction of the hut is the remote effect; the action of the muscular fibres of the man, is the proximate effect; the volition, or activity of desire to preserve himself from cold, is the proximate cause; and the pain of cold, which excited that desire, is the remote cause.

To this, we must add the final cause, as many authors call it, which refers to the motivation for which the previous chain of causes was set in motion. The concept of a final cause, therefore, includes the idea of a rational mind that uses means to achieve its goals; for example, the desire to protect oneself from the painful experience of cold leads a person to build a hut. The act of putting stakes in the ground for walls, using tree branches for rafters, and laying down turf for cover involves a series of consecutive voluntary efforts, all of which are means to achieve a specific outcome. This outcome of keeping warm is called the final cause; the building of the hut is the remote effect; the movement of the man's muscles is the proximate effect; the decision or desire to stay warm is the proximate cause; and the pain of cold, which triggered that desire, is the remote cause.

6. This perpetual chain of causes and effects, whose first link is rivetted to the throne of GOD, divides itself into innumerable diverging branches, which, like the nerves arising from the brain, permeate the most minute and most remote extremities of the system, diffusing motion and sensation to the whole. As every cause is superior in power to the effect, which it has produced, so our idea of the power of the Almighty Creator becomes more elevated and sublime, as we trace the operations of nature from cause to cause, climbing up the links of these chains of being, till we ascend to the Great Source of all things.

6. This endless chain of causes and effects, with the first link connected to the throne of GOD, branches out into countless paths, much like the nerves that extend from the brain, reaching the smallest and most distant parts of the system, spreading motion and sensation everywhere. Since every cause is stronger than its resulting effect, our understanding of the power of the Almighty Creator becomes more profound and awe-inspiring as we follow the workings of nature from cause to cause, climbing up the links of this chain of existence until we reach the Great Source of all things.

Hence the modern discoveries in chemistry and in geology, by having traced the causes of the combinations of bodies to remoter origins, as well as those in astronomy, which dignify the present age, contribute to enlarge and amplify our ideas of the power of the Great First Cause. And had those ancient philosophers, who contended that the world was formed from atoms, ascribed their combinations to certain immutable properties received from the hand of the Creator, such as general gravitation, chemical affinity, or animal appetency, instead of ascribing them to a blind chance; the doctrine of atoms, as constituting or composing the material world by the variety of their combinations, so far from leading the mind to atheism, would strengthen the demonstration of the existence of a Deity, as the first cause of all things; because the analogy resulting from our perpetual experience of cause and effect would have thus been exemplified through universal nature.

Thus, the modern discoveries in chemistry and geology, by tracing the causes of how substances combine to deeper origins, along with those in astronomy that elevate our current era, help to broaden and enhance our understanding of the power of the Great First Cause. If those ancient philosophers who claimed that the world was made from atoms had attributed their combinations to certain unchanging properties given by the Creator, such as general gravitation, chemical affinity, or animal instincts, instead of attributing them to random chance, the theory of atoms constituting or making up the material world through various combinations would, far from leading the mind to atheism, actually strengthen the case for the existence of a Deity as the ultimate cause of everything. This is because the analogy drawn from our constant experience of cause and effect would have been demonstrated throughout the natural world.

The heavens declare the glory of GOD, and the firmament sheweth his handywork! One day telleth another, and one night certifieth another; they have neither speech nor language, yet their voice is gone forth into all lands, and their words into the ends of the world. Manifold are thy works, O LORD! in wisdom hast thou made them all. Psal. xix. civ.

The heavens proclaim the glory of GOD, and the sky reveals his creations! One day tells another, and one night confirms the next; they don't use words or speech, yet their message has spread across all the earth, and their words reach the ends of the world. Your works are numerous, O LORD! In wisdom, you have made them all. Psal. xix. civ.



SECT. XL.

On the OCULAR SPECTRA of Light and Colours, by Dr. R. W. Darwin, of Shrewsbury. Reprinted, by Permission, from the Philosophical Transactions, Vol. LXXVI. p. 313.

On the OCULAR SPECTRA of Light and Colors, by Dr. R. W. Darwin, of Shrewsbury. Reprinted, by Permission, from the Philosophical Transactions, Vol. LXXVI. p. 313.

Spectra of four kinds. 1. Activity of the retina in vision. 2. Spectra from defect of sensibility. 3. Spectra from excess of sensibility. 4. Of direct ocular spectra. 5. Greater stimulus excites the retina into spasmodic action. 6. Of reverse ocular spectra. 7. Greater stimulus excites the retina into various successive spasmodic actions. 8. Into fixed spasmodic action. 9. Into temporary paralysis. 10. Miscellaneous remarks; 1. Direct and reverse spectra at the same time. A spectral halo. Rule to predetermine the colours of spectra. 2. Variation of spectra from extraneous light. 3. Variation of spectra in number, figure, and remission. 4. Circulation of the blood in the eye is visible. 5. A new way of magnifying objects. Conclusion.

Spectra of four kinds. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. Activity of the retina in vision. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. Spectra resulting from reduced sensitivity. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. Spectra resulting from increased sensitivity. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. Of direct ocular spectra. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__. Greater stimuli trigger spasmodic responses in the retina. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_5__. Of reverse ocular spectra. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_6__. Greater stimuli lead the retina to various successive spasmodic actions. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_7__. Resulting in fixed spasmodic action. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_8__. Resulting in temporary paralysis. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_9__. Miscellaneous remarks; __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_10__. Direct and reverse spectra occurring simultaneously. A spectral halo. A rule for predicting the colors of spectra. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_11__. Variation of spectra due to external light. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_12__. Variation of spectra in quantity, shape, and brightness. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_13__. The circulation of blood in the eye is visible. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_14__. A new method for magnifying objects. Conclusion.

When any one has long and attentively looked at a bright object, as at the setting sun, on closing his eyes, or removing them, an image, which resembles in form the object he was attending to, continues some time to be visible; this appearance in the eye we shall call the ocular spectrum of that object.

When someone has stared at a bright object, like the setting sun, for a long time, and then closes their eyes or looks away, an image that looks similar to the object they were focusing on stays visible for a while. We'll refer to this appearance in the eye as the ocular spectrum of that object.

These ocular spectra are of four kinds: 1st, Such as are owing to a less sensibility of a defined part of the retina; or spectra from defect of sensibility. 2d, Such as are owing to a greater sensibility of a defined part of the retina; or spectra from excess of sensibility. 3d, Such as resemble their object in its colour as well as form; which may be termed direct ocular spectra. 4th, Such as are of a colour contrary to that of their object; which may be termed reverse ocular spectra.

These visual spectra come in four types: 1st, those resulting from reduced sensitivity in a specific part of the retina; or spectrum from a lack of sensitivity. 2nd, those resulting from increased sensitivity in a specific part of the retina; or spectrum from excess sensitivity. 3rd, those that closely resemble their object in both color and shape; which can be called direct visual spectra. 4th, those that have a color opposite to that of their object; which can be referred to as reverse visual spectra.

The laws of light have been most successfully explained by the great Newton, and the perception of visible objects has been ably investigated by the ingenious Dr. Berkeley and M. Malebranche; but these minute phenomena of vision have yet been thought reducible to no theory, though many philosophers have employed a considerable degree of attention upon them: among these are Dr. Jurin, at the end of Dr. Smith's Optics; M. Æpinus, in the Nov. Com. Petropol. V. 10.; M. Beguelin, in the Berlin Memoires, V. II. 1771; M. d'Arcy, in the Histoire de l'Acad. des Scienc. 1765; M. de la Hire; and, lastly, the celebrated M. de Buffon, in the Memoires de l'Acad. des Scien. who has termed them accidental colours, as if subjected to no established laws, Ac. Par. 1743. M. p. 215.

The laws of light have been explained very well by the great Newton, and the understanding of visible objects has been thoroughly explored by the clever Dr. Berkeley and M. Malebranche; however, the intricate details of vision have yet to be linked to any established theory, even though many philosophers have dedicated significant attention to them: among these are Dr. Jurin at the end of Dr. Smith's Optics; M. Æpinus in the Nov. Com. Petropol. V. 10.; M. Beguelin in the Berlin Memoires, V. II. 1771; M. d'Arcy in the Histoire de l'Acad. des Scienc. 1765; M. de la Hire; and finally, the famous M. de Buffon in the Memoires de l'Acad. des Scien., who referred to them as accidental colors, as if they were not governed by any established laws, Ac. Par. 1743. M. p. 215.

I must here apprize the reader, that it is very difficult for different people to give the same names to various shades of colours; whence, in the following pages, something must be allowed, if on repeating the experiments the colours here mentioned should not accurately correspond with his own names of them.

I want to make it clear to the reader that it can be really challenging for different people to agree on names for various shades of colors. Therefore, in the following pages, some understanding should be given if the colors mentioned here do not exactly match the names that you use for them during your own experiments.

I. Activity of the Retina in Vision.

I. The Retina's Role in Vision.

From the subsequent experiments it appears, that the retina is in an active not in a passive state during the existence of these ocular spectra; and it is thence to be concluded, that all vision is owing to the activity of this organ.

From the following experiments, it seems that the retina is in an active state, not a passive one, when these visual spectra occur; therefore, it can be concluded that all vision is the result of the activity of this organ.

1. Place a piece of red silk, about an inch in diameter, as in plate 1, at Sect. III. 1., on a sheet of white paper, in a strong light; look steadily upon it from about the distance of half a yard for a minute; then closing your eyelids cover them with your hands, and a green spectrum will be seen in your eyes, resembling in form the piece of red silk: after some time, this spectrum will disappear and shortly reappear; and this alternately three or four times, if the experiment is well made, till at length it vanishes entirely.

1. Place a piece of red silk, about an inch in diameter, as in plate 1, at Sect. III. 1., on a sheet of white paper, in a strong light; look steadily upon it from about the distance of half a yard for a minute; then closing your eyelids cover them with your hands, and a green spectrum will be seen in your eyes, resembling in form the piece of red silk: after some time, this spectrum will disappear and shortly reappear; and this alternately three or four times, if the experiment is well made, till at length it vanishes entirely.

2. Place on a sheet of white paper a circular piece of blue silk, about four inches in diameter, in the sunshine; cover the center of this with a circular piece of yellow silk, about three inches in diameter; and the center of the yellow silk with a circle of pink silk, about two inches in diameter; and the center of the pink silk with a circle of green silk, about one inch in diameter; and the centre of this with a circle of indigo, about half an inch in diameter; make a small speck with ink in the very center of the whole, as in plate 3, at Sect. III. 3. 6.; look steadily for a minute on this central spot, and then closing your eyes, and applying your hand at about an inch distance before them, so as to prevent too much or too little light from passing through the eyelids, you will see the most beautiful circles of colours that imagination can conceive, which are most resembled by the colours occasioned by pouring a drop or two of oil on a still lake in a bright day; but these circular irises of colours are not only different from the colours of the silks above mentioned, but are at the same time perpetually changing as long as they exist.

2. Place on a sheet of white paper a circular piece of blue silk, about four inches in diameter, in the sunshine; cover the center of this with a circular piece of yellow silk, about three inches in diameter; and the center of the yellow silk with a circle of pink silk, about two inches in diameter; and the center of the pink silk with a circle of green silk, about one inch in diameter; and the centre of this with a circle of indigo, about half an inch in diameter; make a small speck with ink in the very center of the whole, as in plate 3, at Sect. III. 3. 6.; look steadily for a minute on this central spot, and then closing your eyes, and applying your hand at about an inch distance before them, so as to prevent too much or too little light from passing through the eyelids, you will see the most beautiful circles of colours that imagination can conceive, which are most resembled by the colours occasioned by pouring a drop or two of oil on a still lake in a bright day; but these circular irises of colours are not only different from the colours of the silks above mentioned, but are at the same time perpetually changing as long as they exist.

3. When any one in the dark presses either corner of his eye with his finger, and turns his eye away from his finger, he will see a circle of colours like those in a peacock's tail: and a sudden flash of light is excited in the eye by a stroke on it. (Newton's Opt. Q. 16.)

3. When someone in the dark presses either corner of their eye with their finger and looks away from their finger, they will see a circle of colors similar to those in a peacock's tail: and a sudden flash of light is triggered in the eye by the pressure on it. (Newton's Opt. Q. 16.)

4. When any one turns round rapidly on one foot, till he becomes dizzy, and falls upon the ground, the spectra of the ambient objects continue to present themselves in rotation, or appear to librate, and he seems to behold them for some time still in motion.

4. When someone spins around quickly on one foot until they get dizzy and fall to the ground, the images of the surrounding objects keep appearing to spin or sway, and it seems like they can still see them moving for a while.

From all these experiments it appears, that the spectra in the eye are not owing to the mechanical impulse of light impressed on the retina, nor to its chemical combination with that organ, nor to the absorption and emission of light, as is observed in many bodies; for in all these cases the spectra must either remain uniformly, or gradually diminish; and neither their alternate pretence and evanescence as in the first experiment, nor the perpetual changes of their colours as in the second, nor the flash of light or colours in the pressed eye as in the third, nor the rotation or libration of the spectra as in the fourth, could exist.

From all these experiments, it seems that the spectra in the eye are not due to the mechanical impact of light hitting the retina, nor to its chemical reaction with that organ, nor to the absorption and emission of light, like in many materials. In all these cases, the spectra should either stay the same or gradually fade away; however, neither their alternating appearance and disappearance as in the first experiment, nor the constant changes in their colors as in the second, nor the flashes of light or colors in the pressed eye as in the third, nor the rotation or wobbling of the spectra as in the fourth, could occur.

It is not absurd to conceive, that the retina may be stimulated into motion, as well as the red and white muscles which form our limbs and vessels; since it consists of fibres, like those, intermixed with its medullary substance. To evince this structure, the retina of an ox's eye was suspended in a glass of warm water, and forcibly torn in a few places; the edges of these parts appeared jagged and hairy, and did not contract, and become smooth like simple mucus, when it is distended till it breaks; which shews that it consists of fibres; and that its fibrous construction became still more distinct to the sight, by adding some caustic alkali to the water, as the adhering mucus was first eroded, and the hair-like fibres remained floating in the vessel. Nor does the degree of transparency of the retina invalidate the evidence of its fibrous structure, since Leeuwenhoek has shewn that the crystalline humour itself consists of fibres. (Arcana Naturæ, V. 1. p. 70.)

It’s not unreasonable to think that the retina can be stimulated into motion, just like the red and white muscles that make up our limbs and blood vessels. It’s made up of fibers, similar to those muscles, mixed with its medullary substance. To demonstrate this structure, the retina from an ox's eye was hung in a glass of warm water and forcibly torn in a few spots; the edges of these torn areas looked ragged and hairy, and they didn’t contract and become smooth like simple mucus does when it’s stretched until it breaks. This shows it’s made of fibers, and the fibrous structure became even clearer when some caustic alkali was added to the water, as the mucus clinging to it was first eroded, leaving the hair-like fibers floating in the water. Plus, the clarity of the retina doesn’t undermine the proof of its fibrous structure, since Leeuwenhoek has shown that the crystalline humor itself is made of fibers. (Arcana Naturæ, V. 1. p. 70.)

Hence it appears, that as the muscles have larger fibres intermixed with a smaller quantity of nervous medulla, the organ of vision has a greater quantity of nervous medulla intermixed with smaller fibres; and it is probable that the locomotive muscles, as well as the vascular ones, of microscopic animals have much greater tenuity than these of the retina.

Hence it seems that while muscles have larger fibers mixed with less nervous tissue, the eye has more nervous tissue mixed with smaller fibers. It's likely that the movement muscles, as well as the blood vessels, of microscopic animals are much thinner than those in the retina.

And besides the similar laws, which will be shewn in this paper to govern alike the actions of the retina and of the muscles, there are many other analogies which exist between them. They are both originally excited into action by irritations, both are nearly in the same quantity of time, are alike strengthened or fatigued by exertion, are alike painful if excited into action when they are in an inflamed state, are alike liable to paralysis, and to the torpor of old age.

And in addition to the similar laws, which will be shown in this paper to govern both the actions of the retina and the muscles, there are many other similarities between them. Both are initially activated by stimuli, both react in roughly the same amount of time, both can be strengthened or exhausted by use, both can be painful if triggered while inflamed, and both are susceptible to paralysis and the sluggishness that comes with old age.

II. OF SPECTRA FROM DEFECT OF SENSIBILITY.

II. OF SPECTRA FROM DEFECT OF SENSIBILITY.

The retina is not so easily excited into action by less irritation after having been lately subjected to greater.

The retina doesn't respond easily to small irritations after it has just experienced more intense stimulation.

1. When any one passes from the bright daylight into a darkened room, the irises of his eyes expand themselves to their utmost extent in a few seconds of time; but it is very long before the optic nerve, after having been stimulated by the greater light of the day, becomes sensible of the less degree of it in the room; and, if the room is not too obscure, the irises will again contract themselves in some degree, as the sensibility of the retina returns.

1. When someone moves from bright daylight into a dark room, the irises of their eyes open up fully in just a few seconds; however, it takes a substantial amount of time for the optic nerve, which has been activated by the bright sunlight, to register the lower level of light in the room. If the room isn’t too dark, the irises will eventually constrict again as the retina regains sensitivity.

2. Place about half an inch square of white paper on a black hat, and looking steadily on the center of it for a minute, remove your eyes to a sheet of white paper; and after a second or two a dark square will be seen on the white paper, which will continue some time. A similar dark square will be seen in the closed eye, if light be admitted through the eyelids.

2. Place a square of white paper about half an inch on a black hat, and gaze steadily at the center of it for a minute. Then, shift your gaze to a sheet of white paper; after a second or two, a dark square will appear on the white paper, which will last for a while. You will also notice a similar dark square in your closed eye if light gets in through your eyelids.

So after looking at any luminous object of a small size, as at the sun, for a short time, so as not much to fatigue the eyes, this part of the retina becomes less sensible to smaller quantities of light; hence, when the eyes are turned on other less luminous parts of the sky, a dark spot is seen resembling the shape of the sun, or other luminous object which we last beheld. This is the source of one kind of the dark-coloured muscæ volitantes. If this dark spot lies above the center of the eye, we turn our eyes that way, expecting to bring it into the center of the eye, that we may view it more distinctly; and in this case the dark spectrum seems to move upwards. If the dark spectrum is found beneath the centre of the eye, we pursue it from the same motive, and it seems to move downwards. This has given rise to various conjectures of something floating in the aqueous humours of the eyes; but whoever, in attending to these spots, keeps his eyes unmoved by looking steadily at the corner of a cloud, at the same time that he observes the dark spectra, will be thoroughly convinced, that they have no motion but what is given to them by the movement of our eyes in pursuit of them. Sometimes the form of the spectrum, when it has been received from a circular luminous body, will become oblong; and sometimes it will be divided into two circular spectra, which is not owing to our changing the angle made by the two optic axises, according to the distance of the clouds or other bodies to which the spectrum is supposed to be contiguous, but to other causes mentioned in No. X. 3. of this section. The apparent size of it will also be variable according to its supposed distance.

