This is a modern-English version of Birds of the Plains, originally written by Dewar, Douglas. It has been thoroughly updated, including changes to sentence structure, words, spelling, and grammar—to ensure clarity for contemporary readers, while preserving the original spirit and nuance. If you click on a paragraph, you will see the original text that we modified, and you can toggle between the two versions.

Scroll to the bottom of this page and you will find a free ePUB download link for this book.

Birds of the Plains, by Douglas Dewar
THE GREY PELICAN. (PELECANUS PHILIPPENSIS)

THE GREY PELICAN. (PELECANUS PHILIPPENSIS)
(A bird of the Plains)

THE GREY PELICAN (PELECANUS PHILIPPENSIS)
(A bird of the Plains)

Plains Birds

BY DOUGLAS DEWAR, F.Z.S., I.C.S.
WITH SIXTEEN ILLUSTRATIONS
FROM PHOTOGRAPHS OF LIVING BIRDS
BY CAPTAIN F. D. S. FAYRER, I.M.S.

BY DOUGLAS DEWAR, F.Z.S., I.C.S.
WITH SIXTEEN ILLUSTRATIONS
FROM PHOTOGRAPHS OF LIVING BIRDS
BY CAPTAIN F. D. S. FAYRER, I.M.S.

LONDON: JOHN LANE THE BODLEY HEAD
NEW YORK: JOHN LANE COMPANY MCMIX

LONDON: JOHN LANE THE BODLEY HEAD
NEW YORK: JOHN LANE COMPANY 1909

WILLIAM BRENDON AND SON, LTD., PRINTERS, PLYMOUTH

WILLIAM BRENDON AND SON, LTD., PRINTERS, PLYMOUTH

[v]


Foreword

It is easy enough to write a book. The difficulty is to sell the production when it is finished. That, however, is not the author’s business. Nevertheless, the labours of the writer are not over when he has completed the last paragraph of his book. He has, then, in most cases, to find a title for it.

It’s pretty straightforward to write a book. The real challenge is selling it once it’s done. That part, however, isn’t up to the author. Still, a writer’s work isn’t finished when they’ve written the last paragraph of their book. Most of the time, they then need to come up with a title for it.

This, I maintain, should be a matter of little difficulty. I regard a title as a mere distinguishing mark, a brand, a label, a something by which the book may be called when spoken of—nothing more.

This, I believe, should be pretty straightforward. I see a title as just a distinguishing mark, a brand, a label—something used to refer to the book when talking about it—nothing more.

According to this view, the value of a title lies, not in its appropriateness to the subject-matter, but in its distinctiveness.

According to this view, the value of a title comes not from how well it fits the subject, but from how unique it is.

To illustrate: some years ago a lady entered a bookseller’s shop and asked for “Drummond’s latest book—Nux Vomica.” The bookseller without a word handed her Lux Mundi.

To illustrate: some years ago, a woman walked into a bookstore and asked for "Drummond's latest book—Nux Vomica." The bookseller silently handed her Lux Mundi.

To my way of thinking Lux Mundi is a good title inasmuch as no other popular book has one like it. So distinctive is it that even when different words were substituted the bookseller at once knew what was intended. That the view here put forward does not [vi] find favour with the critics may perhaps be inferred by the exception many of them took to the title of my last book—Bombay Ducks.

To me, Lux Mundi is a great title because no other popular book has one quite like it. It's so unique that even when different words were used, the bookseller immediately knew what was meant. The fact that the perspective presented here isn't popular with critics might be suggested by the objections many of them had to the title of my last book—Bombay Ducks.

While commending my view to their consideration, I have on this occasion endeavoured to meet them by resorting to a more orthodox designation. I am, doubtless, pursuing a risky policy. Most of the reviewers were kind enough to say that Bombay Ducks was a good book with a bad title. When criticising the present work they may reverse the adjectives. Who knows?

While I urge them to consider my perspective, I’ve tried this time to use a more conventional title. I’m certainly taking a chance. Most of the reviewers were nice enough to say that Bombay Ducks was a good book with a bad title. When they critique this current work, they might switch the adjectives. Who knows?

D. D.

D. D.

[vii]

CONTENTS

PAGE
I. British Birds in the Indian Plains 1
II. The Blue Bird 10
III. Sparrows in the daycare 16
IV. Caring for Young Birds After They Leave the Nest 23
V. The Secretary Bird 29
VI. The Sarus Crane 35
VII. The Stability of Species 40
VIII. The Amadavat 46
IX. The Nutmeg Bird 52
X. The Did he do it? 56
XI. Cobbler or Fashion Designer? 62
XII. A Colorful Crow 68
XIII. Current Species Creation 73
XIV. Honeysuckles 78
XV. A Wood Cutter 84
XVI. A Feathered Runner 89
XVII. A Characterful Bird 94
XVIII. Swifts 99
XIX. Robotic Birds 104
XX. Playing Cuckoo Bird 111
XXI. The Koel Bird 117
XXII. India's Common Doves 124
XXIII. Doves on a Porch 130
XXIV. The Goldfinch 135
XXV. The Barn Owl 140
XXVI. A Tree-top Accident 145
XXVII. Two Little Birds 150
XXVIII. The Paradise Flycatcher 156
XXIX. Birds of Prey 163
XXX. Ducks 168
XXXI. A Deposed Monarch 173
XXXII. Birds in the Rain 178
XXXIII. The Weaver Bird 183
XXXIV. Green Parrots 190
XXXV. Sparrows Nesting 197
XXXVI. A Fabulous Trickster 202
XXXVII. The Emerald Bee-eater 208
XXXVIII. Do animals think? 213
XXXIX. A Few Overlooked Craftsmen 219
40. Birds in Their Nests 224
XLI. Bulbuls 229
XLII. The Indian Corby 235
[ix]


ILLUSTRATIONS

The Gray Pelican (Pelecanus philippensis), a Plains Bird Frontispiece
The White-breasted Kingfisher (Halcyon smyrnensis) 4
The Redshank Bird (Totanus calidris), one of the British birds found in India 8
The Indian Roller, also known as the "Blue Jay" (Coracias indica) 12
The Indian Adjutant (Leptoptilus dubius) 28
The Indian Adjutant (Leptoptilus dubius) 34
Loten's Sunbird (Arachnechthra lotenia) 78
(Note the long curved bill, adapted to insertion in flowers.)
The Yellow Sunbird (Arachnechthra zeylonica) 80
Nest of Loten's Sunbird 82
(Notice that it is built in a spider’s web.)
Loten’s Sunbird (Hen) is about to enter the nest. 90
The Indian Spotted Owl (Athene brama) 94
The Indian Rice Bird (Ardeola grayii) 114
The Common Kingfisher (Alcedo ispida), one of the British birds found in India 144
The Indian Kite (Milvus govinda) 148
The Gray-necked Crow (Corvus splendens) 190
The Bengal Red-whiskered Bulbul (Otocompsa emeria) 230

BIRDS OF THE PLAINS

[1]


BRITISH BIRDS ON THE PLAINS OF INDIA

Most birds are cosmopolitans and belong to no nationality. Strictly speaking, there is only one British bird, only one bird found in the British Isles and nowhere else, and that is the red grouse (Tetrao scoticus).

Most birds are cosmopolitan and don’t belong to any one nationality. To be precise, there is only one British bird, only one bird found in the British Isles and nowhere else, and that is the red grouse (Tetrao scoticus).

For this reason some apology seems necessary for the heading of this article. “Birds common to the Plains of India and the British Isles” would doubtless be a more correct title. However, I write as an Englishman. When I meet in a foreign land a bird I knew in England I like to set that bird down as a fellow-countryman.

For this reason, some apology seems necessary for the title of this article. “Birds common to the Plains of India and the British Isles” would definitely be a more accurate title. However, I write as an Englishman. When I encounter a bird I recognized from England in a foreign land, I like to consider that bird a fellow countryman.

In India most of the familiar birds: the thrush, the blackbird, the robin redbreast, the wren, the chaffinch, and the blue tit are conspicuous by their absence; their places being taken by such strange forms as mynas, bulbuls, seven sisters, parakeets, etc. The Englishman is therefore prone to exaggerate the differences between the avifauna of his own country and that of India. The [2] dissimilarity is indeed great, but not so great as is generally supposed.

In India, many of the common birds like the thrush, blackbird, robin, wren, chaffinch, and blue tit are noticeably absent; they are replaced by unusual species such as mynas, bulbuls, seven sisters, parakeets, and more. As a result, an Englishman tends to exaggerate the differences between the birdlife in his own country and that of India. While the differences are indeed significant, they aren’t as vast as most people think.

A complete list of British birds comprises some four hundred species; of these nearly one-half occur in India. But a list of British species is apt to be a misleading document. You may keep a sharp look-out in England for a lifetime without ever setting eyes on many of the so-called British birds. Every feathered thing that has been blown by contrary winds, or whose dead body has been washed by the waves, on to the shores of Albion has been appropriated as a British species. This sounds very hospitable. Unfortunately the hospitality is of a dubious nature, seeing that every casual bird visitor promptly falls a victim to the gun of some self-styled naturalist. Having slaughtered his “feathered friend” the aforesaid naturalist proceeds to boast in the press of his exploit.

A complete list of British birds includes about four hundred species; nearly half of these can be found in India. However, a list of British species can be misleading. You could keep a close eye in England for a lifetime without ever seeing many of the so-called British birds. Every bird that's been blown in by strong winds or whose dead body has washed ashore in England has been labeled as a British species. This seems very welcoming. Unfortunately, the hospitality is questionable, as every rare bird visitor quickly becomes a target for some self-proclaimed naturalist's gun. After killing his "feathered friend," that naturalist then boasts about his achievement in the press.

I do not deem it correct to speak of these occasional visitors as British birds. On the other hand, I think we may legitimately call the birds we see constantly in England, at certain or all seasons of the year, English birds. Of these many are also found in India. More of them occur in the Punjab than in any other part of the country because of our long cold weather, and because, as the crow flies, if not as the sahib travels, the Punjab is nearer England than is any other province.

I don’t think it’s right to refer to these occasional visitors as British birds. However, I believe we can accurately call the birds we frequently see in England, at certain times or all seasons of the year, English birds. Many of these are also found in India. More of them are present in the Punjab than in any other part of the country due to our long cold weather, and because, as the crow flies, if not as the sahib travels, the Punjab is closer to England than any other province.

The ubiquitous sparrow first demands our attention. This much-abused little bird is, thanks to his “push,” quite as much at home in the “Gorgeous East” as he is in England. He is certainly not quite so abundant out here; the crows and spotted owlets take care of [3] that. They are very fond of sparrow for breakfast. Nevertheless, Passer domesticus is quite plentiful enough and is ever ready to nest inside one’s bungalow.

The common sparrow immediately grabs our attention. This often-overlooked little bird is just as comfortable in the "Gorgeous East" as it is in England, thanks to its adaptability. It's definitely less common out here; the crows and spotted owls make sure of that because they enjoy sparrows for breakfast. Still, Passer domesticus is quite plentiful and always eager to nest in a bungalow.

The Indian cock sparrow differs slightly in appearance from the English bird, having more white on the sides of his neck. This is not, as might be supposed, due to the fact that he is not coated with soot to such an extent as the cockney bird. Every widely distributed species, including man, has its local peculiarities, due to climatic influences, isolation, and other causes. If the isolation be maintained long enough the process of divergence continues until the various races differ from one another to such an extent as to be called species. Local races are incipient species, species in the making. The barn owl (Strix flammea) is another case in point. This is a familiar owl in England, and is common out here, but not nearly so abundant as the little spotted owlet that makes night hideous by its caterwaulings. The Indian barn owl, which, in default of barns, haunts mosques, temples, deserted buildings, and even secluded verandahs, differs from our English friend in having stronger claws and feet, and the breast spotted instead of plain white. These trivial differences are not usually considered sufficient to justify the division of the barn owl into two species.

The Indian cock sparrow looks a bit different from the English one, having more white on the sides of its neck. This isn’t, as one might think, because it’s not as coated in soot as the cockney bird. Every widely spread species, including humans, has its local quirks due to climate, isolation, and other factors. If isolation lasts long enough, the process of divergence continues until different races vary enough to be regarded as distinct species. Local races are budding species, species in the making. The barn owl (Strix flammea) is another example. This owl is well-known in England and is common here, but it’s not nearly as widespread as the little spotted owlet that disturbs the night with its loud calls. The Indian barn owl, which, in the absence of barns, roams around mosques, temples, abandoned buildings, and even quiet verandahs, differs from our English counterpart by having stronger claws and feet, and a breast that’s spotted rather than plain white. These minor differences aren’t usually considered enough to split the barn owl into two species.

Some of our English birds assume diminutive proportions in India, as, for example, the kingfisher and the raven. This may perhaps be attributed to the enervating Indian climate. The common kingfisher (Alcedo ispida) is exceedingly common in all parts of India except the Punjab. It does, indeed, occur in [4] that province, but not abundantly. The commonest kingfisher in the Land of the Five Rivers is the much more splendid white-breasted species (Halcyon smyrnensis), which may be recognised by its beautiful blue wings with a white bar, and by its anything but melodious “rattling scream.”

Some of our English birds are smaller in India, like the kingfisher and the raven. This might be due to the draining Indian climate. The common kingfisher (Alcedo ispida) is very widespread in all areas of India except the Punjab. It does occur in that region, but not in large numbers. The most common kingfisher in the Land of the Five Rivers is the more striking white-breasted species (Halcyon smyrnensis), which can be identified by its stunning blue wings with a white bar and its rather unmelodious “rattling scream.”

This winter the ravens are invading Lahore in very large numbers. It is impossible not to notice the great black creatures as they fly overhead in couples or in companies of six or eight, uttering solemn croaks.

This winter, the ravens are invading Lahore in huge numbers. It's hard not to notice these large black birds as they fly overhead in pairs or groups of six or eight, making serious croaking sounds.

But the Indian raven, large as it is, is a diminutive form; its length is but twenty-four inches as compared with the twenty-eight of its English cousin. Moreover, there are slight anatomical differences between the two races; hence the Indian bird was at one time considered to be a separate species and was called Corvus lawrencii. There certainly does seem to be some justification for this procedure, since the Indian raven has not the solitary, shy, and retiring disposition of the bird at Home. It consorts with those feathered villains the Indian crows, and, like them, thieves from man and delights to tease and annoy birds bigger than itself by pulling their tail! But there exist ravens of all sizes intermediate between the large European form and the small Indian one, so that it is not possible to find a point at which a line may be drawn between them. For this reason the Indian raven is now held to be one and the same species as the English bird—Corvus corax.

But the Indian raven, as large as it is, is actually a smaller version; it measures only twenty-four inches compared to its English counterpart, which is twenty-eight inches long. Additionally, there are slight anatomical differences between the two species; therefore, the Indian bird was once classified as a separate species and called Corvus lawrencii. There does seem to be some basis for this classification since the Indian raven doesn't have the solitary, shy, and reserved nature of the bird back home. It hangs out with those mischievous Indian crows and, like them, steals from people and loves to tease and annoy bigger birds by tugging on their tails! However, there are ravens of all sizes that fall between the large European type and the small Indian one, making it impossible to find a clear line to distinguish them. For this reason, the Indian raven is now considered the same species as the English bird—Corvus corax.

THE WHITE-BREASTED KINGFISHER. (HALCYON SMYRNENSIS)

THE WHITE-BREASTED KINGFISHER. (HALCYON SMYRNENSIS)

White-Breasted Kingfisher (Halcyon smyrnensis)

[5]

Two cousins of the raven, namely, the rook and the jackdaw, also occur in the Punjab. They both visit us in the cold weather and fraternise with the common crows. The rook may be readily distinguished from these by the bare whitish patch of skin in front of its face. Last year hundreds of rooks were to be seen in the fields between the big and the little Ravi. They are not so abundant this winter owing to the comparative mildness of the weather.

Two relatives of the raven, the rook and the jackdaw, are also found in the Punjab. They both come around in the cold weather and mingle with the regular crows. You can easily tell a rook apart from them by the bare whitish patch of skin on its face. Last year, there were hundreds of rooks in the fields between the big and the little Ravi. They aren’t as common this winter because the weather has been relatively mild.

The jackdaw is very like Corvus splendens in appearance. It may, however, be easily distinguished by its white eye. There is at present a jackdaw in confinement in the Lahore “Zoo.”

The jackdaw looks quite similar to Corvus splendens. However, you can easily tell them apart by its white eye. Right now, there's a jackdaw in captivity at the Lahore “Zoo.”

The coot (Fulica atra) is another bird common at Home which is also abundant in India. He needs no description, being familiar—too familiar—to every sportsman in India. He is the “black duck” of Thomas Atkins that remains on the jhil after all the duck have disappeared. It is unnecessary to say that the bird is not a duck, but a water-hen that apes the manners of one. His black plumage, white face, and the difficulty he experiences in rising from the water prevent him being confounded with a duck.

The coot (Fulica atra) is another bird commonly found at home and also plentiful in India. He doesn't need much description since he's too familiar to every sportsman in India. He’s the “black duck” that stays on the jhil even after all the other ducks have left. It's worth mentioning that the bird isn’t actually a duck but rather a water-hen that mimics a duck’s behavior. His black feathers, white face, and the struggle he has to take off from the water make it impossible to confuse him with a duck.

Ornithological text-books tell us that the skylark (Alauda arvensis) visits India during the winter. This may be so, but I do not think I have ever seen one in the Punjab. I have seen thousands of the Indian skylark (Alauda gulgula)—a very similar bird, which is said to soar and sing “just as the lark in England does.”

Ornithology textbooks tell us that the skylark (Alauda arvensis) comes to India in the winter. This might be true, but I don't think I've ever spotted one in Punjab. I've seen thousands of the Indian skylark (Alauda gulgula)—a very similar bird, which is said to soar and sing “just like the lark in England does.”

As a rule it soars only at daybreak. There are in India so many birds of prey, ever on the look out for quarry, that our larks are not able to sing with impunity at heaven’s gate. They usually put forth their vocal efforts from a less exalted platform.

As a rule, it only soars at dawn. There are so many birds of prey in India, always on the lookout for something to catch, that our larks can't sing freely at heaven's gate. They usually express themselves from a lower vantage point.

[6]

“The eel’s foe, the heron” (Ardea cinerea), need not detain us long, although he is a common bird in both England and India, for the Punjab is too dry to be a favourite resort of waders. There is, however, a heron in the “Zoo” at Lahore who lives happily enough among the ducks and storks in spite of the way in which the kites worry him when he is at supper.

“The eel’s enemy, the heron” (Ardea cinerea), doesn’t require much of our time, although it’s a common bird in both England and India. The Punjab is just too dry to be a popular place for wading birds. However, there is a heron at the “Zoo” in Lahore who lives quite happily among the ducks and storks, despite the hassle he gets from the kites while he's trying to eat.

The blue-rock pigeon (Columba livia) is another English bird found in the Punjab. This must not be confounded with its cousin (Columba intermedia) the very common Indian blue pigeon, of which so many have taken up their quarters in the Montgomery Hall. The European form is not nearly so abundant, and is distinguished by its paler colour and by the fact that its lower back is white instead of bluish grey.

The blue-rock pigeon (Columba livia) is another English bird found in the Punjab. This shouldn't be confused with its relative (Columba intermedia), the very common Indian blue pigeon, which has settled in large numbers at Montgomery Hall. The European version is much less common and is recognizable by its lighter color and the fact that its lower back is white instead of bluish-gray.

The family of birds of prey affords us a large number of species common to England and India. Almost all the well-known English raptores are found in India—the peregrine falcon, the marsh harrier, the hen-harrier, the merlin, the kestrel, the sparrow-hawk, and the buzzard. All these are considerably more abundant in India than in the British Isles.

The family of birds of prey includes many species that are common in both England and India. Almost all the notable English raptors can be found in India—the peregrine falcon, the marsh harrier, the hen-harrier, the merlin, the kestrel, the sparrowhawk, and the buzzard. These birds are significantly more plentiful in India than in the British Isles.

Thus far we have spoken chiefly of birds that are found in the plains of India all the year round. We have now to deal with migrants. As was to be expected, many of these are common to Hindustan and to England.

Thus far, we've mainly talked about birds that are found in the plains of India all year round. Now we need to discuss migrants. As expected, many of these are common to both Hindustan and England.

Surprising as it may seem, stationary birds are the exception rather than the rule. The majority of species, like viceroys and lieutenant-governors, divide their time more or less equally between two different [7] places. It is by no means always easy to determine whether any particular species is a migrant one or not. The mere fact that specimens of it are seen in any given place at all seasons of the year is not sufficient to prove that it is non-migratory. For the birds of a species we saw six months ago are not necessarily the same ones that we have with us to-day. To take a concrete example, the crested lark (Galerita cristata) is found in Lahore all the year round, but is far more plentiful in summer than in winter, which is the only time when it is seen in England. The species is therefore a migratory one.

Surprisingly, stationary birds are more of an exception than the norm. Most species, like viceroys and lieutenant-governors, split their time pretty evenly between two different places. It’s not always easy to tell whether a certain species migrates or not. Just because you spot them in a location throughout the year doesn’t necessarily mean they don’t migrate. The birds we saw six months ago might not be the same ones we see today. For example, the crested lark (Galerita cristata) is present in Lahore year-round but is much more common in the summer than in the winter, which is the only time it appears in England. Therefore, this species is considered migratory.

The general rule as regards migratory birds is that they breed in the north and then go south for a season to enjoy themselves. Great Britain is further north than India and has a much colder climate, hence we should expect birds to crowd to India for the pleasant cold weather and go to England for the genial summer. This does happen to a large extent. Yet there are surprisingly few birds which winter in India and summer in England. The only common ones that I can call to mind are the wagtails, the pipits, and the quail (Coturnix communis). There are two reasons for this. The first is that migration takes place in a more or less northerly and southerly direction, and the British Isles are not due north of India. The second reason is that England is a long way south of the Arctic Circle. Its winter is therefore not cold enough for the taste of many birds. Geese, ducks, and snipe are cold-loving creatures. Their idea of nice mild weather is the English winter! In order to avoid anything in the [8] shape of heat they migrate very far north in summer, and in winter, being driven southwards by the intense Arctic cold, spread themselves all over the temperate zone. Thus it comes to pass that the full and the jack snipe, the grey lag-goose, the mallard, the gadwall, the pintail and the shoveller ducks, the widgeon and the teal, are winter visitors both to India and the British Isles. But whereas snipe, geese, and most ducks leave India for the hot weather, many of them remain in Great Britain for the summer and nest there. It is probable that the birds which spend the winter in Great Britain go further north to breed, their place in the British Isles being taken by species that have wintered in Africa. The north of Scotland, even, is too far south to serve as a breeding place for some species. The little jack snipe (Gallinago gallinula) is one of these; he never breeds in England, whereas the common or full snipe (Gallinago cœlestis) does. Hence the former is set down as a migrant in England, while the latter is thought to be a permanent resident. In point of fact both are migrants, as we see in India, but while some full snipe find a Scotch summer cool enough for them to breed in, all jack snipe find it insufferably hot.

The general rule about migratory birds is that they breed in the north and then head south for a season to have some fun. Great Britain is farther north than India and has a much colder climate, so we would expect birds to flock to India for the pleasant cooler weather and go to England for the nice summer. This does happen to a large extent. However, there are surprisingly few birds that winter in India and summer in England. The only common ones I can think of are the wagtails, the pipits, and the quail (Coturnix communis). There are two reasons for this. The first is that migration generally happens in a more or less north-south direction, and the British Isles are not directly north of India. The second reason is that England is quite far south of the Arctic Circle. Its winter is therefore not cold enough for the liking of many birds. Geese, ducks, and snipe prefer colder conditions. Their idea of nice mild weather is actually the English winter! To escape any kind of heat, they migrate very far north in summer, and in winter, driven southward by the harsh Arctic cold, they spread throughout the temperate zone. As a result, the full and jack snipe, the greylag goose, the mallard, the gadwall, the pintail and the shoveler ducks, the wigeon and the teal, all winter in both India and the British Isles. However, while snipe, geese, and most ducks leave India for the hot weather, many of them stay in Great Britain for the summer and nest there. It’s likely that the birds that winter in Great Britain go further north to breed, with their spots in the British Isles being taken by species that have wintered in Africa. Even the north of Scotland is too far south to be a breeding ground for some species. The little jack snipe (Gallinago gallinula) is one of these; it never breeds in England, whereas the common or full snipe (Gallinago cœlestis) does. Therefore, the former is labeled as a migrant in England, while the latter is considered a permanent resident. In reality, both are migrants, as we observe in India, but while some full snipe find a Scottish summer cool enough to breed in, all jack snipe find it unbearably hot.

THE REDSHANK. (TOTANUS CALIDRIS)

THE REDSHANK. (TOTANUS CALIDRIS)
(One of the British birds found in India)

THE REDSHANK. (TOTANUS CALIDRIS)
(A British bird also found in India)

[9]

A curious fact regarding snipe in India is that these birds appear in the south earlier than they do in the north. I do not know the earliest date after the end of the hot weather on which a snipe has been shot in the Punjab, but believe it to be considerably later than the last week in August, at which time snipe are regularly shot in the Madras Presidency. This is not what we should have expected. It is but reasonable to suppose that the earliest birds to arrive in India would take up their winter quarters in the north, and that the later arrivals, finding all eligible residences in the north already occupied, would go farther afield. The only explanation of the phenomenon which occurs to me is that the most northerly birds are the first to feel the approaching Arctic winter and so are the first to migrate. These, when they arrive in India, find the northern portion of the peninsula too hot for them, so pass on southwards until they come to the places where the temperature is at that season lower.

A curious fact about snipe in India is that these birds show up in the south earlier than in the north. I’m not sure of the earliest date after the hot weather when a snipe has been shot in the Punjab, but I believe it’s definitely later than the last week of August, which is when snipe are routinely shot in the Madras Presidency. This is not what we would expect. It makes sense to think that the first birds to arrive in India would settle in the north for the winter, and that the later arrivals, finding all the good spots in the north already taken, would move further south. The only explanation I can think of for this phenomenon is that the birds from the north are the first to sense the approaching Arctic winter and therefore start migrating first. When they get to India, they find the northern part of the peninsula too hot for them, so they continue south until they find places with cooler temperatures during that time of year.

This article has already reached an undue length, yet quite a number of birds, more or less common in England and in India, have not been mentioned. On this account I owe apologies to the cuckoo, the stint, the sandpiper, the redshank, the ringed and the Kentish plovers. But the names of these and of eight score others, are they not written in the appendix?

This article has already become too long, yet several birds, more or less common in England and India, haven't been mentioned. For this, I apologize to the cuckoo, the stint, the sandpiper, the redshank, the ringed plover, and the Kentish plover. But aren't the names of these and eighty others listed in the appendix?

[10]


THE BLUE BIRD

As I write my tympanic membranes are being somewhat rudely shaken by the clamorous voices of a brood of young blue jays, which are in a nest somewhere in one of the chimneys of my bungalow.

As I write, my eardrums are being somewhat rudely disturbed by the loud voices of a group of young blue jays, which are in a nest somewhere in one of the chimneys of my bungalow.

From the point of view of the blue jays the site they have chosen for their nursery is an admirable one; indeed, had the architect of the bungalow received a handsome “tip” he could not have provided the birds with more comfortable accommodation.

From the perspective of the blue jays, the location they've picked for their nursery is excellent; honestly, if the designer of the bungalow had gotten a generous "tip," he couldn't have given the birds better living conditions.

The shaft of the chimney is not straight, as, in my humble opinion, it should be. At a few feet from the top it is bent at a right angle, and runs horizontally for a short distance before it again assumes what I consider to be its normal course.

The chimney isn’t straight like it should be, in my opinion. A few feet from the top, it bends at a right angle and runs horizontally for a bit before it goes back to what I think is its normal direction.

The architect was, however, not such a fool as he may appear, for it is quite impossible to clean properly the chimney of his design; it must therefore take fire sooner or later, and the fire may spread and result in the destruction of the house. The re-erection thereof would of course mean more work for the said architect.

The architect wasn't as foolish as he might seem, because it's impossible to properly clean the chimney he designed; it will eventually catch fire, and that fire could spread and destroy the house. Rebuilding it would obviously mean more work for him.