So after looking at any luminous object of a small size, as at the sun, for a short time, so as not much to fatigue the eyes, this part of the retina becomes less sensible to smaller quantities of light; hence, when the eyes are turned on other less luminous parts of the sky, a dark spot is seen resembling the shape of the sun, or other luminous object which we last beheld. This is the source of one kind of the dark-coloured muscæ volitantes. If this dark spot lies above the center of the eye, we turn our eyes that way, expecting to bring it into the center of the eye, that we may view it more distinctly; and in this case the dark spectrum seems to move upwards. If the dark spectrum is found beneath the centre of the eye, we pursue it from the same motive, and it seems to move downwards. This has given rise to various conjectures of something floating in the aqueous humours of the eyes; but whoever, in attending to these spots, keeps his eyes unmoved by looking steadily at the corner of a cloud, at the same time that he observes the dark spectra, will be thoroughly convinced, that they have no motion but what is given to them by the movement of our eyes in pursuit of them. Sometimes the form of the spectrum, when it has been received from a circular luminous body, will become oblong; and sometimes it will be divided into two circular spectra, which is not owing to our changing the angle made by the two optic axises, according to the distance of the clouds or other bodies to which the spectrum is supposed to be contiguous, but to other causes mentioned in No. X. 3. of this section. The apparent size of it will also be variable according to its supposed distance.

As these spectra are more easily observable when our eyes are a little weakened by fatigue, it has frequently happened, that people of delicate constitutions have been much alarmed at them, fearing a beginning decay of their sight, and have thence fallen into the hands of ignorant oculists; but I believe they never are a prelude to any other disease of the eye, and that it is from habit alone, and our want of attention to them, that we do not see them on all objects every hour of our lives. But as the nerves of very weak people lose their sensibility, in the same manner as their muscles lose their activity, by a small time of exertion, it frequently happens, that sick people in the extreme debility of fevers are perpetually employed in picking something from the bed-clothes, occasioned by their mistaking the appearance of these muscæ volitantes in their eyes. Benvenuto Celini, an Italian artist, a man of strong abilities, relates, that having passed the whole night on a distant mountain with some companions and a conjurer, and performed many ceremonies to raise the devil, on their return in the morning to Rome, and looking up when the sun began to rise, they saw numerous devils run on the tops of the houses, as they passed along; so much were the spectra of their weakened eyes magnified by fear, and made subservient to the purposes of fraud or superstition. (Life of Ben. Celini.)

As these visual phenomena are easier to notice when our eyes are a bit tired, it's not uncommon for people with sensitive dispositions to get quite worried about them, fearing that their vision is starting to fail. This can lead them to seek help from unqualified eye doctors. However, I believe these phenomena are not a sign of any other eye disease, and it's simply due to habit and our lack of attention to them that we don't see them all the time. Additionally, just as very weak people's nerves become less sensitive after a short period of exertion, sick people suffering from severe fevers often spend a lot of time trying to brush something off their sheets because they mistake these muscæ volitantes for something else in their eyes. Benvenuto Celini, an Italian artist known for his remarkable talents, tells a story about how he and some friends, including a conjurer, spent the whole night on a distant mountain performing various ceremonies to summon the devil. When they returned to Rome in the morning and looked up as the sun was rising, they saw numerous devils running across the rooftops. Their weakened eyes exaggerated the visual phenomena, which became tools of deception or superstition. (Life of Ben. Celini.)

3. Place a square inch of white paper on a large piece of straw-coloured silk; look steadily some time on the white paper, and then move the centre of your eyes on the silk, and a spectrum of the form of the paper will appear on the silk, of a deeper yellow than the other part of it: for the central part of the retina, having been some time exposed to the stimulus of a greater quantity of white light, is become less sensible to a smaller quantity of it, and therefore sees only the yellow rays in that part of the straw-coloured silk.

3. Place a square inch of white paper on a large piece of straw-colored silk; gaze at the white paper for a while, then shift your focus to the silk, and you'll notice a spectrum shaped like the paper appearing on the silk, appearing a deeper yellow than the surrounding area: this happens because the central part of your retina, having been exposed to a stronger amount of white light for some time, becomes less sensitive to a weaker amount of light, and thus only detects the yellow rays in that area of the straw-colored silk.

Facts similar to these are observable in other parts of our system: thus, if one hand be made warm, and the other exposed to the cold, and then both of them immersed in subtepid water, the water is perceived warm to one hand, and cold to the other; and we are not able to hear weak sounds for some time after we have been exposed to loud ones; and we feel a chilliness on coming into an atmosphere of temperate warmth, after having been some time confined in a very warm room: and hence the stomach, and other organs of digestion, of those who have been habituated to the greater stimulus of spirituous liquor, are not excited into their due action by the less stimulus of common food alone; of which the immediate consequence is indigestion and hypochondriacism.

Facts like these can be seen in other parts of our system: if one hand is warmed while the other is left in the cold, and then both are put in lukewarm water, the water feels warm to one hand and cold to the other; we can’t hear faint sounds for a while after being exposed to loud noises; and we feel cold when we enter a moderately warm environment after being in a very hot room for a while. This is why the stomach and other digestive organs of people who are used to the stronger stimulus of alcoholic beverages aren't stimulated enough by regular food alone; this often leads to indigestion and anxiety.

III. OF SPECTRA FROM EXCESS OF SENSIBILITY.

III. OF SPECTRA FROM EXCESS OF SENSITIVITY.

The retina is more easily excited into action by greater irritation after having been lately subjected to less.

The retina reacts more quickly to stronger stimuli after it has recently been exposed to weaker ones.

1. If the eyes are closed, and covered perfectly with a hat, for a minute or two, in a bright day; on removing the hat a red or crimson light is seen through the eyelids. In this experiment the retina, after being some time kept in the dark, becomes so sensible to a small quantity of light, as to perceive distinctly the greater quantity of red rays than of others which pass through the eyelids. A similar coloured light is seen to pass through the edges of the fingers, when the open hand is opposed to the flame of a candle.

1. If your eyes are shut and completely covered by a hat for a minute or two on a bright day, when you take off the hat, you’ll notice a red or crimson light through your eyelids. In this experiment, after being kept in the dark for a while, the retina becomes so sensitive to even a small amount of light that it clearly perceives more red rays than other colors that come through the eyelids. A similar colored light can also be seen passing through the edges of your fingers when your open hand is held in front of a candle flame.

2. If you look for some minutes steadily on a window in the beginning of the evening twilight, or in a dark day, and then move your eyes a little, so that those parts of the retina, on which the dark frame-work of the window was delineated, may now fall on the glass part of it, many luminous lines, representing the frame-work, will appear to lie across the glass panes: for those parts of the retina, which were before least stimulated by the dark frame-work, are now more sensible to light than the other parts of the retina which were exposed to the more luminous parts of the window,

2. If you stare at a window for several minutes during the early evening twilight or on a dark day, and then slightly shift your gaze so that the areas of your retina that were focused on the dark frame of the window now fall on the glass part, you'll see many bright lines that seem to cross the glass panes. This happens because the areas of the retina that were less stimulated by the dark frame are now more responsive to light compared to the other parts that were exposed to the brighter areas of the window.

3. Make with ink on white paper a very black spot, about half an inch in diameter, with a tail about an inch in length, so as to represent a tadpole, as in plate 2, at Sect. III. 3. 3.; look steadily for a minute on this spot, and, on moving the eye a little, the figure of the tadpole will be seen on the white part of the paper, which figure of the tadpole will appear whiter or more luminous than the other parts of the white paper; for the part of the retina on which the tadpole was delineated, is now more sensible to light, than the other parts of it, which were exposed to the white paper. This experiment is mentioned by Dr. Irwin, but is not by him ascribed to the true cause, namely, the greater sensibility of that part of the retina which has been exposed to the black spot, than of the other parts which had received the white field of paper, which is put beyond a doubt by the next experiment.

3. Make with ink on white paper a very black spot, about half an inch in diameter, with a tail about an inch in length, so as to represent a tadpole, as in plate 2, at Sect. III. 3. 3.; look steadily for a minute on this spot, and, on moving the eye a little, the figure of the tadpole will be seen on the white part of the paper, which figure of the tadpole will appear whiter or more luminous than the other parts of the white paper; for the part of the retina on which the tadpole was delineated, is now more sensible to light, than the other parts of it, which were exposed to the white paper. This experiment is mentioned by Dr. Irwin, but is not by him ascribed to the true cause, namely, the greater sensibility of that part of the retina which has been exposed to the black spot, than of the other parts which had received the white field of paper, which is put beyond a doubt by the next experiment.

4. On closing the eyes after viewing the black spot on the white paper, as in the foregoing experiment, a red spot is seen of the form of the black spot: for that part of the retina, on which the black spot was delineated, being now more sensible to light than the other parts of it, which were exposed to the white paper, is capable of perceiving the red rays which penetrate the eyelids. If this experiment be made by the light of a tallow candle, the spot will be yellow instead of red; for tallow candles abound much with yellow light, which passes in greater quantity and force through the eyelids than blue tight; hence the difficulty of distinguishing blue and green by this kind of candle light. The colour of the spectrum may possibly vary in the daylight, according to the different colour of the meridian or the morning or evening light.

4. When you close your eyes after looking at the black spot on the white paper, as mentioned in the previous experiment, you will see a red spot that takes the shape of the black spot. This happens because the part of the retina where the black spot was is now more sensitive to light than the other areas that were exposed to the white paper, allowing it to perceive the red rays that pass through your eyelids. If you conduct this experiment using the light of a tallow candle, the spot will appear yellow instead of red, since tallow candles emit a lot of yellow light, which penetrates the eyelids more effectively than blue light. This is why it's hard to distinguish between blue and green under this kind of candlelight. The color of the spectrum might also change in daylight, depending on the different colors of the light during midday or in the morning and evening.

M. Beguelin, in the Berlin Memoires, V. II. 1771, observes, that, when he held a book so that the sun shone upon his half-closed eyelids, the black letters, which he had long inspected, became red, which must have been thus occasioned. Those parts of the retina which had received for some time the black letters, were so much more sensible than those parts which had been opposed to the white paper, that to the former the red light, which passed through the eyelids, was perceptible. There is a similar story told, I think, in de Voltaire's Historical Works, of a Duke of Tuscany, who was playing at dice with the general of a foreign army, and, believing he saw bloody spots upon the dice, portended dreadful events, and retired in confusion. The observer, after looking for a minute on the black spots of a die, and carelessly closing his eyes, on a bright day; would see the image of a die with red spots upon it, as above explained.

M. Beguelin, in the Berlin Memoires, V. II. 1771, notes that when he held a book so that the sun shone on his slightly closed eyelids, the black letters he had been looking at for a while turned red. This likely happened because the parts of the retina that had been exposed to the black letters were much more sensitive than the areas that had been against the white paper, making the red light that came through his eyelids noticeable to them. There's a similar story, I think, in de Voltaire's Historical Works, about a Duke of Tuscany who was playing dice with a general from a foreign army and mistakenly thought he saw bloody spots on the dice, which he interpreted as a bad omen, causing him to leave in embarrassment. After looking at the black spots on a die for a minute and then casually closing his eyes on a bright day, the observer would see an image of a die with red spots on it, as described above.

5. On emerging from a dark cavern, where we have long continued, the light of a bright day becomes intolerable to the eye for a considerable time, owing to the excess of sensibility existing in the eye, after having been long exposed to little or no stimulus. This occasions us immediately to contract the iris to its smallest aperture, which becomes again gradually dilated, as the retina becomes accustomed to the greater stimulus of the daylight.

5. After coming out of a dark cave, the brightness of the day feels overwhelming to our eyes for quite a while because they're overly sensitive after being in darkness for so long. This makes us instantly narrow our pupils to their smallest size, which then gradually widen again as our retinas adjust to the stronger light.

The twinkling of a bright star, or of a distant candle in the night, is perhaps owing to the same cause. While we continue to look upon these luminous objects, their central parts gradually appear paler, owing to the decreasing sensibility of the part of the retina exposed to their light; whilst, at the same time, by the unsteadiness of the eye, the edges of them are perpetually falling on parts of the retina that were just before exposed to the darkness of the night, and therefore tenfold more sensible to light than the part on which the star or candle had been for some time delineated. This pains the eye in a similar manner as when we come suddenly from a dark room into bright daylight, and gives the appearance of bright scintillations. Hence the stars twinkle most when the night is darkest, and do not twinkle through telescopes, as observed by Musschenbroeck; and it will afterwards be seen why this twinkling is sometimes of different colours when the object is very bright, as Mr. Melvill observed in looking at Sirius. For the opinions of others on this subject, see Dr. Priestley's valuable History of Light and Colours, p. 494.

The shimmering of a bright star or a distant candle at night might be due to the same reason. As we keep looking at these shining objects, their center gradually appears dimmer because the part of our retina that's exposed to their light becomes less sensitive. Meanwhile, because our eyes are unsteady, the edges of these objects constantly fall on parts of the retina that were just in the dark, making those areas much more sensitive to light than the part where the star or candle has been focused for a while. This can strain the eyes in a way similar to stepping suddenly from a dark room into bright daylight, creating the effect of bright flickers. Therefore, stars twinkle the most when the night is darkest and do not twinkle when viewed through telescopes, as noted by Musschenbroeck. Later, it will be explained why this twinkling sometimes appears in different colors when the object is very bright, as Mr. Melvill noted while observing Sirius. For more opinions on this topic, see Dr. Priestley's valuable History of Light and Colours, p. 494.

Many facts observable in the animal system are similar to these; as the hot glow occasioned by the usual warmth of the air, or our clothes, on coming out of a cold bath; the pain of the fingers on approaching the fire after having handled snow; and the inflamed heels from walking in snow. Hence those who have been exposed to much cold have died on being brought to a fire, or their limbs have become so much inflamed as to mortify. Hence much food or wine given suddenly to those who have almost perished by hunger has destroyed them; for all the organs of the famished body are now become so much more irritable to the stimulus of food and wine, which they have long been deprived of, that inflammation is excited, which terminates in gangrene or fever.

Many observable facts in the animal kingdom are similar to these: like the warm feeling you get from the usual warmth of the air or your clothes when you come out of a cold bath; the pain in your fingers when you get close to the fire after handling snow; and the swollen heels from walking in the snow. That's why people who have been exposed to extreme cold have died when brought near a fire, or their limbs have become so inflamed that they have become gangrenous. Similarly, giving a lot of food or wine suddenly to those who have nearly starved to death can kill them, because all the organs of the starving body have become much more sensitive to the food and wine they’ve been deprived of for so long, leading to inflammation that results in gangrene or fever.

IV. OF DIRECT OCULAR SPECTRA.

IV. OF DIRECT OCULAR SPECTRA.

A quantity of stimulus somewhat greater than natural excites the retina into spasmodic action, which ceases in a few seconds.

A slightly higher level of stimulation than normal makes the retina react in a spasm, but this only lasts for a few seconds.

A certain duration and energy of the stimulus of light and colours excites the perfect action of the retina in vision; for very quick motions are imperceptible to us, as well as very slow ones, as the whirling of a top, or the shadow on a sun-dial. So perfect darkness does not affect the eye at all; and excess of light produces pain, not vision.

A specific amount of time and intensity of light and colors triggers the retina's optimal function in vision; very fast movements are invisible to us, just like very slow ones, such as the spinning of a top or the shadow on a sundial. Complete darkness doesn't impact the eye at all; and too much light causes discomfort, not vision.

1. When a fire-coal is whirled round in the dark, a lucid circle remains a considerable time in the eye; and that with so much vivacity of light, that it is mistaken for a continuance of the irritation of the object. In the same manner, when a fiery meteor shoots across the night, it appears to leave a long lucid train behind it, part of which, and perhaps sometimes the whole, is owing to the continuance of the action of the retina after having been thus vividly excited. This is beautifully illustrated by the following experiment: fix a paper sail, three or four inches in diameter, and made like that of a smoke jack, on a tube of pasteboard; on looking through the tube at a distant prospect, some disjointed parts of it will be seen through the narrow intervals between the sails; but as the fly begins to revolve, these intervals appear larger; and when it revolves quicker, the whole prospect is seen quite as distinct as if nothing intervened, though less luminous.

1. When a fire-coal spins in the dark, it leaves a bright circle in your vision for quite a while; it’s so bright that it’s often mistaken for the continued presence of the object. Similarly, when a shooting star streaks across the night sky, it seems to leave a long bright trail behind it, part of which, and sometimes all of it, is due to the retina continuing to react after being intensely stimulated. This is nicely shown by the following experiment: attach a paper sail, three or four inches wide, made like that of a smoke jack, to a cardboard tube; when you look through the tube at a distant view, you'll see some fragmented parts of it through the narrow gaps between the sails. However, as the sail starts to spin, those gaps appear larger; and when it spins faster, the entire scene becomes as clear as if nothing was in the way, although it’s less bright.

Fig. 3. Fig. 3.

2. Look through a dark tube, about half a yard long, at the area of a yellow circle of half an inch diameter, lying upon a blue area of double that diameter, for half a minute; and on closing your eyes the colours of the spectrum will appear similar to the two areas, as in fig. 3.; but if the eye is kept too long upon them, the colours of the spectrum will be the reverse of those upon the paper, that is, the internal circle will become blue, and the external area yellow; hence some attention is required in making this experiment.

2. Look through a dark tube, about half a yard long, at the yellow circle that’s half an inch in diameter, set against a blue area that’s twice that size, for half a minute. When you close your eyes, you'll see colors of the spectrum similar to the two areas, as shown in fig. 3. However, if you keep your gaze on them for too long, the spectrum colors will flip; the inner circle will turn blue, and the outer area will become yellow. So, you need to pay some attention when doing this experiment.

3. Place the bright flame of a spermaceti candle before a black object in the night; look steadily at it for a short time, till it is observed to become somewhat paler; and on closing the eyes, and covering them carefully, but not so as to compress them, the image of the blazing candle will continue distinctly to be visible.

3. Put a bright spermaceti candle in front of a black object at night; stare at it for a moment until you notice it getting a bit lighter; then, when you close your eyes and cover them gently without pressing, the image of the glowing candle will still be clearly seen.

4. Look steadily, for a short time, at a window in a dark day, as in Exp. 2. Sect. III. and then closing your eyes, and covering them with your hands, an exact delineation of the window remains for some time visible in the eye. This experiment requires a little practice to make it succeed well; since, if the eyes are fatigued by looking too long on the window, or the day be too bright, the luminous parts of the window will appear dark in the spectrum, and the dark parts of the frame-work will appear luminous, as in Exp. 2. Sect. III. And it is even difficult for many, who first try this experiment, to perceive the spectrum at all; for any hurry of mind, or even too great attention to the spectrum itself, will disappoint them, till they have had a little experience in attending to such small sensations.