The blue jays are as satisfied as the designer with the chimney, because the horizontal portion forms a shelf [11] upon which they can lay their eggs. These are visible neither from above nor from below, and they are as inaccessible as invisible, for the chimney is so narrow as to baffle all attempts at ascent or descent on the part of human beings.

The blue jays are just as happy as the designer with the chimney, since the horizontal section creates a shelf where they can lay their eggs. These eggs can’t be seen from above or below, and they’re as unreachable as they are unseen, because the chimney is so narrow that it stops any attempts by humans to climb up or down.

The blue jays make good to my ear what they deny my eye. The young hopefuls utter unceasingly a loud cry resembling that of some creature in distress. This is what I have to listen to all the time I am in the bungalow. Outside, the parent birds make the welkin ring with their raucous voices. Never were father and mother prouder of their offspring or fonder of proclaiming the fact. When not cumbered about much serving they squat either on the roof or on a blue gum tree hard by, and, at regular intervals, utter a short, sharp, harsh “Tshow.” This is emphasised by a jerk of the tail; the blue jay does nothing without first consulting its caudal appendage.

The blue jays provide a sound that my ears appreciate but my eyes don't see. The young ones continuously make a loud cry that sounds like an animal in distress. This is what I have to listen to all the time I’m in the bungalow. Outside, the parent birds make the sky ring with their harsh voices. Never have a mother and father been prouder of their young ones or more eager to announce it. When they’re not busy taking care of them, they perch either on the roof or on a nearby blue gum tree and, at regular intervals, let out a quick, sharp, harsh "Tshow." This is emphasized by a flick of their tail; the blue jay does nothing without first checking with its tail.

On the occasions when I made vain attempts to obtain a look at the young birds the parents took to their wings, and, as they sped through the air, uttered cries so harsh and dry-sounding as to make me feel quite thirsty!

On the times when I tried to catch a glimpse of the young birds, the parents flew off, and as they rushed through the air, they let out cries that were so harsh and dry-sounding that they made me feel really thirsty!

The blue jay is so familiar to us Anglo-Indians as to need no description. We have all admired the bird as it lazily sailed through the air on outstretched pinions of pale blue and rich ultramarine. We have, each of us, watched it perched on a railing looking out for its insect quarry. It is then comparatively inconspicuous, its neck and wing coverts being the hue of a faded port-wine stain. We have seen it pounce upon some object [12] too small for us to distinguish, and either devour it then and there or bear it off in triumph.

The blue jay is so well known to us Anglo-Indians that it hardly needs a description. We’ve all admired the bird as it glided through the air on its wide wings of pale blue and vibrant ultramarine. Each of us has watched it sitting on a railing, searching for its insect prey. At that moment, it blends in more, its neck and wing feathers having the color of a faded port-wine stain. We’ve seen it swoop down on something too small for us to make out and either eat it right away or carry it off in triumph.

We all know that the bird is not a jay at all, that its proper name is the Indian roller (Coracias indica), that it is related to the kingfisher family, and that it is called a jay merely on account of its gaudy plumage.

We all know that the bird isn’t a jay at all, that its real name is the Indian roller (Coracias indica), that it’s related to the kingfisher family, and that it’s called a jay simply because of its flashy feathers.

Next to its colour the most striking thing about the blue jay is its wonderful power of flight. Ordinarily the bird is content to flap along at an easy pace, but, when it likes, it can move for a little as though it were shot out of a catapult; moreover, it is able to completely change its course with startling rapidity; hence even the swiftest birds of prey find it no child’s play to catch a roller bird. A good idea of its aerial performances may be obtained by watching it attack a kite that persists in hovering about in the neighbourhood of the nest. Blue jays, like king-crows and doves, are exceedingly short-tempered when they have young.

Next to its color, the most impressive thing about the blue jay is its incredible flying ability. Usually, the bird is happy to coast along at a relaxed speed, but when it wants to, it can dart off like it's been shot from a catapult; plus, it can change direction with shocking speed. That's why even the fastest birds of prey find it difficult to catch a blue jay. You can get a good sense of its aerial skills by watching it chase a kite that keeps hovering near its nest. Blue jays, like crows and doves, are extremely short-tempered when they have young ones.

This species seems to indulge in very little sleep; it is up betimes, and may be seen about long after every other day bird, with the possible exception of the king-crow, is fast asleep.

This species doesn’t seem to sleep much at all; it’s up early and can be seen long after every other day bird, except maybe the king-crow, is sound asleep.

The blue jay is a good friend to the gardener, since it feeds exclusively on insects and small animals. Jerdon cites as the chief articles of its diet, large insects, grasshoppers, crickets, mantidæ, and beetles, with an occasional field-mouse or shrew. To this list he might have added frogs and small snakes.

The blue jay is a great ally for gardeners because it only eats insects and small animals. Jerdon highlights the main parts of its diet, which include large insects, grasshoppers, crickets, mantids, and beetles, along with an occasional field mouse or shrew. He could also have included frogs and small snakes in that list.

THE INDIAN ROLLER, OR “BLUE JAY.” (CORACIAS INDICA)

THE INDIAN ROLLER, OR “BLUE JAY.” (CORACIAS INDICA)

THE INDIAN ROLLER, OR “BLUE JAY.” (CORACIAS INDICA)

[13]

At most seasons of the year the blue jay strikes one as a rather sluggish bird, being content to squat on a perch for a great part of the day and wait patiently for quarry to come its way. At the breeding season, however, it becomes very sprightly. It is then more than usually vociferous and indulges in a course of aerial gymnastics. It may be seen at these throughout the month of March, now towering high above the earth, then dropping headlong down, to suddenly check itself and sail away, emitting the while the hoarsest and wheeziest notes imaginable, and behaving generally like the proverbial March hare. These performances are either actual love-making or a prelude to it. By the end of March the various birds have sorted themselves out, and then the billing and cooing stage begins.

At most times of the year, the blue jay seems like a pretty lethargic bird, happy to sit on a branch for much of the day and wait patiently for food to come its way. However, during the breeding season, it becomes very lively. It becomes more vocal than usual and engages in some impressive aerial displays. You can see this throughout March, soaring high above the ground, then diving down, only to suddenly pull up and glide away, all while making the loudest, choppy sounds you can imagine, acting like the classic March hare. These antics are either actual courtship or a warm-up for it. By the end of March, the different birds have paired off, and then the flirting phase begins.

At this season the birds are invariably found in pairs; the cock and hen delight to sit side by side on some exposed branch. Like the young couples that moon about Hyde Park on Sundays, blue jays do not mind spooning in public. As the sexes dress alike it is not possible to say which of a couple is the cock and which is the hen. Under such circumstances naturalists always assume that the bird which makes the advances is the cock. I am not at all sure that this assumption is justified. Among human beings the ladies very frequently set their caps at the men. Why should not the fair sex among birds do likewise?

At this time of year, birds are usually seen in pairs; the male and female love to sit next to each other on a visible branch. Just like the young couples strolling through Hyde Park on Sundays, blue jays don’t mind being affectionate in public. Since the males and females look alike, it’s hard to tell which is which. Because of this, naturalists often assume that the bird that takes the initiative is the male. I’m not sure this assumption is accurate. In humans, women often pursue men. Why shouldn’t female birds do the same?

In many species the sexes dress differently, and it is then easy to discover which sex “makes the running,” and in such cases this is by no means always the cock. I have seen one hen paradise flycatcher drive away another and then go and make up to a cock bird. Similarly I have seen two hen orioles behave in a very unladylike manner to one another, all because they both had [14] designs on the same cock. He sat and looked on from a distance at the contest, and would assuredly have purred with delight had he known how to do so! But of this more anon. The blue-jay lovers sit on a branch, side by side, and gaze upon one another with enraptured eyes. Suddenly one of them betakes itself to some other tree, uttering its hoarse screeches as it flies. Its companion follows almost immediately and then begins to bow and scrape, puff out its neck, slowly wave its tail, and utter unmusical cries. The bird which is being thus courted adds its voice to that of its companion. The raucous duet over, silence reigns for a little. Then one of the birds moves on, to be followed by its companion, and the above performance is repeated, and will continue to be repeated dozens of times before the birds give themselves over to family cares.

In many species, males and females look different, making it easy to see which sex is "in charge," and it's not always the male. I've seen one female paradise flycatcher chase off another, only to approach a male. Similarly, I've watched two female orioles act in a very unrefined manner towards each other because they both wanted the same male. He sat back and watched the rivalry from a distance, and he would have surely been pleased if he could express it! But more on that later. The blue jay couples sit on a branch, side by side, gazing at each other with admiring eyes. Suddenly, one flies off to another tree, making its harsh calls as it goes. The other follows almost immediately and starts to bow, puff out its chest, slowly wave its tail, and make awkward sounds. The bird being courted joins in the noise. After their loud duet, there's a moment of silence. Then, one of the birds moves again, followed closely by its partner, and they repeat the whole performance, continuing to do this dozens of times before they settle down to care for their young.

The greatest admirer of the blue jay could not call its nest a work of art. The eggs are laid in a hole in a tree or building. Usually the hole is more or less lined by a promiscuous collection of grass, tow, feathers, and the like, but sometimes the birds are content to lay their eggs in the bare cavity.

The biggest fan of the blue jay wouldn’t describe its nest as a masterpiece. The eggs are typically placed in a hollow in a tree or a building. Usually, this hollow is somewhat lined with a random assortment of grass, fibers, feathers, and similar materials, but sometimes the birds are fine with just laying their eggs in the empty space.

The blue jay, although so brazen over its courtship, strongly objects to having its family affairs pried into, so if you would find its nursery you must, unless you are lucky, exercise some patience. The birds steadfastly refuse to visit the nest when they know they are being watched. If patience be a virtue great, the blue jay is a most virtuous bird, for, if it is aware that it is being observed, it will take up a perch and sit there for hours, mournfully croaking, rather than betray the [15] whereabouts of its eggs or young. Most of the nests I have seen have been discovered by accident. For example, when going along a road I have had occasion to look round suddenly at some bird flying overhead and caught sight of a roller entering a hole in a tree.

The blue jay, while bold in its courtship, is very protective of its family and doesn't like having its private life invaded. So, if you want to find its nest, you'll need to have some patience unless you're lucky. The birds will not approach the nest if they know they're being watched. If patience is a great virtue, then the blue jay is quite virtuous, because if it realizes it's being observed, it will just sit on a branch for hours, making mournful sounds instead of revealing where its eggs or chicks are hidden. Most of the nests I've come across were found by chance. For instance, while walking along a road, I've unexpectedly looked up at a bird flying by and spotted a jay entering a hole in a tree.

Some days ago I was out with a friend, when we saw a hoopoe, with food in its mouth, disappear into a hole in the wall of a Hindu temple. The aperture was about seven feet from the ground, so, in order to look into it, I mounted my friend’s back. While I was investigating the hoopoe’s hole, a blue jay flew out of another hole in the wall within a yard of my face!

A few days ago, I was out with a friend when we spotted a hoopoe with food in its mouth disappearing into a hole in the wall of a Hindu temple. The opening was about seven feet high, so I climbed onto my friend’s back to get a better look. While I was checking out the hoopoe’s hole, a blue jay flew out of another hole in the wall just a foot away from my face!

Like Moses of old, I turned aside to investigate this new wonder, and found that the hole went two and a half feet into the wall, and that its aperture was a square six inches in both length and breadth. The floor of this little alcove was covered with earth and tiny bits of dirty straw, which may or may not have been put there by the blue jay. On this lay a clutch of four glossy white eggs, nearly as large as those laid by the degenerate Indian murghi. Fortunately for those blue jays I am not an egg collector. As it was, I did remove one of them for a lady who was anxious to have it, but this was not missed. Birds cannot count.

Like Moses of old, I stopped to check out this new wonder and discovered that the hole went two and a half feet into the wall, and that its opening was a square six inches in both length and width. The floor of this little alcove was covered with dirt and small pieces of dirty straw, which may or may not have been left by the blue jay. On this sat a clutch of four glossy white eggs, nearly as big as those laid by the degenerate Indian murghi. Luckily for those blue jays, I'm not an egg collector. As it was, I did take one for a woman who was eager to have it, but it wasn’t missed. Birds can’t count.

[16]


Sparrows in the nursery

The sparrow, as every Anglo-Indian knows, is a bird that goes about dumping down nests in sahibs’ bungalows. It is greatly assisted in this noble work by the native of India, who has brought to the acme of perfection the art of jerry-building. In the ramshackle, half-finished modern bungalow the rafters that support the ceiling never, by any chance, fit properly into the walls. There are thus in every room a number of cracks, holes, and crevices in which the sparrows love to nest. As a matter of fact, these are not at all safe nesting places. Apart from the fact that the nest is liable to be pulled down at any moment by an angry human being, the situation is dangerous, because there is nothing to prevent a restless young bird from falling out of the nest and thus terminating a promising career. A few days ago a servant brought me a baby sparrow that had fallen out of a nest in the pantry. I always feel inclined to wring the neck of any sparrow that fate has put within my grasp, for I have many a score to pay off against the species. Upon this occasion, however, I felt mercifully inclined, so took the young bird, which was nearly covered with feathers, and offered it bread soaked in milk. This it swallowed greedily. When [17] the youngster was as full up inside as the Hammersmith ’bus on a wet day, I told the bearer to put it in the cage in which my amadavats dwell. When I left for office I directed the man to feed the new arrival. On my return in the evening the bearer informed me that the young hopeful had declined its food. Now, a young sparrow refuses to eat only when it is stuffed to the brim. It was thus evident that its parents had found it out and were feeding it, in spite of the fact that the nest from which it came was in the pantry on the east side of the house, while its new quarters were in the west verandah.

The sparrow, as every Anglo-Indian knows, is a bird that commonly builds nests in sahibs’ bungalows. It gets a lot of help from the locals, who have perfected the art of shoddy construction. In the rundown, half-finished modern bungalow, the rafters that hold up the ceiling never quite fit into the walls. As a result, every room has numerous cracks, holes, and crevices where sparrows like to nest. In reality, these aren’t very safe nesting spots. Besides the fact that a nest can be destroyed at any moment by an angry human, it’s dangerous because there’s nothing to stop an restless young bird from falling out and ending a promising life. A few days ago, a servant brought me a baby sparrow that had fallen out of a nest in the pantry. I always feel tempted to wring the neck of any sparrow that fate brings my way, as I have plenty of scores to settle with the species. However, this time I felt merciful, so I took the young bird, which was nearly covered with feathers, and offered it bread soaked in milk. It devoured it eagerly. When the little one was as full as a Hammersmith bus on a rainy day, I told the bearer to put it in the cage where my amadavats live. Before heading to the office, I instructed the man to feed the new arrival. When I came back in the evening, the bearer told me that the young hopeful had refused to eat. A young sparrow only turns down food when it’s stuffed to the brim. It was clear that its parents had found it and were feeding it, despite the fact that the nest it came from was in the pantry on the east side of the house, while its new home was in the west verandah.

The next day a second sparrow fell out of the nest in the pantry and was also consigned to the amadavats’ cage. At bed-time that night I took a look at the birds, and found that the two young sparrows had tucked themselves snugly in the seed tin! The next morning a third sparrow from the same nest was brought to me; it was put in the cage along with its brethren. As my office was closed on the day in question, I had the cage placed in front of my study window. I could thus watch the doings of the latest additions to my aviary. The hen sparrow does the lion’s share of the feeding; she works like a slave from morning to night. At intervals, varying from one to ten minutes, throughout the day she appears with a beakful of food, which consists chiefly of green caterpillars.

The next day, a second sparrow fell out of the nest in the pantry and was also put in the amadavats’ cage. At bedtime that night, I checked on the birds and found that the two young sparrows had snuggled up in the seed tin! The next morning, a third sparrow from the same nest was brought to me; it was placed in the cage with its siblings. Since my office was closed that day, I set the cage in front of my study window. This way, I could watch the activities of the newest members of my aviary. The female sparrow does most of the feeding; she works tirelessly from morning to night. At intervals ranging from one to ten minutes throughout the day, she appears with a beak full of food, mainly green caterpillars.

It is the custom to speak of the sparrow as a curse to the husbandman. The bird is popularly supposed to live on grain, fruit, seedlings, and buds—those of [18] valuable plants by preference. There is no denying the fact that the sparrow does devour a certain amount of fruit and grain, but, so far from being a pest, I believe that the good it does by destroying noxious insects far outweighs the harm. Adult sparrows frequently feed on insects. I have watched them hawking flies in company with the swifts, and the skill displayed by the “spadger” showed that his was no ’prentice hand at the game.

It’s common to refer to the sparrow as a nuisance for farmers. People generally think that this bird feeds on grain, fruit, seedlings, and buds—especially those of valuable plants. While it’s true that sparrows do consume some fruit and grain, I believe the benefits they provide by getting rid of harmful insects far exceed the damage they cause. Adult sparrows often eat insects. I’ve seen them catching flies alongside swifts, and the skill the “spadger” showed proved he was no novice at the task.

Sparrow nestlings in the early stages are fed almost exclusively on caterpillars, grubs, and insects. As there are usually five or six baby sparrows in a brood, and as these have appalling appetites, they must consume an enormous number of insects. Let us work out a little sum. We may assume that the sparrow brings at least three caterpillars in each beakful of food she carries to her brood. She feeds them at least fifteen times in the hour, and works for not less than twelve hours in the day. I timed the sparrows in question to commence feeding operations at 5.30 a.m., and when I left the bungalow at 6 p.m. the birds were still at it. Thus the hen sparrow brings in something like 540 insects per diem to her brood. She feeds them on this diet for at least twenty days, so that the brood is responsible for no less than 10,000 insects, mostly caterpillars, before its units are ready to fend for themselves. According to Hume, the sparrow in India brings up two broods in the year. I should have doubled this figure, since the species appears to be always breeding. But it is better to understate than exaggerate. We thus arrive at the conclusion that the hen sparrow [19] destroys each year over 20,000 insects, mostly injurious, in the feeding of her young. Add to this number those she herself consumes, those the cock eats, and those he brings to the nest, and you have a fine insect mortality bill.

Sparrow chicks in the early stages are fed almost entirely on caterpillars, grubs, and insects. Since there are usually five or six baby sparrows in a nest, and since they have huge appetites, they need to eat a massive number of insects. Let’s do some quick math. We can assume that the sparrow brings at least three caterpillars in each beakful of food she carries to her chicks. She feeds them at least fifteen times an hour and works for at least twelve hours a day. I timed the sparrows to start feeding at 5:30 a.m., and when I left the bungalow at 6 p.m., they were still at it. So, the female sparrow brings in about 540 insects per day for her nestlings. She feeds them this way for at least twenty days, meaning the nest is responsible for at least 10,000 insects, mostly caterpillars, before the chicks are ready to take care of themselves. According to Hume, sparrows in India raise two broods a year. I could double this number since the species seems to be breeding constantly. But it’s better to understate than to exaggerate. So, we can conclude that the female sparrow destroys over 20,000 insects each year, mostly harmful ones, while feeding her young. Add to this the number she eats herself, what the male eats, and what he brings to the nest, and you have quite a significant insect death toll.

The movements of the mother bird when feeding her young are so rapid that it is not easy to determine what it is she brings to the nest, even though the objects hang down from her beak; the same applies to the cock. In order to make quite certain of the nature of the food she was bringing, I sought, by frightening her, to make her drop a beakful; accordingly, at one of her visits I tapped the window-pane smartly just as she was about to ram the food down the gaping mouth of a young bird. She flew off chirruping with anger and alarm, but kept her bill tightly closed on the food she was carrying. As the parents had to feed the young ones through the bars of a cage the process required some manipulation, and, in spite of its care, the bird sometimes dropped part of its burden; but, almost before I had time to move, it had dashed down to the ground and retrieved it. However, by dint of careful watching I managed to bang the window immediately after the hen had dropped something of a dark colour. Having frightened her away I rushed outside and found that the object in question was part of a sausage-shaped sac containing a number of tiny green grubs. After a few minutes the hen returned with her beak full. Her fright had made her suspicious, so she perched on the verandah trellis-work and looked around for a little. Nine times she flew towards the cage, but on each occasion her [20] courage failed her, to the intense disgust of her clamouring brood. At the tenth attempt she plucked up sufficient courage to feed the young birds.

The movements of the mother bird while feeding her chicks are so quick that it's hard to tell what she's bringing to the nest, even though the food is hanging from her beak; the same goes for the male. To figure out exactly what she was bringing, I tried to scare her into dropping a beakful of food; so, during one of her visits, I tapped the window sharply just as she was about to stuff the food into the open mouth of a chick. She flew off, chirping in anger and alarm, but kept her beak firmly closed on the food she was carrying. Since the parents had to feed the chicks through the bars of a cage, it required some maneuvering, and despite being careful, the bird sometimes dropped part of its load; but almost before I could react, it darted down to the ground to grab it back. However, by carefully watching, I managed to bang the window right after the hen dropped something dark. After scaring her away, I rushed outside and found that the item was a piece of a sausage-shaped sac filled with tiny green grubs. After a few minutes, the hen came back with her beak full. Her fright had made her cautious, so she perched on the trellis and looked around for a bit. Nine times she flew toward the cage, but each time she lost her nerve, much to the annoyance of her hungry chicks. On the tenth try, she finally gathered enough courage to feed the young birds.

At a subsequent visit she dropped a caterpillar, and I frightened her away before she could retrieve it. I found it to be alive and about an inch in length.

At a later visit, she dropped a caterpillar, and I scared her off before she could pick it up. I discovered that it was alive and about an inch long.

On another occasion I saw her ramming something black down the throat of a young hopeful. Frightening her away, I went outside and found the youthful bird making valiant attempts to swallow a whole mulberry. But it was not often that she gave them fruit; green caterpillars formed quite nine-tenths of what she brought in; the remainder was composed chiefly of grubs, with an occasional grasshopper or moth. As the young grew older the proportion of insect food given to them diminished until, when they were about twenty-two days old, their diet was made up principally of grain.

On another occasion, I saw her forcing something black down the throat of a young bird. Scaring her off, I went outside and found the young bird making brave attempts to swallow an entire mulberry. But she didn't often give them fruit; green caterpillars made up almost ninety percent of what she brought in, with the rest being mostly grubs and the occasional grasshopper or moth. As the young birds got older, the amount of insect food they received decreased until, when they were about twenty-two days old, their diet mostly consisted of grains.

The day on which the third young sparrow was put into the cage was a warm one, so at 2 p.m., when the shade temperature was about 115°, I brought the cage into the comparatively cool bungalow, for the sake of the amadavats. The cock sparrow witnessed the removal of the cage and did not hesitate to give me a bit of his mind. In a minute or so the hen returned with her beak full of green caterpillars. When she found the cage gone, she, too, expressed her opinion of me and of mankind in general in no uncertain terms. It was the last straw. Earlier in the day I had removed one of the baby sparrows from the cage and placed it in a cigar-ash tray outside the cage. The hen had affected not to notice that anything had happened, and [21] fed it in the ash-tray as though she were unconscious of the removal. When, however, the whole cage and its contents disappeared it was quite useless for her to pretend that nothing was wrong, so she treated me to her best “Billingsgate.”

The day the third young sparrow was put into the cage was warm, so at 2 p.m., when the shade temperature reached about 115°, I brought the cage into the cooler bungalow for the sake of the amadavats. The male sparrow saw me move the cage and didn’t hold back his thoughts. A minute later, the female returned with her beak full of green caterpillars. When she discovered the cage was gone, she made it clear how she felt about me and humanity in general. It was the final straw. Earlier that day, I had taken one of the baby sparrows out of the cage and placed it in an ashtray outside the cage. The female acted like she hadn’t noticed anything and fed it in the ashtray as if nothing had happened. However, when the entire cage and all its contents vanished, she couldn’t pretend that everything was fine, so she unleashed her best insults at me.

After the cage had been inside for about three-quarters of an hour the young “spadgers” began to feel the pangs of hunger, and made this known by giving vent to a torrent of chirrups which differed in no way from those that make the adult so offensive. All that the poor mother could do was to answer from the outside. I felt, that afternoon, that I was paying off with interest some of my score against the sparrow.

After the cage had been inside for about forty-five minutes, the young sparrows started to feel hungry and expressed this by chirping loudly, just like the adults do, which can be quite annoying. The poor mother could only respond from the outside. That afternoon, I felt like I was getting back at the sparrows for all the trouble they had caused me.

The next day I did not take the cage into the bungalow, because I wanted to ascertain whether sparrows feed their young throughout the day, or whether they indulge in a noonday siesta. They kept it up, at their respective rates, throughout the day, although the thermometer in the shade must have risen to 115°. After the hen had disburdened herself of the food she brought, she would perch for a moment on the trellis, and pant with open beak as though she were thoroughly exhausted.

The next day I didn't bring the cage into the bungalow because I wanted to find out if sparrows feed their young all day long or if they take a midday nap. They kept going at their own pace throughout the day, even though the temperature in the shade must have hit 115°. After the mother bird dropped off the food she had collected, she would sit for a moment on the trellis, breathing heavily with her beak open as if she were completely worn out.

I have long been trying to ascertain how birds in the nest obtain the liquid they require. Do the succulent caterpillars, on which young sparrows are fed, provide them with sufficient moisture, or do the parents water them? Although I spent several hours in watching those sparrows, I am not able to answer the question satisfactorily. I placed a bowl of water on the ground near the cage, hoping that this would tempt the hen [22] bird to drink, and that I should see her carry some of the liquid to her offspring. But she took no notice of the water. She certainly used to come to the cage sometimes with her beak apparently empty, and yet insert it into the open mouth of a young one. Was she then watering the nestling, or did her beak hold some small seeds that did not protrude? It seems incredible that unfledged birds exposed to the temperature of an Indian summer require no water; nevertheless, I never actually saw any pass from the crop of the parents to those of the youngsters.

I've been trying to figure out how baby birds in the nest get the liquids they need. Do the juicy caterpillars that young sparrows eat provide enough moisture, or do the parents somehow hydrate them? Even after spending hours observing those sparrows, I can't answer that question. I set a bowl of water on the ground near the cage, hoping it would encourage the mother bird to drink and then share some with her chicks. But she completely ignored the water. She definitely came to the cage sometimes with her beak looking empty, yet she would still feed a young one. Was she watering the nestling, or did she have some small seeds in her beak that weren't visible? It seems unbelievable that fledgling birds exposed to Indian summer temperatures need no water; still, I never actually saw any liquid pass from the parents’ crop to their young.

[23]


Caring for Young Birds
AFTER THEY LEAVE THE NEST

It has been urged as an objection to the Darwinian theory that Natural Selection, if that force exists, must tend to destroy species rather than cause new ones to come into being. Nearly all birds leave the nest before they are fully developed. When they first come out of the nursery they have attained neither their full powers of flight nor complete skill in obtaining food. Every young bird, no matter how fine a specimen it be, leaves the nest an inexperienced weakling, and can therefore stand no chance in competition with the fully grown and experienced members of the species. Natural Selection takes an individual as it finds it and pays no attention to potentialities.

It has been argued against the Darwinian theory that Natural Selection, if it exists, is more likely to lead to the extinction of species rather than the emergence of new ones. Almost all birds leave the nest before they are fully developed. When they first leave the nest, they haven't fully mastered flying or feeding themselves. Every young bird, no matter how impressive, leaves the nest an inexperienced weakling and has no chance of competing with the fully grown and experienced members of its species. Natural Selection takes individuals as they are and ignores their potential.

That such an objection should have been urged against the theory of Natural Selection is proof of the fact that naturalists are inclined to forget that, with many, if not all, species of birds, the duties of the parents towards their offspring by no means cease when the young birds leave the nest.

That such an objection has been raised against the theory of Natural Selection shows that naturalists tend to overlook that, for many, if not all, bird species, the responsibilities of the parents towards their young do not stop when the fledglings leave the nest.

The parent birds, in many cases, continue to feed their young long after these are apparently well able to fend for themselves. This fact is not sufficiently emphasised in books on natural history. On the other [24] hand, such works lay stress upon the fact that in many species of birds the parents drive their offspring away from the place of their birth in order that the numbers of the species in the locality shall not outgrow the food supply. How far this is a general characteristic of birds I do not know. What I desire to emphasise is that the driving-away process, when it occurs, does not take place until some time after the young have left the nest. The fact that the parent birds tend the young long after they have left the nest, and even after they are fully capable of holding their own in the struggle for existence, disposes of the above-cited objection to the theory of Natural Selection. Nature is so careful of the young warriors that she prolongs the instinct of parental affection longer than is absolutely necessary. So important is it that the young should have a fair start in life that she errs on the safe side.