4. Look steadily, for a short time, at a window in a dark day, as in Exp. 2. Sect. III. and then closing your eyes, and covering them with your hands, an exact delineation of the window remains for some time visible in the eye. This experiment requires a little practice to make it succeed well; since, if the eyes are fatigued by looking too long on the window, or the day be too bright, the luminous parts of the window will appear dark in the spectrum, and the dark parts of the frame-work will appear luminous, as in Exp. 2. Sect. III. And it is even difficult for many, who first try this experiment, to perceive the spectrum at all; for any hurry of mind, or even too great attention to the spectrum itself, will disappoint them, till they have had a little experience in attending to such small sensations.

The spectra described in this section, termed direct ocular spectra, are produced without much fatigue of the eye; the irritation of the luminous object being soon withdrawn, or its quantity of light being not so great as to produce any degree of uneasiness in the organ of vision; which distinguishes them from the next class of ocular spectra, which are the consequence of fatigue. These direct spectra are best observed in such circumstances that no light, but what comes from the object, can fall upon the eye; as in looking through a tube, of half a yard long, and an inch wide, at a yellow paper on the side of a room, the direct spectrum was easily produced on closing the eye without taking it from the tube; but if the lateral light is admitted through the eyelids, or by throwing the spectrum on white paper, it becomes a reverse spectrum, as will be explained below.

The spectra discussed in this section, called direct ocular spectra, are created without much eye strain; the irritation from the light source is quickly removed, or its brightness isn’t strong enough to cause any discomfort to the eyes. This sets them apart from the next type of ocular spectra, which are caused by fatigue. The best way to observe these direct spectra is in conditions where no light, except for that coming from the object, hits the eye. For example, when looking through a tube about half a yard long and an inch wide at a piece of yellow paper on the wall, the direct spectrum can be easily seen after closing the eye without moving it from the tube. However, if lateral light enters through the eyelids or if the spectrum is projected onto white paper, it turns into a reverse spectrum, as will be explained below.

The other senses also retain for a time the impressions that have been made upon them, or the actions they have been excited into. So if a hard body is pressed upon the palm of the hand, as is practised in tricks of legerdemain, it is not easy to distinguish for a few seconds whether it remains or is removed; and tastes continue long to exist vividly in the mouth, as the smoke of tobacco, or the taste of gentian, after the sapid material is withdrawn.

The other senses also hold on to the impressions they've received or the reactions they've triggered for a while. So, if a solid object is pressed against the palm of the hand, as is done in magic tricks, it can be challenging to tell for a few seconds whether it’s still there or has been taken away. Similarly, tastes can linger vividly in the mouth, like the taste of tobacco smoke or gentian, even after the flavorful substance is gone.

V. A quantity of stimulus somewhat greater than the last mentioned excites the retina into spasmodic action, which ceases and recurs alternately.

V. A somewhat larger amount of stimulus than the one previously mentioned causes the retina to enter a spasm, which alternates between stopping and starting again.

1. On looking for a time on the setting sun, so as not greatly to fatigue the sight, a yellow spectrum is seen when the eyes are closed and covered, which continues for a time, and then disappears and recurs repeatedly before it entirely vanishes. This yellow spectrum of the sun when the eyelids are opened becomes blue; and if it is made to fall on the green grass, or on other coloured objects, it varies its own colour by an intermixture of theirs, as will be explained in another place.

1. When looking for a moment as the sun sets, to avoid straining the eyes too much, you’ll notice a yellow light when your eyes are closed and covered. This light lasts for a while, then fades, only to come back again before it completely disappears. When you open your eyelids, this yellow light turns blue; and if it falls on green grass or other colored things, it changes its color by mixing with theirs, as will be explained later.

2. Place a lighted spermaceti candle in the night about one foot from your eye, and look steadily on the centre of the flame, till your eye becomes much more fatigued than in Sect. IV. Exp. 3.; and on closing your eyes a reddish spectrum will be perceived, which will cease and return alternately.

2. Place a lighted spermaceti candle in the night about one foot from your eye, and look steadily on the centre of the flame, till your eye becomes much more fatigued than in Sect. IV. Exp. 3.; and on closing your eyes a reddish spectrum will be perceived, which will cease and return alternately.

The action of vomiting in like manner ceases, and is renewed by intervals, although the emetic drug is thrown up with the first effort: so after-pains continue some time after parturition; and the alternate pulsations of the heart of a viper are renewed for some time after it is cleared from its blood.

The act of vomiting stops and starts again at intervals, even though the emetic is expelled with the first heave: similarly, afterpains persist for a while after childbirth; and the heartbeat of a viper resumes its rhythm for some time after it has been drained of blood.

VI. OF REVERSE OCULAR SPECTRA.

VI. OF REVERSE OCULAR SPECTRA.

The retina, after having been excited into action by a stimulus somewhat greater them the last mentioned falls into opposite spasmodic action.

The retina, when stimulated by a slightly stronger stimulus than the previous one, reacts with a contrasting spastic response.

The actions of every part of animal bodies may be advantageously compared with each other. This strict analogy contributes much to the investigation of truth; while those looser analogies, which compare the phenomena of animal life with those of chemistry or mechanics, only serve to mislead our inquiries.

The actions of every part of animal bodies can be effectively compared with one another. This close similarity greatly aids in the search for truth, while those broader comparisons that link the phenomena of animal life to chemistry or mechanics tend to confuse our investigations.

When any of our larger muscles have been in long or in violent action, and their antagonists have been at the same time extended, as soon as the action of the former ceases, the limb is stretched the contrary way for our ease, and a pandiculation or yawning takes place.

When any of our larger muscles have been under long or intense activity, and their opposing muscles have been stretched at the same time, as soon as the action of the first ones stops, the limb is stretched in the opposite direction for our comfort, and a stretch or yawn happens.

By the following observations it appears, that a similar circumstance obtains in the organ of vision; after it has been fatigued by one kind of action, it spontaneously falls into the opposite kind.

By the following observations, it seems that a similar situation occurs in the eye; after it has been tired from one type of activity, it naturally shifts to the opposite kind.

1. Place a piece of coloured silk, about an inch in diameter, on a sheet of white paper, about half a yard from your eyes; look steadily upon it for a minute; then remove your eyes upon another part of the white paper, and a spectrum will be seen of the form of the silk thus inspected, but of a colour opposite to it. A spectrum nearly similar will appear if the eyes are closed, and the eyelids shaded by approaching the hand near them, so as to permit some, but to prevent too much light falling on them.

1. Place a piece of colored silk, about an inch in diameter, on a sheet of white paper, about half a yard away from your eyes. Look at it steadily for a minute, then shift your gaze to another part of the white paper, and you’ll see a spectrum that resembles the shape of the silk you just looked at, but in a color that’s opposite to it. A similar spectrum will appear if you close your eyes and shade your eyelids by bringing your hand close to them, allowing some light in but not too much.

Red silk produced a green spectrum.

Red silk produced a green spectrum.

Green produced a red one.

Green made a red one.

Orange produced blue.

Orange made blue.

Blue produced orange.

Blue made orange.

Yellow produced violet.

Yellow made violet.

Violet produced yellow.

Violet created yellow.

That in these experiments the colours of the spectra are the reverse of the colours which occasioned them, may be seen by examining the third figure in Sir Isaac Newton's Optics, L. II. p. 1, where those thin laminæ of air, which reflected yellow, transmitted violet; those which reflected red, transmitted a blue green; and so of the rest, agreeing with the experiments above related.

That in these experiments, the colors of the spectra are the opposite of the colors that caused them, can be seen by looking at the third figure in Sir Isaac Newton's Optics, L. II. p. 1, where those thin layers of air that reflected yellow transmitted violet; those that reflected red transmitted a blue-green; and so on, matching the experiments mentioned above.

2. These reverse spectra are similar to a colour, formed by a combination of all the primary colours except that with which the eye has been fatigued in making the experiment: thus the reverse spectrum of red must be such a green as would be produced by a combination of all the other prismatic colours. To evince this fact the following satisfactory experiment was made. The prismatic colours were laid on a circular pasteboard wheel, about four inches in diameter, in the proportions described in Dr. Priestley's History of Light and Colours, pl. 12. fig. 83. except that the red compartment was entirely left out, and the others proportionably extended so as to complete the circle. Then, as the orange is a mixture of red and yellow, and as the violet is a mixture of red and indigo, it became necessary to put yellow on the wheel instead of orange, and indigo instead of violet, that the experiment might more exactly quadrate with the theory it was designed to establish or confute; because in gaining a green spectrum from a red object, the eye is supposed to have become insensible to red light. This wheel, by means of an axis, was made to whirl like a top; and on its being put in motion, a green colour was produced, corresponding with great exactness to the reverse spectrum of red.

2. These reverse spectra are like a color made by mixing all the primary colors except the one that has fatigued the eye during the experiment: therefore, the reverse spectrum of red would produce a green that comes from combining all the other prismatic colors. To demonstrate this fact, the following effective experiment was conducted. The prismatic colors were arranged on a circular cardboard wheel about four inches in diameter, in the proportions described in Dr. Priestley's History of Light and Colours, pl. 12. fig. 83, except that the red section was completely omitted, and the others were proportionally extended to complete the circle. Since orange is a mixture of red and yellow, and violet is a mixture of red and indigo, it was necessary to place yellow on the wheel instead of orange and indigo instead of violet, in order for the experiment to align more closely with the theory it aimed to prove or disprove; because in obtaining a green spectrum from a red object, the eye is thought to have become insensitive to red light. This wheel was set up to spin like a top using an axis, and when it was set in motion, a green color appeared, closely matching the reverse spectrum of red.

3. In contemplating any one or these reverse spectra in the closed and covered eye, it disappears and re-appears several times successively, till at length it entirely vanishes, like the direct spectra in Sect. V.; but with this additional circumstance, that when the spectrum becomes faint or evanescent, it is instantly revived by removing the hand from before the eyelids, so as to admit more light: because then not only the fatigued part of the retina is inclined spontaneously to fall into motions of a contrary direction, but being still sensible to all other rays of light, except that with which it was lately fatigued, is by these rays at the same time stimulated into those motions which form the reverse spectrum.

3. In contemplating any one or these reverse spectra in the closed and covered eye, it disappears and re-appears several times successively, till at length it entirely vanishes, like the direct spectra in Sect. V.; but with this additional circumstance, that when the spectrum becomes faint or evanescent, it is instantly revived by removing the hand from before the eyelids, so as to admit more light: because then not only the fatigued part of the retina is inclined spontaneously to fall into motions of a contrary direction, but being still sensible to all other rays of light, except that with which it was lately fatigued, is by these rays at the same time stimulated into those motions which form the reverse spectrum.

From these experiments there is reason to conclude, that the fatigued part of the retina throws itself into a contrary mode of action, like oscitation or pandiculation, as soon as the stimulus which has fatigued it is withdrawn; and that it still remains sensible, that is, liable to be excited into action by any other colours at the same time, except the colour with which it has been fatigued.

From these experiments, we can conclude that the tired part of the retina shifts to an opposite mode of action, similar to yawning or stretching, as soon as the stimulus that fatigued it is removed; and it still remains responsive, meaning it can be triggered into action by any other colors at the same time, except for the color that caused the fatigue.

VII. The retina after having been excited into action by a stimulus somewhat greater than the last mentioned falls into various successive spasmodic actions.

VII. The retina, after being triggered by something a bit stronger than what was mentioned earlier, goes through a series of repeated spasms.

1. On looking at the meridian sun as long as the eyes can well bear its brightness, the disk first becomes pale, with a luminous crescent, which seems to librate from one edge of it to the other, owing to the unsteadiness of the eye; then the whole phasis of the sun becomes blue, surrounded with a white halo; and on closing the eyes, and covering them with the hands, a yellow spectrum is seen, which in a little time changes into a blue one.

1. When looking at the midday sun for as long as the eyes can handle its brightness, the disk first turns pale, showing a bright crescent that shifts from one edge to the other because of the unsteadiness of the eye. Then the entire sun appears blue, surrounded by a white halo. When you close your eyes and cover them with your hands, you see a yellow spectrum, which soon changes to a blue one.

M. de la Hire observed, after looking at the bright sun, that the impression in his eye first assumed a yellow appearance, and then green, and then blue; and wishes to ascribe these appearances to some affection of the nerves. (Porterfield on the Eye, Vol. I. p. 313.)

M. de la Hire noticed, after staring at the bright sun, that the impression in his eye first looked yellow, then green, and finally blue; he wants to attribute these colors to some sort of nerve reaction. (Porterfield on the Eye, Vol. I. p. 313.)

2. After looking steadily on about an inch square of pink silk, placed on white paper, in a bright sunshine, at the distance of a foot from my eyes, and closing and covering my eyelids, the spectrum of the silk was at first a dark green, and the spectrum of the white paper became of a pink. The spectra then both disappeared; and then the internal spectrum was blue; and then, after a second disappearance, became yellow, and lastly pink, whilst the spectrum of the field varied into red and green.

2. After focusing on about a one-inch square of pink silk, placed on white paper, in bright sunlight and keeping it a foot away from my eyes while closing and covering my eyelids, the spectrum of the silk initially appeared dark green, and the spectrum of the white paper turned pink. Then both spectra vanished; next, the internal spectrum was blue; after a second disappearance, it changed to yellow, and finally pink, while the spectrum of the field shifted to red and green.

These successions of different coloured spectra were not exactly the same in the different experiments, though observed, as near as could be, with the same quantity of light, and other similar circumstances; owing, I suppose, to trying too many experiments at a time; so that the eye was not quite free from the spectra of the colours which were previously attended to.

These series of different colored spectra weren't exactly the same in the various experiments, even though they were observed as closely as possible with the same amount of light and similar conditions. I guess this was due to running too many experiments at once, so the eye wasn't completely free from the colors seen earlier.

The alternate exertions of the retina in the preceding section resembled the oscitation or pandiculation of the muscles, as they were performed in directions contrary to each other, and were the consequence of fatigue rather than of pain. And in this they differ from the successive dissimilar exertions of the retina, mentioned in this section, which resemble in miniature the more violent agitations of the limbs in convulsive diseases, as epilepsy, chorea S. Viti, and opisthotonos; all which diseases are perhaps, at first, the consequence of pain, and have their periods afterwards established by habit.

The alternating activities of the retina described in the previous section were like the stretching or yawning of muscles, as they occurred in opposing directions and were due to fatigue rather than pain. This sets them apart from the different successive activities of the retina mentioned in this section, which are similar to the more intense movements of limbs seen in convulsive disorders like epilepsy, chorea, and opisthotonos; all of these conditions might initially stem from pain, but their patterns are later established by habit.

VIII. The retina, after having been excited into action by a stimulus somewhat greater than the last mentioned falls into a fixed spasmodic action, which continues for some days.

VIII. The retina, after being activated by a trigger that's slightly more intense than the previous one, enters a phase of continuous spasmodic activity that continues for several days.

1. After having looked long at the meridian sun, in making some of the preceding experiments, till the disks faded into a pale blue, I frequently observed a bright blue spectrum of the sun on other objects all the next and the succeeding day, which constantly occurred when I attended to it, and frequently when I did not previously attend to it. When I closed and covered my eyes, this appeared of a dull yellow; and at other times mixed with the colours of other objects on which it was thrown. It may be imagined, that this part of the retina was become insensible to white light, and thence a bluish spectrum became visible on all luminous objects; but as a yellowish spectrum was also seen in the closed and covered eye, there can remain no doubt of this being the spectrum of the sun. A similar appearance was observed by M. Æpinus, which he acknowledges he could give no account of. (Nov. Com. Petrop. V. 10. p. 2. and 6.)

1. After staring at the midday sun for a long time during some of the earlier experiments, until the disks faded into a light blue, I often noticed a bright blue spectrum of the sun on other objects throughout the next day and the day after. This spectrum consistently appeared when I focused on it and often when I didn’t. When I closed and covered my eyes, it looked dull yellow; at other times, it blended with the colors of different objects it was cast onto. One might think that this part of the retina had become insensitive to white light, making the bluish spectrum visible on all bright objects. However, since I also saw a yellowish spectrum with my eyes closed, there's no doubt this was the spectrum of the sun. A similar phenomenon was noted by M. Æpinus, who admitted he couldn’t explain it. (Nov. Com. Petrop. V. 10. p. 2. and 6.)

The locked jaw, and some cataleptic spasms, are resembled by this phenomenon; and from hence we may learn the danger to the eye by inspecting very luminous objects too long a time.

The locked jaw and some cataleptic spasms are similar to this phenomenon; and from this, we can learn about the danger to the eye from looking at very bright objects for too long.

IX. A quantity of stimulus greater than the preceding induces a temporary paralysis of the organ of vision.

IX. A greater amount of stimulus than before causes a temporary paralysis of the eye.

1. Place a circular piece of bright red silk, about half an inch in diameter, on the middle of a sheet of white paper; lay them on the floor in a bright sunshine, and fixing your eyes steadily on the center of the red circle, for three or four minutes, at the distance of four or six feet from the object, the red silk will gradually become paler, and finally cease to appear red at all.

1. Put a circular piece of bright red silk, about half an inch wide, in the center of a sheet of white paper; place them on the floor in bright sunlight, and focusing your eyes steadily on the center of the red circle for three or four minutes, from a distance of four to six feet, the red silk will gradually fade, and eventually you won't see it as red at all.

2. Similar to these are many other animal facts; as purges, opiates, and even poisons, and contagious matter, cease to stimulate our system, after we have been habituated to their use. So some people sleep undisturbed by a clock, or even by a forge hammer in their neighbourhood: and not only continued irritations, but violent exertions of any kind, are succeeded by temporary paralysis. The arm drops down after violent action, and continues for a time useless; and it is probable, that those who have perished suddenly in swimming, or in scating on the ice, have owed their deaths to the paralysis, or extreme fatigue, which succeeds every violent and continued exertion.

2. Similar to this, there are many other animal facts; just like how laxatives, painkillers, and even poisons, along with contagious materials, stop affecting our bodies after we've gotten used to them. Some people sleep soundly through the ticking of a clock or even the sound of a hammer nearby: and not just constant irritations, but also violent physical activities can lead to temporary paralysis. The arm goes limp after intense activity and remains useless for a while; it's likely that those who have drowned unexpectedly while swimming or skating on ice died due to paralysis or extreme exhaustion that follows any intense and prolonged effort.

X. MISCELLANEOUS REMARKS.

X. MISCELLANEOUS REMARKS.

There were some circumstances occurred in making these experiments, which were liable to alter the results of them, and which I shall here mention for the assistance of others, who may wish to repeat them.

There were some circumstances that occurred while conducting these experiments that could change the results, and I’ll mention them here to help others who may want to repeat them.

1. Of direct and inverse spectra existing at the same time; of reciprocal direct spectra; of a combination of direct and inverse spectra; of a spectral halo; rules to pre-determine the colours of spectra.