The parent birds often keep feeding their young long after the chicks seem ready to take care of themselves. This point isn't highlighted enough in natural history books. Conversely, those books emphasize how many bird species push their young away from their birthplace to prevent overpopulation in the area that could exceed the available food. I'm not sure how widespread this behavior is among birds. What I want to highlight is that the process of pushing them away doesn't happen until some time after the young have left the nest. The fact that parent birds care for their young long after they've fledged and even when they're fully capable of surviving on their own counters the previous argument against the theory of Natural Selection. Nature is so protective of young birds that she extends the instinct of parental affection longer than necessary. It’s so crucial for the young to have a fair chance at life that she plays it safe.

It is common knowledge that foster-parents feed cuckoos when these have grown so large that, in order to reach the mouth of their spurious babes, the little foster-mothers have to perch on their shoulders.

It’s well-known that foster parents feed cuckoo chicks when they have grown so large that, to reach the mouths of their fake offspring, the little foster mothers have to sit on their shoulders.

The sight of a tiny bird feeding the great parasite is laughable, but it is also most instructive. It demonstrates how thoroughly bird mothers perform their duties.

The sight of a small bird feeding the large parasite is funny, but it's also very educational. It shows just how dedicated bird mothers are to their responsibilities.

Crows tend their young ones for weeks after they have left the nest. I have had ample opportunity of satisfying myself as to this.

Crows take care of their young for weeks after they leave the nest. I've had plenty of chances to confirm this.

It was my custom in Madras to breakfast on the verandah. A number of crows used to assemble daily to watch operations and to pick up the pieces of food [25] thrown to them. They would go farther when the opportunity occurred, and commit petty larceny.

It was my routine in Madras to have breakfast on the verandah. Every day, a group of crows would gather to watch what was happening and to grab the bits of food tossed to them. They would wander off when they got the chance and steal little things.

The crows were all grey-necked ones, with the exception of two belonging to the larger black species. But these latter are comparatively shy birds, and consequently used to hang about on the outskirts of the crowd.

The crows were all grey-necked, except for two that belonged to the larger black species. However, these black crows were relatively shy birds, so they tended to stay on the edges of the crowd.

Among the grey-necked crows was a family of four—the parents and two young birds. Every day, without fail, they used to visit the verandah; the two young birds made more noise than all the rest of the crows put together. They were easily recognisable, firstly, by their more raucous voices, and, secondly, by the pink inside of the mouth. When I first noticed them they were so old that, in size, they were very nearly equal to the mother. Further, the grey of the neck was sharply differentiated from the black portions of the plumage, showing that they had left the nest some time ago.

Among the grey-necked crows was a family of four—the parents and two young birds. Every day, without fail, they came to the porch; the two young birds made more noise than all the other crows combined. They were easy to recognize, first by their louder voices, and second by the pink inside their mouths. When I first spotted them, they were so old that they were almost the same size as the mother. Additionally, the grey of the neck was clearly different from the black parts of the plumage, indicating that they had fledged some time ago.

Unfortunately I did not make a note of the day on which they first put in an appearance. I can, however, safely say that they visited my verandah regularly for some weeks, during the whole of which time the mother bird fed them most assiduously. It was ludicrous to see the great creatures sidle up to mamma when she had seized a piece of toast, and open their red mouths, often pecking at one another out of jealousy.

Unfortunately, I didn't note the day when they first showed up. I can, however, confidently say that they visited my porch regularly for several weeks, during which time the mother bird took great care in feeding them. It was funny to see the big birds shuffle up to mom when she grabbed a piece of toast, opening their red mouths and often pecking at each other out of jealousy.

They were obviously well able to look after themselves; their flight was as powerful as that of the mother bird, yet she treated them as though they were infants, incapable of doing anything for themselves.

They clearly knew how to take care of themselves; their flight was just as strong as that of the mother bird, yet she acted as if they were babies, unable to do anything on their own.

[26]

At the beginning of the cold weather I changed my quarters, so was not able to witness the break-up of the crow family. Probably this did not occur until the following spring, when nesting operations commenced.

At the start of the cold weather, I moved to a different place, so I didn’t get to see the crow family break up. That probably didn’t happen until the next spring, when they started nesting.

The feeding of the young after they have left the nest and are full-grown is not confined to crows.

The feeding of the young after they’ve left the nest and are fully grown isn’t just something crows do.

I was walking one morning along a shady lane when I noticed on the grass by the roadside a bird which I did not recognise. It was a small creature, clothed in black and white, which tripped along like a wagtail. It had no tail, but it wagged the hind end of its body just as a sandpiper does. While I was trying to identify this strange creature, a young pied wagtail came running up to it with open mouth, into which the first bird popped something. I then saw that the unknown bird was simply a pied wagtail (Motacilla maderaspatensis) which had lost her tail! The young bird was fully as large as the mother, and having a respectable tail, which it wagged in a very sedate manner, looked far more imposing. The parts of the plumage which were black in the mother were brownish grey in the young bird. The white eyebrow was not so well defined in the youngster as in the adult, while the former had rather more white in the wing, but as regards size there was nothing to choose between the two. The young bird remained in close attendance on the mother. It was able to keep pace with her as she dashed after a flying insect. It ran after her begging continually for food. The mother swallowed most of the flies she caught, but now and again put one into the mouth of the young bird, but she [27] did so very severely, as if she were saying, “You are far too old to be fed; it is no use to pretend you cannot catch insects, you are a naughty, lazy, little bird!” But the lackadaisical air of the young one expressed more plainly than words: “Oh, mother, it tires me to chase insects. They move so fast. I have tried, but have caught so few, and am very hungry.”

I was walking one morning down a shady lane when I noticed a bird on the grass by the roadside that I didn’t recognize. It was a small creature, dressed in black and white, moving like a wagtail. It didn’t have a tail, but it wiggled the back end of its body just like a sandpiper. While I was trying to figure out what this strange bird was, a young pied wagtail ran up to it with its mouth open, and the first bird dropped something into its mouth. I then realized that the unknown bird was simply a pied wagtail (Motacilla maderaspatensis) that had lost its tail! The young bird was just as big as the mother, and with a nice tail that it shook in a very composed way, it looked much more impressive. The parts of the plumage that were black in the mother were brownish-grey in the young bird. The white eyebrow wasn’t as distinct in the young one as in the adult, while the youngster had a bit more white in its wing, but in terms of size, they were about the same. The young bird stayed close to the mother. It was able to keep up with her as she dashed after a flying insect. It ran after her, constantly begging for food. The mother caught most of the flies but occasionally fed one to the young bird, and she did so quite sternly, as if to say, “You’re too old to be fed; there’s no point in pretending you can’t catch insects; you’re a naughty, lazy little bird!” But the young bird’s lackadaisical attitude said more clearly than words: “Oh, mother, it’s exhausting to chase insects. They move so quickly. I’ve tried, but I’ve caught so few, and I’m really hungry.”

For several minutes the young wagtail followed the mother; then something arrested its attention, so that it tarried behind its parent. The mother moved away, apparently glad to be rid of the troublesome child for a little. Then she suddenly flew off. Presently the young wagtail looked round for its mother, and I was interested to see what would happen when it noticed that she had flown away. My curiosity was soon satisfied. Directly the young bird perceived that the mother had gone, it set itself most philosophically to catch insects, which it did with all the skill of an old bird, turning, twisting, doubling, with the elegance of an experienced wagtail.

For several minutes, the young wagtail followed its mother; then something caught its attention, causing it to fall behind. The mother moved on, seemingly relieved to be away from her pesky chick for a bit. Then she suddenly took off. Soon, the young wagtail looked around for its mother, and I was curious to see what would happen when it realized she was gone. My curiosity was quickly satisfied. As soon as the young bird noticed that the mother had left, it calmly focused on catching insects, doing so with the skill of a seasoned bird, turning, twisting, and dodging with the grace of an experienced wagtail.

I describe these two little incidents, not as anything wonderful, but as examples of what is continually going on in the world around us.

I mention these two small events, not as anything remarkable, but as examples of what is constantly happening in the world around us.

The parental instinct is probably developed in some birds more than in others, but I believe that in all cases the affection of a bird mother for her young persists long after they have left the nest, and for some time after they are fully capable of looking after themselves.

The parenting instinct is likely stronger in some birds than in others, but I believe that in every case, a bird mother’s love for her young lasts long after they leave the nest and even for a while after they can take care of themselves.

Birds are born with many instincts, but they have much to learn both before and after they leave the nest. [28] It is not until their education is complete, until the mother bird has taught them all she herself knows, until they are as strong or stronger than she, that the young birds are driven away and made to look after themselves.

Birds are born with various instincts, but they still have a lot to learn both before and after they leave the nest. [28] Only when their education is finished, once the mother bird has taught them everything she knows, and when they are as strong or stronger than she is, are the young birds pushed away and expected to fend for themselves.

THE INDIAN ADJUTANT. (LEPTOPTILUS DUBIUS)

THE INDIAN ADJUTANT. (LEPTOPTILUS DUBIUS)

THE INDIAN ADJUTANT. (LEPTOPTILUS DUBIUS)

[29]


The Adjutant Bird

The adjutant bird (Leptoptilus dubius) is one of Nature’s little jokes. It is a caricature of a bird, a mixture of gravity and clownishness. Everything about it is calculated to excite mirth—its weird figure, its great beak, its long, thin legs, its conspicuous pouch, its bald head, and every attitude it strikes. The adjutant bird is a stork which has acquired the habits of the vulture. Forsaking to a large extent frogs and such-like delicacies, which constitute the normal diet of its kind, it lives chiefly upon offal. Now, most, if not all, birds which feed on carrion have the head and neck devoid of feathers. This arrangement, if not ornamental, is very useful. The bare head and neck are, as “Eha” remarks, “the sleeves tucked up for earnest work.” The adjutant forms no exception to the rule, it wears the badge of its profession. But let me here give a full description of this truly comic bird. It stands five feet in its stockings. Its bill is over a foot in length and correspondingly massive. As we have seen, the whole head and neck are bare, except for a few feathers scattered over it like the hairs on an elephant’s head. The bare skin is not lacking in colour. On the forehead it is blackish; it becomes saffron-yellow on the upper neck, while lower [30] down it turns to brick-red. There is a ruff of white feathers round the base of the neck. This ruff, of course, appears entirely out of place and adds to the general grotesqueness of the bird. The back and wings are ashy black, becoming slaty grey at the breeding season. The lower parts are white.

The adjutant bird (Leptoptilus dubius) is one of nature's little jokes. It's a caricature of a bird, blending seriousness with clumsiness. Everything about it is designed to make you laugh—its strange shape, huge beak, long, skinny legs, noticeable pouch, bald head, and every pose it strikes. The adjutant bird is a stork that has picked up the habits of a vulture. Largely giving up on frogs and similar delicacies that are a typical part of its diet, it primarily feeds on carrion. Most, if not all, birds that eat dead animals have heads and necks without feathers. This arrangement, while not stylish, is very practical. The bare head and neck are, as “Eha” puts it, “sleeves tucked up for serious work.” The adjutant follows this rule, proudly displaying its professional badge. But let me give you a complete description of this truly amusing bird. It stands five feet tall. Its bill is over a foot long and correspondingly massive. As we’ve seen, the entire head and neck are bare, except for a few feathers scattered on it like the hairs on an elephant's head. The bare skin has some color. It’s blackish on the forehead, turning saffron-yellow on the upper neck, and further down, it becomes brick-red. There’s a ruff of white feathers around the base of the neck. This ruff looks entirely out of place and adds to the bird's overall ridiculousness. The back and wings are ashy black, turning slate gray during breeding season, while the lower parts are white.

As if the creature, thus arrayed, were not sufficiently comic, Nature has given it a great pouch which dangles from the neck. This is over a foot in length and hangs down like a bag when inflated. It is red in colour, spotted with black. Its situation naturally leads one to believe that it is connected with the gullet, that it is a receptacle into which the bird can hastily pass the garbage it swallows pending more complete disposal. But it is nothing of the sort. It does not communicate directly with the œsophagus. Knowing this, one is able to appreciate to the full the splendid mendacity of the writer to Chambers’s Journal in 1861, who declares that he witnessed an adjutant swallow a crow which he watched “pass into the sienna-toned pouch of the gaunt avenger. He who writes saw it done.”

As if the creature, dressed like this, weren't already ridiculous, Nature has given it a large pouch that hangs from its neck. It's over a foot long and dangles like a bag when it's inflated. It's red with black spots. Its position naturally makes one think it's linked to the throat, serving as a place for the bird to quickly store its food waste until it can properly dispose of it. But that's not the case at all. It doesn't connect directly to the esophagus. Knowing this makes it easier to appreciate the incredible falsehood told by the writer for Chambers’s Journal in 1861, who claims he saw an adjutant swallow a crow, which he observed "pass into the sienna-toned pouch of the gaunt avenger. He who writes saw it done."

Note the last sentence. The scribe was evidently of opinion that people would not believe him, so thought to clinch matters by bluffing! But, to do him justice, it is quite possible that he did see an adjutant swallow a crow, for other observers have witnessed this, but the remainder of the story rests upon the sandy foundation of the imagination. If the truth must be told, we do not know for certain what the use of this pouch is. Blyth suggested that it is analogous to the air cell attached to one lung only of the python or the boa-constrictor, [31] and, as in that case, no doubt supplies oxygen to the lungs during protracted meals. The bird can thus “guzzle” to its heart’s content without having to stop every now and then to take a “breather.”

Note the last sentence. The scribe clearly thought that people wouldn't believe him, so he decided to exaggerate! But, to be fair, it's entirely possible he did see an adjutant swallow a crow, as other witnesses have reported this, but the rest of the story rests on shaky ground of imagination. To be honest, we don't know for sure what the purpose of this pouch is. Blyth suggested that it’s similar to the air cell connected to only one lung of the python or boa constrictor, [31] and, like in that case, it likely provides oxygen to the lungs during long meals. This way, the bird can “guzzle” to its heart's content without having to pause occasionally for a “breather.”

But we must return to the appearance of the bird, for the account of this is not yet complete, since no mention has been made of the eye. This is white and very small, and so gives the bird a wicked, knowing expression, like that of an elephant. Colonel Cunningham speaks of “the malignantly sneaking expression of the pallid eyes.” This is perhaps a little severe on the adjutant, but it is, I fear, quite useless to deny the fact that he has “a canister look in his heye.”

But we need to go back to the bird's appearance, as the description isn't finished yet since we haven't talked about the eye. It's small and white, giving the bird a sly, clever look, similar to that of an elephant. Colonel Cunningham refers to “the sneakily malicious expression of the pale eyes.” That might be a bit harsh on the adjutant, but unfortunately, it’s hard to deny that he has “a canister look in his eye.”

A mere description of the shape and colouring of the adjutant does not give any idea of his comicality. It is his acts rather than his appearance that make him so ludicrous. Except when floating high above the earth on his great pinions the bird always looks grotesque. To say that he, as he walks along, recalls a hunchbacked old man who is deliberately “clowning” is to give a hopelessly inadequate idea of the absurdity of his movements. Lockwood Kipling is nearer the mark when he says: “For grotesque devilry of dancing the Indian adjutant beats creation. Don Quixote or Malvolio were not half so solemn or mincing, and yet there is an abandonment and lightness of step, a wild lift in each solemn prance, which are almost demoniacal. If it were possible for the most angular, tall, and demure of elderly maiden ladies to take a great deal too much champagne and then to give a lesson in ballet dancing, with occasional pauses of acute sobriety, perhaps some [32] faint idea might be conveyed of the peculiar quality of the adjutant’s movements.”

A simple description of the shape and color of the adjutant doesn’t capture his comical nature. It’s his actions, not his looks, that make him so ridiculous. Unless he’s soaring high above the ground on his large wings, the bird always appears silly. Comparing his walk to that of a hunchbacked old man trying to be funny doesn’t even come close to conveying the absurdity of his movements. Lockwood Kipling puts it better when he says: “For the absurd dancing of the Indian adjutant, nothing beats it. Don Quixote or Malvolio weren’t nearly as serious or stiff, yet there’s a carefree quality to his step, a wild lift in each serious dance, that’s almost devilish. If the most angular, tall, and prim old maid were to drink way too much champagne and then teach a ballet class, occasionally pausing to appear perfectly sober, maybe we’d get a hint of the unique quality of the adjutant’s movements.”

Sometimes the bird struts along solemnly with bent back and forwardly pointed bill, at others it will jump or skip along with outstretched wings and clap its beak. It cannot even stand still without striking ludicrous attitudes. Seen from behind, it looks like a little hunch-backed old man with very thin legs, dressed in a grey swallow-tail coat. Adjutants sometimes vary the monotony of existence by standing on one leg; occasionally they sit down, stretching their long legs out in front, and looking “as though they were kneeling wrong side foremost.”

Sometimes the bird walks around seriously with a hunched back and a bill that's pointed forward; other times, it jumps or skips with its wings outstretched and claps its beak. It can't even stay still without striking silly poses. From behind, it looks like a little hunchbacked old man with very thin legs, dressed in a gray tailcoat. Adjutants sometimes mix up the dull routine by standing on one leg; occasionally, they sit down, stretching their long legs out in front of them, looking “as if they were kneeling the wrong way.”

Colonel Cunningham gives a most entertaining account of the habits of these birds, many of which used, until quite recently, to be seen about Calcutta. My observations are chiefly confined to birds in captivity; this perhaps accounts for the fact that they do not agree in all respects with those of the Colonel. According to him, adjutants “are singularly ill-tempered birds, constantly squabbling with one another, even in the absence of any cause of competition, such as favourite roosts or specially savoury stores of offal. Even whilst several of them are standing quietly about, sunning themselves and apparently buried in deep thought, a quarrel will suddenly arise for no apparent reason; and then you may see two monstrous fowls begin to pace around, cautiously stalking one another, and watching for a favourable opportunity of striking and buffeting with beak and wings. The expression of slow malignity with which such duellists regard one [33] another is gruesome, and the injuries resulting from the fray are often ghastly; blinded eyes and bloody cockscombs being matters of everyday occurrence.”

Colonel Cunningham provides a really entertaining description of the behavior of these birds, many of which were recently seen around Calcutta. My observations mainly focus on birds in captivity; this might explain why my findings don't entirely match the Colonel's. According to him, adjutants "are remarkably irritable birds, constantly bickering with each other, even when there's no reason for competition, like preferred roosts or particularly tasty scraps. Even when several of them are calmly hanging out, sunbathing and seemingly lost in thought, a fight can suddenly break out for no clear reason; then you can see two massive birds start to circle each other, carefully stalking and looking for a good chance to hit and shove with their beaks and wings. The expression of slow malice with which these duelists stare at each other is unsettling, and the injuries from the fights are often horrific; blinded eyes and bloody combs are common sights."

Captive adjutants seem to be most placid birds. There are three of them in the “Zoo” at Lahore, kept in a large park-like enclosure, and I have never seen these fighting. They appear to be always, if not on the best of terms, at any rate, indifferent to one another. The three will stand for many minutes at a time in a row, motionless as statues. Sometimes a male and a female will huddle up to one another and remain thus, with their heads almost touching, looking like caricatures of Darby and Joan.

Captive adjutants seem to be some of the calmest birds. There are three of them in the “Zoo” at Lahore, kept in a large park-like enclosure, and I have never seen them fight. They seem to always be, if not the best of friends, at least indifferent to each other. The three will stand in a row for many minutes, motionless like statues. Sometimes a male and a female will snuggle up to each other and stay that way, with their heads almost touching, resembling caricatures of Darby and Joan.

The table manners of adjutants, like those of most other carrion feeders, are not polite. I will therefore not attempt to describe them. In the good old days, feeding adjutants used to be a favourite pastime of Mr. Thomas Atkins at Calcutta. I regret to have to say that his motives were not always purely philanthropic. To connect two pieces of meat by a long string and then throw them among a crowd of adjutants savours of practical joking. One bird, of course, swallows one piece of meat, while a second adjutant secures the other morsel. All goes well until each of the birds tries to go its own way—then a tug-of-war results, fraught with gastronomical disturbance to the combatants.

The table manners of adjutants, like those of most other scavengers, are not graceful. So, I won't bother describing them. Back in the day, feeding adjutants was a favorite pastime of Mr. Thomas Atkins in Calcutta. I unfortunately have to say that his intentions weren't always entirely generous. Tying two pieces of meat together with a long string and then tossing them into a group of adjutants feels like a prank. One bird, of course, gobbles up one piece of meat, while another adjutant grabs the other piece. Everything seems fine until both birds try to fly away with their food—then a tug-of-war ensues, resulting in quite the mess for the competitors.

Adjutants are nowhere very abundant; they are nevertheless spread over the whole of Northern India, but do not appear to be found so far south as Madras. Another species, however—the smaller adjutant [34] (L. javanicus)—has been observed on the Malabar coast.

Adjutants aren’t very common, but they can be found all over Northern India. However, they don’t seem to reach as far south as Madras. Another species, though— the smaller adjutant [34] (L. javanicus)—has been spotted along the Malabar coast.

Some natives make adjutant-catching their profession. The birds are captured on account of their down-like feathers, which are of considerable commercial value.

Some locals have made catching adjutants their job. The birds are captured for their soft feathers, which are quite valuable in the market.

The catcher fits the skin of an adjutant over his head and shoulders, and in this attire creeps up to a company of the birds as they stand half-asleep, knee-deep in water. Great is the surprise of the unsuspecting birds when one of them is unceremoniously seized by the wolf in the adjutant’s skin.

The catcher puts on the skin of an adjutant over his head and shoulders, and in this outfit, sneaks up on a group of birds that are dozing, standing knee-deep in water. The unsuspecting birds are greatly surprised when one of them is suddenly grabbed by the wolf in the adjutant's skin.

THE INDIAN ADJUTANT. (LEPTOPTILUS DUBIUS)

THE INDIAN ADJUTANT. (LEPTOPTILUS DUBIUS)

Indian Adjutant. (Leptoptilus dubius)

[35]


THE SARUS

Having discoursed upon the adjutant, it seems but fitting that we should turn our attention to another long-shanked gentleman—the sarus. The adjutant is, as we have seen, a stork, while the sarus is a crane. I do not know whether this conveys very much information to the average mind. Most people will, I imagine, “give it up” if asked, “What is the difference between a stork and a crane?” Yet there are considerable differences between the two; they belong to different families, and, like rival tradesmen of the same name, “have no connection with one another.” I do not propose to detail the anatomical differences between storks and cranes, for the excellent reason that I myself do not know them all, nor have I the least intention of acquiring such knowledge. It forms part of the dry bones of science, and these are best left to museum ornithologists to squabble over. There are, however, one or two simple points which suffice to enable us to distinguish at a glance a crane from a stork. The hind toe of the stork is well developed, while that of the crane is small and does not touch the ground; the consequence is that the stork likes to rest on trees, while the crane prefers to stand on terra firma on its flat feet. [36] The nostrils of the crane are half-way down the beak, while they are at the base in the bill of the stork. The crane nests on the ground; the stork builds in a tree. Young storks are helpless creatures, while little cranes hop and run about from the moment they leave the egg. Lastly, the crane has a voice, a fine loud voice, a voice that can be heard a mile away, a voice like a trumpet, for its windpipe is coiled. King stork, on the other hand, has no voice; when he wants to make a joyful noise he is obliged to clap together his great mandibles.

Having talked about the adjutant, it seems right that we should focus on another tall guy—the sarus. The adjutant is, as we’ve seen, a stork, while the sarus is a crane. I’m not sure how much that means to the average person. Most people will probably throw their hands up and say, “What’s the difference between a stork and a crane?” But there are significant differences between the two; they belong to different families and, like competing businesses with the same name, “have no connection with one another.” I don’t plan to go into the anatomical differences between storks and cranes because I don’t know them all, nor do I intend to learn. That stuff is just dry scientific detail, best left to museum ornithologists to argue over. However, there are a couple of simple points that can help us tell a crane from a stork at a glance. The stork has a well-developed hind toe, while the crane’s is small and doesn’t touch the ground; this means the stork likes to rest in trees, while the crane prefers to stand on solid ground with its flat feet. The nostrils of the crane are halfway down the beak, while they are at the base of the stork's bill. The crane nests on the ground; the stork builds its nest in a tree. Young storks are helpless, while little cranes are up and running around the moment they hatch. Finally, the crane has a voice, a loud, powerful voice that can be heard a mile away, a trumpet-like voice because its windpipe is coiled. King stork, on the other hand, has no voice; when he wants to make noise, he has to clap his large mandibles together. [36]

Cranes have been favourites with man from time immemorial. The result is that ancient and mediæval writers have plenty to say about them. Now the naturalist of old considered himself in honour bound to attribute some wonderful characteristic to every beast of which he wrote. If he did not know of any clever thing done by any creature, he invented something for it to do. This method had the advantage of making natural history a very exciting and interesting study. Cranes were supposed to perform all manner of tricks with stones. As we have seen, they are blessed with powerful voices, and, like other loud-voiced people, find it difficult to keep silent. They are fully persuaded that silence is golden; but, when it comes to acting up to this belief, the flesh proves itself very frail. Thus it came to pass that the sagacious birds, when migrating, used to stop up their mouths with stones. As they are far too well-bred to speak with the mouth full, they were able to maintain a decorous silence when travelling.

Cranes have been favorites of humans for a really long time. As a result, ancient and medieval writers had a lot to say about them. Back in the day, naturalists felt it was their duty to give every creature a remarkable trait. If they couldn’t think of anything impressive about an animal, they made something up. This approach made natural history a much more exciting and engaging subject. Cranes were thought to perform all sorts of tricks with stones. As we know, they have strong voices, and like other loud people, they struggle to stay quiet. They believe that silence is valuable; however, when it comes to actually being quiet, it’s much harder for them. So, the clever birds, while migrating, would fill their mouths with stones. Since they’re too well-mannered to speak with their mouths full, they could maintain a proper silence while traveling.

[37]

I can cite plenty of authority for this statement. There is, in particular, no less a personage than “Robert Tanner, Gent. Practitioner in Astrologie and Physic.” “The cranes,” he writes, “when they fly out of Cilicia, over the mountain Taurus, carried in their mouths a pebble stone, lest by their chattering they should be seized upon by eagles.”

I can point to a lot of sources to back up this claim. In particular, none other than “Robert Tanner, Gentleman, Practitioner in Astrology and Medicine.” He writes, “The cranes, when they fly out of Cilicia, over the Taurus mountains, carry a pebble in their mouths so that their chattering doesn’t attract eagles.”

The cranes had yet another use for their stones. When the main body were resting at night, sentinels were posted to guard against surprise, so that the company could go to sleep in security. To ensure necessary vigilance, the sentinels stood on one foot and held in the other a large stone. If they inadvertently nodded, their muscles relaxed and the stone dropped. This, of course, used to wake them up. Even Alexander the Great was glad to learn a lesson from the cranes. He used to go to roost with, not a stone in his hands, but a silver ball, as more befitting his royal dignity. On the slightest movement the ball would fall and he wake up. Thus it was that he never overslept himself. We do not do such heroic things nowadays; nor do cranes.

The cranes had another use for their stones. When the main group was resting at night, sentinels were assigned to guard against surprises, so the others could sleep securely. To maintain necessary vigilance, the sentinels stood on one leg and held a large stone in the other. If they accidentally nodded off, their muscles would relax, and the stone would drop. This would, of course, wake them up. Even Alexander the Great found a lesson in the cranes’ method. He would go to sleep not with a stone in his hands, but with a silver ball, as it suited his royal status better. At the slightest movement, the ball would fall, waking him up. This way, he never overslept. We don’t do such heroic things these days; nor do cranes.