1. Of direct and inverse spectra existing simultaneously; of reciprocal direct spectra; of a mix of direct and inverse spectra; of a spectral halo; guidelines to predict the colors of spectra.

a. When an area, about six inches square, of bright pink Indian paper, had been viewed on an area, about a foot square, of white writing paper, the internal spectrum in the closed eye was green, being the reverse spectrum of the pink paper; and the external spectrum was pink, being the direct spectrum of the pink paper. The same circumstance happened when the internal area was white, and external one pink; that is, the internal spectrum was pink, and the external one green. All the same appearances occurred when the pink paper was laid on a black hat.

a. When a section about six inches square of bright pink Indian paper was viewed against a section about a foot square of white writing paper, the internal spectrum in the closed eye appeared green, which was the opposite spectrum of the pink paper; while the external spectrum was pink, reflecting the actual spectrum of the pink paper. The same situation occurred when the internal section was white and the external one was pink; in that case, the internal spectrum was pink, and the external one was green. All of these effects were also observed when the pink paper was placed on a black hat.

b. When six inches square of deep violet polished paper was viewed on a foot square of white writing paper, the internal spectrum was yellow, being the reverse spectrum of the violet paper, and the external one was violet, being the direct spectrum of the violet paper.

b. When a six-inch square of deep violet polished paper was placed on a foot square of white writing paper, the inner spectrum appeared yellow, which is the opposite spectrum of the violet paper, while the outer spectrum was violet, reflecting the direct spectrum of the violet paper.

c. When six inches square of pink paper was viewed on a foot square of blue paper, the internal spectrum was blue, and the external spectrum was pink; that is, the internal one was the direct spectrum of the external object, and the external one was the direct spectrum of the internal object, instead of their being each the reverse spectrum of the objects they belonged to.

c. When a six-inch square of pink paper was placed on a square foot of blue paper, the inner spectrum appeared blue, and the outer spectrum appeared pink; in other words, the inner spectrum was the direct spectrum of the external object, and the outer spectrum was the direct spectrum of the internal object, rather than each being the reverse spectrum of the objects they were associated with.

d. When six inches square of blue paper were viewed on a foot square of yellow paper, the interior spectrum became a brilliant yellow, and the exterior one a brilliant blue. The vivacity of the spectra was owing to their being excited both by the stimulus of the interior and exterior objects; so that the interior yellow spectrum was both the reverse spectrum of the blue paper, and the direct one of the yellow paper; and the exterior blue spectrum was both the reverse spectrum of the yellow paper, and the direct one of the blue paper.

d. When a six-inch square of blue paper was placed on a foot square of yellow paper, the inside spectrum appeared a bright yellow, while the outside one looked a bright blue. The intensity of the spectra was due to both the influence of the inner and outer objects; thus, the inner yellow spectrum was both the opposite spectrum of the blue paper and the direct one of the yellow paper, while the outer blue spectrum was both the opposite spectrum of the yellow paper and the direct one of the blue paper.

e. When the internal area was only a square half-inch of red paper, laid on a square foot of dark violet paper, the internal spectrum was green, with a reddish-blue halo. When the red internal paper was two inches square, the internal spectrum was a deeper green, and the external one redder. When the internal paper was six inches square, the spectrum of it became blue, and the spectrum of the external paper was red.

e. When the inside area was just a half-inch square of red paper placed on a square foot of dark violet paper, the internal spectrum was green, with a reddish-blue halo. When the red internal paper was two inches square, the internal spectrum deepened to a richer green, and the external one appeared redder. When the internal paper grew to six inches square, its spectrum shifted to blue, while the spectrum of the external paper remained red.

f. When a square half-inch of blue paper was laid on a six-inch square of yellow paper, the spectrum of the central paper in the closed eye was yellow, incircled with a blue halo. On looking long on the meridian sun, the disk fades into a pale blue surrounded with a whitish halo.

f. When a half-inch square of blue paper was placed on a six-inch square of yellow paper, the color seen in a closed eye was yellow, surrounded by a blue halo. After staring at the midday sun for a while, the disk turns into a light blue with a whitish halo around it.

These circumstances, though they very much perplexed the experiments till they were investigated, admit of a satisfactory explanation; for while the rays from the bright internal object in exp. a. fall with their full force on the center of the retina, and, by fatiguing that part of it, induce the reverse spectrum, many scattered rays, from the same internal pink paper, fall on the more external parts of the retina, but not in such quantity as to occasion much fatigue, and hence induce the direct spectrum of the pink colour in those parts of the eye. The same reverse and direct spectra occur from the violet paper in exp. b.: and in exp. c. the scattered rays from the central pink paper produce a direct spectrum of this colour on the external parts of the eye, while the scattered rays from the external blue paper produce a direct spectrum of that colour on the central part of the eye, instead of these parts of the retina falling reciprocally into their reverse spectra. In exp. d. the colours being the reverse of each other, the scattered rays from the exterior object falling on the central parts of the eye, and there exciting their direct spectrum, at the same time that the retina was excited into a reverse spectrum by the central object, and this direct and reverse spectrum being of similar colour, the superior brilliancy of this spectrum was produced. In exp. e. the effect of various quantities of stimulus on the retina, from the different respective sizes of the internal and external areas, induced a spectrum of the internal area in the center of the eye, combined of the reverse spectrum of that internal area and the direct one of the external area, in various shades of colour, from a pale green to a deep blue, with similar changes in the spectrum of the external area. For the same reasons, when an internal bright object was small, as in exp. f. instead of the whole of the spectrum of the external object being reverse to the colour of the internal object, only a kind of halo, or radiation of colour, similar to that of the internal object, was spread a little way on the external spectrum. For this internal blue area being so small, the scattered rays from it extended but a little way on the image of the external area of yellow paper, and could therefore produce only a blue halo round the yellow spectrum in the center.

These situations, while quite confusing during the experiments until they were examined, can be explained clearly; because while the rays from the bright internal object in exp. a. strike the center of the retina with full intensity, fatiguing that area and creating the reverse spectrum, many scattered rays from the same internal pink paper reach the outer parts of the retina, but not enough to cause significant fatigue, leading to the direct spectrum of the pink color in those areas of the eye. The same reverse and direct spectra are produced from the violet paper in exp. b.: and in exp. c. the scattered rays from the central pink paper create a direct spectrum of this color on the outer parts of the eye, while the scattered rays from the outer blue paper create a direct spectrum of that color on the center of the eye, rather than these areas of the retina yielding their reverse spectra. In exp. d. the colors being opposites, the scattered rays from the outer object reach the center parts of the eye, stimulating their direct spectrum, while at the same time the retina is excited into a reverse spectrum by the central object, and since this direct and reverse spectrum share a similar color, the greater brilliance of this spectrum results. In exp. e. the effect of varying stimulus levels on the retina, due to the different respective sizes of the internal and external areas, resulted in a spectrum of the internal area at the center of the eye, made up of the reverse spectrum of that internal area and the direct one of the external area, in shades of color ranging from pale green to deep blue, accompanied by similar changes in the spectrum of the external area. For the same reasons, when the bright internal object was small, as in exp. f. instead of the entire spectrum of the external object being the reverse of the color of the internal object, only a kind of halo, or radiation of color, similar to that of the internal object, was faintly spread over the external spectrum. Since this internal blue area was so small, the scattered rays from it reached only a short distance on the image of the external yellow paper area, thus only producing a blue halo around the yellow spectrum in the center.

If any one should suspect that the scattered rays from the exterior coloured object do not intermix with the rays from the interior coloured object, and thus affect the central part of the eye, let him look through an opake tube, about two feet in length, and an inch in diameter, at a coloured wall of a room with one eye, and with the other eye naked; and he will find, that by shutting out the lateral light, the area of the wall seen through a tube appears as if illuminated by the sunshine, compared with the other parts of it; from whence arises the advantage of looking through a dark tube at distant paintings.

If anyone doubts that the scattered light from an external colored object doesn’t blend with the light from an internal colored object and thus impact the central part of the eye, let them look through an opaque tube, about two feet long and an inch in diameter, at a colored wall in a room with one eye, while keeping the other eye uncovered; they will see that by blocking out the side light, the section of the wall viewed through the tube appears as if it’s lit by sunshine compared to the other areas. This demonstrates the benefit of looking through a dark tube at distant paintings.

Hence we may safely deduce the following rules to determine before-hand the colours of all spectra. 1. The direct spectrum without any lateral light is an evanescent representation of its object in the unfatigued eye. 2. With some lateral light it becomes of a colour combined of the direct spectrum of the central object, and of the circumjacent objects, in proportion to their respective quantity and brilliancy. 3. The reverse spectrum without lateral light is a representation in the fatigued eye of the form of its objects, with such a colour as would be produced by all the primary colours, except that of the object. 4. With lateral light the colour is compounded of the reverse spectrum of the central object, and the direct spectrum of the circumjacent objects, in proportion to their respective quantity and brilliancy.

Therefore, we can confidently establish the following rules to determine the colors of all spectra beforehand. 1. The direct spectrum, without any side light, is a fleeting representation of its object in an unfatigued eye. 2. With some side light, it becomes a color that combines the direct spectrum of the central object and that of surrounding objects, according to their respective amounts and brightness. 3. The reverse spectrum, without side light, is a representation in the fatigued eye of the shape of its objects, with a color made up of all the primary colors except for that of the object itself. 4. With side light, the color is a mixture of the reverse spectrum of the central object and the direct spectrum of the surrounding objects, based on their respective amounts and brightness.

2. Variation and vivacity of the spectra occasioned by extraneous light.

2. Variation and liveliness of the spectra caused by outside light.

The reverse spectrum, as has been before explained, is similar to a colour, formed by a combination of all the primary colours, except that with which the eye has been fatigued in making the experiment: so the reverse spectrum of red is such a green as would be produced by a combination of all the other prismatic colours. Now it must be observed, that this reverse spectrum of red is therefore the direct spectrum of a combination of all the other prismatic colours, except the red; whence, on removing the eye from a piece of red silk to a sheet of white paper, the green spectrum, which is perceived, may either be called the reverse spectrum of the red silk, or the direct spectrum of all the rays from the white paper, except the red; for in truth it is both. Hence we see the reason why it is not easy to gain a direct spectrum of any coloured object in the day-time, where there is much lateral light, except of very bright objects, as of the setting sun, or by looking through an opake tube; because the lateral external light falling also on the central part of the retina, contributes to induce the reverse spectrum, which is at the same time the direct spectrum of that lateral light, deducting only the colour of the central object which we have been viewing. And for the same reason, it is difficult to gain the reverse spectrum, where there is no lateral light to contribute to its formation. Thus, in looking through an opake tube on a yellow wall, and closing my eye, without admitting any lateral light, the spectra were all at first yellow; but at length changed into blue. And on looking in the same manner on red paper, I did at length get a green spectrum; but they were all at first red ones: and the same after looking at a candle in the night.

The reverse spectrum, as previously explained, is like a color made by mixing all the primary colors, except for the one that has fatigued the eye during the experiment. So, the reverse spectrum of red corresponds to a green that would result from combining all the other prismatic colors. It should be noted that this reverse spectrum of red is essentially the direct spectrum of a combination of all the other prismatic colors, minus the red. Therefore, when you shift your gaze from a piece of red silk to a white sheet of paper, the green spectrum you see can be called either the reverse spectrum of the red silk or the direct spectrum of all the rays from the white paper, excluding the red; in reality, it is both. This explains why it's challenging to get a direct spectrum of any colored object during the day, when there’s a lot of lateral light, except for very bright objects, like the setting sun, or by looking through an opaque tube. The lateral external light also hits the central part of the retina, contributing to the reverse spectrum, which is also the direct spectrum of that lateral light, only minus the color of the central object we're observing. For the same reason, it’s hard to achieve the reverse spectrum when there’s no lateral light to aid in its formation. For instance, when looking through an opaque tube at a yellow wall and closing my eye without allowing any lateral light in, the spectra initially appeared yellow but eventually changed to blue. Similarly, when viewing red paper in the same way, I eventually saw a green spectrum, although they all started as red; the same occurred after looking at a candle at night.

The reverse spectrum was formed with greater facility when the eye was thrown from the object on a sheet of white paper, or when light was admitted through the closed eyelids; because not only the fatigued part of the retina was inclined spontaneously to fall into motions of a contrary direction; but being still sensible to all other rays of light except that with which it was lately fatigued, was by these rays stimulated at the same time into those motions which form the reverse spectrum. Hence, when, the reverse spectrum of any colour became faint, it was wonderfully revived by admitting more light through the eyelids, by removing the hand from before them: and hence, on covering the closed eyelids, the spectrum would often cease for a time, till the retina became sensible to the stimulus of the smaller quantity of light, and then it recurred. Nor was the spectrum only changed in vivacity, or in degree, by this admission of light through the eyelids; but it frequently happened, after having viewed bright objects, that the spectrum in the closed and covered eye was changed into a third spectrum, when light was admitted through the eyelids: which third spectrum was composed of such colours as could pass through the eyelids, except those of the object. Thus, when an area of half an inch diameter of pink paper was viewed on a sheet of white paper in the sunshine, the spectrum with closed and covered eyes was green; but on removing the hands from before the closed eyelids, the spectrum became yellow, and returned instantly again to green, as often as the hands were applied to cover the eyelids, or removed from them: for the retina being now insensible to red light, the yellow rays passing through the eyelids in greater quantity than the other colours, induced a yellow spectrum; whereas if the spectrum was thrown on white paper, with the eyes open, it became only a lighter green.

The reverse spectrum was easier to create when the eye was shifted away from the object onto a sheet of white paper, or when light was allowed in through closed eyelids. This was because the fatigued part of the retina naturally tended to move in the opposite direction. Additionally, since it could still detect all other light rays apart from the one that had just caused fatigue, these rays prompted the retina into motions that created the reverse spectrum. So, when the reverse spectrum of any color started to fade, it was remarkably revived by letting more light in through the eyelids or by removing the hand that was covering them. Conversely, covering the closed eyelids often made the spectrum disappear for a while until the retina became sensitive to the smaller amount of light, at which point it would reappear. Furthermore, the spectrum not only changed in intensity or degree with this admission of light through the eyelids; it often happened that after looking at bright objects, the spectrum in the closed and covered eye transformed into a third spectrum when light came through the eyelids. This third spectrum consisted of colors that could penetrate the eyelids, except for the colors of the object. For instance, when a half-inch area of pink paper was viewed against a white sheet in the sunshine, the spectrum with closed and covered eyes appeared green; but when the hands were removed from the closed eyelids, the spectrum changed to yellow and instantly returned to green whenever the hands were applied to cover or were removed from the eyelids. This occurred because the retina was now insensitive to red light, and the yellow rays passing through the eyelids in larger amounts than the other colors produced a yellow spectrum; whereas, if the spectrum was directed onto white paper with the eyes open, it would only show a lighter green.

Though a certain quantity of light facilitates the formation of the reverse spectrum, a greater quantity prevents its formation, as the more powerful stimulus excites even the fatigued parts of the eye into action; otherwise we should see the spectrum of the last viewed object as often as we turn our eyes. Hence the reverse spectra are best seen by gradually approaching the hand near the closed eyelids to a certain distance only, which must be varied with the brightness of the day, or the energy of the spectrum. Add to this, that all dark spectra, as black, blue, or green, if light be admitted through the eyelids, after they have been some time covered, give reddish spectra, for the reasons given in Sect. III. Exp. 1.

Though a certain quantity of light facilitates the formation of the reverse spectrum, a greater quantity prevents its formation, as the more powerful stimulus excites even the fatigued parts of the eye into action; otherwise we should see the spectrum of the last viewed object as often as we turn our eyes. Hence the reverse spectra are best seen by gradually approaching the hand near the closed eyelids to a certain distance only, which must be varied with the brightness of the day, or the energy of the spectrum. Add to this, that all dark spectra, as black, blue, or green, if light be admitted through the eyelids, after they have been some time covered, give reddish spectra, for the reasons given in Sect. III. Exp. 1.

From these circumstances of the extraneous light coinciding with the spontaneous efforts of the fatigued retina to produce a reverse spectrum, as was observed before, it is not easy to gain a direct spectrum, except of objects brighter than the ambient light; such as a candle in the night, the setting sun, or viewing a bright object through an opake tube; and then the reverse spectrum is instantaneously produced by the admission of some external light; and is as instantly converted again to the direct spectrum by the exclusion of it. Thus, on looking at the setting sun, on closing the eyes, and covering them, a yellow spectrum is seen, which is the direct spectrum of the setting sun; but on opening the eyes on the sky, the yellow spectrum is immediately changed into a blue one, which is the reverse spectrum of the yellow sun, or the direct spectrum of the blue sky, or a combination of both. And this is again transformed into a yellow one on closing the eyes, and so reciprocally, as quick as the motions of the opening and closing eyelids. Hence, when Mr. Melvill observed the scintillations of the star Sirius to be sometimes coloured, these were probably the direct spectrum of the blue sky on the parts of the retina fatigued by the white light of the star. (Essays Physical and Literary, p. 81. V. 2.)

From these circumstances of the external light coinciding with the natural attempts of the tired retina to create a reverse spectrum, as noted earlier, it isn’t easy to obtain a direct spectrum, except from objects that are brighter than the surrounding light; such as a candle at night, the setting sun, or viewing a bright object through an opaque tube; and then the reverse spectrum is instantly produced when some external light is allowed in; and it is just as quickly changed back to the direct spectrum when the external light is blocked. For example, when looking at the setting sun, if you close and cover your eyes, you see a yellow spectrum, which is the direct spectrum of the setting sun; but when you open your eyes to the sky, the yellow spectrum immediately shifts to a blue one, which is the reverse spectrum of the yellow sun, or the direct spectrum of the blue sky, or a mix of both. This yellow spectrum transforms back again when you close your eyes, and so it continues to switch back and forth as fast as you can open and close your eyelids. Therefore, when Mr. Melvill noticed that the star Sirius sometimes appeared colorful, these colors were likely the direct spectrum of the blue sky affecting the parts of the retina that were exhausted by the star's white light. (Essays Physical and Literary, p. 81. V. 2.)

When a direct spectrum is thrown on colours darker than itself, it mixes with them; as the yellow spectrum of the setting sun, thrown on the green grass, becomes a greener yellow. But when a direct spectrum is thrown on colours brighter than itself, it becomes instantly changed into the reverse spectrum, which mixes with those brighter colours. So the yellow spectrum of the setting sun thrown on the luminous sky becomes blue, and changes with the colour or brightness of the clouds on which it appears. But the reverse spectrum mixes with every kind of colour on which it is thrown, whether brighter than itself or not; thus the reverse spectrum, obtained by viewing a piece of yellow silk, when thrown on white paper, was a lucid blue green; when thrown on black Turkey leather, becomes a deep violet. And the spectrum of blue silk, thrown on white paper, was a light yellow; on black silk was an obscure orange; and, the blue spectrum, obtained from orange-coloured silk, thrown on yellow, became a green.

When a direct light spectrum is cast onto colors that are darker than it, it blends with them; for example, the yellow light from the setting sun on green grass turns into a greener yellow. However, when a direct light spectrum is cast onto colors that are brighter than it, it quickly turns into the reverse spectrum, which mixes with those brighter colors. So, the yellow light from the setting sun hitting the bright sky turns blue and shifts depending on the color or brightness of the clouds it reflects on. The reverse spectrum mixes with all kinds of colors that it hits, whether they are brighter or not; for instance, the reverse spectrum that you get from looking at a piece of yellow silk, when projected onto white paper, appears as a bright blue-green; when cast onto black leather, it turns into a deep violet. And the spectrum from blue silk, when thrown on white paper, looks like a light yellow; on black silk, it appears as a dark orange; and the blue spectrum generated from orange silk, when cast onto yellow, turns green.

In these cases the retina is thrown into activity or sensation by the stimulus of external colours, at the same time that it continues the activity or sensation which forms the spectra; in the same manner as the prismatic colours, painted on a whirling top, are seen to mix together. When these colours of external objects are brighter than the direct spectrum which is thrown upon them, they change it into the reverse spectrum, like the admission of external light on a direct spectrum, as explained above. When they are darker than the direct spectrum, they mix with it, their weaker stimulus being inefficient to induce the reverse spectrum.

In these cases, the retina is activated or experiences sensations from the external colors, while still continuing to process the sensations that create the afterimages; much like how the prismatic colors painted on a spinning top appear to blend together. When these external colors are brighter than the direct spectrum cast upon them, they transform it into the opposite spectrum, similar to allowing external light into a direct spectrum, as previously discussed. When they are darker than the direct spectrum, they combine with it, their weaker stimulus not being strong enough to trigger the opposite spectrum.

3. Variation of spectra in respect to number, and figure, and remission.

3. Change in spectra regarding quantity, shape, and reflection.

Fig. 4. Fig. 4.

When we look long and attentively at any object, the eye cannot always be kept entirely motionless; hence, on inspecting a circular area of red silk placed on white paper, a lucid crescent or edge is seen to librate on one side or other of the red circle: for the exterior parts of the retina sometimes falling on the edge of the central silk, and sometimes on the white paper, are less fatigued with red light than the central part of the retina, which is constantly, exposed to it; and therefore, when they fall on the edge of the red silk, they perceive it more vividly. Afterwards, when the eye becomes fatigued, a green spectrum in the form of a crescent is seen to librate on one side or other of the central circle, as by the unsteadiness of the eye a part of the fatigued retina falls on the white paper; and as by the increasing fatigue of the eye the central part of the silk appears paler, the edge on which the unfatigued part of the retina occasionally falls will appear of a deeper red than the original silk, because it is compared with the pale internal part of it. M. de Buffon in making this experiment observed, that the red edge of the silk was not only deeper coloured than the original silk; but, on his retreating a little from it, it became oblong, and at length divided into two, which must have been owing to his observing it either before or behind the point of intersection of the two optic axises. Thus, if a pen is held up before a distant candle, when we look intensely at the pen two candles are seen behind it; when we look intensely at the candle two pens are seen. If the sight be unsteady at the time of beholding the sun, even though one eye only be used, many images of the sun will appear, or luminous lines, when the eye is closed. And as some parts of these will be more vivid than others, and some parts of them will be produced nearer the center of the eye than others, these will disappear sooner than the others; and hence the number and shape of these spectra of the sun will continually vary, as long as they exist. The cause of some being more vivid than others, is the unsteadiness of the eye of the beholder, so that some parts of the retina have been longer exposed to the sunbeams. That some parts of a complicated spectrum fade and return before other parts of it, the following experiment evinces. Draw three concentric circles; the external one an inch and a half in diameter, the middle one an inch, and the internal one half an inch; colour the external and internal areas blue, and the remaining one yellow, as in Fig. 4.; after having looked about a minute on the center of these circles, in a bright light, the spectrum of the external area appears first in the closed eye, then the middle area, and lastly the central one; and then the central one disappears, and the others in inverted order. If concentric circles of more colours are added, it produces the beautiful ever changing spectrum in Sect. I. Exp. 2.

When we look long and attentively at any object, the eye cannot always be kept entirely motionless; hence, on inspecting a circular area of red silk placed on white paper, a lucid crescent or edge is seen to librate on one side or other of the red circle: for the exterior parts of the retina sometimes falling on the edge of the central silk, and sometimes on the white paper, are less fatigued with red light than the central part of the retina, which is constantly, exposed to it; and therefore, when they fall on the edge of the red silk, they perceive it more vividly. Afterwards, when the eye becomes fatigued, a green spectrum in the form of a crescent is seen to librate on one side or other of the central circle, as by the unsteadiness of the eye a part of the fatigued retina falls on the white paper; and as by the increasing fatigue of the eye the central part of the silk appears paler, the edge on which the unfatigued part of the retina occasionally falls will appear of a deeper red than the original silk, because it is compared with the pale internal part of it. M. de Buffon in making this experiment observed, that the red edge of the silk was not only deeper coloured than the original silk; but, on his retreating a little from it, it became oblong, and at length divided into two, which must have been owing to his observing it either before or behind the point of intersection of the two optic axises. Thus, if a pen is held up before a distant candle, when we look intensely at the pen two candles are seen behind it; when we look intensely at the candle two pens are seen. If the sight be unsteady at the time of beholding the sun, even though one eye only be used, many images of the sun will appear, or luminous lines, when the eye is closed. And as some parts of these will be more vivid than others, and some parts of them will be produced nearer the center of the eye than others, these will disappear sooner than the others; and hence the number and shape of these spectra of the sun will continually vary, as long as they exist. The cause of some being more vivid than others, is the unsteadiness of the eye of the beholder, so that some parts of the retina have been longer exposed to the sunbeams. That some parts of a complicated spectrum fade and return before other parts of it, the following experiment evinces. Draw three concentric circles; the external one an inch and a half in diameter, the middle one an inch, and the internal one half an inch; colour the external and internal areas blue, and the remaining one yellow, as in Fig. 4.; after having looked about a minute on the center of these circles, in a bright light, the spectrum of the external area appears first in the closed eye, then the middle area, and lastly the central one; and then the central one disappears, and the others in inverted order. If concentric circles of more colours are added, it produces the beautiful ever changing spectrum in Sect. I. Exp. 2.

From hence it would seem, that the center of the eye produces quicker remissions of spectra, owing perhaps to its greater sensibility; that is, to its more energetic exertions. These remissions of spectra bear some analogy to the tremors of the hands, and palpitations of the heart, of weak people: and perhaps a criterion of the strength of any muscle or nerve may be taken from the time it can be continued in exertion.

From this, it appears that the center of the eye produces faster fading of images, possibly due to its greater sensitivity; that is, because of its more vigorous activity. These fading images are somewhat similar to the tremors of the hands and the palpitations of the heart found in weaker individuals: and perhaps a measure of the strength of any muscle or nerve can be determined by how long it can maintain exertion.

4. Variation of spectra in respect to brilliancy; the visibility of the circulation of the blood in the eye.

4. Changes in spectra based on brightness; the visibility of blood flow in the eye.

1. The meridian or evening light makes a difference in the colours of some spectra; for as the sun descends, the red rays, which are less refrangible by the convex atmosphere, abound in great quantity. Whence the spectrum of the light parts of a window at this time, or early in the morning, is red; and becomes blue either a little later or earlier; and white in the meridian day; and is also variable from the colour of the clouds or sky which are opposed to the window.

1. The evening light changes the colors of some spectra; as the sun sets, the red rays, which bend less in the curved atmosphere, are much more prominent. This is why the light coming through a window during this time, or early in the morning, appears red; it turns blue either a bit later or earlier; and it’s white during midday. The color can also change based on the color of the clouds or sky outside the window.

2. All these experiments are liable to be confounded, if they are made too soon after each other, as the remaining spectrum will mix with the new ones. This is a very troublesome circumstance to painters, who are obliged to look long upon the same colour; and in particular to those whose eyes, from natural debility, cannot long, continue the same kind of exertion. For the same reason, in making these experiments, the result becomes much varied if the eyes, after viewing any object, are removed on other objects for but an instant of time, before we close them to view the spectrum; for the light from the object, of which we had only a transient view, in the very time of closing our eyes acts as a stimulus on the fatigued retina; and for a time prevents the defined spectrum from appearing, or mixes its own spectrum with it. Whence, after the eyelids are closed, either a dark field, or some unexpected colours, are beheld for a few seconds, before the desired spectrum becomes distinctly visible.

2. All these experiments can get mixed up if they're done too quickly after each other since the leftover spectrum will blend with the new ones. This is a real hassle for painters, who have to focus on the same color for a long time; it's especially challenging for those whose eyes, due to natural weakness, can't sustain that kind of strain for very long. For the same reason, when doing these experiments, the results change a lot if your eyes, after looking at any object, shift to other objects for even just a moment before you close them to see the spectrum. The light from the object we only glanced at acts as a stimulus on the tired retina at the moment we close our eyes, which temporarily prevents the defined spectrum from showing up or mixes its own spectrum in. Therefore, after closing the eyelids, you either see a dark field or some unexpected colors for a few seconds before the desired spectrum becomes clearly visible.

3. The length of time taken up in viewing an object, of which we are to observe the spectrum, makes a great difference in the appearance of the spectrum, not only in its vivacity, but in its colour; as the direct spectrum of the central object, or of the circumjacent ones, and also the reverse spectra of both, with their various combinations, as well as the time of their duration in the eye, and of their remissions or alternations, depend upon the degree of fatigue the retina is subjected to. The Chevalier d'Arcy constructed a machine by which a coal of fire was whirled round in the dark, and found, that when a luminous body made a revolution in eight thirds of time, it presented to the eye a complete circle of fire; from whence he concludes, that the impression continues on the organ about the seventh part of a second. (Mem. de l'Acad. des Sc. 1765.) This, however, is only to be considered as the shortest time of the duration of these direct spectra; since in the fatigued eye both the direct and reverse spectra, with their intermissions, appear to take up many seconds of time, and seem very variable in proportion to the circumstances of fatigue or energy.

3. The amount of time spent looking at an object from which we want to observe the spectrum significantly affects the appearance of that spectrum, not just in how vivid it is but also in its color. This includes the direct spectrum of the main object and those around it, as well as the reverse spectra of both, along with their various combinations. The duration they last in our vision, and any fading or alternating effects, depend on how fatigued the retina is. The Chevalier d'Arcy created a device that spun a piece of coal in the dark and discovered that when a light source made a complete rotation in eight-thirds of a second, it created a complete circle of fire for the eye. He concluded that the impression lingers on the eye for about one-seventh of a second. (Mem. de l'Acad. des Sc. 1765.) However, this should be considered the shortest duration for these direct spectra, as in a fatigued eye, both the direct and reverse spectra, along with their pauses, can last for several seconds and seem quite variable based on the level of fatigue or energy.

4. It sometimes happens, if the eyeballs have been rubbed hard with the fingers, that lucid sparks are seen in quick motion amidst the spectrum we are attending to. This is similar to the flashes of fire from a stroke on the eye in fighting, and is resembled by the warmth and glow, which appears upon the skin after friction, and is probably owing to an acceleration of the arterial blood into the vessels emptied by the previous pressure. By being accustomed to observe such small sensations in the eye, it is easy to see the circulation of the blood in this organ. I have attended to this frequently, when I have observed my eyes more than commonly sensible to other spectra. The circulation may be seen either in both eyes at a time, or only in one of them; for as a certain quantity of light is necessary to produce this curious phenomenon, if one hand be brought nearer the closed eyelids than the other, the circulation in that eye will for a time disappear. For the easier viewing the circulation, it is sometimes necessary to rub the eyes with a certain degree of force after they are closed, and to hold the breath rather longer than is agreeable, which, by accumulating more blood in the eye, facilitates the experiment; but in general it may be seen distinctly after having examined other spectra with your back to the light, till the eyes become weary; then having covered your closed eyelids for half a minute, till the spectrum is faded away which you were examining, turn your face to the light, and removing your hands from the eyelids, by and by again shade them a little, and the circulation becomes curiously distinct. The streams of blood are however generally seen to unite, which shews it to be the venous circulation, owing, I suppose, to the greater opacity of the colour of the blood in these vessels; for this venous circulation is also much more easily seen by the microscope in the tail of a tadpole.

4. Sometimes, if you rub your eyeballs hard with your fingers, you might see bright sparks moving quickly amid the colors you're focusing on. This is similar to the flashes you see after getting hit in the eye during a fight and is like the warmth and redness that appears on your skin after rubbing it, probably because more arterial blood flows into the vessels that were previously compressed. By getting used to noticing these small sensations in your eyes, it's easy to observe the blood circulation in this organ. I've paid attention to this often, especially when my eyes are more sensitive to different colors. You can see the circulation in both eyes at once or just one. If one hand is closer to your closed eyelids than the other, the circulation in that eye will temporarily disappear due to the amount of light needed to create this interesting effect. To view the circulation more easily, it can help to rub your eyes gently with a bit of pressure while they are closed and to hold your breath a bit longer than comfortable, as this brings more blood into the eye and aids the experiment. Generally, though, you can see it clearly after examining other colors with your back to the light until your eyes get tired. Then, cover your closed eyelids for half a minute until the colors you were looking at fade away, turn your face to the light, and after removing your hands from your eyelids, shade them slightly again, and you'll see the circulation clearly. The blood streams usually seem to combine, indicating it's the venous circulation, probably because the blood in these vessels is darker and less transparent; this venous circulation is also much easier to observe under a microscope in a tadpole's tail.

5. Variation of spectra in respect to distinctness and size; with a new way of magnifying objects.

5. Changes in spectra regarding clarity and size; along with a new method for enlarging objects.

1. It was before observed, that when the two colours viewed together were opposite to each other, as yellow and blue, red and green, &c. according to the table of reflections and transmissions of light in Sir Isaac Newton's Optics, B. II. Fig. 3. the spectra of those colours were of all others the most brilliant, and best defined; because they were combined of the reverse spectrum of one colour, and of the direct spectrum of the other. Hence, in books printed with small types, or in the minute graduation of thermometers, or of clock-faces, which are to be seen at a distance, if the letters or figures are coloured with orange, and the ground with indigo; or the letters with red, and the ground with green; or any other lucid colour is used for the letters, the spectrum of which is similar to the colour of the ground; such letters will be seen much more distinctly, and with less confusion, than in black or white: for as the spectrum of the letter is the same colour with the ground on which they are seen, the unsteadiness of the eye in long attending to them will not produce coloured lines by the edges of the letters, which is the principal cause of their confusion. The beauty of colours lying in vicinity to each other, whose spectra are thus reciprocally similar to each colour, is owing to this greater ease that the eye experiences in beholding them distinctly; and it is probable, in the organ of hearing, a similar circumstance may constitute the pleasure of melody. Sir Isaac Newton observes, that gold and indigo were agreeable when viewed together; and thinks there may be some analogy between the sensations of light and sound. (Optics, Qu. 14.)

1. It was previously noted that when two colors are viewed together and are opposites, like yellow and blue, or red and green, according to the table of reflections and transmissions of light in Sir Isaac Newton's Optics, B. II. Fig. 3, the spectra of these colors are the brightest and most well-defined of all. This is because they combine the reverse spectrum of one color with the direct spectrum of the other. Therefore, in books printed with small fonts, or in the fine gradations of thermometers or clock faces that need to be viewed from a distance, if the letters or numbers are colored orange with an indigo background, or red with a green background, or use any other bright color for the letters that resembles the color of the background, those letters will be much easier to see and less confusing than if they were black or white. This is because when the color of the letters matches the background, the eye's instability when focusing on them for a long time won't create colored edges around the letters, which is a main factor in their confusion. The aesthetics of colors that are close to one another, whose spectra are similarly related, is due to the greater ease with which the eye can observe them clearly; and it’s likely that a similar concept exists in the ear when it comes to the enjoyment of melody. Sir Isaac Newton noted that gold and indigo looked nice together and believed there might be some parallel between the sensations of light and sound. (Optics, Qu. 14.)

In viewing the spectra of bright objects, as of an area of red silk of half an inch diameter on white paper, it is easy to magnify it to tenfold its size: for if, when the spectrum is formed, you still keep your eye fixed on the silk area, and remove it a few inches further from you, a green circle is seen round the red silk: for the angle now subtended by the silk is less than it was when the spectrum was formed, but that of the spectrum continues the same, and our imagination places them at the same distance. Thus when you view a spectrum on a sheet of white paper, if you approach the paper to the eye, you may diminish it to a point; and if the paper is made to recede from the eye, the spectrum will appear magnified in proportion to the distance.

When looking at the spectra of bright objects, like a small area of red silk that’s half an inch in diameter on white paper, it's easy to enlarge it to ten times its size. If you focus on the silk area while the spectrum is being formed and then move the silk a few inches away from you, you’ll see a green circle around the red silk. This happens because the angle created by the silk is smaller than it was when the spectrum formed, but the spectrum's angle remains the same, and our minds assume they're at the same distance. So, when you observe a spectrum on a white sheet of paper, if you bring the paper closer to your eye, it may shrink down to a point. Conversely, if you move the paper away from your eye, the spectrum will appear larger in relation to the distance.

Fig. 5. Fig. 5.

I was surprised, and agreeably amused, with the following experiment. I covered a paper about four inches square with yellow, and with a pen filled with a blue colour wrote upon the middle of it the word BANKS in capitals, as in Fig. 5, and sitting with my back to the sun, fixed my eyes for a minute exactly on the center of the letter N in the middle of the word; after closing my eyes, and shading them somewhat with my hand, the word was distinctly seen in the spectrum in yellow letters on a blue field; and then, on opening my eyes on a yellowish wall at twenty feet distance, the magnified name of BANKS appeared written on the wall in golden characters.

I was surprised and pleasantly entertained by the following experiment. I took a square piece of paper about four inches across and covered it with yellow. Then, using a pen filled with blue ink, I wrote the word BANKS in all caps in the center of it, as shown in Fig. 5. Sitting with my back to the sun, I focused my gaze on the center of the letter N for one minute. After closing my eyes and shading them a bit with my hand, I could clearly see the word in yellow letters against a blue background in the spectrum. When I opened my eyes to look at a yellowish wall about twenty feet away, the name BANKS appeared magnified and written across the wall in golden letters.

Conclusion.

Conclusion.