Cranes are birds which will not stand nonsense. The pigmies used to go egg-collecting among them; the result of this was, to translate Homer:—

Cranes are birds that won’t put up with nonsense. The pygmies used to go collecting eggs among them; the result of this was, to translate Homer:—

When inclement winters vex the plain,

When harsh winters trouble the plain,

With piercing frosts, or thick descending rain,

With sharp frosts, or heavy falling rain,

To warmer seas the cranes embodied fly,

To warmer seas, the cranes fly.

With noise and order, through the midway sky:

With noise and order, through the middle of the sky:

To pigmy nations wound and death they bring.

To small nations, they bring harm and death.

Notice that as the cranes were on the war-path there was no necessity for them to fill their mouths with [38] stones; they wanted all their lung power to bark at their pigmy foes.

Notice that as the cranes were preparing for battle, they didn't need to fill their mouths with [38] stones; they needed all their lung power to shout at their tiny enemies.

Having considered cranes as they are not, it behoves us to glance at them as they are. The sarus is a handsome creature. It stands over five feet high. The general colour of the plumage is a beautiful French grey. The head and long neck are devoid of feathers, but are covered with numerous tiny crimson warts or papillæ. These assume a deeper hue at the breeding season, which occurs from July to September. There is a patch of grey on the sides of the head. The throat and a ring round the nape are covered with black hairs.

Having looked at cranes the wrong way, it's time to see them for what they really are. The sarus crane is a striking bird. It stands more than five feet tall. The overall color of its feathers is a lovely shade of French grey. Its head and long neck are featherless, covered instead with lots of small crimson bumps. These become a darker red during the breeding season, which takes place from July to September. There’s a patch of grey on the sides of its head, and its throat and the area around its neck have black hair.

Saruses feed upon vegetable substances, insects, earthworms, frogs, lizards, and other small reptiles, with an occasional snake thrown in by way of condiment. “This,” remarks Babu Ram Brama Sanyal, “shows the kind of accommodation they must have.”

Saruses eat plant materials, insects, earthworms, frogs, lizards, and other small reptiles, with an occasional snake added for flavor. “This,” says Babu Ram Brama Sanyal, “shows the level of adaptability they must have.”

Saruses are not gregarious birds, but hunt in couples and are said to mate for life. It is further asserted that when one of a pair is killed the other pines away and dies. I believe this to be true, although I cannot vouch for it, and am certainly not going to put the statement to the test by shooting one of a pair: for these cranes are such tame, confiding birds that to shoot them savours strongly of murder.

Saruses aren’t social birds; they hunt in pairs and are believed to mate for life. It’s also said that when one of a pair is killed, the other grieves and dies. I believe this is true, though I can’t confirm it, and I definitely won’t test the claim by shooting one of a pair. These cranes are so gentle and trusting that shooting them feels pretty much like murder.

According to Jerdon, a young sarus is not bad eating, but old birds are worthless for the table. Lucky old birds! Saruses thrive very well in captivity. As they habitually indulge in all manner of eccentric dances they make most amusing pets. They are usually gentle and let strangers caress them and tickle their heads. But I [39] always let others try this on for the first time with a strange crane, because some birds resent this head-tickling and, to again quote from the worthy Babu above mentioned, “appear to exist only as it were for pecking at everything, bird, beast, and man: children being the special object of their wrath.”

According to Jerdon, a young sarus is pretty good to eat, but older birds aren't worth anything for the table. Lucky old birds! Saruses do really well in captivity. Since they often engage in all kinds of quirky dances, they make really entertaining pets. They’re usually gentle and allow strangers to pet them and scratch their heads. But I always let others try this first with a strange crane, because some birds don’t like being petted on the head and, to quote the esteemed Babu mentioned earlier, “seem to exist solely for the purpose of pecking at everything—birds, animals, and humans; children being especially targeted by their ire.”

There are two cranes in the “Zoo” at Lahore; they are a most mischievous couple. They used to be kept with the ducks and geese, and amused themselves by rooting up all freshly planted rushes. At feeding time it was their habit to hop from one dish of food to another with outstretched wings and thus frighten off the ducks and secure the lion’s share for themselves. They were then removed to the enclosure where the adjutants are. They started playing tricks on these, but the adjutant has a powerful beak which he is quite ready to use when necessity arises. The result is that the saruses are not on speaking terms with the adjutants.

There are two cranes in the “Zoo” at Lahore; they are quite a mischievous pair. They used to be kept with the ducks and geese, having fun by tearing up all the freshly planted rushes. At feeding time, they would hop from one dish of food to another with their wings spread wide, scaring off the ducks and grabbing the biggest portions for themselves. They were then moved to the enclosure with the adjutants. They started playing pranks on them, but the adjutant has a strong beak that he’s not afraid to use when needed. As a result, the saruses don’t get along with the adjutants.

Unlike the adjutant, whose nest is a huge platform of sticks placed on the top of a very lofty tree, the sarus builds its nursery on the ground. This takes the form of a large cone, several feet in diameter at the base and two or three feet high. It is composed of reeds, rushes, and straw, and placed by preference in shallow water. Great care is taken to keep the eggs above water level. If, as is apt to happen in India, heavy rain comes on after the completion of the nest, the parents speedily set to work to raise the eggs by adding more material to that upon which they rest.

Unlike the adjutant, which builds a massive nest of sticks high up in a tall tree, the sarus creates its nest on the ground. It's shaped like a large cone, several feet wide at the base and two or three feet tall. It’s made of reeds, rushes, and straw, and is usually placed in shallow water. The parents are very careful to keep the eggs above water level. If, as often happens in India, heavy rain falls after the nest is completed, the parents quickly start adding more materials to raise the eggs above the water.

[40]


SPECIES STABILITY

If two crows be taken to an ornithologist and he be told that one of them was caught in the Himalayas while the other was captured in Madras, he will not be able to tell which individual came from which area: in other words, the crows of Madras resemble those of the Himalayas. This, of course, is no unusual phenomenon. The same may be said of the myna, the king-crow, and a great many other birds and beasts. Yet the phenomenon is a remarkable one if we take into account the facts of variation.

If an ornithologist is given two crows and told that one was caught in the Himalayas and the other in Madras, he won’t be able to tell which one came from where: in other words, the crows from Madras look just like those from the Himalayas. This isn’t an uncommon occurrence. The same applies to the myna, the king-crow, and many other birds and animals. Still, it is quite striking when we consider the facts of variation.

If several hundred thousand crows be collected and carefully examined, it will be found that no two of them resemble one another in all respects. This being so, we should expect the crows of Madras to differ from those of the Himalayas, since the two environments are so dissimilar. We may say with tolerable certainty that no intercrossing takes place between the crows of the two localities: for these birds are stay-at-home creatures, and do not wander far afield. In this case, therefore, it is not intercrossing that has prevented the origin of local races.

If we gather several hundred thousand crows and examine them closely, we will find that no two look exactly alike in every way. Given this, we can expect the crows in Madras to be different from those in the Himalayas, since the two environments are so different. We can say with reasonable certainty that there is no interbreeding between the crows from these two areas because these birds tend to stay in one place and don't roam far. Therefore, in this instance, it’s not interbreeding that has stopped the development of local varieties.

A consideration of the main causes which conduce to the stability of species may not be devoid of interest; [41] for the subject is one which has hitherto attracted but little attention. Since the Darwinian hypothesis was given to the world we have heard so much of variation and the origin of new species that the other phenomenon—that of the fixity of species—in spite of varying environments has been almost entirely overlooked. Yet it was just this feature of animal life that attracted the attention of the older zoologists and led them to believe that species had been created once and for all, and that, when created, they were immutably fixed.

Considering the main factors that contribute to the stability of species might actually be quite interesting; [41] this topic has generally received little attention. Since the Darwinian hypothesis was introduced, we've heard a lot about variation and the emergence of new species, while the other phenomenon—the stability of species—despite changing environments has been almost entirely ignored. Yet, it was precisely this aspect of animal life that caught the interest of earlier zoologists and led them to believe that species were created once and for all and that, once created, they remained unchanged.

Most biologists, if asked to explain the comparative fixity of species, the slowness of evolution, would, I think, refer to the fact that variations appear to take place indiscriminately in all directions. Take, for example, a large number of birds of any species and measure any one organ, let us say the first primary wing feather. Suppose the average length be six inches. We shall find that in a considerable percentage of the individuals measured the wing is exactly six inches in length: that six inches is what we may call the favourite or fashionable length of the wing. The next commonest lengths will be 5.99 and 6.01 inches, and so on. We shall find that only a very small percentage of the individuals have wings shorter than 5½ inches or longer than 6½ inches; and if we measured a thousand individuals we probably should not find any in which the wing was shorter than five inches or longer than seven.

Most biologists, when asked to explain why species seem stable and evolution happens slowly, would likely point out that variations appear to occur randomly in all directions. For instance, if we take a large sample of birds from any species and measure a specific part, like the first primary wing feather, let’s say the average length is six inches. We’ll see that a significant percentage of the birds measured have a wing that’s exactly six inches long: this is what we could call the preferred or typical length of the wing. The next most common lengths would be 5.99 and 6.01 inches, and so on. We would find that only a small percentage of the birds have wings shorter than 5½ inches or longer than 6½ inches; and if we measured a thousand individual birds, we probably wouldn’t find any with wings shorter than five inches or longer than seven.

Now, the commonly accepted theory is that in those cases where there is free interbreeding the long-winged varieties and the short-winged varieties tend to neutralise [42] one another, hence no change in character takes place. The effects of variation are swamped by intercrossing. It is only when intercrossing is checked, as when natural selection weeds out certain varieties, that evolution occurs.

Now, the widely accepted theory is that in situations where there is free mixing, the long-winged varieties and the short-winged varieties tend to balance each other out, so no change in characteristics happens. The effects of variation are overwhelmed by interbreeding. Evolution only occurs when interbreeding is limited, such as when natural selection eliminates certain varieties.

This theory, of course, explains, or helps to explain, why species are so stable; but it involves the assumption that there is no such thing as sexual selection among animals in a state of nature. The theory assumes that individuals mate in a haphazard manner, that a long-winged hen is as likely to select a short-winged husband as a long-winged one. Are we justified in assuming this? At present there is little evidence on the subject. Evidence can only be procured by measuring a number of pairs of birds that have mated, and seeing whether large hens mate chiefly with large cocks or with small cocks, or indifferently with large or small cock-birds.

This theory, of course, explains, or helps to explain, why species are so stable; but it assumes that there is no such thing as sexual selection among animals in the wild. The theory assumes that individuals mate randomly, meaning a long-winged hen is just as likely to choose a short-winged mate as a long-winged one. Do we have a good reason to believe this? Right now, there's not much evidence on the topic. Evidence can only be gathered by examining a number of pairs of birds that have mated and seeing whether larger hens mostly pair with larger roosters, smaller roosters, or if they don't care about size at all when choosing a mate.

That sexual selection is a reality and not a mere hypothesis there can, I think, be but little doubt. It is with the theory that supposes that the females alone exercise selection that I feel compelled to quarrel. The male selects his partner just as much as the female selects hers. The choice is mutual.

That sexual selection is real and not just a theory is pretty clear, I believe. What I disagree with is the theory that suggests only females make the selection. Males choose their partners just as much as females choose theirs. The selection is mutual.

In the Zoological Gardens at Lahore there are a number of ordinary coloured peacocks and a number of albinos. No coloured hen will mate with a coloured cock if she is allowed to exercise a choice between him and an albino. Here, then, is a clear example of sexual selection.

In the Lahore Zoo, there are several regular colored peacocks and some albino ones. A colored hen will not mate with a colored cock if she can choose between him and an albino. This is a clear example of sexual selection.

Professor Karl Pearson has spent much time in trying [43] to discover whether there is such a thing as sexual selection—what we may call unconscious selection—among human beings. His experiments tend to show that there is.

Professor Karl Pearson has dedicated a lot of time to exploring whether sexual selection—what we might refer to as unconscious selection—exists among humans. His experiments suggest that it does.

If we take a thousand married men whose stature is not less than six feet, and a thousand also who are none of them taller than 5 ft. 8 in., we shall find that the average height of the wives of the former is greater than that of the wives of the shorter men.

If we take a thousand married men who are at least six feet tall, and another thousand who are all shorter than 5 ft. 8 in., we will see that the average height of the wives of the taller men is greater than that of the wives of the shorter men.

If wild animals display a similar characteristic, it is evident that to say that intercrossing swamps variation and causes species to remain stable is not altogether accurate; for, if like select like as partners, we should expect a number of races to rapidly arise, or, at any rate, three races—a large, medium, and small one. So far, however, as we can see, species display no such tendency. We are therefore driven to the conclusion either that there is among species in a state of nature no tendency for like individuals to select like as their partners, or, if there be such a tendency, there is some force at work which counteracts it.

If wild animals show a similar trait, it’s clear that saying that interbreeding blurs variation and keeps species stable isn’t completely accurate; because, if similar individuals pair up, we would expect a number of races to quickly develop, or at least three races—a large one, a medium one, and a small one. However, as far as we can observe, species don’t show such a tendency. We are therefore led to conclude that either there isn’t a tendency for like individuals to choose like partners in nature, or if such a tendency exists, there’s some force that counters it.

It may be thought that the case of the peafowl in the Lahore “Zoo” tends to show that among animals it is dissimilarity, not similarity, that attracts, for the coloured hens mate with white cocks in preference to those like themselves.

It might be believed that the situation with the peafowl at the Lahore “Zoo” suggests that in the animal kingdom, it’s differences, not similarities, that draw individuals together, as the colored hens prefer to mate with white cocks rather than those that look like them.

As a matter of fact the hens select the white cocks, not because they are white, but because of the strength of the sexual instincts of these latter. The white cocks continually show off before the hens; the sexual desire is developed more highly in them [44] than in the ordinary cocks, and it is this that attracts the hens.

As a matter of fact, the hens choose the white cocks, not because they are white, but because of the strength of the sexual instincts in those cocks. The white cocks constantly display themselves in front of the hens; their sexual desire is much stronger than in regular cocks, and that's what draws the hens in. [44]

We must also bear in mind that abnormal variations have a strong tendency to perpetuate themselves. If a white cock mates with an ordinary peahen, the majority of the offspring are pure white.

We also need to remember that unusual variations tend to continue existing. If a white rooster mates with a regular peahen, most of the offspring will be pure white.

If there be such a thing as sexual selection, and if it be, as I believe, the strongest, the most mettlesome individuals, those in which the sexual instincts reach the highest development, that attract the opposite sex, then the question arises: is there any connection between these characteristics and the size and colour of their possessor? We are not in possession of sufficient data to answer this question in the affirmative. Nevertheless I believe that such a relation does exist.

If sexual selection really exists, and if, as I believe, the strongest and most vibrant individuals with the most developed sexual instincts attract the opposite sex, then we must ask: is there a connection between these traits and the size and color of the individuals who have them? We don't have enough data to answer this definitively. Still, I believe there is a relationship.

The researches of Professor Pearson seem to point to the fact that there exists a definite relation between variation and fertility. For every species there is a mode or typical size and form, and from this there are deviations in all directions, and, speaking generally, the greater the deviation from the mode the less the fertility of the individual.

The studies by Professor Pearson indicate that there is a clear relationship between variation and fertility. Every species has a standard size and shape, and there are variations in all directions from this standard. Generally speaking, the greater the deviation from the standard, the lower the individual’s fertility.

If this be a general law we have here a very potent factor tending to make species stable. Those individuals which deviate least from the common type are the most fertile; they produce the most offspring; moreover, they are the most numerous, hence they, by sheer force of numbers, keep a species stable. The abnormal individuals are comparatively few in number, and they beget comparatively few of their kind, so have no chance of establishing themselves and crushing out [45] the normal type, unless natural selection steps in to their aid.

If this is a general law, we have a strong factor that helps keep species stable. The individuals that differ the least from the common type are the most fertile; they produce the most offspring. Plus, they are the most numerous, so their sheer numbers help maintain the stability of the species. The abnormal individuals are relatively few in number, and they produce relatively few of their kind, so they don’t stand a chance of establishing themselves and pushing out the normal type unless natural selection helps them. [45]

Is comparative infertility the result of feebleness of the sexual instinct? If so, sexual selection must be conducive to the stability of species.

Is comparative infertility due to a weakness of the sexual instinct? If that’s the case, then sexual selection must play a role in the stability of species.

For if the rule be the greater the deviation of an individual from the normal the less the development in it of the sexual instinct and the less its fertility, it follows that an abnormal organism is less likely to find a mate than a normal individual is; and if it do succeed in forming a union, that union will probably produce less than the average number of offspring.

For if the principle is that the greater the deviation of a person from the norm, the less their sexual instinct develops and the lower their fertility, it follows that an abnormal organism is less likely to find a partner than a normal individual; and if it does manage to form a union, that union will probably result in fewer than the average number of offspring.

[46]


THE AMADAVAT

“Gentlemen,” said a Cambridge professor to his class, “I regret that owing to the forgetfulness of my assistant, I am unable to show you a specimen of the shell of the mollusc of which we are speaking. You have, however, but to step into the parlour of any seaside lodging-house and on the mantelpiece you will see two of the shells in question.” Every undergraduate immediately knew what the shell was like; so will my readers at once recognise the bird of which I write when I inform them that the amadavat is the little red bird with white spots that occurs in every aviary in India. The bird is, indeed, not all red, but the bill is bright red and there are patches of this colour all over the plumage—more in the cock than in the hen, and more in the former in the breeding season than at other times. Thus the general effect is that of a red bird; hence the native name Lal munia, which, being interpreted, is the red munia. This is the proper English name of the bird, although fanciers frequently call it the red waxbill. Men of science know it as Sporæginthus amandava. I may say here that the name avadavat or amadavat is derived from Ahmedabad, whence great numbers used to be exported, for the bird is a great favourite in England.

“Gentlemen,” said a Cambridge professor to his class, “I’m sorry that, due to my assistant’s forgetfulness, I can’t show you a specimen of the shell of the mollusc we’re discussing. However, if you just step into the parlor of any seaside lodge, you’ll see two of the shells on the mantelpiece.” Every undergraduate immediately knew what the shell looked like; my readers will also recognize the bird I’m writing about when I tell them that the amadavat is the small red bird with white spots that can be found in every aviary in India. The bird isn’t entirely red, but it has a bright red bill and patches of that color all over its feathers—more so in the male than the female, and more in the male during the breeding season than at other times. So the overall effect is that of a red bird; hence the native name Lal munia, which translates to the red munia. This is the proper English name for the bird, although bird lovers often call it the red waxbill. Scientists refer to it as Sporæginthus amandava. I should mention that the name avadavat or amadavat comes from Ahmedabad, where a lot of these birds used to be exported, as they are very popular in England.

[47]

It is the cage bird of India par excellence. Hundreds of thousands of amadavats must at this moment be living in captivity. The bird takes to cage life as a Scotsman to whisky. Within five minutes of capture the little creature is contentedly eating its seed and singing quite gaily. This is no exaggeration. I was recently out with a friend when we came upon a small boy catching munias. We saw captured a fine cock which my friend purchased for two annas. Not happening to have a cage in his pocket, he put the tiny creature into a fold of his handkerchief and placed the remainder of the handkerchief in his pocket. While we were walking home our captive began twittering in answer to his companions who were still free. If this be not philosophical behaviour, I do not know what is.

It’s the quintessential caged bird of India. Right now, hundreds of thousands of amadavats must be living in captivity. The bird adjusts to life in a cage as easily as a Scotsman enjoys whisky. Within five minutes of being captured, the little creature is happily eating its seeds and singing cheerfully. This isn’t an exaggeration. I was out with a friend recently when we saw a small boy catching munias. We witnessed a fine male being captured, which my friend bought for two annas. Since he didn’t have a cage with him, he placed the tiny bird in a fold of his handkerchief and put the rest of the handkerchief in his pocket. While we were walking home, our captive began chirping back to his companions who were still free. If this isn't philosophical behavior, I don't know what is.

Nothing is easier than to catch munias. All that is required is the common, pyramidal-shaped, four-anna wicker cage in which birds are usually carried about in India. To the base of one of the walls of this a flap is attached by a hinge. The flap is the same size and shape as the wall of the cage, and composed of a frame over which a narrow-meshed string net is stretched. A string is fastened to the apex of the flap. The cage, with a captive bird inside, is placed in the open so that the flap rests on the ground. On this some groundsel is thrown. In a few minutes a passing amadavat is attracted to the cage by the song of the bird inside. The new-comer at once begins to feed on the groundsel. Then the bird-catcher, who is seated a few yards away, pulls the string sharply, so that the flap closes over the side of the cage and thus the bird is secured. It is then [48] placed inside the cage and the flap again set. In this manner a dozen or more amadavats can be captured in an hour. As nine red munias are sold for a rupee, and as they will live for years in captivity and cost next to nothing to keep, it is not surprising that they are popular pets.

Nothing is easier than catching munias. All you need is a common, pyramid-shaped, four-anna wicker cage typically used for transporting birds in India. One of the walls has a flap attached with a hinge. The flap is the same size and shape as the wall of the cage and consists of a frame covered with a narrow-meshed string net. A string is attached to the top of the flap. The cage, with a captured bird inside, is placed outside so that the flap rests on the ground. Some groundsel is scattered on it. In a few minutes, a passing amadavat is drawn to the cage by the song of the bird inside. The newcomer immediately starts to eat the groundsel. The bird-catcher, sitting a few yards away, then pulls the string sharply, causing the flap to close over the side of the cage, securing the bird. It is then placed inside the cage, and the flap is set again. This way, a dozen or more amadavats can be caught in an hour. Since nine red munias sell for a rupee and they can live for years in captivity at very little cost, it's no surprise they are popular pets.

Moreover, the amadavat is no mean songster. “Eha” is, I think, a little severe on the bird when he states that “fifty in a cage make an admirable chorus.” The bird is small, so is its voice, but what there is of the latter is exceedingly sweet. Were its notes only louder the bird would be in the first rank as a songster. A rippling stream of cheery twitters emanates unceasingly from a cage of munias. The birds seem never to tire. The cock frequently utters, in addition to this perpetual twitter, a warble of five or six notes. The birds love to huddle together in a row on a perch and twitter in chorus. Suddenly the chorus ceases; one of the birds raises his head above the level of the others and sings a solo, while the rest listen in silence with the air of connoisseurs. When he has finished, another bird has a “turn,” then another. The whole performance always puts me in mind of one of those impromptu concerts which soldiers are so fond of getting up.

Moreover, the amadavat is quite the singer. “Eha” is, I think, a bit harsh on the bird when he says that “fifty in a cage make an admirable chorus.” The bird is small, and so is its voice, but what it does have is incredibly sweet. If its notes were just a bit louder, this bird would rank among the top songbirds. A cheerful stream of twitters continuously flows from a cage of munias. The birds never seem to tire. The male often adds a warble of five or six notes to this constant twittering. The birds enjoy sitting close together on a perch, chirping in chorus. Suddenly the chorus stops; one bird raises its head above the others and sings a solo while the rest listen in silence like connoisseurs. Once it finishes, another bird takes a “turn,” and then another. The whole act always reminds me of those impromptu concerts that soldiers love to put on.

Quite apart from their song, munias afford him who keeps them much pleasure, because they are most amusing birds to watch. They are very fond of heat. They are happiest when the thermometer stands at about a hundred. When they huddle together for the sake of warmth, all are content except the two end birds, who are kept warm only on one [49] side. No bird, therefore, likes to be an outside one of a row. If two or three, sitting close together, are joined by another, this last does not take up a position at the end of the line. He knows a trick worth two of that. He perches on the backs of two in the middle and tries to wedge himself in between them. Sometimes he succeeds. Sometimes he does not. When he does succeed he frequently upsets the equilibrium of the whole row.

Quite aside from their song, munias bring a lot of joy to those who keep them because they are entertaining birds to observe. They really enjoy warmth. They are happiest when the temperature is around a hundred degrees. When they huddle together to stay warm, everyone is happy except for the two birds on the ends, who only get warmth on one side. No bird likes to be on the outside of a line. If two or three are sitting closely together and another one joins them, the newcomer doesn’t take a spot at the end. He knows a trick better than that. He sits on the backs of the two in the middle and tries to squeeze himself in between them. Sometimes he succeeds. Sometimes he doesn’t. When he does succeed, he often disrupts the balance of the whole line.

Needless to say, the birds roost huddled together, and at bed-time there is great manœuvring to avoid an outside position. Each tries to get somewhere in the middle, and, in order to do so, adopts one of two methods. He either flops on top of birds already in position, and, if he cannot wedge himself in, sleeps with one foot on the back of one bird and the other on its neighbour’s back. The birds do not seem to mind being sat upon in this way. The other method is for the two outer birds to press inwards until one of those in the middle of the row is squeezed so hard as to lose its foothold and be violently ejected upwards. The bird thus jockeyed out of its position then hops to one end and in its turn begins to push inwards, and so the process continues until the birds grow too sleepy to struggle any more. All this contest is conducted without a sound. There is no bickering or squabbling. The only thing I know like it is the contest in the dining-room of an Indian hotel, when two “boys,” each belonging to a different master, seize a dish simultaneously. Each is determined to secure that dish, and neither dares utter a sound for fear of [50] angering his Sahib. Thus they struggle in grim silence. Eventually one is victorious and walks off in triumph with the dish. The defeated servant at once accepts the situation; so is it with a munia ejected from a central position.

Needless to say, the birds roost close together, and at bedtime, there's a lot of movement to avoid being on the outside. Each one tries to get somewhere in the middle and does so using one of two methods. They either flop on top of birds already in place, and if they can't squeeze in, they sleep with one foot on the back of one bird and the other on the back of a neighbor. The birds don’t seem to mind being sat on this way. The other method involves the two outer birds pushing inward until one of those in the middle gets squeezed so hard that it loses its footing and gets pushed upward. The bird that gets pushed out then hops to one end and starts pushing inward again, and this process continues until the birds are too sleepy to keep struggling. All of this happens without a sound. There’s no arguing or fighting. The only thing I can compare it to is the contest in the dining room of an Indian hotel, when two “boys,” each serving a different master, grab for a dish at the same time. Each is determined to get that dish, and neither dares to make a sound for fear of upsetting his Sahib. So they struggle in complete silence. Eventually, one wins and walks off triumphantly with the dish. The defeated servant immediately accepts the situation; it’s the same with a munia pushed out of a central position.

Although amadavats are widely distributed in India and fairly common in most parts of the country, they usually escape notice on account of their small size. When flying overhead they are probably mistaken for sparrows. Moreover, they do not often visit gardens; they prefer open country.

Although amadavats are widely found in India and are fairly common in many areas, they often go unnoticed because of their small size. When flying overhead, they are likely mistaken for sparrows. Additionally, they don't frequently visit gardens; they prefer open countryside.

Amadavats belong to the finch family, to the great tribe which includes the sparrow, the canary, and the weaver-bird. By their coarse, stout beak, tapering to a point, you may know them. The use of this big beak is to husk grain. Finches do not gobble up their seed whole as pigeons or fowls do; they carefully husk each grain before swallowing it. Hence the meal of a bird of this family is a somewhat protracted affair. He who keeps an aviary should remember this and provide his birds with several seed-boxes, otherwise one or two bullies (for there are bullies even among tiny birds) are apt to monopolise the food.

Amadavats are part of the finch family, which includes sparrows, canaries, and weaver birds. You can identify them by their thick, strong beaks that taper to a point. This large beak is used for husking grain. Unlike pigeons or chickens that swallow their seeds whole, finches carefully husk each grain before eating it. So, mealtime for a bird in this family tends to take a while. Anyone who keeps an aviary should keep this in mind and provide their birds with several seed boxes; otherwise, one or two bullies (and yes, even small birds can be bullies) may end up hogging all the food.

He should also bear in mind that Nature does not provide her feathered children with teeth. Seed-eating birds, therefore, habitually swallow small stones and pieces of grit. These perform the function of millstones inside the bird. From this it follows that it is cruel to keep seed-eating birds without supplying them with sand and grit.

He should also remember that Nature doesn't give her feathered creatures teeth. Seed-eating birds, therefore, commonly swallow small stones and bits of grit. These act like millstones inside the bird. This means that it's cruel to keep seed-eating birds without providing them with sand and grit.

The bone of a cuttle-fish, tied to the wall of the cage, [51] is much appreciated by all the finch tribe and helps to keep them in condition.

The bone of a cuttlefish, attached to the cage wall, [51] is highly valued by all the finch species and helps to keep them healthy.