It was observed by the learned M. Sauvage (Nosol. Method. Cl. VIII. Ord. i.) that the pulsations of the optic artery might be perceived by looking attentively on a white wall well illuminated. A kind of net-work, darker than the other parts of the wall, appears and vanishes alternately with every pulsation. This change of the colour of the wall he well ascribes to the compression of the retina by the diastole of the artery. The various colours produced in the eye by the pressure of the finger, or by a stroke on it, as mentioned by Sir Isaac Newton, seem likewise to originate from the unequal pressure on various parts of the retina. Now as Sir Isaac Newton has shewn, that all the different colours are reflected or transmitted by the laminæ of soap bubbles, or of air, according to their different thickness or thinness, is it not probable, that the effect of the activity of the retina may be to alter its thickness or thinness, so as better to adapt it to reflect or transmit the colours which stimulate it into action? May not muscular fibres exist in the retina for this purpose, which may be less minute than the locomotive muscles of microscopic animals? May not these muscular actions of the retina constitute the sensation of light and colours; and the voluntary repetitions of them, when the object is withdrawn, constitute our memory of them? And lastly, may not the laws of the sensations of light, here investigated, be applicable to all our other senses, and much contribute to elucidate many phenomena of animal bodies both in their healthy and diseased state; and thus render this investigation well worthy the attention of the physician, the metaphysician, and the natural philosopher?

It was noted by the knowledgeable M. Sauvage (Nosol. Method. Cl. VIII. Ord. i.) that you can observe the pulsations of the optic artery by looking closely at a bright white wall. A kind of darker pattern appears and disappears with each pulse. He attributes this change in the wall's color to the compression of the retina caused by the artery's diastole. The different colors seen in the eye when pressure is applied, such as by a finger or a quick tap, as mentioned by Sir Isaac Newton, also seem to come from the uneven pressure on various parts of the retina. Now, as Sir Isaac Newton has shown, all the different colors are reflected or transmitted by the layers of soap bubbles or air, depending on their thickness. Isn't it likely that the activity of the retina changes its thickness to better reflect or transmit the colors that activate it? Might there be muscle fibers in the retina for this purpose that are even smaller than the movement muscles of microscopic creatures? Do these muscle actions in the retina create the sensation of light and color, and when repeated voluntarily after an object is gone, do they create our memory of them? Finally, could the principles of light sensation studied here apply to all our other senses and help clarify many phenomena in animal bodies, whether healthy or diseased? This investigation seems to be worth the attention of physicians, philosophers, and natural scientists alike.

November 1, 1785.

November 1, 1785.



Dum, Liber! astra petis volitans trepidantibus alis,

Dum, Liber! You soar to the stars with your trembling wings,

Irruis immemori, parvula gutta, mari.

Irruis immemori, parvula gutta, mari.

Me quoque, me currente rotâ revolubilis ætas

Me too, me running on the spinning wheel of time.

Volverit in tenebras,—i, Liber, ipse sequor.

Volverit in tenebras,—i, Liber, I follow myself.



INDEX TO THE SECTIONS OF PART FIRST.

A.

A.

Abortion from fear, xxxix. 6. 5.

Abortion due to fear, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Absorbent vessels, xxiii. 3. xxix. 1.

Absorbent containers, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— regurgitate their fluids, xxix. 2.

regurgitate their fluids, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— their valves, xxix. 2.

—— their valves, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— communicate with vena portarum, xxvii. 2.

communicate with portal vein, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Absorption of solids, xxxiii. 3. 1. xxxvii.

Absorption of solids, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— of fluids in anasarca, xxxv. 1. 3.

—— of fluids in swelling, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Accumulation of sensorial power, iv. 2. xii. 5. 2.

Building sensory power, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Activity of system too great, cure of, xii. 6.

Activity of system too great, cure of, xii. 6.

—— too small, cure of, xii. 7.

too small, cure for, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Age, old, xii. 3. 1. xxxvii. 4.

Age, elderly, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Ague fit, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— how cured by bark, xii. 3. 4.

—— how healed by bark, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— periods, how occasioned, xii. 2. 3. xxxii. 3. 4.

—— periods, how they happened, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Ague cakes, xxxii. 7. xxxii. 9.

Ague cakes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Air, sense of fresh, xiv. 8.

Fresh air, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— injures ulcers, xxviii. 2.

injures ulcers, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— injected into veins, xxxii. 5.

—— injected into veins, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Alcohol deleterious, xxx. 3.

Alcohol harmful, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Alliterations, why agreeable, xxii. 2.

Alliterations, why it's cool, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Aloes in lessened doses, xii. 3. 1.

Aloes in smaller doses, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

American natives indolent, xxxi. 2.

American natives lazy, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— narrow shouldered, xxxi. 1.

narrow-shouldered, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Analogy intuitive, xvii. 3. 7.

Intuitive analogy, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Animals less liable to madness, xxxiii. 1.

Animals less prone to madness, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— less liable to contagion, xxxiii. 1.

less likely to catch infections, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— how to teach, xxii. 3. 2.

how to teach, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— their similarity to each other, xxxix. 4. 8.

—— their similarity to each other, xxxix. 4. 8.

—— their changes after nativity, xxxix. 4. 8.

—— their changes after birth, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— their changes before nativity, xxxix. 4. 8.

—— their changes before birth, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— less liable to contagious diseases, why, xxxiii. 1. 5.

—— less liable to contagious diseases, why, xxxiii. 1. 5.

—— less liable to delirium and insanity, why, xxxiii. 1. 5.

—— less liable to delirium and insanity, why, xxxiii. 1. 5.

—— easier to preserve than to reproduce, xxxvii.

—— easier to preserve than to reproduce, xxxvii.

—— food, distaste of, xxviii. 1.

food aversion, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— appetency, xxxix. 4. 7.

Desire, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Antipathy, x. 2. 2.

Dislike, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Aphthæ, xxviii.

Aphthae, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Apoplexy, xxxiv. 1. 7.

Stroke, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— not from deficient irritation, xxxii. 2. 1.

—— not from lack of irritation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Appetites, xi. 2. 2. xiv. 8.

Appetites, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Architecture, xxii. 2. xvi. 10.

Architecture, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Arts, fine, xxii. 2.

Fine arts, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Asparagus, its smell in urine, xxix.

Asparagus, its scent in urine, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Association defined, ii. 2. 11. iv. 7. v. 2.

Association defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— associate motions, x.

associate movements, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— stronger than irritative ones, xxiv. 2. 8.

—— stronger than annoying ones, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— formed before nativity, xi. 3.

—— formed before birth, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— with irritative ones, xxiv. 2. 8.

—— with annoying ones, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— with retrograde ones, xxv. 7. xxv. 10. xxv. 15.

—— with retrograde ones, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— diseases from, xxxv.

diseases from, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Asthma, xviii. 15.

Asthma, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Attention, language of, xvi. 8. 6.

Attention, language of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Atrophy, xxviii.

Atrophy, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Aversion, origin of, xi. 2. 3.

Aversion, its origin, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

B.

B.

Balance ourselves by vision, xx. 1.

Balance ourselves by vision, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Bandage increases absorption, xxxiii. 3. 2.

Bandage boosts absorption, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Barrenness, xxxvi. 2. 3.

Barrenness, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Battement of sounds, xx. 7.

Sound waves, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Bath, cold. See Cold Bath.

Cold bath. See Cold Bath.

Beauty, sense of, xvi. 6. xxii. 2.

Beauty, sense of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Bile-ducts, xxx.

Bile ducts, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— stones, xxx. 1. 3.

—— stones, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— regurgitates into the blood, xxiv. 2. 7.

—— regurgitates into the blood, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— vomiting of, xxx. 1. 3.

vomiting of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Birds of passage, xvi. 12.

Migratory birds, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— nests of, xvi. 13.

nests of __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— colour of their eggs, xxxix. 5.

—— color of their eggs, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Biting in pain, xxxiv. 1. 3.

Biting down in pain, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of mad animals, xxxiv. 1. 3.

—— of crazy animals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Black spots on dice appear red, xl. 3.

Black spots on dice appear red, xl. 3.

Bladder, communication of with the intestines, xxix. 3.

Bladder, communication of with the intestines, xxix. 3.

—— of fish, xxiv. 1. 4.

—— of fish, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Blood, transfusion of in nervous fevers, xxxii. 4.

Blood, transfusion of in nervous fevers, xxxii. 4.

—— deficiency of, xxxii. 2. and 4.

lack of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— from the vena portarum into the intestines, xxvii. 2.

—— from the vena portarum into the intestines, xxvii. 2.

—— its momentum, xxxii. 5. 2.

—— its momentum, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— momentum increased by venesection, xxxii. 5. 4.

momentum increased by bloodletting, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— drawn in nervous pains, xxxii. 5. 4.

—— drawn in anxious pain, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— its oxygenation, xxxviii.

—— its oxygenation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Breasts of men, xiv. 8.

Men's breasts, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Breathing, how learnt, xvi. 4.

Breathing, how to learn, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Brutes differ from men, xi. 2. 3. xvi. 17.

Brutes differ from humans, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Brutes. See Animals.

Brutes. See Animals.

Buxton bath, why it feels warm, xii. 2. 1. xxxii. 3. 3.

Buxton bath, why it feels warm, xii. 2. 1. xxxii. 3. 3.

C.

C.

Capillary vessels are glands, xxvi. 1.

Capillary vessels are glands, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Catalepsy, xxxiv. 1. 5.

Catalepsy, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Catarrh from cold skin, xxxv. 1. 3. xxxv. 2. 3.

Cold skin catarrh, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— from thin caps in sleep, xviii. 15.

—— from thin caps in sleep, xviii. 15.

Catenation of motions defined, ii. 2. 11. iv. 7.

Defined motion sequences, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— cause of them, xvii. 1. 3.

—— cause of them, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— described, xvii.

—— described, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— continue some time after their production, xvii. 1. 3.

—— continue some time after their production, xvii. 1. 3.

—— voluntary ones dissevered in sleep, xvii. 1. 12. xvii. 3. 7.

—— voluntary ones dissevered in sleep, xvii. 1. 12. xvii. 3. 7.

Cathartics, external, their operation, xxix. 7. 6.

Cathartics, external, their function, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Causation, animal, defined, ii. 2. 11. iv. 7.

Causation, animal, defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Cause of causes, xxxix. 4. 8.

Cause of causes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Causes inert and efficient, xxxix. 8. 2.

Causes inactive and effective, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— active and passive, xxxix. 8. 3.

active and passive, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— proximate and remote, xxxix. 8. 4.

— nearby and far, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Chick in the egg, oxygenation of, xxxviii. 2.

Chick in the egg, oxygenation of, xxxviii. 2.

Child riding on a stick, xxxiv. 2. 6.

Kid riding a stick, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Chilness after meals, xxi. 3. xxxv. 1. 1.

Chill after meals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Cholera, case of, xxv. 13.

Cholera case, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Circulation in the eye visible, xl. 10. 4.

Eye circulation visible, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Cold in the head, xii. 6. 5.

Cold in the head, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— perceived by the teeth, xxxii. 3. 1. xiv. 6.

—— sensed by the teeth, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— air, uses of in fevers, xxxii. 3. 3.

—— air, uses of in fevers, xxxii. 3. 3.

—— feet, produces coryza, xxxv. 2. 3. xxxv. 1. 3.

—— feet, causes cold symptoms, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— bath, why it strengthens, xxxii. 3. 2.

—— bath, its benefits, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— short and cold breathing in it, xxxii. 3. 2.

—— short and cold breathing in it, xxxii. 3. 2.

—— produces a fever-fit, xxxii. 3. 2.

—— causes a fever fit, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— fit of fever the consequence of hot fit, xxxii. 9. 3.

—— fit of fever the consequence of hot fit, xxxii. 9. 3.

—— bathing in pulmonary hæmorrhage, xxvii. 1.

—— bathing in pulmonary hemorrhage, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— fits of fever, xxxii. 4. xxxii. 9. xvii. 3. 3.

—— fever spikes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

Colours of animals, efficient cause of, xxxix. 5. 1.

Colours of animals, efficient cause of, xxxix. 5. 1.

—— of eggs from female imagination, xxxix. 5. 1.

—— of eggs from female imagination, xxxix. 5. 1.

—— of the choroid coat of the eye, xxxix. 5. 1.

—— of the choroid coat of the eye, xxxix. 5. 1.

—— of birds nests, xvi. 13.

—— of bird nests, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Comparing ideas, xv. 3.

Comparing ideas, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Consciousness, xv. 3. 4.

Consciousness, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— in dreams, xviii. 13.

—— in dreams, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Consent of parts. See Sympathy.

Consent of parts. See Sympathy.

Consumption, its temperament, xxxi. 1. and 2.

Consumption, its vibe, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— of dark-eyed patients, xxvii. 2.

—— of dark-eyed patients, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of light-eyed patients, xxviii. 2.

—— of light-eyed patients, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— is contagious, xxxiii. 2. 7.

—— is viral, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Contagion, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_4__.

—— does not enter the blood, xxxiii. 2. 10. xxii. 3. 3.

—— does not enter the blood, xxxiii. 2. 10. xxii. 3. 3.

Contraction and attraction, iv. 1.

Contraction and attraction, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of fibres produces sensation, iv. 5. xii. 1. 6.

—— of fibers produces sensation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— continues some time, xii. 1. 5.

—— continues for a while, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— alternates with relaxation, xii. 1. 3.

—— alternates with downtime, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Convulsion, xvii. 1. 8. xxxiv. 1. 1. and 4. iii. 5. 8.

Convulsion, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__.

—— of particular muscles, xvii. 1. 8.

—— of specific muscles, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— periods of, xxxvi. 3. 9.

periods of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Coryza. See Catarrh.

Coryza. See Rhinitis.

Cough, nervous, periods of, xxxvi. 3. 9.

Cough, anxious, intervals of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Cramp, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Critical days from lunations, xxxvi. 4.

Critical days from lunar cycles, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

D.

D.

Darkish room, why we see well in it, xii. 2. 1.

Darkish room, why we see well in it, xii. 2. 1.

Debility sensorial and stimulatory, xii. 2. 1.

Sensory and stimulatory weakness, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— direct and indirect of Dr. Brown, xii. 2. 1. xxxii. 3. 2.

—— direct and indirect of Dr. Brown, xii. 2. 1. xxxii. 3. 2.

—— See Weakness.

—— See Vulnerability.

—— from drinking spirits, cure of, xii. 7. 8.

—— from drinking spirits, cure of, xii. 7. 8.

—— in fevers, cure of, xii. 7. 8.

— in fevers, remedy for, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Deliberation, what, xxxiv. 1.

Deliberation, what’s up, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Delirium, two kinds of, xxxiii. 1. 4. xxxiv. 2. 2.

Delirium, two types of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— cases of, iii. 5. 8.

—— cases of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— prevented by dreams, xviii. 2.

—— stopped by dreams, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Desire, origin of, xi. 2. 3.

Desire, its origin, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Diabetes explained, xxix. 4.

Understanding diabetes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— with bloody urine, xxvii. 2.

—— with bloody pee, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— in the night, xviii. 15.

—— at night, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Diarrhœa, xxix. 4.

Diarrhea, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Digestion, xxxiii. 1. xxxvii.

Digestion, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— strengthened by emetics, xxxv. 1. 3.

strengthened by vomit-inducers, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— strengthened by regular hours, why, xxxvi. 2. 1.

—— strengthened by regular hours, why, xxxvi. 2. 1.

Digitalis, use of in dropsy, xxix. 5. 2.

Digitalis for treating edema, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Distention acts as a stimulus, xxxii. 4.

Distention acts as a trigger, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— See Extension.

Check Extension.

Distinguishing, xv. 3.

Distinguishing, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Diurnal circle of actions, xxv. 4.

Daily cycle of actions, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Doubting, xv. 3.

Doubtful, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Dreams, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— their inconsistency, xviii. 17.

—— their inconsistency, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— no surprise in them, xviii. 17.

no surprises in them, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— much novelty of combination, xviii. 9.

—— a lot of new combinations, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Dropsies explained, xxix. 5. 1.

Dropsies explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Dropsy cured by insanity, xxxiv. 2. 7.

Dropsy cured by madness, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— cure of, xxix. 5. 2.

cure for, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Drunkards weak till next day, xvii. 1. 7.

Drunkards weak until the next day, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— stammer, and stagger, and weep, xii. 4. 1. xxi. 4.

—— stammer, and stagger, and weep, xii. 4. 1. xxi. 4.

—— see objects double, why, xxi. 7.

see double, why, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— become delirious, sleepy, stupid, xxi. 5.

become dizzy, tired, foolish, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Drunkenness. See Intoxication, xxi.

Intoxication. See Drunkenness, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— diminished by attention, xxi. 8.

diminished by attention, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Dyspnœa in cold bath, xxxii. 3. 2.

Shortness of breath in cold bath, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

E.

E.

Ear, a good one, xvi. 10.

Ear, a good one, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— noise in, xx. 7.

—— noise in, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Eggs of frogs, fish, fowl, xxxix. 2.

Frog, fish, bird eggs, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of birds, why spotted, xxxix. 5.

—— of birds, why are they spotted, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— with double yolk, xxxix. 4. 4.

— with a double yolk, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Electricity, xii. 1. xiv. 9.

Electricity, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— jaundice cured by it, xxx. 1. 2.

jaundice cured by it, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Embryon produced by the male, xxxix. 2.

Embryo produced by the male, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— consists of a living fibre, xxxix. 4.

—— consists of a living fibre, xxxix. 4.

—— absorbs nutriment, receives oxygen, xxxix. 1.

—— absorbs nutrients, receives oxygen, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— its actions and sensations, xvi. 2.

—— its actions and feelings, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Emetic. See Vomiting.

Emetic. See Vomiting.

Emotions, xi. 2. 2.

Feelings, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Ennui, or tædium vitæ, xxxiv. 2. 3. xxxiii. 1. 1. xxxix. 6.

Boredom, or tædium vitæ, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

Epileptic fits explained, xxxiv. 1. 4. xxvii. 2.

Epileptic seizures explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— in sleep, why, xviii. 14. & 15.

—— in sleep, why, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. & __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Equinoxial lunations, xxxii. 6.

Equinox moon phases, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Excitability perpetually varies, xii. 1. 7.

Excitability constantly changes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— synonymous to quantity of sensorial power, xii. 1. 7.

—— synonymous to quantity of sensorial power, xii. 1. 7.

Exercise, its use, xxxii. 5. 3.

Exercise, its benefits, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Exertion of sensorial power defined, xii. 2. 1.

Exertion of sensory power defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Existence in space, xiv. 2. 5.

Existence in space, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Extension, sense of, xiv. 7.

Extension, sense of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Eyes become black in some epilepsies, xxvii. 2.

Eyes become black in some epilepsies, xxvii. 2.

F.

F.

Face, flushing of after dinner, xxxv. 1. 1.

Face flushing after dinner, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— why first affected in small-pox, xxxv. 1. 1.

—— why first affected in small-pox, xxxv. 1. 1.

—— red from inflamed liver, xxxv. 2. 2.

—— red from inflamed liver, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Fainting fits, xii. 7. 1. xiv. 7.