The nest of the amadavat is a large ball of fibrous material, somewhat carelessly put together, with a hole at one side by way of entrance. Winter is the season in which to look for the nests, but they are not easy to find, being well concealed in low bushes. Six pure white glossless eggs are usually laid.

The amadavat's nest is a big, messy ball made of fibrous material, with an entrance on one side. Winter is the best time to search for these nests, but they can be hard to spot since they're well hidden in low bushes. Typically, six pure white, non-shiny eggs are laid.

[52]


THE NUTMEG BIRD

The nutmeg bird or spotted munia (Uroloncha punctulata) is second only to the amadavat as an aviary favourite. The two species are almost invariably caged. This is, perhaps, the reason why I was once gravely assured by a lady that the spotted munia is the hen; and the amadavat the cock of one and the same species! Needless to say, the birds, although relatives, belong to different genera. The stouter bill of the spotted munia proclaims this. In colour the beak is bluish black or dark slate colour, and contrasts strongly with the chocolate-brown of the head, neck, back, wings, and tail. The breast is white with a number of black rings, which give it the appearance of a nutmeg-grater, hence the popular name of the bird. Fanciers go one better and call it the spice bird. If in years to come the former name be forgotten, etymologists will put their wise heads together and puzzle and wrangle over the derivation of the name “spice bird”!

The nutmeg bird or spotted munia (Uroloncha punctulata) is only surpassed by the amadavat as a favorite in aviaries. These two species are almost always kept in cages. This might be why a woman once seriously insisted to me that the spotted munia is a female and the amadavat is a male of the same species! Of course, even though they are related, they belong to different genera. The spotted munia’s thicker bill makes this clear. Its beak is a bluish-black or dark slate color, which contrasts strongly with the chocolate-brown of its head, neck, back, wings, and tail. The breast is white with several black rings, giving it the look of a nutmeg grater, which is how the bird got its popular name. Bird enthusiasts even call it the spice bird. If the former name gets forgotten in the future, language experts will likely come together to debate the origin of the term “spice bird”!

The habits of the spotted munia are those of the amadavat. Like the latter, it seems to thrive in captivity; it also loves warmth, and likes to go to roost with a warm companion on each side of it. Red and spotted munias live together very amicably in a cage; but as [53] the latter, owing to their less showy plumage, are usually in a minority, they have to be content with outside positions at roosting-time. Sometimes my munias take it into their tiny heads to sleep on a perch which runs across a corner of the cage, and is barely long enough to accommodate them all. There are several other finer and longer perches, but, for some reason or other, they seem to prefer this one. Possibly its breadth is better adapted to the grip of their feet than that of any of the others. I may here say, in parenthesis, for the benefit of those who keep cage birds, that every cage should contain several perches of varying diameter, so as to permit the inmates of the cage the luxury of a change of grip.

The habits of the spotted munia are similar to those of the amadavat. Like the amadavat, it seems to do well in captivity; it also enjoys warmth and prefers to nestle with a warm companion on each side of it. Red and spotted munias get along very well in a cage, but since the latter tend to have less colorful feathers, they usually end up in the minority and have to settle for the outer spots when it's time to roost. Sometimes my munias decide to sleep on a perch that runs across a corner of the cage, which is barely long enough for them all. There are several other nicer and longer perches available, but for some reason, they seem to prefer this one. It could be that its width is better suited for their grip than the others. I should mention, for those who keep caged birds, that every cage should have several perches of varying diameters, allowing the birds the comfort of changing their grip.

Well, when a dozen birds persist in roosting on a perch intended only to seat ten, at least one of them is unable to find room on the perch, and is obliged either to sleep on the backs of some of his companions or make-believe that he is roosting on the perch. This latter feat is accomplished by the bird clutching hold of the two wires between which the perch passes and maintaining himself at an angle of 45° with the vertical. In this attitude a bird will sometimes sleep! Of course, its body is in part resting on that of its neighbour, but, allowing for this, a more uncomfortable position is inconceivable to a human being. The spotted munia, however, seems to find it tolerably comfortable.

Well, when a dozen birds keep trying to sit on a perch meant for only ten, at least one of them can't find a spot and has to either sleep on the backs of some of their friends or pretend to be roosting on the perch. This latter trick is done by the bird gripping the two wires that the perch runs between and holding itself at a 45° angle to the vertical. In this position, a bird will sometimes sleep! Sure, part of its body is resting on its neighbor, but even with that, a more uncomfortable position is hard to imagine for a human. However, the spotted munia seems to find it pretty comfortable.

Birds sleep standing, often on one leg. Did this require any appreciable muscular effort on the part of the bird there could be no rest in such an attitude, and the bird would fall off its perch as soon as it went to [54] sleep. As a matter of fact, the muscles and tendons of a bird’s hind-limb are so arranged that, to use the words of Mr. F. W. Headley, “when the leg bends at the ankle, there is a pull upon the tendons, the muscles are stretched, the toes are bent and grasp the perch on which the bird sits. Thus he is maintained by his own weight, which bends the leg and so causes the toes to grip.” Thanks to this feature of their anatomy, passerine birds are able to sleep on branches of trees out of reach of prowling beasts of prey.

Birds sleep while standing, often on one leg. If this required any significant muscular effort from the bird, it wouldn’t be able to rest in such a position and would fall off its perch as soon as it dozed off. In reality, the muscles and tendons in a bird’s hind leg are arranged in a way that, as Mr. F. W. Headley puts it, “when the leg bends at the ankle, it creates tension on the tendons, stretches the muscles, and bends the toes to grip the perch the bird is on. So it’s held in place by its own weight, which bends the leg and makes the toes grip.” Because of this anatomical feature, songbirds can sleep on tree branches that are out of reach of lurking predators.

The great force with which a bird grasps its perch is worthy of note. As every hawker is aware, a falcon, when carried on the wrist, grips the leather gauntlet so tightly as to almost stop the circulation of the blood in the hand of the carrier. A fox cannot open its mouth when once its snout is in the iron grip of an eagle. Examples of the power of the grip of the foot of a passerine bird will occur to every one who has had much to do with our feathered friends. Crows habitually roost in the topmost branches of trees, which must be very violently shaken in a gale of wind; yet the birds never seem to lose their hold.

The strong way a bird holds onto its perch is worth noting. As any falconer knows, a falcon gripping the leather glove on someone's wrist holds on so tightly that it nearly cuts off blood circulation in the carrier's hand. A fox can't open its mouth once an eagle has its beak locked onto its snout. Anyone who has spent time with birds will recall the strength of a songbird's grip. Crows typically roost in the highest branches of trees, which get shaken violently in strong winds, yet the birds never seem to lose their hold.

I have said that the habits of the spotted munia are those of the amadavat; what was said of the latter applies to the former, with one exception. The spotted munia is no songster. Those who keep the bird must have seen him go through all the motions of singing, with a considerable display of energy, but scarcely a sound seems to issue. You may perhaps hear the feeblest noise, like that made by a wheezy and decrepit mosquito. When you see the bird’s mandibles [55] moving nineteen to the dozen with scarcely a sound issuing, you are inclined to think that he is either playing dumb crambo or that he has taken leave of his senses. Nothing of the kind. The bird is singing his top notes, which are doubtless greatly appreciated by his mate. Sound is, as we all know in this scientific age, vibration appreciable to the ear. Air is the usual vibrating medium. Only certain vibrations are perceptible to the human auditory organ. Those having a recurrence of below thirty or above sixteen thousand per second do not produce the sensation of sound to the average human ear. There are thus numbers of vibrations continually going on which are lost to us; to this category belong the vibrations in the air produced by the vocal cords of the spotted munia. The ear of a bird is constituted very differently from that of man, so that it is not surprising if birds can hear certain sounds imperceptible to us human beings. I may here say that the range of the human ear varies greatly in different individuals. Some men can hear vibrations of which the recurrence is but fifteen in the second, while others are said to appreciate notes caused by forty thousand vibrations per second. I have a friend who cannot hear a black partridge when it is calling; its notes are too high for the unusually limited range of his ear. I do not know if there are any people to whom the note of the nutmeg bird sounds quite loud; if there be, and these lines meet their eye, I hope they will give their brethren of more limited capacity the benefit of their experience.

I’ve mentioned that the habits of the spotted munia are similar to those of the amadavat; what was said about the latter applies to the former, with one exception. The spotted munia doesn’t sing. Anyone who has kept this bird must have seen it go through all the motions of singing, energetically flapping its beak, but hardly any sound comes out. You might catch a faint noise, like the sound of a wheezy old mosquito. When you see the bird's beak moving rapidly with hardly any noise, you might think it's either playing dumb or has lost its mind. But that's not the case. The bird is actually singing its highest notes, which are surely appreciated by its mate. Sound is, as we all know today, vibration that can be perceived by the ear. Air is the typical medium for these vibrations. Only certain vibrations can be detected by the human ear. Frequencies below thirty or above sixteen thousand per second don’t create a sensation of sound for most people. Therefore, there are many vibrations happening all the time that we can’t hear; these include the vibrations in the air caused by the vocal cords of the spotted munia. A bird’s ear is structured quite differently from a human's, so it makes sense that birds can hear some sounds that we can't. I should also mention that the range of hearing in humans varies widely among individuals. Some people can hear frequencies as low as fifteen per second, while others can perceive sounds from as high as forty thousand vibrations per second. I have a friend who can’t hear a black partridge when it calls; its notes are too high for his unusually limited hearing range. I'm not sure if there are people who find the nutmeg bird’s call quite loud; if there are, and they happen to read this, I hope they’ll share their insights with those of us who have more limited hearing.

[56]


THE DID HE DO IT

Mr. “did-he-do-it” is a dandy of the first water. I should like to add “and so is his wife,” for she dresses exactly as he does, and is every bit as particular regarding her personal appearance, but owing to the peculiarity of our Anglo-Saxon tongue, it is incorrect to apply the term “dandy” to a lady, and there appears to be no feminine equivalent of it. I must therefore be content to say that Mrs. Did-he-do-it is a dressy little person. Before describing the attire of the Did-he-do-it let me say that the bird is correctly styled the red-wattled lapwing. Ornithologists used to call it Lobivanellus goensis, but this was found to be a bit of a mouthful for even an ornithologist; accordingly the bird is now named Sarcogrammus indicus for short.

Mr. "did-he-do-it" is a real dandy. I’d like to add “and so is his wife,” since she dresses just like him and is equally concerned about her appearance, but because of the quirks of our Anglo-Saxon language, it’s not correct to call a woman a “dandy,” and there doesn’t seem to be a feminine version of the term. So, I’ll just say that Mrs. Did-he-do-it is a stylish little lady. Before I describe the Did-he-do-it's outfits, let me mention that the bird is properly called the red-wattled lapwing. Ornithologists used to refer to it as Lobivanellus goensis, but that was a bit too complicated, even for them; so now the bird is commonly known as Sarcogrammus indicus.

The Did-he-do-it belongs to the noble family of plovers. Its head, neck, and upper back are black, and the under parts are white. A broad white band runs down each side of the neck from the eye to join the white of the under parts. The wings are of a beautiful greenish-bronze hue; the legs are bright yellow. The beak is crimson-red, as is the forwardly pointing wattle which forms so conspicuous a feature of the bird’s physiognomy. The lapwing is thus an easy [57] bird to identify. Even if you cannot see him, you know he is there the moment you hear his loud, shrill “Did he do it, pity to do it.” The only bird with which he can possibly be confounded is his cousin, the yellow-wattled lapwing (Sarciophorus malabaricus). This latter, however, has a yellow wattle and one syllable less in its cry.

The Did-he-do-it belongs to the noble family of plovers. Its head, neck, and upper back are black, and its underbelly is white. A broad white band extends down each side of the neck from the eye to connect with the white of the underbelly. The wings are a stunning greenish-bronze color, and the legs are bright yellow. The beak is a striking crimson-red, as is the prominent wattle that stands out on the bird’s face. The lapwing is therefore an easy bird to identify. Even if you can't see it, you know it's there the moment you hear its loud, shrill “Did he do it, pity to do it.” The only bird that might be confused with it is its cousin, the yellow-wattled lapwing (Sarciophorus malabaricus). This one, however, has a yellow wattle and has one syllable less in its call.

The Did-he-do-it is a bird which frequents open plains in the neighbourhood of water. I have never seen it perched on a tree, and as it does not possess the luxury of a hind toe, I imagine that, like the old lady after a rough Channel crossing, it likes to feel itself on “terra cotta.

The Did-he-do-it is a bird that hangs out in open fields near water. I’ve never seen it sitting in a tree, and since it doesn’t have a hind toe, I figure that, like the old lady after a tough boat ride across the Channel, it prefers to be on “terra cotta.

This bird is not likely to be seen within municipal limits, but it is fairly abundant outside Madras. It feeds chiefly upon insects and small crustacea. It is not a gluttonous fowl. “Eha” declares that you never find it where there is food and that it does without sleep, since you never catch it napping. Jerdon, however, informs us that in the South of India it is said to sleep on its back with its legs in the air—a distinctly undignified position for a dandy. It sleeps thus so as to be able to catch on its toes the sky in case this should happen to fall down. As “Eha” says, the chief point about this truly native yarn is that it is impossible to contradict it, for who has seen a lapwing asleep?

This bird is rarely spotted within city limits, but it's quite common outside of Madras. Its diet mainly consists of insects and small crustaceans. It's not a greedy bird. “Eha” states that you’ll never find it where there's food and that it never sleeps, since you never see it taking a nap. Jerdon, on the other hand, tells us that in South India, people say it sleeps on its back with its legs up in the air—a pretty undignified position for a fancy bird. It sleeps this way so it can catch the sky with its toes in case it ever comes crashing down. As “Eha” points out, the main takeaway from this local tale is that it can't be disproven, because who has actually seen a lapwing asleep?

The nesting habits of the Did-he-do-it are most interesting. Strictly speaking, it does not build a nest. It scrapes a cavity, about a quarter of an inch deep, in some stony place. This is the nest. Round it there are a few pieces of kankar or some twigs; [58] whether these are brought thither by the bird, or have merely been brushed there in the making of the cavity, I know not. Very frequently the nest is situated in the ballast of the railway line. Sometimes it is so placed that the footboard of every carriage passes over the head of the sitting bird. There is no accounting for tastes! Four eggs are usually laid; they are much more pointed at one end than at the other, and are invariably placed in the nest so as to form a star, the blunt ends projecting outwards and the thin ends nearly meeting at the centre.

The nesting habits of the Did-he-do-it are really interesting. Technically, it doesn’t actually build a nest. It scrapes out a shallow hole, about a quarter of an inch deep, in a stony spot. This is considered the nest. Around it, there are a few pieces of kankar or some twigs; [58] I’m not sure if the bird brings these or if they’re just moved there while making the hole. Often, the nest is found in the ballast of the railway line. Sometimes it’s placed so that the footboard of every train car passes right over the sitting bird. It’s hard to explain preferences! Four eggs are usually laid; they’re much more pointed at one end than the other and are always arranged in the nest to form a star shape, with the blunt ends sticking out and the thin ends nearly touching in the middle.

Lapwings’ eggs are protectively coloured. Being laid in the open and not hidden away in a nest, it is important that they should not be conspicuous, otherwise they would soon be espied and devoured by some egg-eating creature. Thus they are coloured so as to assimilate with their surroundings. The ground colour is greenish and is boldly splotched with sepia, some of the splotches being darker than others. The eggs are dull and not glossy, hence are very difficult to distinguish from the stones which lie round about them. From the above description it will be seen that the Did-he-do-it’s egg is very like that of his cousin the English plover, whose eggs are held to be so great a delicacy. Why these eggs are so much esteemed I do not know. I suspect that it is because they are difficult to find, and so costly. If tripe and onions cost fifty shillings a pound, this dish would probably form the pièce de résistance of every millionaire’s banquet.

Lapwing eggs are camouflaged. Since they are laid in the open rather than hidden in a nest, it’s crucial that they don’t stand out; otherwise, they’d quickly be spotted and eaten by some egg-loving animal. So, they are colored to blend in with their environment. The base color is greenish, splashed boldly with sepia, with some spots darker than others. The eggs are dull rather than shiny, making them hard to distinguish from the stones around them. From this description, you can see that the Did-he-do-it’s egg looks a lot like that of its relative, the English plover, whose eggs are considered a delicacy. I don’t know why these eggs are so highly valued. I suspect it’s because they’re hard to find, making them expensive. If tripe and onions were fifty shillings a pound, this dish would probably be the highlight of every millionaire’s dinner party.

The eggs of the Did-he-do-it, then, are interesting as forming perfect examples of protectively coloured [59] objects. As I have previously remarked, the theory of protective colouration has my deepest sympathy. It is an unfortunate jade upon which every biologist seems to think that he is entitled to take free rides; the result is that the poor beast’s ribs are cutting through its skin! For example, every bird’s egg is supposed to be protectively coloured—even the gorgeous shining blue egg laid by the seven sisters, which is, in truth, about as much protectively coloured as the I Zingari Cricket Club blazer is. The majority of eggs are laid in nests which are either covered in or more or less well concealed among foliage, hence there is no necessity for them to be protectively coloured. Dame Nature is free to exercise on them to the uttermost her artistic temperament, with the result that there are few things more beautiful than a collection of birds’ eggs.

The eggs of the Did-he-do-it are interesting because they are perfect examples of protective coloring. As I mentioned earlier, I really support the theory of protective coloration. Unfortunately, it’s a confusing topic that every biologist feels they can weigh in on, and the result is that the poor animal's ribs are sticking out through its skin! For instance, every bird's egg is believed to be protectively colored—even the stunning, shiny blue egg laid by the seven sisters, which is actually about as protectively colored as the I Zingari Cricket Club blazer. Most eggs are laid in nests that are either covered or somewhat hidden among leaves, so there’s really no need for them to be protectively colored. Mother Nature can fully express her artistic side with them, resulting in very few things being more beautiful than a collection of bird eggs.

So well do the eggs of the lapwing assimilate with their surroundings, that, if you would discover a clutch of them, your only chance is to watch the actions of the possessors of the nest. But the Did-he-do-it is a wily bird, and if you are not very cute he will live up to his name by “doing you in the eye.” He does not, like babblers and bulbuls, make a tremendous noise as you approach the nest. He assumes a nonchalant, I might say jaunty, air, hoping thereby to put the intruder off the scent. The other day I had the pleasure of circumventing a couple of lapwings. Feeling tolerably certain that a pair had a nest on a flat piece of ground near a canal bank, I determined to find that nest. My wife accompanied me. On arriving at the spot we took cover under some trees and scanned the horizon with [60] field-glasses, but saw no trace of a lapwing. I began to think I had made a mistake. After a time we walked on towards the canal; when we had gone some three hundred yards my wife noticed a bird on a ridge by the canal. By the aid of glasses I saw it was a Did-he-do-it. We both dropped down and watched. The bird had “spotted” us, for he had assumed the air of an old sailor who is smoking a pipe over a mug of beer, the air of a man without a care in the world. Presently he quietly disappeared behind the little ridge. We then made a big detour so as to reach the other side of this. Having arrived there we sat behind a tree. The lapwing was now eyeing us suspiciously. We affected to take no notice of him. Presently a second Did-he-do-it came out from behind a clump of low plants only to disappear into it almost immediately, and then ostentatiously reappear after a few seconds. Had we not known the wiles of the lapwing we should have located the nest behind that clump. But we knew better and waited. One of the birds again disappeared behind the clump, but emerged at the other side and strolled along very slowly; presently it came to some stones, where it stood motionless for a few seconds. It then sat down, or rather slowly sank into a sitting position. There was no doubt that the bird was now on the nest. We made for it. As we approached, the bird that was not on the nest flew off, making a noise with the object of putting us off the scent. The lapwing on the nest quietly got up and strolled off without a sound. On arriving at the place where she had been sitting we found three eggs. I took one of them for a lady who was [61] anxious to have one. Meanwhile both birds had flown away without making any noise. Having examined the nest, we returned to our watching place. In about ten minutes the bird was again sitting quite happily. She had not missed the egg.

The eggs of the lapwing blend so well with their environment that, if you want to find a clutch, your best bet is to watch the behavior of the parent birds. But the Did-he-do-it is a clever bird, and if you’re not careful, it will outsmart you. Unlike babblers and bulbuls, it doesn’t make a loud noise when you approach the nest. Instead, it takes on a relaxed, almost cheerful demeanor, trying to throw off any intruders. The other day I managed to outwit a couple of lapwings. I felt pretty confident that they had a nest on some flat ground near a canal, so I set out to find it. My wife came along with me. When we got to the area, we hid under some trees and scanned the landscape with binoculars but saw no sign of a lapwing. I started to think I had been mistaken. After a while, we walked toward the canal; about three hundred yards away, my wife spotted a bird on a ridge by the canal. Looking through the binoculars, I identified it as a Did-he-do-it. We both crouched down and watched. The bird had noticed us; it was acting like a laid-back sailor, smoking a pipe and enjoying a drink, completely carefree. Soon it quietly disappeared behind a little ridge. We then took a long route to reach the other side. Once there, we sat behind a tree. The lapwing was now eyeing us warily. We pretended not to notice it. Then a second Did-he-do-it came out from behind a group of low plants, only to vanish back into it almost right away, and then showed itself again a few seconds later. If we hadn’t known the tricks of the lapwing, we might have thought the nest was behind that clump. But we waited. One of the birds went behind the clump again but came out the other side and slowly walked along; eventually, it stopped at some stones and stood still for a few seconds. Then it sat down, or rather lowered itself into a sitting position. There was no doubt the bird was now on the nest. We moved in. As we got closer, the bird that wasn’t on the nest flew off, making noise to distract us. The lapwing on the nest quietly got up and walked away without a sound. When we reached the spot where she had been, we found three eggs. I took one for a lady who wanted one. Meanwhile, both birds had flown off without making a sound. After examining the nest, we returned to our hiding spot. In about ten minutes, the bird was back, sitting contentedly. She hadn’t noticed that an egg was missing.

[62]


Shoemaker or seamstress?

The disagreement between the popular and the scientific name of the tailor-bird (Orthotomus sutorius) must, I suppose, be attributed to the fact that the average ornithologist is not learned in the Classics. I freely admit that I did not notice the discrepancy until it was pointed out to me. Orthotomus sutorius means, not the tailoring, but the cobbling Orthotomus. It was, I believe, Forester who, considerably over a century ago, gave the bird the specific name which it now possesses, or rather the allied name, sutoria. If he wrote this in mistake for sartoria, the error was a stroke of genius, since the bird should certainly be called the cobbler rather than the tailor. The so-called sewing of the nest is undoubtedly a great performance for a little bird that does not possess a workbox. Nevertheless, if the dirzie who squats in the verandah did not work more neatly than the tailor-bird he would soon lose his place. Orthotomus sutorius does not sew leaves one to another, it merely cobbles them together, much as the “boy” cobbles together the holes in his master’s socks.

The disagreement between the common and scientific name of the tailor-bird (Orthotomus sutorius) is probably due to the fact that most birdwatchers aren't well-versed in classical languages. I admit I didn’t notice the difference until it was pointed out to me. Orthotomus sutorius actually means not tailoring, but cobbling Orthotomus. I believe it was Forester who, over a century ago, gave the bird its current name or rather the related name, sutoria. If he mistakenly wrote this instead of sartoria, it was a brilliant mistake, since the bird should rightly be called the cobbler rather than the tailor. The so-called sewing of the nest is certainly an impressive feat for such a small bird that doesn't have a toolbox. Still, if the dirzie sitting on the porch didn’t work more neatly than the tailor-bird, he wouldn’t keep his job for long. Orthotomus sutorius doesn’t sew leaves together; it just cobbles them together, much like a kid mending the holes in his master's socks.

When last I wrote about the tailor-bird, I had honestly to admit that I did not know how the bird did its work. [63] My attitude towards its sewing was then that of the child who sings—

When I last wrote about the tailor-bird, I honestly had to admit that I didn’t understand how the bird did its work. [63] My attitude towards its sewing was then that of a child who sings—

Twinkle, twinkle, little star,

Twinkle, twinkle, little star,

How I wonder what you are!

How I wonder what you are!

To-day I can boast with the learned astronomer—

To day I can brag with the educated astronomer—

Twinkle, twinkle, little star,

Twinkle, twinkle, little star,

Now we all know what you are!

Now we all know what you are!

for I have found out how the bird does its sewing.

for I have discovered how the bird does its sewing.

Some months ago Mr. G. A. Pinto, a very keen ornithologist, informed me that a tailor-bird built regularly every year in the verandah in front of his drawing-room window. He told me that he had never thought of watching the stitching operation, and was much surprised when I informed him that, so far as I knew, no one had ever observed the complete process. He said that as the bird would undoubtedly begin building shortly, he would follow the whole process from the other side of the window. He was as good as his word. It is thanks to his patient watching that I am in a position to pen this article. Towards the end of May the hen tailor-bird began “prospecting” for a likely site, for the hen alone works at the nest, and selected a Dracæna plant on the left-hand side of the entrance to the verandah. One of the leaves of the plant was so curved that its terminal half was parallel with the ground. Upon this she commenced operations. The first thing she did was to make with her sharp little beak a number of punctures along each edge of the leaf. In this particular case the punctures took the form of longitudinal slits, owing to the fact that the veins of the [64] Dracæna leaf run longitudinally. In leaves of different texture the punctures take other shapes. Having thus prepared the leaf, she disappeared for a little and returned with a strand of cobweb. One end of this she wound round the narrow part of the leaf that separated one of the punctures from the edge; having done this, she carried the loose end of the strand across the under surface of the leaf to a puncture on the opposite side, where she attached it to the leaf and thus drew the edges a little way together. She then proceeded to connect most of the other punctures with those opposite to them, so that the leaf took the form of a tunnel converging to a point. The under surface of the leaf formed the roof and sides of the tunnel or arch. There was no floor to this, since the edges of the leaf did not meet below, the gap between them being bridged by strands of cobweb. This was a full day’s work for the little bird, and more than sufficient to disqualify her for membership in any trade union.

A few months ago, Mr. G. A. Pinto, an enthusiastic birdwatcher, told me that a tailor-bird builds its nest every year on the verandah outside his living room window. He mentioned that he had never thought about observing the stitching process and was surprised when I told him that, to my knowledge, no one had ever seen the entire process. He said that since the bird would likely start building soon, he would watch the whole thing from the other side of the window. He kept his promise. Thanks to his patient observation, I am able to write this article. Towards the end of May, the female tailor-bird began “scouting” for a good location, as only the female works on the nest, and chose a Dracæna plant on the left side of the entrance to the verandah. One of the leaves was so curved that its tip was parallel to the ground. She began her work there. The first thing she did was make several punctures along each edge of the leaf with her sharp little beak. In this case, the punctures were long slits because the veins of the [64] Dracæna leaf run lengthwise. On leaves with different textures, the punctures take other shapes. After preparing the leaf, she disappeared for a moment and returned with a strand of cobweb. One end of this she wrapped around the narrow part of the leaf, connecting one of the punctures to the edge; then she brought the loose end of the strand across the underside of the leaf to a puncture on the opposite side, where she attached it to the leaf, slightly pulling the edges together. She then continued to connect most of the other punctures to their counterparts, shaping the leaf into a tunnel converging to a point. The underside of the leaf formed the roof and sides of the tunnel or arch. There was no floor since the edges of the leaf didn’t meet below, with the gap between them bridged by strands of cobweb. This was a full day's work for the little bird, more than enough to disqualify her from joining any trade union.

She next went on to line with cotton this cul-de-sac which she had made in the leaf. She, of course, commenced by filling the tip, and the weight of the lining soon caused the hitherto horizontal leaf to hang downwards, so that it eventually became almost vertical, with the tip pointing towards the ground. When lining the nest the bird made a number of punctures in the leaf, through which she poked the lining with her beak, the object of this being to keep the lining in situ. It was Mr. Pinto who first called my attention to these punctures in the body of the leaf. He informed me that he had never seen a tailor-bird’s nest in which the lining [65] did not thus project through holes in the leaf, and that when searching for such nests he always looked out for this. My subsequent observations have tended to confirm his statement.

She then proceeded to line the cul-de-sac she created in the leaf with cotton. Naturally, she started by filling the tip, and the weight of the lining soon caused the previously horizontal leaf to droop downwards, eventually making it almost vertical, with the tip pointing towards the ground. While lining the nest, the bird made several punctures in the leaf, through which she pushed the lining with her beak, aiming to keep the lining in situ. It was Mr. Pinto who first pointed out these punctures in the leaf to me. He told me that he had never seen a tailor-bird’s nest where the lining didn’t project through holes in the leaf, and that when searching for nests like these, he always looked for this. My later observations have supported his claim.

All this time the edges of the leaf that formed the nest had been held together by the thinnest strands of cobweb, and it is a mystery how these can have stood the strain. However, before the lining was completed, the bird proceeded to strengthen them by connecting the punctures on opposite edges of the leaf with threads of cotton. Her modus operandi was to push one end of a thread through a puncture on one edge and the other end through a puncture on the opposite edge of the leaf. The cotton used is soft and frays easily, so that that part of it which is forced through a tiny aperture issues as a fluffy knob, which looks like a knot and is usually taken for such. As a matter of fact, the bird makes no knots; she merely forces a portion of the cotton strand through a puncture, and the silicon which enters into the composition of the leaf catches the soft, minute strands of the cotton and prevents them from slipping.

All this time, the edges of the leaf that made up the nest had been held together by the thinnest strands of cobweb, and it’s a mystery how they managed to withstand the pressure. However, before the lining was finished, the bird began to reinforce them by connecting the punctures on opposite edges of the leaf with threads of cotton. Her modus operandi was to push one end of a thread through a puncture on one edge and the other end through a puncture on the opposite edge of the leaf. The cotton used is soft and frays easily, so the part that gets pushed through a tiny hole comes out as a fluffy knob, which looks like a knot and is usually mistaken for one. In reality, the bird doesn’t make any knots; she simply forces a section of the cotton strand through a puncture, and the silicon in the leaf's composition catches the soft, tiny strands of cotton and keeps them from slipping.

Every one must have noticed how brittle a dead leaf is. This brittleness is due to the silicon which is deposited in the epidermis of the leaf. When the leaf is green the silicon is not so obvious; it is nevertheless there. Some leaves take up more silicon than others; grasses, for example, contain so much that many will cut one’s hand if roughly plucked. I imagine that the tailor-bird usually selects for her nest a leaf or leaves in which there is plenty of silicon. Thus the bird does not make a knot [66] as is popularly supposed, nor is there any necessity for her to do so. Sometimes the connecting threads of cotton are sufficiently long to admit of their being passed to and fro, in which case the bird utilises the full length.

Everyone must have noticed how fragile a dead leaf is. This fragility is due to the silicon deposited in the leaf's outer layer. When the leaf is green, the silicon isn't as noticeable, but it's still there. Some leaves absorb more silicon than others; grasses, for example, have so much that they can cut your hand if you try to pull them roughly. I imagine the tailor-bird usually picks a leaf or leaves with plenty of silicon for her nest. So, the bird doesn’t actually tie a knot as people often think, nor is there any need for her to do so. Sometimes the connecting threads of cotton are long enough to be passed back and forth, allowing the bird to use the full length.

I may mention that when the nest, the building of which I have attempted to describe, was about three parts finished, Mr. Pinto noticed that the bird had ceased to work at it. He was surprised and disappointed. He then discovered that the little builder was at work on a Dracæna plant on the right-hand side of the entrance to the verandah, not two yards distant from the first nest. He was much astonished at the strange behaviour of the bird, and still more so when, the next day, she had resumed work at her first nest, which she completed, leaving the second unfinished at the stage when the punctures had been made and the edges of the leaf drawn together by strands of cobweb. Presently an explanation of the bird’s unusual behaviour occurred to him. His dog which, ordinarily, is chained up at one end of the verandah, was, on the day the tailor-bird left her first nest, fastened up in the middle of the verandah, so that the bird while working at her nest would be within its reach. She evidently objected to this, so began a new nest; but next day, when the dog had been removed, she returned to her more advanced nursery. This accident of chaining up the dog for one day in the middle of the verandah was particularly fortunate, for it enabled me to examine carefully a nest in an early state of construction.

I should mention that when the nest, which I’ve tried to describe, was about three-quarters done, Mr. Pinto noticed that the bird had stopped working on it. He was surprised and disappointed. He then saw that the little builder was focused on a Dracæna plant on the right side of the entrance to the verandah, not even two yards away from the first nest. He was very surprised by the bird's strange behavior, and even more so when, the next day, she returned to work on her first nest and finished it, leaving the second one incomplete at the stage where the punctures had been made and the edges of the leaf tied together with strands of cobweb. Soon, an explanation for the bird’s unusual behavior came to him. His dog, which is usually chained at one end of the verandah, was secured in the middle that day when the tailor-bird left her first nest, making it so the bird would be within the dog's reach while working on her nest. She clearly didn’t like that, so she started a new nest. However, the next day, once the dog was removed, she went back to her more developed nursery. This incident of chaining the dog up in the middle of the verandah for just one day turned out to be really fortunate, as it allowed me to closely examine a nest in its early stages of construction.

This account must, I fear, close with a tragedy. [67] When the little cobbler had been sitting on her eggs for about ten days one of the garden coolies broke them, out of mischief, and thought he had done a clever thing. He is now a sadder if not a wiser rascal!

This story, I’m afraid, has to end with a tragedy. [67] After the little cobbler had been sitting on her eggs for about ten days, one of the garden workers broke them, just for fun, thinking he was being clever. He’s now a sadder, if not a wiser, troublemaker!

[68]


A Colorful Crow

From bough to bough the restless magpie roves,

From branch to branch, the restless magpie roams,

And chatters as she flies.

And chats as she flies.

The magpie has been well called a crow in gay attire. The two species are related, and, as regards character, they are “birds of a feather.” Both are bold, bad creatures, both rogues, thieves, and villains, and, as such, both appeal to me. The magpie with which we are familiar in England can scarcely be called an Indian bird. It does disport itself in happy Kashmir, and has been seen in the uninviting tract of land over which the Khan of Khelat presides. But India, as defined in the Income Tax Act, extends neither to Kashmir nor to Baluchistan, hence Pica rustica may decline to be considered an Indian subject. In this land of many trials his place is taken by his cousins the tree-pies. One of these—the Indian tree-pie (Dendrocita rufa)—is distributed throughout the plains of India, at least, so the books tell us. As a matter of fact, I have never seen the bird in or about Madras. This is curious, for Madras is a garden city (I speak not of Georgetown), and the bird ought to revel in the well-wooded compounds which beautify the capital of the Southern Presidency. Lest its absence from Madras [69] be attributed to the profession tax, let me say that the best legal authorities are of opinion that the bird would not be liable to pay the tax. Not that it would make any difference if the bird were liable. If I know him aright, he would say to the importunate tax collector, “Go and get your hair cut,” or words to that effect. Nor is there, so far as I can see, anything in the much-abused climate of Madras to frighten away the bird. Perhaps the doves are too much for him. If there be one thing more than another calculated to disturb the easily upset equilibrium of the gentle dove it is the sight of a tree-pie. In those places where it occurs you may, any day of the week, see one of these long-tailed rascals being pursued and buffeted by a pair of irate and hysterically screaming doves. In this particular case the doves have some excuse for their anger. The tree-pie, or the Indian magpie as Jerdon calls him, is, to use a colloquialism, dead-nuts on a new-laid egg for his breakfast, and, as doves always display their oological productions on a shakedown in a tree, and as I defy even a museum ornithologist to discover any trace of protective colouration about the aforesaid oological treasures, we cannot be surprised if the tree-pie thinks that doves lay eggs for his especial benefit. Even if the tree-pie does not happen to have been breakfasting off their eggs the doves have ample excuse for chastising him, for does not tradition tell us that Noah’s curse is upon the bird? The rascal flatly refused to enter the Ark with the other birds, so that the Patriarch had actually to send Japhet to catch it!

The magpie is often described as a crow in fancy clothing. The two species are related, and in terms of behavior, they are "birds of a feather." Both are bold, mischievous creatures—rogues, thieves, and villains—and that's what appeals to me about them. The magpie we know in England can hardly be called an Indian bird. It does enjoy itself in beautiful Kashmir and has been spotted in the less inviting territory ruled by the Khan of Khelat. However, since India, as defined in the Income Tax Act, doesn't include Kashmir or Baluchistan, Pica rustica may not be considered an Indian resident. In this land of many challenges, its place is filled by its relatives, the tree-pies. One of these—the Indian tree-pie (Dendrocita rufa)—is said to be found throughout the plains of India, at least according to the books. In reality, I’ve never seen the bird around Madras. This is odd because Madras is a garden city (not counting Georgetown), and the bird should thrive in the well-wooded areas that beautify the capital of the Southern Presidency. To avoid blaming its absence on the profession tax, let me clarify that the best legal experts believe the bird wouldn't have to pay it. Not that it would matter if it did; if I know him well enough, he would tell the annoying tax collector, “Go and get your hair cut,” or something like that. So far as I can see, there's nothing in the frequently criticized climate of Madras that would scare the bird away. Maybe the doves are too much for him. If there’s one thing that tends to disturb the gentle dove, it’s the appearance of a tree-pie. In areas where it’s found, you can see one of these long-tailed troublemakers relentlessly chased and harassed by a pair of furious, screaming doves any day of the week. In this case, the doves have some reason to be upset. The tree-pie, or Indian magpie as Jerdon calls it, is, to put it colloquially, quite keen on a new-laid egg for breakfast. Since doves typically display their eggs on a nest in a tree, and I challenge even a museum ornithologist to find any protective coloring on those eggs, it’s not surprising that the tree-pie thinks doves lay eggs especially for him. Even if the tree-pie hasn’t actually been eating their eggs, the doves have plenty of reasons to scold him; after all, tradition says Noah cursed the bird. The rascal stubbornly refused to enter the Ark with the other birds, so the Patriarch had to send Japhet to catch it!

Unfortunately, the tree-pie does not draw the line [70] at eggs. It is said that it makes no bones about devouring a young bird. I have never seen the creature commit this enormity, but Jerdon is my authority for the fact that “Mr. Smith” has known a bird to enter a covered verandah of a house and nip off half a dozen young geraniums, visit a cage of small birds, begin by stealing the grain, and end by killing and eating the birds, and repeating these visits daily until destroyed. Facilis est descensus Averni.

Unfortunately, the tree-pie doesn’t stop at just eating eggs. It’s said that it has no problem devouring a young bird. I’ve never actually seen it do something so terrible, but Jerdon is my source for the fact that “Mr. Smith” once saw a bird enter a covered porch of a house, nibble off half a dozen young geraniums, visit a cage of small birds, start by stealing the grain, and then end up killing and eating the birds, coming back every day until it was stopped. Facilis est descensus Averni.

This is only one side of the bird’s character. I have seen a tree-pie literally obey the Biblical doctrine of turning the smitten cheek to the smiter; nor, so far as I know, did it, like the well-brought-up boy, after having allowed its second cheek to be smitten, take off its coat and thrash the smiter. The bird in question sat motionless on a branch with a seraphic smile on its face, and appeared to be ignorant of the fact that two little furies, in the shape of fantailed flycatchers, were making puny pecks at its plumage.

This is just one side of the bird’s personality. I’ve seen a tree-pie genuinely follow the Biblical teaching of turning the other cheek; and as far as I know, it didn’t, like a well-mannered kid, after letting its other cheek be hit, take off its coat and fight back. The bird in question sat still on a branch with a serene smile on its face, seeming unaware that two tiny little pests, looking like fantailed flycatchers, were pecking at its feathers.

But before discoursing further upon the merits and demerits of our crow in colours, let me describe him. What applies to him applies to her. To the human eye there is no external difference between the two sexes. This by way of introduction. The tree-pie is a foot and a half long, one foot being tail and the remaining inches body. The head, neck, and breast are sooty brown, and the greater part of the remaining plumage is reddish fawn. The wings are brown and silver-grey. The tail is ashy grey broadly tipped with black. It is impossible to mistake a tree-pie; there is no other bird like it. Its flight is very characteristic, consisting of half [71] a dozen rapid flaps of the wing followed by a little sail. The two middle tail feathers are much longer than the others, the pair next to the middle ones are the second longest, and the outer ones shortest of all. The bird, like all others, spreads out its tail during flight, and the expanded tail gives it a curious appearance.

But before I talk more about the pros and cons of our colorful crow, let me describe him. What I say about him applies to her too. To the human eye, there’s no external difference between the two sexes. This is just an introduction. The tree-pie is about a foot and a half long, with one foot being the tail and the remaining inches being the body. The head, neck, and breast are a sooty brown, while most of the rest of the feathers are a reddish fawn. The wings are brown and silver-grey. The tail is ashy grey with broad black tips. You can't mistake a tree-pie; there's no other bird like it. Its flight is distinctive, consisting of a series of rapid flaps followed by a little glide. The two middle tail feathers are much longer than the others, the pair next to them are the second longest, and the outer ones are the shortest. Like all birds, it spreads its tail during flight, and the expanded tail gives it a curious look.

The Indian tree-pie, as its name implies, dwells principally in trees, and spends most of its time in picking insects off the leaves and branches. When fruit is in season, it feeds largely on that. It moves with great agility from branch to branch, but it frequently descends to the ground to feed and drink. It does not, I think, ever accompany cattle, as does our poor, persecuted magpie at home. It is a sociable bird and is frequently seen in little companies of six or seven.

The Indian tree-pie, as its name suggests, primarily lives in trees and spends most of its time picking insects off the leaves and branches. When fruit is in season, it mainly feeds on that. It moves swiftly from branch to branch, but often comes down to the ground to eat and drink. I don’t think it ever follows cattle like our unfortunate, harassed magpie does back home. It’s a social bird and is often seen in small groups of six or seven.

Like all socially inclined birds, it is very conversational. It has a great variety of notes, many of which are harsh and angry-sounding, others are whistling, metallic calls, acceptable to the human ear. The commonest of these sounds something like coch-lee, coch-lee. If, in a place where magpies abound, you hear any new and strange cry, you are tolerably safe in attributing it to one of those birds.

Like all social birds, it’s very chatty. It has a wide range of sounds, some of which are harsh and angry, while others are whistling, metallic calls that are pleasing to the human ear. The most common of these sounds is something like coch-lee, coch-lee. If you’re in an area with a lot of magpies and hear a new and unusual cry, you can pretty confidently say it’s one of those birds.

The Indian pie is not so expert a nest-builder as its European cousin. This latter, it will be remembered, builds a large domed structure of prickly twigs with an entrance at one side, well protected by thorns. I have not been able to discover why this bird is at such pains to protect the entrance to its nursery. It is so aggressive and pugnacious that no sane thing in feathers [72] would dream of attempting to rob its nest. One ornithologist has put forth the brilliant suggestion that the protection is against its brother magpies. I cannot accept this, for I take it as an axiom that where one magpie can enter, there can another. We must also bear in mind that the Indian species manage to thrive very well in spite of their roofless nests.

The Indian magpie isn't as skilled at building nests as its European counterpart. The European magpie, as you may recall, creates a large dome-shaped nest made of sharp twigs with an entrance on one side, well-guarded by thorns. I haven't been able to figure out why this bird goes to such lengths to protect its nursery entrance. It's so aggressive and combative that no sensible bird would even think about trying to steal from its nest. One ornithologist suggested that the protection is meant to guard against its fellow magpies. I can't accept this idea, since I believe it's a given that if one magpie can enter, another can too. We also need to remember that the Indian species thrive just fine despite their nests being roofless.

[73]


CURRENT SPECIES CREATION

The ornithological world is peopled by two classes of human beings. There are those who study nature inside the museum with the microscope and the scalpel; and there are those who love to observe birds in the open and study their habits. The former, if kept in their place, perform a very useful function, for they co-ordinate and elaborate the observations of the field naturalist. They should be most useful servants to him. Unfortunately these museum men are growing very powerful, and, like trade unions, are beginning to dictate to their masters. Indeed, they bid fair to become the masters and turn the field naturalists into their slaves. The chief aim of the arm-chair or museum ornithologist appears to be the multiplication of new species. Nowadays more species seem to be brought into being by these men than by natural selection. When they are not manufacturing new species, they are tampering with those that already exist.

The world of birdwatching is divided into two groups of people. There are those who study nature in the museum using microscopes and scalpel, and then there are those who enjoy watching birds in the wild and learning about their behavior. The former, if they stay in their lane, serve a very important role by organizing and refining the observations made by field naturalists. They could be great support for these naturalists. Unfortunately, these museum researchers are gaining a lot of influence and, like trade unions, are starting to boss around their superiors. In fact, they seem likely to become the leaders and turn field naturalists into their subordinates. The main goal of the armchair or museum ornithologist seems to be to create new species. Nowadays, it feels like more species are being invented by these individuals than through natural selection. When they’re not busy creating new species, they’re altering the ones that already exist.

I have repeatedly had occasion to speak of the marvellous, kaleidoscopic changes undergone by ornithological terminology—changes which are the despair of the field naturalist. I am not a statistician, but at a rough guess I should say that every species of bird has its name [74] changed about once in each decade. The object of having a classical terminology is that naturalists of all countries shall have a common name for every bird and beast, and thus not be at cross-purposes when conversing or corresponding. But this object is most successfully defeated when the classical name is continually undergoing alteration. It is practically impossible for any one but the professional ornithologist to keep pace with these changes. A poor dilettante like myself has not a look in. For example, I received by the last mail[1] the latest issue of the Avicultural Society’s Magazine and noticed in it an article on the collared turtle-dove of Burma. Wondering what this bird might be, I looked at its scientific name and found it to be Turtur decaocta. I looked this up in both Jerdon and the Fauna of British India, but could not find it; nor could I see any mention of the collared turtle-dove. On reading through the paper I found, to my astonishment, that the bird referred to was our familiar friend the common or garden Indian ring-dove, which for years has been called Turtur risorius. Risorius was a name good enough for Jerdon, Hume, Vidal, Legge, Barnes, Reid, Davison, and a hundred other good ornithologists; but because, forsooth, one Salvadori would like a change, we shall, I suppose, be obliged to adopt the latest new-fangled appellation.

I’ve often found myself talking about the amazing, ever-changing terminology in bird watching—a situation that frustrates field naturalists. I’m not a statistician, but I’d guess that every bird species has its name changed about once every ten years. The goal of having a standard terminology is so that naturalists from all over can use the same name for every bird and animal, avoiding confusion when they talk or write to each other. However, this purpose is completely undermined when the standard name keeps changing. It’s almost impossible for anyone other than a professional ornithologist to keep up with these changes. A casual enthusiast like me stands no chance. For instance, I recently received the latest issue of the Avicultural Society’s Magazine and saw an article about the collared turtle-dove from Burma. Curious about which bird this was, I checked its scientific name and found it to be Turtur decaocta. I looked this up in both Jerdon and the Fauna of British India, but couldn’t find it; I didn’t see any mention of the collared turtle-dove either. As I read through the article, I was surprised to discover that the bird in question was our well-known friend, the common Indian ring-dove, which has been referred to as Turtur risorius for years. The name risorius was good enough for Jerdon, Hume, Vidal, Legge, Barnes, Reid, Davison, and many other credible ornithologists; but now, it seems, because one Salvadori wants a change, we’ll all have to adopt this latest trendy name.

The museum ornithologist has yet another craze. He sees that there must be some limit to the present multiplication of species, so he has hit upon the brilliant idea of making sub-species. Just as the inhabitants of [75] every town and village have little local peculiarities, so have birds of the same species which live in different provinces. The latest idea is to make each of these a different sub-species with a special name of its own. In the near future the scientific name of every bird will be composed of three parts, the generic, the specific, and the sub-specific. Thus Mr. T. H. Newman has discovered that the skin round the eye of the ring-dove of Burma is not whitish, as it is in India, but yellow; Mr. Newman therefore manufactures a new sub-species, which he calls Turtur decaocta xanthocyclus as opposed to the Indian bird which he calls Turtur decaocta douraca. We may consider ourselves lucky that he has not made a new species of the Burmese bird!

The museum ornithologist has a new obsession. He believes there should be a limit to the current explosion of species, so he's come up with the clever idea of creating sub-species. Just like the people in every town and village have unique local traits, birds of the same species living in different areas have their own distinctions. The latest concept is to designate each of these as a different sub-species, each with its own special name. Soon, the scientific name for every bird will consist of three parts: the generic, the specific, and the sub-specific. For example, Mr. T. H. Newman discovered that the skin around the eye of the ring-dove from Burma is not white like it is in India, but yellow; therefore, Mr. Newman creates a new sub-species, which he names Turtur decaocta xanthocyclus, in contrast to the Indian bird, which he labels Turtur decaocta douraca. We should consider ourselves fortunate that he hasn't created a new species for the Burmese bird!

This is not an isolated case. Almost every unfortunate species in the universe is being split up into a dozen or more sub-species. Any local variation in the colour of the plumage is considered sufficient justification for the formation of a sub-species, and we shall undoubtedly, ere long, hear of sub-sub-species!!

This isn't an isolated incident. Nearly every unfortunate species in the universe is being divided into a dozen or more sub-species. Any local change in the color of the feathers is seen as enough reason to create a sub-species, and soon enough, we'll definitely hear about sub-sub-species!!

The hopeless thing is that any Juggins can make new sub-species. It is as easy as falling out of a tree. Let me show how it is done. Take the common sparrow. This pushing little bird, this “feathered Hooligan,” as Mr. Finn calls him, is found all over the world, and every one is able to recognise the sparrow wherever he meets him as the same bird that insults people in London. But the sparrows of each country have their little peculiarities. For example, the cock sparrow in India has more white on his neck than his brother in [76] England. Hence we may make a sub-species of the Indian bird and call him Passer domesticus indicus.

The frustrating part is that anyone can create new sub-species. It's as easy as falling out of a tree. Let me show you how it's done. Take the common sparrow. This pushy little bird, this "feathered troublemaker," as Mr. Finn calls him, is found all over the world, and anyone can recognize the sparrow wherever they see it as the same bird that annoys people in London. But the sparrows in each country have their own little quirks. For example, the male sparrow in India has more white on his neck than his counterpart in England. Therefore, we can create a sub-species of the Indian bird and call him Passer domesticus indicus.

Now, close and patient observation during a prolonged sojourn in Madras has convinced me that the sparrow in the Southern Presidency (I will no longer call it the Benighted Presidency, for experience has shown me that there are other parts of India far more benighted) is quite twenty per cent. more impudent than the sparrows in Northern India. Hence we have no option but to make a sub-sub-species of him. Let us call him Passer domesticus indicus maderaspatensis. We may go even a step further. The sparrows that hold chorus along the ledges of the iron rafters of the Connemara Hotel are far more insulting and exasperating than any other sparrows I have set eyes upon. This surely is quite sufficient provocation for making a sub-sub-sub-species of those birds. I propose to call them Passer domesticus indicus maderaspatensis connemara hotelwalla—a name which I am sure will be received with acclamation both by sparrows and human beings.

Now, after closely and patiently observing during an extended stay in Madras, I’m convinced that the sparrows in the Southern Presidency (I won’t refer to it as the Benighted Presidency anymore, as I’ve realized there are other parts of India that are far more backward) are about twenty percent more arrogant than the sparrows in Northern India. Therefore, we have no choice but to classify them as a sub-sub-species. Let's call them Passer domesticus indicus maderaspatensis. We can even go a step further. The sparrows that chirp along the ledges of the iron rafters at the Connemara Hotel are much more rude and annoying than any other sparrows I’ve ever seen. This is definitely enough reason to create a sub-sub-sub-species for those birds. I suggest we name them Passer domesticus indicus maderaspatensis connemara hotelwalla—a title that I’m sure will be celebrated by both sparrows and humans alike.

But enough of this foolery. The multiplication of species is really a very serious matter, for it is likely to deter sane persons from taking up the most delightful of studies. If the ornithological societies of every country in the world would combine to suppress the evil, it could easily be put down. But there is, I fear, no likelihood of such combination, because these societies are composed mostly of museum ornithologists, and it is too much to expect of these men that they will voluntarily suppress their chief enjoyment in life. To persuade them to act in this altruistic manner it will be [77] necessary to offer them a quid pro quo. The only quid that suggests itself to me is to invite each of them to name a bird after himself. Let the name of every known species (I mean proper and indisputable species) be put in a hat and let each member draw one out. The bird he draws will henceforth be called after him. If any birds are left undrawn after every man has shed his name on one species, the remainder could be balloted for, and thus some lucky dogs would be able to give their name to two birds. When this is once done, it should be made an offence punishable with death to change the specific name of any feathered thing. Newly discovered birds and beasts could, as heretofore, be named after the happy discoverer. This proposal will, if adopted, cure the evil. My point is that it does not matter a jot what a bird be called; the important thing is to give it a fixed and immutable name, so that we poor field naturalists shall know where we are.

But enough of this nonsense. The increase in species is a serious issue because it might discourage sensible people from pursuing one of the most enjoyable studies. If ornithological societies from every country would work together to tackle this problem, it could easily be resolved. However, I fear there's little chance of such cooperation, as these societies mainly consist of museum ornithologists, and it's unrealistic to expect them to willingly give up their main passion in life. To convince them to act in this selfless way, we would need to offer something in return. The only incentive I can think of is to let each of them name a bird after themselves. Let every known species (I mean proper and undeniable species) be placed in a hat, and each member can draw one. The bird they draw will be named in their honor. If any birds remain after everyone has claimed one, the leftovers could be raffled off, allowing some fortunate individuals to name two birds. Once this is accomplished, it should be made a serious offense to change the specific name of any bird. Newly discovered birds and animals could still be named after their discoverers, as before. This suggestion, if accepted, would solve the problem. My point is that the name of a bird doesn’t matter at all; what’s important is to assign it a fixed and unchanging name, so that we poor field naturalists will know where we stand.

[1]Written towards the end of 1906.
[78]


HONEYSUCKERS

Honeysuckers are birds that have adopted the manner of living of the butterfly, and a charming mode of life it is. To flit about in the sunshine and drink sweet draughts of the nectar that lies hidden away at the base of the petals of flowers is indeed an idyllic existence.

Honeysuckers are birds that have taken on a lifestyle similar to that of butterflies, and it's a lovely way to live. To flutter around in the sunshine and sip the sweet nectar tucked away at the bottom of flower petals is truly a dreamy life.

The sunbird, as the honeysucker is frequently called, is provided with a curved beak and a long tubular tongue to enable it the better to rob cup-like blossoms of their honey. The bird must perforce be very small and light, or it would find it impossible to reach the nectar of many flowers. As a matter of fact, it is almost as light as air, so is able to support itself on one flower when drinking honey from another. Sometimes, if no perch be available, the little honeysucker will hover in the air on rapidly vibrating wings and thus extract the sweets from a flower. In this attitude it looks very like a butterfly. I may here mention that sunbirds do not live exclusively upon honey: they vary this diet with minute insects which they pick off flowers and leaves.

The sunbird, often called the honeysucker, has a curved beak and a long tubular tongue that allow it to easily steal honey from cup-shaped flowers. The bird has to be very small and light; otherwise, it wouldn’t be able to reach the nectar of many blossoms. In fact, it’s almost as light as air, which lets it support itself on one flower while sipping honey from another. Sometimes, if there’s no place to land, the little honeysucker will hover in the air with its rapidly flapping wings and extract the sweet nectar from a flower. In this position, it looks a lot like a butterfly. It’s worth noting that sunbirds don’t live only on honey; they also mix things up by eating tiny insects that they pick off flowers and leaves.

LOTEN’S SUNBIRD. (ARACHNECHTHRA LOTENIA)

LOTEN’S SUNBIRD. (ARACHNECHTHRA LOTENIA)
(Note the long curved bill, adapted to insertion in flowers)

LOTEN'S SUNBIRD (ARACHNECHTHRA LOTENIA)
(Note the long curved bill, which is made for accessing flowers)

[79]

Honeysuckers are frequently called humming-birds by Anglo-Indians. This is not correct. Humming-birds are confined to the New World, and are smaller and more ethereal than our little honeysuckers, but their methods of feeding are so similar that the mistake is a pardonable one.

Honeysuckers are often referred to as hummingbirds by Anglo-Indians. This isn’t accurate. Hummingbirds are found only in the New World and are smaller and more delicate than our small honeysuckers. However, their feeding methods are so alike that the confusion is understandable.