Fainting spells, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Fear, language of, xvi. 8. 1.

Fear, the language of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— a cause of fever, xxxii. 8.

a cause of fever, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— cause of, xvii. 3. 7.

reason for, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Fetus. See Embryon, xvi. 2. xxxix. 1.

Fetus. See Embryo, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Fevers, irritative, xxxii. 1.

Fevers, irritative, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— intermittent, xxxii. 1. xxxii. 3.

—— sporadic, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— sensitive, xxxiii. 1.

—— sensitive, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— not an effort of nature for relief, xxxii. 10.

—— not an effort of nature for relief, xxxii. 10.

—— paroxysms of, xii. 7. 1. xii. 2. 3. xii. 3. 5.

—— outbursts of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— why some intermit and not others, xxxvi. 1.

—— why some intermit and not others, xxxvi. 1.

—— cold fits of, xxxii. 4. xxxii. 9. xvii. 3. 3.

—— cold fits of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— periods of, xxxvi. 3.

—— periods of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— have solar or lunar periods, xxxii. 6.

—— have solar or lunar periods, xxxii. 6.

—— source of the symptoms of, xxxii. 1.

—— source of the symptoms of, xxxii. 1.

—— prostration of strength in, xii. 4. 1. xxxii. 3. 2.

—— prostration of strength in, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— cure of, xii. 6. 1.

cure for, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— how cured by the bark, xii. 3. 4.

—— how cured by the bark, xii. 3. 4.

—— cured by increased volition, xii. 2. 4. xxxiv. 2. 8.

cured by increased motivation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— best quantity of stimulus in, xii. 7. 8.

—— best quantity of stimulus in, xii. 7. 8.

Fibres. See Muscles.

Fibers. See Muscles.

Fibres, their mobility, xii. 1. 7. xii. 1. 1.

Fibers, their movement, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— contractions of, vi. xii. 1. 1.

—— contractions of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— four classes of their motions, vi.

—— four classes of their motions, vi.

—— their motions distinguished from sensorial ones, v. 3.

—— their motions distinguished from sensorial ones, v. 3.

Figure, xiv. 2. 2. iii. 1.

Figure, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Fish, their knowledge, xvi. 14.

Fish, their wisdom, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Foxglove, its use in dropsies, xxix. 5. 2.

Foxglove, its use in edema, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— overdose of, xxv. 17.

overdose of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Free-will, xv. 3. 7.

Free will, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

G.

G.

Gall-stone, xxv. 17.

Gallstone, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— See Bile-stones.

See gallstones.

Generation, xxxiii. 1. xxxix.

Generation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Gills of fish, xxxviii. 2.

Fish gills, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Glands, xxiii. 2.

Glands, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— conglobate glands, xxiii. 3.

conglomerate glands, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— have their peculiar stimulus, xi. 1.

have their unique appeal, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— their senses, xiv. 9. xxxix. 6.

—— their senses, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— invert their motions, xxv. 7.

invert their motions, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— increase their motions, xxv. 7.

increase their movements, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Golden rule for exhibiting wine, xii. 7. 8.

Golden rule for showing wine, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— for leaving off wine, xii. 7. 8.

—— for skipping wine, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Gout from inflamed liver, xxxv. 2. 2. xviii. 16. xxiv. 2. 8.

Gout from liver inflammation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— in the stomach, xxiv. 2. 8. xxv. 17.

—— in the stomach, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— why it returns after evacuations, xxxii. 4.

—— why it returns after evacuations, xxxii. 4.

—— owing to vinous spirit only, xxi. 10.

—— owing to vinous spirit only, xxi. 10.

—— periods of, xxxvi. 3. 6.

—— periods of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Grinning in pain, xxxiv. 1. 3.

Grinning through the pain, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Gyration on one foot, xx. 5. and 6.

One-foot spin, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

H.

H.

Habit defined, ii. 2. 11. iv. 7.

Habit defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Hæmorrhages, periods of, xxxvi. 3. 11.

Periods of bleeding, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from paralysis of veins, xxvii. 1. and 2.

—— from paralysis of veins, xxvii. 1. and 2.

Hair and nails, xxxix. 3. 2.

Hair and nails, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— colour of, xxxix. 5. 1.

colour of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Harmony, xxii. 2.

Harmony, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Head-achs, xxxv. 2. 1.

Headaches, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Hearing, xiv. 4.

Hearing, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Heat, sense of, xiv. 6. xxxii. 3. 1.

Heat, feeling of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— produced by the glands, xxxii. 3.

—— produced by the glands, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— external and internal, xxxii. 3. 1.

—— external and internal, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— atmosphere of heat, xxxii. 3. 1.

—— hot atmosphere, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— increases during sleep, xviii. 15.

—— increases during sleep, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Hemicrania, xxxv. 2. 1.

Hemicrania, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from decaying teeth, xxxv. 2. 1.

—— from decayed teeth, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Hepatitis, cause of, xxxv. 2. 3.

Cause of hepatitis, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Hereditary diseases, xxxix. 7. 6.

Genetic disorders, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Hermaphrodite insects, xxxix. 5.

Hermaphrodite insects, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Herpes, xxviii. 2.

Herpes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from inflamed kidney, xxxv. 2. 2.

—— from inflamed kidney, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Hilarity from diurnal fever, xxxvi. 3. 1.

Hilarity from daytime fever, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Hunger, sense of, xiv. 8.

Sense of hunger, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Hydrophobia, xxii. 3. 3.

Fear of water, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Hypochondriacism, xxxiii. 1. 1. xxxiv. 2. 3.

Hypochondria, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

I.

I.

Ideas defined, ii. 2. 7.

Ideas defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— are motions of the organs of sense, iii. 4. xviii. 5. xviii. 10. xviii. 6.

—— are motions of the organs of sense, iii. 4. xviii. 5. xviii. 10. xviii. 6.

—— analogous to muscular motions, iii. 5.

— similar to muscle movements, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— continue some time, xx. 6.

—— continue for a while, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— new ones cannot be invented, iii. 6. 1.

—— new ones cannot be invented, iii. 6. 1.

—— abstracted ones, iii. 6. 4.

—— abstract ones, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— inconsistent trains of, xviii. 17.

inconsistent train schedules, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— perish with the organ of sense, iii. 4. 4.

—— perish with the organ of sense, iii. 4. 4.

—— painful from inflammation of the organ, iii. 5. 5.

—— painful from inflammation of the organ, iii. 5. 5.

—— irritative ones, vii. 1. 4. vii. 3. 2. xv. 2. xx. 7.

—— annoying ones, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__.

—— of resemblance, contiguity, causation, viii. 3. 2. x. 3. 3.

—— of similarity, proximity, causation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— resemble the figure and other properties of bodies, xiv. 2. 2.

—— resemble the figure and other properties of bodies, xiv. 2. 2.

—— received in tribes, xv. 1.

received in groups, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of the same sense easier combined, xv. 1. 1.

—— of the same sense easier combined, xv. 1. 1.

—— of reflection, xv. 1. 6. ii. 2. 12.

—— of reflection, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Ideal presence, xv. 1. 7.

Perfect presence, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Identity, xv. 3. 5. xviii. 13.

Identity, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Iliac passion, xxv. 15.

Iliac passion, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Imagination, viii. 1. 2. xv. 1. 7. xv. 2. 2.

Imagination, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— of the male forms the sex, xxxix. 6.

—— of the male forms the sex, xxxix. 6.

Imitation, origin of, xii. 3. 3. xxxix. 5. xxii. 3. xvi. 7.

Imitation, source of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_3__.

Immaterial beings, xiv. 1. xiv. 2. 4.

Immaterial beings, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Impediment of speech, xvii. 1. 10. xvii. 2. 10.

Speech impediment, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Infection. See Contagion.

Infection. See Contagion.

Inflammation, xii. 2. 3. xxxiii. 2. 2.

Inflammation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— great vascular exertion in, xii. 2. 1.

great blood flow in, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— not from pains from defect of stimulus, xxxiii. 2. 3.

—— not from pains from defect of stimulus, xxxiii. 2. 3.

—— of parts previously insensible, xii. 3. 7.

—— of parts previously unaware, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— often distant from its cause, xxiv. 2. 8.

—— often distant from its cause, xxiv. 2. 8.

—— observes solar days, xxxii. 6.

—— observes solar days, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of the eye, xxxiii. 3. 1.

—— of the eye, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of the bowels prevented by their continued action in sleep, xviii. 2.

—— of the bowels prevented by their continued action in sleep, xviii. 2.

Inoculation with blood, xxxiii. 2. 10.

Blood inoculation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Insane people, their great strength, xii. 2. 1.

Crazy people, their great strength, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Insanity (see Madness) pleasurable one, xxxiv. 2. 6.

Insanity (see Madness) enjoyable one, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Insects, their knowledge, xvi. 15. and 16.

Insects, their knowledge, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__, and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— in the heads of calves, xxxix. 1.

—— in the heads of calves, xxxix. 1.

—— class of, xxxix. 4. 8.

class of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Instinctive actions defined, xvi. 1.

Instinctive actions explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Intestines, xxv. 3.

Intestines, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Intoxication relieves pain, why, xxi. 3.

Drinking eases pain, right? __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from food after fatigue, xxi. 2.

—— from food after fatigue, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— diseases from it, xxi. 10.

—— diseases from it, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— See Drunkenness.

See Alcoholism.

Intuitive analogy, xvii. 3. 7.

Intuitive comparison, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Invention, xv. 3. 3.

Invention, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Irritability increases during sleep, xviii. 15.

Irritability increases during sleep, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Itching, xiv. 9.

Itchy, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

J.

J.

Jaundice from paralysis of the liver, xxx. 1. 2.

Jaundice from paralysis of the liver, xxx. 1. 2.

—— cured by electricity, xxx. 1. 2.

cured by electricity, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Jaw-locked, xxxiv. 1. 5.

Jaw clenched, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Judgment, xv. 3.

Judgment, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

K.

K.

Knowledge of various animals, xvi. 11.

Knowledge of different animals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

L.

L.

Lachrymal sack, xvi. 8. xxiv. 2. 2. and 7.

Lacrimal sac, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

Lacteals, paralysis of, xxviii.

Lacteal paralysis, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— See Absorbents.

See Absorbents.

Lady playing on the harpsichord, xvii. 2.

Woman playing the harpsichord, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— distressed for her dying bird, xvii. 2. 10.

—— distressed for her dying bird, xvii. 2. 10.

Language, natural, its origin, xvi. 7. & 8.

Language, natural, its origin, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. & __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— of various passions described, xvi. 8.

—— of various interests described, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— artificial, of various animals, xvi. 9.

artificial, of different animals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— theory of, xxxix. 8. 3.

—— theory of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Lapping of puppies, xvi. 4.

Puppy licking, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Laughter explained, xxxiv. 1. 4.

Laughter explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from tickling, xvii. 3. 5. xxxiv. 1. 4.

—— from tickling, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— from frivolous ideas, xxxiv. 1. 4. xviii. 12.

—— from silly ideas, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Life, long, art of producing, xxxvii.

Life, long, is the art of creating, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Light has no momentum, iii. 3. 1.

Light has no momentum, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Liquor amnii, xvi. 2. xxxviii. 2.

Amniotic fluid, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— is nutritious, xxxviii. 3.

—— is healthy, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— frozen, xxxviii. 3.

—— frozen, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Liver, paralysis of, xxx. 1. 4.

Liver paralysis, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— large of geese, xxx. 1. 6.

—— flock of geese, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Love, sentimental, its origin, xvi. 6.

Love, emotional, its origin, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— animal, xiv. 8. xvi. 5.

—— animal, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Lunar periods affect diseases, xxxii. 6.

Moon phases affect diseases, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Lust, xiv. 8. xvi. 5.

Lust, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Lymphatics, paralysis of, xxviii.

Lymphatic paralysis, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— See Absorbents.

See Absorbents.

M.

M.

Mad-dog, bite of, xxii. 3. 3.

Mad-dog bite, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Madness, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Magnetism, xii. 1. 1.

Magnetism, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Magnifying objects, new way of, xl. 10. 5.

New way of magnifying objects, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Male animals have teats, xxxix. 4. 8.

Male animals have nipples, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— pigeons give milk, xxxix. 4. 8.

pigeons produce milk, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Man distinguished from brutes, xi. 2. 3. xvi. 17.

Man distinguished from animals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Material world, xiv. 1. xiv. 2. 5. xviii. 7.

Material world, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

Matter, penetrability of, xiv. 2. 3.

Matter, how penetrable it is, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— purulent, xxxiii. 2. 4.

pus-filled, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Measles, xxxiii. 2. 9.

Measles, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Membranes, xxvi. 2.

Membranes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Memory defined, ii. 2. 10. xv. 1. 7. xv. 3.

Memory defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

Menstruation by lunar periods, xxxii. 6.

Menstruation by moon cycles, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Miscarriage from fear, xxxix. 6. 5.

Miscarriage due to fear, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Mobility of fibres, xii. 1. 7.

Mobility of fibers, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Momentum of the blood, xxxii. 5. 2.

Blood circulation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— sometimes increased by venesection, xxxii. 5. 4.

—— sometimes increased by bloodletting, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Monsters, xxxix. 4. 4. and 5. 2.

Monsters, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— without heads, xxxviii. 3.

—— without heads, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Moon and sun, their influence, xxxii. 6.

Moon and sun, their impact, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Mortification, xxxiii. 3. 3.

Embarrassment, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Motion is either cause or effect, i. xiv. 2. 2.

Motion is either cause or effect, i. xiv. 2. 2.

—— primary and secondary, i.

primary and secondary, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— animal, i. iii. 1.

—— animal, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— propensity to, xxii. 1.

tendency to, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— animal, continue some time after their production, xvii. 1. 3.

—— animal, continue some time after their production, xvii. 1. 3.

—— defined, a variation of figure, iii. 1. xiv. 2. 2. xxxix. 8.

—— defined, a variation of figure, iii. 1. xiv. 2. 2. xxxix. 8.

Mucus, experiments on, xxvi. 1.

Mucus, experiments on, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— secretion of, xxvi. 2.

—— secretion of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Mules, xxxix. 4. 5. and 6. xxxix. 5. 2.

Mules, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

Mule plants, xxxix. 2.

Mule plants, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Muscæ volitantes, xl. 2.

Floaters, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Muscles constitute an organ of sense, xiv. 7. ii. 2. 4.

Muscles constitute an organ of sense, xiv. 7. ii. 2. 4.

—— stimulated by extension, xi. 1. xiv. 7.

stimulated by extension, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— contract by spirit of animation, xii. 1. 1. and 3.

—— contract by spirit of animation, xii. 1. 1. and 3.

Music, xvi. 10. xxii. 2.

Music, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Musical time, why agreeable, xii. 3. 3.

Musical time, why so agreeable, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

N.

N.

Nausea, xxv. 6.

Nausea, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Nerves and brain, ii. 2. 3.

Nerves and brain, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— extremities of form the whole system, xxxvii. 3.

—— extremities of form the whole system, xxxvii. 3.

—— are not changed with age, xxxvii. 4.

—— are not changed with age, xxxvii. 4.

Nervous pains defined, xxxiv. 1. 1.

Nerve pain defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Number defined, xiv. 2. 2.

Number defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Nutriment for the embryon, xxxix. 5. 2.

Nutrition for the embryo, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Nutrition owing to stimulus, xxxvii. 3.

Nutrition based on stimulus, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— by animal selection, xxxvii. 3.

—— through selective breeding, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— when the fibres are elongated, xxxvii. 3.

—— when the fibres are elongated, xxxvii. 3.

—— like inflammation, xxxvii. 3.

—— like inflammation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

O.

O.

Objects long viewed become faint, iii. 3. 2.

Objects we've long seen fade, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Ocular spectra, xl.

Ocular spectra, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Oil externally in diabætes, xxix. 4.

Oil externally in diabetes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Old age from inirritability, xxxvii.

Old age from irritability, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Opium is stimulant, xxxii. 2. 2.

Opium is a stimulant, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— promotes absorption after evacuation, xxxiii. 3. 1.

—— promotes absorption after use, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— in increasing doses, xii. 3. 1.

—— in higher doses, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Organs of sense, ii. 2. 5. and 6.

Senses, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Organs when destroyed cease to produce ideas, iii. 4. 4.

Organs when destroyed cease to produce ideas, iii. 4. 4.

Organic particles of Buffon, xxxvii. 3. xxxix. 3. 3.

Organic particles of Buffon, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Organ-pipes, xx. 7.

Organ pipes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Oxygenation of the blood, xxxviii.

Blood oxygenation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

P.

P.

Pain from excess and defect of motion, iv. 5. xii. 5. 3. xxxiv. 1. xxxv. 2. 1.

Pain from excess and defect of motion, iv. 5. xii. 5. 3. xxxiv. 1. xxxv. 2. 1.

—— not felt during exertion, xxxiv. 1. 2.

—— not felt during exercise, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from greater contraction of fibres, xii. 1. 6.

—— from greater contraction of fibres, xii. 1. 6.

—— from accumulation of sensorial power, xii. 5. 3.

—— from accumulation of sensorial power, xii. 5. 3.

—— from light, pressure, heat, caustics, xiv. 9.

—— from light, pressure, heat, caustics, xiv. 9.

—— in epilepsy, xxxv. 2. 1.

—— in epilepsy, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— distant from its cause, xxiv. 2. 8.

distant from its cause, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from stone in the bladder, xxxv. 2. 1.

—— from stone in the bladder, xxxv. 2. 1.

—— of head and back from defect, xxxii. 3.

—— of head and back from defect, xxxii. 3.

—— from a gall-stone, xxxv. 2. 1. xxv. 17.

—— from a gallstone, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— of the stomach in gout, xxv. 17.

—— of the stomach in gout, xxv. 17.

—— of shoulder in hepatitis, xxxv. 2. 4.

—— shoulder pain in hepatitis, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— produces volition, iv. 6.

—— drives willpower, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Paleness in cold fit, xxxii. 3. 2.

Pale in cold fit, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Palsies explained, xxxiv. 1. 7.

Palsy explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Paralytic limbs stretch from irritation, vii. 1. 3.

Paralyzed limbs stretch from irritation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— patients move their sound limb much, xii. 5. 1.

—— patients move their sound limb much, xii. 5. 1.

Paralysis from great exertion, xii. 4. 6.

Exhaustion-induced paralysis, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from less exertion, xii. 5. 6.

—— from less effort, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of the lacteals, xxviii.

—— of the lacteals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of the liver, xxx. 1. 4.

—— of the liver, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of the right arm, why, xxxiv. 1. 7.

—— of the right arm, why, xxxiv. 1. 7.

—— of the veins, xxvii. 2.

—— of the veins, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Particles of matter will not approach, xii. 1. 1.

Particles of matter will not approach, xii. 1. 1.

Passions, xi. 2. 2.