As every one knows, butterflies and bees, in return for the honey they receive, render service to the flowers by carrying the pollen from the stamen of one to the stigma of the other and thus bring about cross-fertilisation, which most botanists believe to be essential to the well-being of a species. Honeysuckers probably perform a similar service, for, as they flit from flower to flower, their little heads may be seen to be well dusted with yellow pollen.

As everyone knows, butterflies and bees, in exchange for the honey they get, help the flowers by transferring pollen from the stamen of one flower to the stigma of another, leading to cross-fertilization, which most botanists think is crucial for a species' survival. Honeysuckers likely do a similar job, as they move from flower to flower, and you can often see their little heads coated in yellow pollen.

Sunbirds are found all over India, but they are most plentiful in the South, being essentially tropical birds; they are merely summer visitors to the Punjab; when the short, cold winter days come, they leave that province and betake themselves to some milder clime.

Sunbirds are found all across India, but they are most abundant in the South, as they are primarily tropical birds. They only visit the Punjab during the summer; when the brief, cold winter days arrive, they leave that region and head to a warmer place.

Three species may be seen in our Madras gardens—Loten’s, the purple, and the yellow honeysucker.

Three types can be found in our Madras gardens—Loten’s, the purple, and the yellow honeysucker.

Of the cocks of the first and second species (Arachnechthra lotenia and A. asiatica) it may perhaps be said that they are clothed in purple and fine linen, for their plumage is a deep, rich purple with a sheen and a gloss like that on a brand-new silk hat. Sometimes the bird looks black, at others green, and more frequently mauve, according to the intensity of the light and the angle at which the sun’s rays fall upon it. It is not very easy to distinguish between these two sunbirds unless specimens are held in the hand, when the violet-black abdomen of the purple species can be easily distinguished from the snuff-brown lower parts of [80] Loten’s. However, the latter has a much longer and stouter beak, and is very abundant in Madras, while the purple bird is comparatively rare, so that the Madrassi is fairly safe in setting down all the purple birds he sees as Loten’s honeysuckers. If, however, he espies a purple sunbird, with an unusually short bill, a bird that sings like a canary, he may be certain that that particular one is A. asiatica. If the cock Loten’s sunbird is clothed in purple and fine linen, that of the yellow species (A. zeylonica) may be said to be arrayed in a coat of many colours, each of which is so beautiful as to defy imitation by the painter. There is a patch on the crown which appears metallic lilac in some lights and emerald-green in others. His neck and upper back are dull crimson, the lower back, chin, and throat are brilliant metallic purple. The tail and wing feathers are dark brown. There is a maroon collar below the throat, and the plumage from this collar downwards is bright yellow. Verily, Solomon in all his glory was not arrayed like one of these.

Of the male birds of the first and second types (Arachnechthra lotenia and A. asiatica), it could be said that they are dressed in purple and fine linen, as their feathers have a deep, rich purple hue with a shine and gloss similar to that of a brand-new silk hat. Sometimes the bird appears black, at other times green, and more often mauve, depending on the light and the angle of the sun’s rays hitting it. It's not very easy to tell these two sunbirds apart unless you hold them in your hand, where the violet-black belly of the purple species is easily distinguishable from the snuff-brown lower parts of Loten’s. However, Loten’s has a much longer and sturdier beak, and is quite common in Madras, while the purple bird is relatively rare, so the people in Madras are generally safe in assuming that all the purple birds they see are Loten's honeysuckers. If, however, they spot a purple sunbird with an unusually short beak, a bird that sings like a canary, they can be sure that it is A. asiatica. While Loten's sunbird is dressed in purple and fine linen, the male of the yellow species (A. zeylonica) can be said to be wearing a coat of many colors, each so beautiful that it defies imitation by any painter. There is a patch on the crown that looks metallic lilac in some lights and emerald green in others. Its neck and upper back are a dull crimson, while the lower back, chin, and throat are a brilliant metallic purple. The tail and wing feathers are dark brown. There’s a maroon collar beneath the throat, and the feathers from this collar down are bright yellow. Truly, Solomon in all his glory was not dressed like one of these.

THE YELLOW SUNBIRD. (ARACHNECHTHRA ZEYLONICA)

THE YELLOW SUNBIRD. (ARACHNECHTHRA ZEYLONICA)

THE YELLOW SUNBIRD (Arachnethra zeylonica)

[81]

The hens of all three species are homely-looking birds, difficult to distinguish one from the other. The upper plumage of each is dingy brown and the lower parts dull yellow. Many ornithologists declare that sexual dimorphism, such as is here displayed, is due to the greater need of the hen for protection when sitting on the eggs. These people allege that if the hens of brightly plumaged species were as showy as the cocks, they would be conspicuous objects when brooding, and so fall easy victims to birds of prey. This is a theory typical of the arm-chair naturalist, or of him who studies nature through the grimy panes of a museum window. Like all such theories, it is tempting at first sight, but is untenable because it fails to take cognisance of facts with which every field-naturalist should be acquainted. In the first place, birds of prey rarely attack stationary objects: they look out for moving quarry. Secondly, the cock of many species, such as the paradise flycatcher (Terpsiphone paradisi), although he is far more showy than the hen, sits on the eggs in the open nest quite as much as she does. In this case what is sauce for the goose is sauce for the gander; if she needs protective colouring, so does he. It is true that the cock sunbird never takes a turn on the nest; he is not a family man, but a gay young spark, who goes about bravely attired, with his hand upon the handle of his sword, ready to draw it upon the least provocation. A more pugnacious little bird does not exist. While the hen is laboriously building the wonderful little nest, he spends his time in drinking and revelry, with an occasional visit to the growing nursery to criticise its construction. Hence it might seem that, in the case of the sunbird, the above-mentioned explanation of the sexual dimorphism is the true one. Unfortunately, the nest is not an open one, but a little mango-shaped structure with an entrance at the side, so that the hen when sitting in it is not visible from above. In this case, therefore, as in so many others, we must seek a new explanation of this difference in the appearance of the cocks and hens.

The hens of all three species are rather plain-looking birds, making it hard to tell them apart. Their upper feathers are a dull brown, while the underparts are a muted yellow. Many ornithologists argue that the sexual dimorphism seen here happens because hens need more protection while incubating their eggs. They claim that if hens of brightly colored species were as flashy as the roosters, they would stand out while brooding and become easy targets for predators. This theory reflects the thinking of armchair naturalists, or those who observe nature through a museum's dirty windows. Like many such theories, it seems convincing at first glance but falls apart because it ignores facts that every field naturalist should know. For starters, birds of prey rarely go after stationary objects; they hunt for moving targets. Also, the male of many species, like the paradise flycatcher (Terpsiphone paradisi), while much more colorful than the hen, spends just as much time in the open nest sitting on the eggs. In this case, what's good for the goose is good for the gander; if she needs protective coloring, so does he. It is true that the male sunbird never sits on the nest; he’s not the nurturing type but rather a flashy young dude, strutting around in his fancy attire, ready to defend himself at any moment. No other small bird is as combative as he is. While the hen is hard at work building their beautiful little nest, he’s off drinking and partying, occasionally stopping by the nursery to critique its construction. So, it might seem that in the case of the sunbird, the earlier explanation for sexual dimorphism is accurate. Unfortunately, the nest isn’t open; it’s a small mango-shaped structure with a side entrance, making the hen hidden from view when she’s inside. Therefore, as in many other cases, we need to find a new explanation for the differences in appearance between the males and females.

The nest is in shape and size like a mango. It hangs down from the end of a branch, or any other convenient object. It is composed of dried grass, [82] leaves, cocoons, bits of paper, and any kind of rubbish, held together by means of cobweb and some glutinous substance. There is an entrance at the side, over which is a little porch that serves to keep out rain and sun, but this porch is seen in every nest, even when the bird builds, as it very frequently does, in a verandah. A sunbird recently made its nest in the verandah of a friend of mine; the latter came to me and expressed his contempt for the intellect of the little architect, since she had been fool enough to construct a porch, although the nest was built under cover. He forgot that the building of nests is largely an instinctive act, that each bird builds on a fixed plan, learned by it in “the school of the woods.”

The nest is shaped and sized like a mango. It hangs down from the end of a branch or any convenient object. It's made of dried grass, [82] leaves, cocoons, bits of paper, and various scraps, all held together by cobwebs and some sticky substance. There's an entrance on the side, topped by a little porch that helps keep out the rain and sun, but this porch can be found in every nest, even when the bird builds, as it often does, in a verandah. A sunbird recently built its nest in the verandah of a friend of mine. My friend came to me, expressing his disdain for the little architect's intelligence, since she was foolish enough to add a porch, even though the nest was sheltered. He overlooked the fact that nest-building is mostly instinctual, and each bird follows a set plan it learned in “the school of the woods.”

The nest is cosily lined with cotton down. No attempt is made to conceal it; nevertheless it frequently escapes the notice of human beings, because it does not look like a nest; one is apt to mistake it for a mass of dried grass and rubbish that has become caught in a branch. A sunbird in my compound completely covered her nest with the paper shavings that had once formed the packing for a tin of biscuits. The khansamah, when opening the tin, had, after the manner of his kind, pitched the shavings out of the window of the cook-house.

The nest is comfortably lined with cotton down. There's no effort to hide it; however, it often goes unnoticed by people because it doesn’t really look like a nest. One might easily mistake it for a clump of dried grass and debris caught in a branch. A sunbird in my yard completely covered her nest with paper shavings that had once been packing for a tin of biscuits. The khansamah, when opening the tin, had, like he usually does, tossed the shavings out the window of the cookhouse.

NEST OF LOTEN’S SUNBIRD

NEST OF LOTEN’S SUNBIRD
(Notice that it is built in a spider’s web)

NEST OF LOTEN’S SUNBIRD
(Keep in mind that it’s created in a spider’s web)

[83]

It is doubtful whether predacious creatures mistake the sunbird’s nest for a mass of rubbish; but it is so well placed that they cannot get at it. It is invariably situated sufficiently far above the ground to be out of reach of a four-legged animal; it hangs from an outstanding branch so that no crow or kite can get a foothold anywhere near it, and the squirrel who ventured to trust himself on to the nest would, I believe, look very foolish when attacked by the owners.

It’s questionable whether predators confuse the sunbird’s nest for a pile of garbage; but it’s positioned so well that they can’t access it. It’s always high enough off the ground to be out of reach of four-legged animals; it hangs from a prominent branch, so no crow or kite can find a foothold nearby, and any squirrel daring enough to approach the nest would likely look pretty foolish when the owners defend it.

As is usually the case with birds that build covered nests, the hen is not at all shy. If her nursery happens to be in a verandah, she will sit in it with her head out of the window, and watch with interest the owners of the bungalow taking afternoon tea three feet below her.

As is usually the case with birds that build covered nests, the hen isn't shy at all. If her nursery happens to be on a verandah, she'll perch there with her head out the window and keenly observe the owners of the bungalow enjoying afternoon tea just three feet below her.

[84]


A woodcutter

Not the least of the many benefits which birds confer upon man is the unceasing warfare which the majority of them wage upon insects. Insects may be said to dominate the earth; they fill every nook and cranny of it, preying upon all other living things which they outnumber. If this is the state of affairs when hundreds of millions of insects are devoured daily by their arch-foes, the fowls of the air, what would it be were there no birds? The earth would certainly not be inhabited by men.

Not least among the many benefits that birds provide to humans is their constant battle against insects. Insects can be said to dominate the planet; they occupy every corner and crevice, preying on all other living things, which they outnumber. If this is the situation with hundreds of millions of insects being eaten each day by their biggest enemies, the birds, what would it be like if there were no birds? The earth would definitely not be a place where humans could live.

Most insectivorous birds specialise, that is to say, lay themselves out to catch a particular class of insect. Swifts, swallows, and flycatchers have developed phenomenal mastery over the air, so prey upon flying insects. Mynas, hoopoes, “blue jays,” magpie-robins, and others feed upon the hexapod hosts that crawl on the ground. Not a few birds confine their attention to the creeping things that inhabit the bark of trees. Such are the wryneck, the tree-creeper, and the woodpecker. Of these the woodpecker is chief. A mighty insect hunter is he, one who tracks down his quarry and drags him out of his lair. How must the insects which lie hidden away in the crevices of the bark tremble as they hear [85] this feathered Nimrod battering at the walls of their citadel!

Most insect-eating birds specialize, meaning they focus on catching a specific type of insect. Swifts, swallows, and flycatchers have developed incredible skills in the air to prey on flying insects. Mynas, hoopoes, "blue jays," magpie-robins, and others eat the six-legged creatures that crawl on the ground. Many birds concentrate on the crawling things that live on the bark of trees. These include the wryneck, the tree-creeper, and the woodpecker. Among them, the woodpecker stands out. He is a powerful insect hunter, tracking down his prey and pulling it out of its hiding spot. You can imagine how the insects hidden in the crevices of the bark must tremble when they hear [85] this feathered hunter pounding at the walls of their fortress!

No bird is better adapted than the woodpecker to the work which nature has given him. He is a perfect hunting machine, constructed for work in trees. Note the ease with which he moves over the upright trunk. His sharp claws can obtain a foothold on almost any surface. I have seen a golden-backed woodpecker hunting insects on a smooth well-wheel!

No bird is better suited than the woodpecker for the job nature has assigned to it. It's a fantastic hunting machine, built for working in trees. Just look at how effortlessly it climbs up the straight trunk. Its sharp claws can grip almost any surface. I've seen a golden-backed woodpecker hunting insects on a smooth well-wheel!

His tail, which is short and composed of very stiff feathers, acts almost like a third leg. The bristle-like feathers stick in the crevices of the bark and enable the bird to maintain his position while he hammers away with might and main. His head is his hammer and his beak his chisel. The chisel is fixed rigidly in the hammer so that none of the force of the blow is lost. It is exhilarating to watch a woodpecker at work. He stands with his legs wide apart, the tip of his tail pressed firmly against the bark, and puts all he knows into each stroke, drawing his head back as far as it will go and then letting drive. The manner in which his strokes follow one another puts me in mind of the clever way in which workmen drive an iron bar into a macadamised road by raining upon it blows with sledge-hammers. Almost before the hammer of the first striker is off the head of the bar the second has struck it, this is immediately followed by the hammer of the third, then, without a pause, the first hammerer gets his second blow home, and so they continue until a halt is called. As a small boy I would stand for hours watching the operation. I am ashamed to do so now, so [86] have to content myself with observing woodpeckers at work! There are few things more fascinating to watch than an operation in which skill and brute force are deftly combined.

His tail, which is short and made of very stiff feathers, acts almost like a third leg. The bristle-like feathers stick into the bark's crevices and help the bird hold its position while it hammers away with all its might. Its head is its hammer and its beak is its chisel. The chisel is fixed securely in the hammer, so none of the force of the blow is wasted. It’s thrilling to watch a woodpecker at work. It stands with its legs wide apart, the tip of its tail pressed firmly against the bark, putting everything it has into each strike, pulling its head back as far as it can and then letting it fly. The way its strokes follow each other reminds me of how workers drive an iron bar into a paved road by hitting it repeatedly with sledgehammers. Almost before the first worker's hammer is off the bar, the second one has struck it, quickly followed by the third, and then, without a pause, the first worker lands a second blow, and they continue this way until a stop is called. As a little kid, I would stand for hours watching this process. I'm embarrassed to do that now, so I have to settle for watching woodpeckers at work! There are few things more captivating than watching a task where skill and raw power are skillfully combined.

Even more useful than the beak as a weapon is the woodpecker’s tongue. This is such an important organ that its owner is known in some parts of England as the tongue bird. It is so long that there is a special apparatus at the back of the bird’s head for stowing it away. Its surface is studded with backwardly pointing bristles and the whole covered with sticky saliva. When the woodpecker espies a crack in the bark it inserts into it the long ribbon-like tongue. To this the luckless insects stick and are ruthlessly dragged out to their doom.

Even more effective than its beak as a weapon is the woodpecker’s tongue. This organ is so important that in some parts of England, the woodpecker is referred to as the tongue bird. Its tongue is so long that there’s a special area at the back of the bird’s head for storing it. The surface is covered with backward-facing bristles and coated in sticky saliva. When the woodpecker spots a crack in the bark, it inserts its long, ribbon-like tongue into it. Insects that get stuck are mercilessly dragged out to their end.

The commonest woodpecker in India is the beautiful golden-backed species (Brachypternus aurantius). The head and crest of the cock are bright crimson, the upper back is a beautiful golden yellow, hence the popular name of the bird. The lower back and tail are black; the wing feathers are black and golden yellow, spotted with white, and the sides of the head show a white background on which there is a network of black lines and streaks.

The most common woodpecker in India is the stunning golden-backed species (Brachypternus aurantius). The male's head and crest are a bright crimson, the upper back is a gorgeous golden yellow, which is why it has that popular name. The lower back and tail are black; the wing feathers are black and golden yellow, dotted with white, and the sides of the head have a white background with a pattern of black lines and streaks.

The hen differs from the cock in having the top of the head black with small white triangular spots.

The hen is different from the rooster in that the top of her head is black with small white triangular spots.

The golden-backed woodpecker is one of our noisiest birds. It constantly utters its loud screaming call, which is similar to that of the white-breasted kingfisher. Its flight, like that of most, if not all woodpeckers, is laborious and noisy, the whir of its wings being audible at a [87] considerable distance. The bird gives one or two vigorous flaps of its wings and thus moves in an upward direction, then it sails and sinks; a few more flaps again send it upwards, and so it continues until it reaches the tree trunk for which it is bound.

The golden-backed woodpecker is one of our loudest birds. It constantly makes its loud, screaming call, which is similar to that of the white-breasted kingfisher. Its flight, like that of most, if not all woodpeckers, is strenuous and noisy, with the sound of its wings being heard from quite a distance. The bird takes one or two strong flaps of its wings to move upwards, then it glides and descends; a few more flaps send it up again, and it keeps doing this until it reaches the tree trunk it’s heading for.

I do not think that the woodpecker ever takes a sustained flight. It is seen at its best when on the stem of a tree, over which it moves with wonderful ease in a series of silent jerks, like a mechanical toy. It always keeps its head pointing heavenwards and hops or jerks itself upwards, downwards, or sideways, with equal ease, just as though it went by clockwork. It sometimes ventures on the ground, from which it digs out insects. On the earth it progresses in the same jerky manner.

I don't think the woodpecker ever flies for long distances. It’s most impressive when it's on the trunk of a tree, where it moves effortlessly in quick, silent bursts, like a robot. It always keeps its head facing up and hops or jerks itself up, down, or sideways with the same ease, almost as if it's moving by clockwork. Sometimes it goes to the ground to dig out insects. On the ground, it moves in that same jerky way.

I have never seen a woodpecker sitting like an ordinary bird on a perch. It is often seen on branches, but always lengthwise, never sitting across the branch. It can move along the under surface of a horizontal bough as easily as a fly walks on the ceiling.

I have never seen a woodpecker resting like a regular bird on a perch. It often sits on branches, but always lengthwise, never across the branch. It can move along the underside of a horizontal bough as easily as a fly walks on the ceiling.

I sometimes wonder how woodpeckers roost. Do they sleep hanging on to the trunk of some tree, do they sit lengthwise on a branch as a nightjar does, or do they repair to some hole? I should be inclined to favour the last of these alternatives but for the fact that woodpeckers seem to excavate a new nest every year. This would not be necessary if each bird had a hole in which it slept at night.

I sometimes wonder how woodpeckers sleep. Do they hang onto the trunk of a tree, sit along a branch like a nightjar, or do they go into a hole? I would lean toward the last option, but woodpeckers seem to make a new nest every year. That wouldn’t be necessary if each bird had a hole to sleep in at night.

Sometimes the bird digs out the whole of its nest, but this is not usual. The woodpecker belongs to the “labouring classes,” and, true to the traditions of its caste, it is averse to work, so generally utilises a ready-made [88] cavity. It taps away at tree after tree until it comes upon a place in a trunk that sounds hollow; it then proceeds to excavate a neat, round passage leading to this hollow. In this ready-made cavity it deposits its white eggs, not troubling to add any lining to the nesting chamber.

Sometimes the bird digs out its entire nest, but that's not common. The woodpecker is part of the "working class," and, true to its roots, it dislikes hard work, so it usually makes use of an existing cavity. It taps away at tree after tree until it finds a spot in a trunk that sounds hollow; it then creates a neat, round passage leading to this hollow. In this pre-existing cavity, it lays its white eggs, not bothering to add any lining to the nesting area.

Woodpeckers in England suffer much at the hands of rascally starlings. These latter nest in holes, but not of their own making. If they cannot find any ready-made hollow they listen for the hammering of a woodpecker. They wait until he has completed the nest, and then take possession while his back is turned. When the rightful owner returns the starling looks out of the entrance with finely simulated indignation and asks the woodpecker what he means by intruding. In vain does the latter expostulate. J’y suis, j’y reste is the attitude of the starling. The result is that our feathered carpenter, not being over-valorous, retires and proceeds to hew out another nest. Woodpeckers in India do not suffer such treatment, for starlings do not breed in this country. Their cousins, the mynas, are not so impudent. The only Indian birds which nest in holes, and have sufficient impudence to eject a woodpecker, are the green parrots; but these breed in January, so that their family cares for the year are over long before the woodpecker begins nest building.

Woodpeckers in England have a tough time dealing with sneaky starlings. The starlings nest in holes, but they don't make them themselves. If they can't find an empty hole, they listen for the sound of a woodpecker pecking away. They wait until the woodpecker finishes building the nest and then take over while he's not looking. When the rightful owner comes back, the starling peeks out of the entrance with a fake look of outrage and questions the woodpecker about his "intrusion." No matter how much the woodpecker protests, it’s pointless. The starling’s attitude is essentially, "I’m here, and I’m staying." As a result, our feathered carpenter, not wanting to fight, backs down and starts carving out a new nest. Woodpeckers in India don’t face this problem because starlings don’t breed there. Their relatives, the mynas, aren’t as bold. The only Indian birds that nest in holes and have the audacity to kick out a woodpecker are the green parrots, but they breed in January, so their nesting season is done long before woodpeckers start building their nests.

[89]


A Fast Bird

Which is the most difficult bird to shoot? You may put this question to a dozen sportsmen; probably no two will name the same bird, and each will be able to give excellent reasons why the particular fowl he mentions is the hardest to hit. The reason for this diversity of opinion is simply that there exists no bird more difficult to shoot than all others. Even as beauty is said to be in the eye of the beholder, so does the difficulty, or otherwise, of shooting any particular species depend upon the idiosyncrasies of the would-be slayer. To some shooters all birds, with the possible exception of the coot, are difficult to bring down, while others are able to make every flying thing appear an easy mark.

Which bird is the hardest to shoot? You could ask a dozen hunters this question, and it's likely no two will give the same answer. Each will have solid reasons for why the bird they mention is the toughest to hit. The reason for this variety of opinions is simple: there isn't one bird that’s more difficult to shoot than all the others. Just as beauty is said to be in the eye of the beholder, the challenge of shooting any specific species depends on the quirks of the shooter. For some hunters, all birds—except maybe the coot—are tricky to take down, while others make every flying creature seem like an easy target.

To my way of thinking the chukor (Caccabis chucar) takes a lot of hitting, but this species receives much help on account of its mountainous habitat. It is difficult to hit even a hoary old peacock if the bird gets up when you, already pumped to exhaustion by a stiff climb, are engaged in scrambling from one terraced field to another with your gun at “safe.” The chukor, thanks to the fact, conclusively proved by our friend Euclid, that any two sides of a triangle are greater than the third, enjoys so great an advantage over the [90] wingless shikari that it would be a contemptible creature were it not difficult to shoot. Were I the leader of a covey of chukor, I should thoroughly enjoy an attempt to shoot me. Having taken up a strategic position near the summit of a steep hill, I should squat there in full view until the sportsman had by laborious effort climbed to a spot some hundred and twenty yards from where I was sitting; I should then gracefully retire with my retinue across the khud to the opposite hill, and watch with interest the shooter clamber down one limb of an isosceles triangle and swarm up the other. Some time before he had completed the operation I should again proceed to give him a practical demonstration of the fact that the base of certain triangles is considerably shorter than the sum of the other two sides.

In my opinion, the chukor (Caccabis chucar) is really tough to catch, but it gets a lot of help because of its mountainous home. It's hard to hit even an old peacock if the bird takes off when you’re already out of breath from a steep climb and trying to move between terraced fields with your gun on “safe.” The chukor has a significant advantage over the wingless shikari, thanks to the fact, proven by our friend Euclid, that any two sides of a triangle are longer than the third. It would be a pitiful creature if it weren’t so tricky to shoot. If I were the leader of a flock of chukor, I would really enjoy seeing someone try to shoot me. I would take a strategic position near the top of a steep hill and stay in plain sight until the hunter, after a tiring climb, got about a hundred and twenty yards away from me. Then I would elegantly move with my group across the khud to the other hill and watch with interest as the shooter climbed down one side of an isosceles triangle and scrambled up the other. Long before he finished, I would once again demonstrate that the base of certain triangles is much shorter than the sum of the other two sides.

LOTEN’S SUNBIRD (HEN) ABOUT TO ENTER NEST

LOTEN’S SUNBIRD (HEN) ABOUT TO ENTER NEST

LOTEN'S SUNBIRD (HEN) ABOUT TO ENTER NEST

[91]

If you take away from the chukor his natural advantages I am inclined to think that the grey partridge (Francolinus pondicerianus) is the more difficult bird to shoot. This species is common in most parts of India, yet I do not remember ever having heard of any one making a big bag of grey partridge. Some there are who say that the bird is not worth shooting. If these good folk mean that the shooting of the partridge involves so large an expenditure of ammunition as to deter them from the undertaking I am inclined to agree with them. Given a fair field in the shape of a plain well studded with prickly pear, there is, in my opinion, no bird more difficult to hit than the grey partridge. It is, like all game birds proper, a very rapid flier for a short distance. But it is not so much this which makes it hard to shoot as the rapidity with which it can run along the ground and the close manner in which it lies up. According to Mr. Lockwood Kipling, the grey partridge, as it runs, “suggests a graceful girl tripping along with a full skirt well held up.” In a sense the simile is a good one, for the lower plumage of the partridge is curiously “full,” and so does make the bird look as though it were holding up its skirts. But until graceful young ladies are able to gather up their ample skirts and sprint the “hundred” two or three yards inside “level time,” it will be inaccurate to compare the tripping gait of the one to the speedy motion of the other. The grey partridge is a winged sprinter, a feathered Camilla. It can for a short distance hold its own comfortably against a galloping horse. Frequently have I come upon a covey, feeding in the open and giving vent to the familiar call, and have immediately proceeded to stalk it in the hopes of obtaining a couple of good shots. Before getting within range, one of the birds invariably “spots” me and gives the alarm. The calling immediately ceases and the partridges walk briskly to cover. The instant they disappear I dash towards the cover, hoping to surprise and flush them, but they run three yards to my two, and by the time I reach the bushes into which they betook themselves they are laughing at me from afar.

If you take away the chukor's natural advantages, I think the grey partridge (Francolinus pondicerianus) is the harder bird to shoot. This species is common in most parts of India, but I don't recall ever hearing of anyone getting a large bag of grey partridge. Some people say that the bird isn't worth shooting. If these folks mean that hunting the partridge requires so much ammunition that they avoid the effort, I tend to agree. Given a fair field, like a plain dotted with prickly pear, I believe there's no bird harder to hit than the grey partridge. It’s a very fast flier for a short distance, like all proper game birds. However, it’s not just that which makes it difficult to shoot; it’s also how quickly it can run on the ground and how closely it hides. According to Mr. Lockwood Kipling, the grey partridge, as it runs, “suggests a graceful girl tripping along with a full skirt well held up.” In a way, that comparison is fitting because the lower plumage of the partridge looks “full,” making the bird seem as though it’s lifting its skirts. But until graceful young ladies can gather up their long skirts and sprint the “hundred” in just a few yards, it's not accurate to compare the one’s tripping walk to the other’s swift movement. The grey partridge is like a winged sprinter, a feathered Camilla. For a short distance, it can easily keep pace with a galloping horse. I’ve often come across a covey, feeding in the open and making their familiar calls, and immediately started stalking them, hoping for a couple of good shots. Before I can get within range, one of the birds always “spots” me and raises the alarm. The calling stops right away and the partridges quickly make their way to cover. The moment they vanish, I rush toward the cover, hoping to catch them off guard, but they run three yards for every two I manage, and by the time I reach the bushes where they hid, they're already mocking me from a distance.