Passions, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— connate, xvi. 1.

connate, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Pecking of chickens, xvi. 4.

Chicken pecking, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Perception defined, ii. 2. 8. xv. 3. 1.

Perception explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Periods of agues, how formed, xxxii. 3. 4.

Periods of fevers, how formed, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of diseases, xxxvi.

—— of diseases, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of natural actions and of diseased actions, xxxvi.

—— of natural actions and of diseased actions, xxxvi.

Perspiration in fever-fits, xxxii. 9. See Sweat.

Fever sweat, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. See Sweat.

Petechiæ, xxvii. 2.

Petechiae, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Pigeons secrete milk in their stomachs, xxxix. 4. 8.

Pigeons secrete milk in their stomachs, xxxix. 4. 8.

Piles, xxvii. 2.

Stacks, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Placenta a pulmonary organ, xxxviii. 2.

Placenta as a lung organ, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Pleasure of life, xxxiii. 1. xxxix. 5.

Joy of life, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— from greater fibrous contractions, xii. 1. 6.

—— from stronger fibrous contractions, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— what kind causes laughter, xxxiv. 1. 4.

—— what kind triggers laughter, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— what kind causes sleep, xxxiv. 1. 4.

—— what kind causes sleep, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Pleurisy, periods of, xxxvi. 3. 7.

Pleurisy, episodes of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— cause of, xxxv. 2. 3.

cause of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Prometheus, story of, xxx. 3.

Prometheus, the story of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Prostration of strength in fevers, xii. 4. 1.

Weakness during fevers, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Pupils of the eyes large, xxxi. 1.

Large pupils, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Pulse quick in fevers with debility, xii. 1. 4. xii. 5. 4. xxxii. 2. 1.

Pulse quick in fevers with debility, xii. 1. 4. xii. 5. 4. xxxii. 2. 1.

—— in fevers with strength, xxxii. 2.

—— in fevers with strength, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from defect of blood, xxxii. 2. 3. xii. 1. 4.

—— from blood defect, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— weak from emetics, xxv. 17.

—— weak from medications, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Q.

Q.

Quack advertisements injurious. Preface.

Fake ads harmful. Preface.

Quadrupeds have no sanguiferous lochia, xxxviii. 2.

Quadrupeds have no blood discharge, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— have nothing similar to the yolk of egg, xxxix. 1.

—— have nothing similar to the yolk of egg, xxxix. 1.

R.

R.

Rhaphania, periods of, xxxvi. 3. 9.

Rhaphania, times of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Reason, ix. 1. 2. xv. 3.

Reason, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Reasoning, xv. 3.

Reasoning, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Recollection, ii. 2. 10. ix. 1. 2. xv. 2. 3.

Memory, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

Relaxation and bracing, xxxii. 3. 2.

Chill and invigorating, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Repetition, why agreeable, xii. 3. 3. xxii. 2.

Repetition, why it's good, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Respiration affected by attention, xxxvi. 2. 1.

Breathing influenced by focus, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Restlessness in fevers, xxxiv. 1. 2.

Restlessness with fever, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Retrograde motions, xii. 5. 5. xxv. 6. xxix. 11.

Retrograde motion, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— of the stomach, xxv. 6.

—— of the stomach, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of the skin, xxv. 9.

—— of the skin, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of fluids, how distinguished, xxix. 8.

—— of liquids, how refined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— how caused, xxix. 11. 5.

—— how it happened, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— vegetable motions, xxix. 9.

—— veggie movements, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Retina is fibrous, iii. 2. xl. 1.

Retina is fibrous, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— is active in vision, iii. 3. xl. 1.

—— is active in vision, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— excited into spasmodic motions, xl. 7.

—— excited into erratic movements, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— is sensible during sleep, xviii. 5. xix. 8.

—— is sensible while sleeping, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Reverie, xix. 1. xxxiv. 3.

Daydream, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— case of a sleep-walker, xix. 2.

case of a sleepwalker, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— is an epileptic disease, xix. 9.

—— is an epilepsy condition, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Rhymes in poetry, why agreeable, xxii. 2.

Rhymes in poetry, why nice, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Rheumatism, three kinds of, xxvi. 3.

Rheumatism, three types of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Rocking young children, xxi. 3.

Rocking little kids, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Ruminating animals, xxv. 1.

Ruminant animals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

S.

S.

Saliva produced by mercury, xxiv. 1.

Saliva made by mercury, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— by food, xxiv. 1. 1.

—— by food, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— by ideas, xxiv. 1. 2. and 5.

—— by ideas, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— by disordered volition, xxiv. 1. 7.

—— by random choice, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Schirrous tumours revive, xii. 2. 2.

Schirrous tumors reappear, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Screaming in pain, xxxiv. 1. 3.

Screaming in pain, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Scrophula, its temperament, xxxi. 1.

Scrofula, its temperament, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Scurvy of the lungs, xxvii. 2.

Lung scurvy, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sea-sickness, xx. 4.

Sea sickness, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— stopped by attention, xx. 5.

—— caught my attention, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Secretion, xxxiii. 1. xxxvii.

Secretion, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— increased during sleep, xviii. 16.

increased while sleeping, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Seeds require oxygenation, xxxviii. 2.

Seeds need oxygen, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sensation defined, ii. 2. 9. v. 2. xxxix. 8. 4.

Sensation defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— diseases of, xxxiii.

diseases of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from fibrous contractions, iv. 5. xii. 1. 6.

—— from fibrous contractions, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— in an amputated limb, iii. 6. 3.

—— in a missing limb, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— affects the whole sensorium, xi. 2.

—— affects the entire sensory system, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— produces volition, iv. 6.

—— creates willpower, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sensibility increases during sleep, xviii. 15.

Awareness increases during sleep, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sensitive motions, viii. xxxiii. 2. xxxiv. 1.

Sensitive actions, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— fevers of two kinds, xxxiii. 1. 2.

two types of fevers, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— ideas, xv. 2. 2.

Modern ideas, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sensorium defined, ii. 2. 1.

Sensorium explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Senses correct one another, xviii. 7.

Senses validate each other, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— distinguished from appetites, xxxiv. 1. 1.

—— distinguished from cravings, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sensorial power. See Spirit of Animation.

Sensorial power. See Spirit of Animation.

—— great expence of in the vital motions, xxxii. 3. 2.

—— great expence of in the vital motions, xxxii. 3. 2.

—— two kinds of excited in sensitive fevers, xxxiii. 1. 3.

—— two kinds of excited in sensitive fevers, xxxiii. 1. 3.

—— powers defined, v. 1.

—— powers defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— motions distinguished from fibrous motions, v. 3.

—— motions distinguished from fibrous motions, v. 3.

—— not much, accumulated in sleep, xviii. 2.

—— not much, accumulated in sleep, xviii. 2.

—— powers, accumulation of, xii. 5. 1.

—— powers, accumulation of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— exhaustion of, xii. 4. 1.

exhaustion of __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— wasted below natural in hot fits, xxxii. 9. 3.

—— wasted below natural in hot fits, xxxii. 9. 3.

—— less exertion of produces pain, xii. 5. 3.

—— less exertion of produces pain, xii. 5. 3.

—— less quantity of it, xii. 5. 4.

fewer of it, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sensual motions distinguished from muscular, ii. 2. 7.

Sensual movements separated from muscular, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sex owing to the imagination of the father, xxxix. 7. 6. xxxix. 6. 3. xxxix. 6. 7. xxxix. 5.

Sex owing to the imagination of the father, xxxix. 7. 6. xxxix. 6. 3. xxxix. 6. 7. xxxix. 5.

Shingles from inflamed kidney, xxxv. 2. 2.

Shingles from swollen kidney, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Shoulders broad, xxxi. 1. xxxix. 7. 6.

Shoulders broad, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Shuddering from cold, xxxiv. 1. 1. and 2.

Shivering from cold, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. and __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Sight, its accuracy in men, xvi. 6.

Sight, its precision in people, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Skin, skurf on it, xxvi. 1.

Skin, flakes on it, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sleep suspends volition, xviii. 1.

Sleep suspends will, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— defined, xviii. 21.

—— defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— remote causes, xviii. 20.

remote causes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— sensation continues in it, xviii. 2.

—— sensation continues in it, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from food, xxi. 1.

—— from food, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from rocking, uniform sounds, xxi. 1.

—— from rocking, uniform sounds, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from wine and opium, xxi. 3.

from wine and opium, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— why it invigorates, xii. 5. 1.

—— why it energizes, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— pulse slower and fuller, xxxii. 2. 2.

—— pulse slower and stronger, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— interrupted, xxvii. 2.

—— interrupted, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from breathing less oxygene, xviii. 20.

—— from breathing less oxygen, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from being whirled on a millstone, xviii. 20.

—— from being whirled on a millstone, xviii. 20.

—— from application of cold, xviii. 20.

—— from cold application, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— induced by regular hours, xxxvi. 2. 2.

—— caused by regular hours, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sleeping animals, xii. 2. 2.

Sleeping animals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sleep-walkers. See Reverie, xix. 1.

Sleepwalkers. See Reverie, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Smallpox, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— eruption first on the face, why, xxxv. 1. 1. xxxiii. 2. 10.

—— eruption first on the face, why, xxxv. 1. 1. xxxiii. 2. 10.

—— the blood will not infect, xxxiii. 2. 10.

—— the blood will not infect, xxxiii. 2. 10.

—— obeys lunations, xxxvi. 4.

follows moon phases, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Smell, xiv. 5. xvi. 5.

Smell, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Smiling, origin of, xvi. 8. 4.

Smiling, origin of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Solidity, xiv. 2. 1.

Solidity, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Somnambulation. See Reverie, xix. 1.

Sleepwalking. See Daydream, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Space, xiv. 2. 2.

Space, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Spasm, doctrine of, xxxii. 10.

Doctrine of spasm, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Spectra, ocular, xl.

Spectra, vision, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— mistaken for spectres, xl. 2.

mistaken for ghosts, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— vary from long inspection, iii. 3. 5.

vary from lengthy inspection, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Spirit of animation. See Sensorial Power.

Spirit of animation. See Sensorial Power.

—— of animation causes fibrous contraction, iv. 2. ii. 2. 1. xiv. 2. 4.

—— of animation causes fibrous contraction, iv. 2. ii. 2. 1. xiv. 2. 4.

—— possesses solidity, figure, and other properties of matter, xiv. 2. 4.

—— possesses solidity, figure, and other properties of matter, xiv. 2. 4.

Spirits and angels, xiv. 2. 4.

Spirits and angels, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Stammering explained, xvii. 1. 10. xvii. 2. 10.

Stuttering explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Stimulus defined, ii. 2. 13. iv. 4. xii. 2. 1.

Stimulus explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— of various kinds, xi. 1.

—— of different types, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— with lessened effect, xii. 3. 1.

—— with reduced effect, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— with greater effect, xii. 3. 3.

—— with more impact, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— ceases to produce sensation, xii. 3. 6.

—— stops producing sensation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Stomach and intestines, xxv.

Stomach and intestines, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— inverted by great stimulus, xxv. 6.

inverted by major stimulation, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— its actions decreased in vomiting, xxxv. 1. 3.

—— its actions decreased in vomiting, xxxv. 1. 3.

—— a blow on it occasions death, xxv. 17.

—— a blow on it occasions death, xxv. 17.

Stools black, xxvii. 2.

Black stools, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Strangury, xxxv. 2. 1.

Strangury, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sucking before nativity, xvi. 4.

Sucking before birth, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Suckling children, sense of, xiv. 8.

Sucking infants, awareness of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Suggestion defined, ii. 2. 10. xv. 2. 4.

Suggestion defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Sun and moon, their influence, xxxii. 6.

Sun and moon, their impact, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Surprise, xvii. 3. 7. xviii. 17.

Surprise, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Suspicion attends madness, xxxiv. 2. 4.

Madness is accompanied by suspicion, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Swallowing, act of, xxv. 1. xvi. 4.

Swallowing, act of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Sweat, cold, xxv. 9. xxix. 6.

Sweat, cold, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— in hot fit of fever, xxxii. 9.

—— in hot fit of fever, xxxii. 9.

—— in a morning, why, xviii. 15.

—— in the morning, why, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sweaty hands cured by lime, xxix. 4. 9.

Sweaty hands solved with lime, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Swinging and rocking, why agreeable, xxi. 3.

Swinging and rocking, why enjoyable, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Sympathy, xxxv. 1.

Sorry, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Fainting, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

T.

T.

Tædium vitæ. See Ennui.

Boredom with life. See Ennui.

Tape-worm, xxxix. 2. 3.

Tape worm, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Taste, sense of, xiv. 5.

Sense of taste, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Tears, secretion of, xxiv.

Tears, secretion of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from grief, xvi. 8. 2.

from grief, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from tender pleasure, xvi. 8. 3.

from sweet pleasure, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from stimulus of nasal duct, xvi. 8. xxiv. 2. 4.

—— from stimulus of nasal duct, xvi. 8. xxiv. 2. 4.

—— by volition, xxiv. 2. 6.

—— by choice, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Teeth decaying cause headachs, xxxv. 2. 1.

Decaying teeth cause headaches, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Temperaments, xxxi.

Temperaments, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Theory of medicine, wanted. Preface.

Theory of medicine, needed. Preface.

Thirst, sense of, xiv. 8.

Feeling thirsty, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— why in dropsies, xxix. 5.

—— why in drops, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Tickle themselves, children cannot, xvii. 3. 5.

Children can’t tickle themselves, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Tickling, xiv. 9.

Tickling, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Time, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— lapse of, xv. 3. 6.

—— lapse of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— poetic and musical, why agreeable, xxii. 2.

—— poetic and musical, why agreeable, xxii. 2.

—— dramatic, xviii. 12.

dramatic, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Tooth-edge, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

Touch, sense of, xiv. 2. 1.

Touch, sense of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— liable to vertigo, xxi. 9.

prone to dizziness, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of various animals, xvi. 6.

—— of different animals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Trains of motions inverted, xii. 5. 5.

Inverted motion trains, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Transfusion of blood in nervous fever, xxxii. 4.

Transfusion of blood in nervous fever, xxxii. 4.

Translations of matter, xxix. 7.

Translations of matter, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Typhus, best quantity of stimulus in, xii. 7. 8.

Typhus, best quantity of stimulus in, xii. 7. 8.

—— periods of observe lunar days, xxxii. 6.

—— periods of observe lunar days, xxxii. 6.

U.

U.

Ulcers, art of healing, xxxiii. 3. 2.

Ulcers, healing techniques, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of the lungs, why difficult to heal, xxviii. 2.

—— of the lungs, why difficult to heal, xxviii. 2.

Uniformity in the fine arts, why agreeable, xxii. 2.

Uniformity in the fine arts, why agreeable, xxii. 2.

Urine pale in intoxication, xxi. 6.

Urine pale when drunk, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— paucity of in anasarca, why, xxix. 5.

—— paucity of in anasarca, why, xxix. 5.

—— its passage from intestines to bladder, xxix. 3.

—— its passage from intestines to bladder, xxix. 3.

—— copious during sleep, xviii. 15.

heavy during sleep, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

V.

V.

Variation, perpetual, of irritability, xii. 2. 1.

Variation, endless, of irritability, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Vegetable buds are inferior animals, xiii. 1.

Vegetable buds are lesser beings, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— exactly resemble their parents, xxxix.

exactly like their parents, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— possess sensation and volition, xiii. 2.

— have feeling and will, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— have associate and retrograde motions, xiii. 4. xxix. 9.

—— have associate and retrograde motions, xiii. 4. xxix. 9.

—— their anthers and stigmas are alive, xiii. 5.

—— their anthers and stigmas are alive, xiii. 5.

—— have organs of sense and ideas, xiii. 5.

—— have organs of sense and ideas, xiii. 5.

—— contend for light and air, xxxix. 4. 8.

—— contend for light and air, xxxix. 4. 8.

—— duplicature of their flowers, xxxix. 4. 4.

duplicate of their flowers, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Veins are absorbents, xxvii. 1.

Veins absorb nutrients, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— paralysis of, xxvii. 1.

—— paralysis of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Venereal orgasm of brutes, xxxii. 6.

Venereal pleasure of animals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Venesection in nervous pains, xxxii. 5. 4.

Bloodletting for nerve pain, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Verbs of three kinds, xv. 3. 4.

Three types of verbs, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Verses, their measure, xxii. 2.

Verses, their rhythm, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Vertigo, xx.

Dizziness, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— defined, xx. 11.

defined, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— in looking from a tower, xx. 1.

—— in looking from a tower, xx. 1.

—— in a ship at sea, xx. 4.

—— in a ship at sea, xx. 4.

—— of all the senses, xxi. 9.

—— of all the senses, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— by intoxication, xxxv. 1. 2.

by getting drunk, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Vibratory motions perceived after sailing, xx. 5. xx. 10.

Feeling seasick after sailing, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Vinegar makes the lips pale, xxvii. 1.

Vinegar makes lips pale, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Vis medicatrix of nature, xxxix. 4. 7.

Healing power of nature, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Vision, sense of, xiv. 3.

Vision, sense of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Volition defined, v. 2. xxxiv. 1.

Volition explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— affects the whole sensorium, xi. 2.

—— affects the whole sensory system, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— diseases of, xxxiv.

diseases of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Voluntarity, xi. 2. 4.

Voluntary, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Voluntary motions, ix. xxxiv. 1.

Voluntary movements, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Voluntary ideas, xv. 2. 3.

Voluntary concepts, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— criterion of, xi. 2. 3. xxxiv. 1.

criterion of, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

Vomiting from vertigo, xx. 8.

Nauseous from vertigo, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— from drunkenness, xx. 8. xxi. 6.

—— from drinking, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__.

—— by intervals, xxv. 8.

—— at intervals, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— by voluntary efforts, xxv. 6.

—— through voluntary efforts, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— of two kinds, xxxv. 1. 3.

—— of two types, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— in cold fit of fever, xxxii. 9. 1.

—— in cold fit of fever, xxxii. 9. 1.

—— stopped by quicksilver, xxv. 16.

—— stopped by Quicksilver, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— weakens the pulse, xxv. 17.

—— slows the pulse, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

W.

W.

Waking, how, xviii. 14.

Waking up, how, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Walking, how learnt, xvi. 3.

Walking, how to learn, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Warmth in sleep, why, xviii. 15.

Warmth in sleep, why not, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

Weakness defined, xii. 1. 3. xii. 2. 1. xxxii. 3. 2.

Weakness explained, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_1__. __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_2__.

—— cure of, xii. 7. 8.

—— treatment for, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

—— See Debility.

See Weakness.

Wit producing laughter, xxxiv. 1. 4.

Humor creates laughter, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.

World generated, xxxix. 4. 8.

World created, __A_TAG_PLACEHOLDER_0__.



END OF THE FIRST VOLUME.

END OF VOLUME ONE.




        
        
    
Download ePUB

If you like this ebook, consider a donation!