Then the way in which a partridge will sometimes lie up in comparatively thin cover is remarkable. One day, when shooting snipe at sunrise, I surprised a partridge feeding in a field. I fired, but apparently did not hit the bird, for it disappeared into a clump of palm trees and prickly pear. Taking up a position close to [92] this clump, I instructed my beaters to throw stones into it. This they did, but half a dozen stones, to say nothing of as many chunks of clay and the most frantic yells and shouts, elicited no response from the partridge. I therefore moved on, and the moment I had turned my back on the clump the bird flew out! This is typical of my experience as a partridge shooter; the birds almost invariably get up from cover at a moment when I cannot possibly take a shot at them. Well might I sing with Cowper—

Then the way a partridge sometimes hides in relatively sparse cover is impressive. One day, while hunting snipe at sunrise, I surprised a partridge feeding in a field. I shot at it, but clearly didn’t hit the bird, as it vanished into a group of palm trees and prickly pear. I positioned myself near this group and told my beaters to throw stones into it. They did, but despite half a dozen stones, as well as some chunks of clay and lots of frantic yelling, the partridge didn’t respond at all. So, I moved on, and the second I turned my back on the clump, the bird flew out! This is typical of my experience as a partridge shooter; the birds almost always take off from cover just when I can’t possibly shoot at them. It is no wonder I could sing with Cowper—

I stride o’er the stubble each day with my gun

I walk over the stubble every day with my gun.

Never ready to shoot till the covey is flown.

Never ready to shoot until the group has taken off.

For these reasons partridge shooting is to me a particularly exasperating form of sport. There are few things more annoying than to hear—“the partridge burst away on whirring wings,” from a bush on which you have just turned your back after having thrown into it half the contents of a ploughed field!

For these reasons, partridge shooting is especially frustrating for me. There are few things more annoying than hearing, “the partridge burst away on whirring wings,” from a bush you just walked away from after throwing half the contents of a plowed field into it!

I am not a bloodthirsty individual, and enjoy watching birds through a field-glass quite as much as, if not more than, shooting them with a gun, but there is something in the call of the grey partridge which makes me want to shoot him. His shrill “pateela, pateela, pateela,” seems to be a challenge. Grahame sings—

I’m not someone who craves violence, and I find just as much pleasure in watching birds through binoculars as I do in hunting them, if not more. However, there's something about the call of the grey partridge that makes me want to shoot it. Its sharp “pateela, pateela, pateela” feels like a challenge. Grahame sings—

Cheerily

Cheerfully

The partridge now her tuneless call repeats.

The partridge now repeats her tune-less call.

For “cheerily” write “cheekily” and you have a good description of the call of our Indian grey partridge, which may be heard in Madras every morning during the winter months.

For “cheerily” write “cheekily” and you have a good description of the call of our Indian grey partridge, which can be heard in Chennai every morning during the winter months.

[93]

This bird does not build an elaborate nest. There is no necessity for it to do so. A nest is a nursery in which young birds are for a time sheltered from the dangers that beset them in the world. When they have developed sufficiently to be able to look after themselves they leave the nest.

This bird doesn’t create a fancy nest. There’s no need for it to do so. A nest is like a nursery where young birds are temporarily kept safe from the dangers of the outside world. Once they’ve grown enough to take care of themselves, they leave the nest.

It is one of the characteristics of the gallinaceous family of birds, which includes grouse, poultry, pea- and guinea-fowl, pheasants, turkeys, and quail, that their young are able to run about almost immediately after issuing from the egg. They are born covered with down, and are thus at first very unlike their parents. They are in reality larvæ, that is to say, embryonic forms which are able to fend for themselves with little or no assistance from their parents. They change into the adult form, not hidden away in a nursery, but in the open world.

It’s one of the traits of the gallinaceous family of birds, which includes grouse, poultry, pea and guinea fowl, pheasants, turkeys, and quail, that their young can start running around almost right after they hatch. They’re born covered in down, so they look quite different from their parents at first. They are essentially larvae, meaning they’re embryonic forms that can take care of themselves with little to no help from their parents. They transition into adults not tucked away in a nursery, but out in the open world.

The nest, then, of the partridge is a very insignificant affair. It is usually a depression in the ground, so shallow as to be barely perceptible, and always well concealed in a bush or tuft of grass. Sometimes the eggs are laid on the bare soil, but more usually the depression is lined with grass or leaves. Occasionally the lining is so thick as to form a regular pad. From six to nine whitish eggs are laid. These do not match the ground or material on which they lie, hence cannot be considered as examples of protective colouring. Their safety depends on the fact that they are hidden away under a bush or tuft of grass. The hen, too, is a very close sitter, and her plumage assimilates well with the surroundings of the nest.

The nest of the partridge is quite simple. It's usually just a shallow depression in the ground, barely noticeable, and always well hidden in a bush or clump of grass. Sometimes the eggs are laid directly on the bare soil, but more often the depression is lined with grass or leaves. Occasionally, the lining is thick enough to create a proper pad. Typically, the hen lays between six to nine whitish eggs. These eggs don’t blend in with the ground or materials around them, so they can’t really be called examples of protective coloring. Their safety relies on being hidden under a bush or clump of grass. The hen also sits very closely on the nest, and her feathers blend well with the surroundings.

[94]


A UNIQUE BIRD

I have hinted more than once at the possibility of there being some understanding between the architect of my bungalow and the feathered folk. On this hypothesis alone am I able to account for the presence of a rectangular hole in the porch, about eight feet above the level of the ground, a hole caused by the deliberate omission of one or two bricks. The scramble for this cavity by those species of birds which build in holes is as great as that of Europeans to secure bungalows in a Presidency town. Last year a pair of spotted owlets (Athene brama) secured the prize and reared up a noisy brood of four. These were regarded with mingled feelings by the human inhabitants of the bungalow. On the one hand, a bird more amusing than the clownish little owlet does not exist; on the other, it is excessively noisy. Each member of the family talks gibberish at the top of its voice, sixteen to the dozen, and as all will persist in speaking at once, the result is a nocturnal chorus that will bear comparison with the efforts of the cats which enliven the Londoner’s back yard.

I’ve hinted more than once that there might be some sort of connection between the architect of my bungalow and the birds. This idea is the only way I can explain the rectangular hole in the porch, about eight feet off the ground, which was caused by purposely leaving out a brick or two. The competition for this hole among the birds that build in cavities is just as fierce as the scramble for bungalows among Europeans in a Presidency town. Last year, a pair of spotted owlets (Athene brama) won the prize and raised a noisy brood of four. The human residents of the bungalow had mixed feelings about them. On one hand, there’s no bird more entertaining than the silly little owlet; on the other hand, they’re incredibly loud. Each family member yells nonsense at the top of their lungs, all talking over each other, which results in a nighttime chorus that rivals the racket made by the cats that liven up a Londoner’s backyard.

THE INDIAN SPOTTED OWLET. (ATHENE BRAMA)

THE INDIAN SPOTTED OWLET. (ATHENE BRAMA)

THE INDIAN SPOTTED OWLET (ATHENE BRAMA)

[95]

This year a couple of mynas (Acridotheres tristis) secured the highly desirable nesting site. Immediately on entering into possession they proceeded to cover the floor of the cavity with a collection of rubbish, composed chiefly of rags, grass, twigs, and bits of paper. There was no attempt at arranging this rubbish, it was bundled pell-mell into the hole and four pretty blue eggs were laid on top of it.

This year, a pair of mynas (Acridotheres tristis) claimed a highly sought-after nesting spot. As soon as they moved in, they covered the bottom of the cavity with a bunch of junk, mainly made up of rags, grass, twigs, and scraps of paper. There was no effort to organize this junk; it was just stuffed haphazardly into the hole with four lovely blue eggs laid on top of it.

One might suppose that the more intelligent the bird the greater the degree of architectural skill it would display. This, however, is not the case. Were it so, crows, mynas, and parrots would build palatial nests.

One might think that the smarter the bird, the better it would be at building nests. However, that's not true. If it were, crows, mynas, and parrots would construct lavish nests.

Mynas do not always nestle in holes in buildings; they are content with any kind of a cavity, whether it be in a building, a tree, or a sandbank. In default of a hole they are content with a ledge, provided it be covered with a roof. A few years ago a pair of mynas reared up a brood on a ledge in the much-frequented verandah of the Deputy Commissioner’s Court at Fyzabad.

Mynas don't always nest in holes in buildings; they're fine with any kind of cavity, whether it's in a building, a tree, or a sandbank. If there's no hole, they're okay with a ledge, as long as it has a roof over it. A few years ago, a pair of mynas raised a brood on a ledge in the busy verandah of the Deputy Commissioner's Court in Fyzabad.

To return to the nest in my porch. The eggs in due course gave rise to four nestlings of the ordinary ugly, triangular-mouthed, alderman-stomached variety. When they were nearly ready to leave the nest I took away two of them by way of rent for the use of my bungalow. This action was in complete accord with oriental custom. In India the landlord has, from time immemorial, taken from his tenants a portion of their produce as rent or land revenue. The Congress will doubtless declare that in levying 50 per cent. of the family brood I assessed the family too highly; but I defy even a Bengali orator to take 33 per cent. of four young mynas. I might, it is true, have assessed the rent at 25 per cent., but the life of a solitary myna cannot be a very happy one, so I took two, a cock and a hen.

To go back to the nest on my porch. The eggs eventually hatched into four pretty standard nestlings with that typical ugly look, triangular mouths, and big bellies. When they were almost ready to leave the nest, I took two of them as rent for using my bungalow. This was totally in line with Eastern custom. In India, landlords have historically taken a share of their tenants' produce as rent or land revenue. The Congress will probably argue that by taking 50 percent of the family, I overvalued them; however, I challenge any Bengali speaker to take 33 percent of four young mynas. I could have set the rent at 25 percent, but a lone myna probably wouldn’t be very happy, so I took two, a male and a female.

[96]

To the ordinary observer the cock myna is as like the hen as one pea is like any other pea. To one, however, who has an eye for such things, the bigger head and more massive body of the cock render him easily recognisable when in company with his sisters. The brood consisted of two cocks and two hens, so that I made a fair division. Some there are who may question the ethics of my action. I would remind such that, incredible as it may seem, the parent birds, in all probability, did not miss the two young ones. Birds cannot count. Even the wily crow is unable to “spot” the extra egg which the koel has surreptitiously introduced into the nest. It is, of course, possible that although those mynas could not count, they missed the two young birds to the extent of noticing that something was wrong with their brood. If they did all I can say is that they concealed their feelings in an admirable manner, for they continued to feed the remaining young as though nothing had happened. If it be thought incredible that the young birds were not missed, is it not equally hard to believe that not one of the lower animals can tell the difference between two and three? If a dog has three bones before him and you remove one of them, he will not miss it unless he sees you remove it!

To the casual observer, the male myna looks just like the female, much like two peas in a pod. However, to someone who's paying attention, the male stands out with his larger head and bulkier body when he's with the females. The group had two males and two females, so I split them fairly. Some might wonder about the ethics of my choice. I’d like to point out that, unbelievable as it sounds, the parent birds probably didn’t even notice those two missing chicks. Birds can’t count. Even the clever crow can't detect the extra egg that the koel secretly places in its nest. Of course, it’s possible that while those mynas couldn't count, they did feel something was off with their brood. If that’s the case, all I can say is they hid their feelings remarkably well, since they kept feeding the rest of the chicks as if nothing had happened. If it seems hard to believe that the young birds went unnoticed, isn’t it just as hard to think that none of the lower animals can tell the difference between two and three? If a dog has three bones in front of him and you take one away, he won’t notice unless he sees you do it!

A chaprassi was appointed to nurse my two young mynas, with instructions to keep them until they should become somewhat more presentable. At the end of three weeks they were adjudged fit to appear in public, being somewhat smaller and rather lanky editions of their parents, with the patch behind the eye white instead of yellow. Having been taken from the nest they were [97] perfectly tame, showing no fear of man, and readily accepting food from the hand.

A chaprassi was hired to take care of my two young mynas, with the task of keeping them until they were a bit more presentable. After three weeks, they were deemed ready to go out in public, looking like smaller and leaner versions of their parents, with the patch behind their eyes white instead of yellow. Since they had been taken from the nest, they were completely tame, showing no fear of people, and happily accepting food from my hand. [97]

Young nestlings display no fear of man, and do not appear to mind being handled by a human being; but as they grow older they learn to fear all strange creatures, hence it is that captive birds taken from the nest are always tamer than those which are caught after they are fledged. It was amusing to see the way in which my young mynas ran towards the chaprassi when he called “Puppy, puppy.” “Puppy” is apparently a term applied by native servants indiscriminately to any kind of pet kept by a sahib.

Young nestlings show no fear of humans and don't seem to mind being handled; however, as they get older, they start to fear all unfamiliar creatures. That's why birds taken from the nest are usually tamer than those caught after they have fledged. It was funny to watch how my young mynas ran toward the chaprassi when he called out “Puppy, puppy.” “Puppy” is a term that local servants use for any kind of pet belonging to a sahib.

Mynas make excellent pets because they are so alert and vivacious, and, above all, because they have so much character.

Mynas make great pets because they are very alert and lively, and most importantly, because they have so much personality.

A myna is a self-assertive bird, a bird that will stand no nonsense.

A myna is a bold bird, one that won't put up with any nonsense.

I know of few things more amusing than to witness a pair of mynas give a snake a bit of their minds as they waltz along beside it in a most daring manner.

I know of few things more entertaining than watching a pair of mynas confidently strut alongside a snake, giving it a piece of their mind in a daring way.

Owing to the self-assertion of the myna he is apt to be quarrelsome.

Due to the assertiveness of the myna, it tends to be argumentative.

Street brawls are, I regret to say, by no means uncommon. In these two or three mynas attack one another so fiercely that they get locked together and roll over and over—a swearing, struggling ball of brown, yellow, and white.

Street fights are, unfortunately, pretty common. In these scuffles, two or three mynas go after each other so aggressively that they end up tangled together, rolling around in a swearing, struggling ball of brown, yellow, and white.

The myna, although by no means a songster, is able to emit a great variety of notes, all of which must be familiar to every Anglo-Indian.

The myna, while not a great singer, can produce a wide range of sounds, all of which every Anglo-Indian must know.

A bird which can produce a large number of sounds [98] is almost invariably a good mimic, and the common myna is no exception to this rule. In this respect, however, he does not compare favourably with the grackles or hill-mynas, as they are commonly called. These can imitate any sound, from the crack of a whip and the exhortations of a bullock-cart driver to the throat-clearing operation in which our Indian brethren so frequently indulge.

A bird that can make a wide variety of sounds [98] is usually a great mimic, and the common myna is no different. However, in this aspect, it doesn’t stack up against the grackles or hill-mynas, as they are often referred to. Those can copy any sound, from the crack of a whip and the commands of a bullock-cart driver to the throat-clearing habit that our Indian friends often engage in.

[99]


SWIFTS

Swifts are extraordinary birds; there are no others like unto them; they are the most mysterious of the many mysterious products of natural selection; their athletic feats transcend the descriptive powers of the English language. What adjective is there of suitable application to a bird that speeds through the air without an appreciable effort at the rate of a hundred miles an hour, that traverses a thousand miles every day of its existence?

Swifts are amazing birds; there are none like them; they are the most mysterious of all the mysterious results of natural selection; their athletic abilities go beyond what the English language can describe. What word can accurately describe a bird that flies effortlessly at a speed of a hundred miles an hour, covering a thousand miles every day of its life?

These wonderful birds are everywhere common, yet much of their life history requires elucidation.

These amazing birds are quite common, yet a lot of their life history still needs to be clarified.

Probably not one man in fifty is able to distinguish between a swallow and a swift. Some think that “swift” and “swallow” are synonymous terms, while others believe that a swift is a kind of black swallow. As a matter of fact, the swift differs more widely from the swallow than the crow does from the canary. There is, it is true, a very strong professional likeness between the swift and the swallow, but this likeness is purely superficial; it is merely the resemblance engendered by similar modes of obtaining a livelihood. Both swallows and swifts feed exclusively on minute insects which they catch upon the wing, hence both have a large gape, light, slender bodies, and long, powerful wings. But speedy though it be, the swallow is not in the same [100] class with the swift as a flyer. When both birds are in the hand nothing is easier than to tell a swift from a swallow or a martin. The latter have the ordinary passerine foot, which consists of three forwardly directed toes and a backwardly directed one. This foot enables a bird to perch, so that one frequently sees swallows seated on telegraph wires. But one never sees a swift on a perch, because all its four toes point forward. It cannot even walk. It spends its life in the air. It eats and drinks on the wing, it does everything, except sleeping and incubating, in the air.

Probably not one person in fifty can tell the difference between a swallow and a swift. Some people think that "swift" and "swallow" are just different words for the same thing, while others think a swift is a type of black swallow. In reality, the swift is more different from the swallow than a crow is from a canary. It's true that swifts and swallows look quite similar, but that resemblance is only skin deep; it comes from the way they both catch their food. Both swallows and swifts feed solely on tiny insects that they catch while flying, so they both have large mouths, slim bodies, and long, strong wings. But even though swallows are fast, they’re not in the same league as swifts when it comes to flying ability. When you hold both birds in your hand, it’s easy to tell a swift apart from a swallow or a martin. The latter have the typical passerine foot, which has three toes pointing forward and one toe pointing backward. This foot allows the bird to perch, so you often see swallows sitting on telephone wires. But you never see a swift perched because all its four toes point forward. It can’t even walk. It spends its whole life in the air. It eats and drinks while flying, and does everything except sleep and incubate its eggs while in the air.

But it is not often that one has a swallow or swift in the hand; it is difficult to get near enough to them to put salt on the tail, so that it is necessary to have some means of distinguishing them when sailing through the air. There is a very marked difference in the manner in which these birds use their wings. This is inimitably described by Mr. E. H. Aitken: “As a swallow darts along, its wings almost close against its sides at every stroke, and it looks like a pair of scissors opening and shutting. Now a swift never closes its wings in this way. It whips the air rapidly with the points of them, but they are always extended and evenly curved from tip to tip like a bow, the slim body of the bird being the arrow.” As a swift speeds through the air it looks something like an anchor, with a short shaft and enormous flukes. If this be borne in mind, it is scarcely possible to mistake a swift for a swallow. Swifts are abundant in Calcutta, but one is not likely to come across a swallow there except when the moon happens to be blue.

But you don’t often get to hold a swallow or a swift; it’s tough to get close enough to put salt on their tails, so it’s important to have a way to tell them apart when you see them flying. There’s a clear difference in how these birds use their wings. Mr. E. H. Aitken describes it perfectly: “As a swallow darts along, its wings almost close against its sides with each stroke, making it look like a pair of scissors opening and shutting. A swift, on the other hand, never closes its wings this way. It rapidly whips the air with the tips of its wings, which are always extended and evenly curved from tip to tip like a bow, while its slim body acts as the arrow.” As a swift zooms through the air, it looks a bit like an anchor, with a short shaft and large flukes. Keeping this in mind, it’s hard to confuse a swift with a swallow. Swifts are common in Calcutta, but you won’t likely find a swallow there unless the moon is blue.

The two swifts commonly seen in Calcutta are the [101] Indian swift (Cypselus affinis) and the palm swift (C. batassiensis).

The two types of swifts often spotted in Calcutta are the [101] Indian swift (Cypselus affinis) and the palm swift (C. batassiensis).

The latter need not detain us long. It is a small and weak edition of the former. It builds a cup-shaped nest on the under side of the great fan-like leaves of the toddy palm.

The latter doesn’t need to take up much of our time. It’s a smaller and weaker version of the former. It creates a cup-shaped nest on the underside of the large fan-like leaves of the toddy palm.

The Indian swift is, in size and appearance, much like the swift which visits England every summer, except for the fact that it has a white patch on the lower part of the back. The chin is white, but all the rest of the plumage, with the exception of the above-mentioned patch, is black or smoky brown.

The Indian swift is similar in size and appearance to the swift that comes to England every summer, except it has a white patch on the lower part of its back. The chin is white, but the rest of the feathers, apart from the mentioned patch, are black or smoky brown.

This bird nests in colonies in the verandahs of houses and inside deserted buildings. The nest is a cup-shaped structure, usually built under an eave in the angle which a roof-beam makes with the wall. Thus the swift finds, ready-made, a roof and a couple of walls, and has merely to add the floor and remaining walls, in one of which it leaves a hole by way of entrance to the nursery. Thus the swift reverses the usual order of things, which is to erect a nest on some foundation such as a branch or ledge.

This bird builds its nests in groups on the porches of houses and inside vacant buildings. The nest is a cup-shaped structure, typically made under an eave where a roof beam meets the wall. This way, the swift finds a ready-made roof and two walls, needing only to add the floor and remaining walls, one of which has a hole for an entrance to the nursery. The swift essentially flips the usual process on its head, which is to construct a nest on a foundation like a branch or ledge.

As we have seen, all four toes of the swift are forwardly directed and each is terminated by a sharp hook-like claw. Thus the swift is able to cling with ease to such a vertical surface as that of a wall, and is therefore quite independent of any ledge or perch. The nest is a conglomeration of grass, straw, and feathers, which are made to adhere to one another, and to the building to which the nest is attached, by the cement-like saliva of the bird.

As we've observed, all four toes of the swift point forward, and each one ends in a sharp, hook-like claw. This allows the swift to easily cling to vertical surfaces like walls, making it completely independent of any ledge or perch. The nest is a mix of grass, straw, and feathers, which stick together and to the surface where the nest is built, thanks to the bird's cement-like saliva.

[102]

Some species of swift build their homes entirely of their glutinous saliva, and so manufacture “edible birds’ nests.” The Indian swift, however, utilises all manner of material by way of economising its saliva.

Some species of swifts build their nests entirely from their sticky saliva, creating what's known as “edible birds’ nests.” The Indian swift, however, uses various materials to save on its saliva.

Nest building is a slow process. Each tiny piece of material has to be separately stuck on to the structure, and the saliva, which is, of course, liquid when first secreted, takes about five minutes to dry. During the whole of this time the bird remains motionless, holding in situ whatever it is adding to the structure.

Nest building is a slow process. Each tiny piece of material has to be individually attached to the structure, and the saliva, which is liquid when first secreted, takes about five minutes to dry. During this entire time, the bird stays still, holding in place whatever it's adding to the structure.

I once timed a pair of swifts at work, and found that on an average they took forty-five minutes in bringing each new piece of material. Much of this time was undoubtedly spent in seeking for food, for so active a bird as the swift must have an enormous appetite, and, as it feeds on the minutest of insects, must consume thousands of them in the course of the day, each of which has to be caught separately. But, even allowing for this, the rate at which the material is added is very slow. Some naturalists declare that the swift is unable to pick anything off the ground. If this be so, the labour of obtaining material must be great, for the creature must fly about until it espies a feather or piece of straw floating in the air.

I once timed a pair of swifts while they were working and found that, on average, they took forty-five minutes to bring each new piece of material. A lot of that time was definitely spent looking for food because such an active bird as the swift must have a huge appetite. Since it eats tiny insects, it must catch thousands of them throughout the day, each one needing to be caught individually. But even considering that, the rate at which they gather material is pretty slow. Some naturalists claim that swifts can’t pick anything off the ground. If that’s true, then finding material must be quite a task, as the bird has to fly around until it spots a feather or piece of straw floating in the air.

I am not yet in a position to say whether it is really impossible for the bird to pick anything from off the ground. I have never seen it do so, and it is a fact that the birds will, when building, eagerly seize anything floating in the air. On the other hand, the helplessness of the swift when placed upon the ground has been much exaggerated. It is said that the bird, if put upon [103] a flat surface, is unable to rise and will remain there until it dies. Quite recently some Indian swifts were brought to me and I placed one of them on my desk. In less than twenty seconds the bird was flying about in the room. Then, again, the grasping powers of its hook-like claws have been somewhat magnified. The bird in question made several unsuccessful attempts to cling on to the whitewashed wall, and eventually fell to the floor, where it was seized and then liberated in the open. It flew off none the worse for its adventure. Nevertheless, its claws are very sharp; the bird in question stuck them quite unpleasantly into me when I held it. A swift can certainly cling to any vertical surface that is the least rough.

I can't say for sure if it's really impossible for the bird to pick something up off the ground. I've never seen it do that, and it's true that birds tend to grab onto anything floating in the air when they're building. However, the idea that swifts can't manage when they're on the ground is largely exaggerated. They say that if you put a bird on a flat surface, it won't be able to take off and will end up staying there until it dies. Recently, I had some Indian swifts brought to me, and I placed one on my desk. Within twenty seconds, the bird was flying around the room. Also, the strength of its hook-like claws has been somewhat overstated. The bird tried several times to cling to the white wall but eventually fell to the floor, where I caught it and then set it free outside. It flew away just fine after that. Still, its claws are very sharp; it dug them into me quite painfully when I held it. A swift can definitely cling to any slightly rough vertical surface.

Unlike most birds, swifts use their nests as houses and sleep in them at night. One frequently hears issuing from the rafters in the dead of night the piercing scream so characteristic of swifts. This disposes of the silly story, so prevalent, that at evening time the swifts mount into the higher layers of the atmosphere and there sleep on the wing.

Unlike most birds, swifts use their nests as homes and sleep in them at night. One often hears the piercing scream that is so typical of swifts coming from the rafters in the dead of night. This puts to rest the ridiculous story, which is so common, that at dusk the swifts rise to the higher levels of the atmosphere and sleep while flying.

In conclusion, I must mention the characteristic flight of swifts just before sundown. The birds close the day in what has been called “a jubilant rout”; as if they had not already taken sufficient exercise, they fly at a breakneck pace round about the building in which their nests are placed, dodging in and out of the pillars of the verandah, and fill the air with their shivering screams. This seems to be a characteristic of swifts wherever they are found.

In conclusion, I have to mention the unique flight of swifts just before sunset. The birds end the day in what has been described as "a jubilant chase"; as if they haven't already gotten enough exercise, they zoom around the building where their nests are, weaving in and out of the pillars on the porch, filling the air with their loud cries. This seems to be a trait of swifts no matter where they are.

[104]


Birds as machines

The sudden change that comes over the nature of most birds at the nesting season is, perhaps, the most wonderful phenomenon in nature. Active, restless birds, which normally spend the whole day on the wing, are content to sit motionless in a cramped position upon the nest for hours together. Birds of prey, whose nature it is to devour every helpless creature that comes within their grasp, behave most tenderly towards their young, actually disgorging swallowed food in order to provide them with a meal. Timid birds become bold. Those which under ordinary circumstances will not permit a human being to approach near them, will sometimes, while brooding, actually allow themselves to be lifted off the nest.

The sudden change in most birds during nesting season is probably one of the most amazing things in nature. Active, restless birds that usually spend the whole day flying around are content to sit still in a cramped position on the nest for hours. Birds of prey, which normally hunt and eat any helpless creature that gets too close, become very gentle with their young, even regurgitating food to feed them. Timid birds turn bold. Those that usually won't let a human get close will sometimes allow themselves to be picked up from the nest while they're brooding.

At the breeding season intelligence, which counsels self-preservation, gives way before the parental instinct, which causes birds to expose themselves to danger, and, in some cases, even to sacrifice their lives for the sake of their offspring.

During breeding season, intelligence that advises self-preservation takes a backseat to the parental instinct, leading birds to expose themselves to danger and, in some cases, even sacrifice their lives for their young.

From the construction of the nest until the time when the young ones are fledged the actions of the parent birds are, at any rate in the neighbourhood of the nest, those of automata, rather than of creatures endowed with intelligence.

From the building of the nest until the young birds are fully fledged, the behavior of the parent birds, at least around the nest, resembles that of robots more than that of beings with intelligence.

[105]

On this hypothesis alone are many of the actions of nesting birds comprehensible.

On this theory alone, many of the behaviors of nesting birds make sense.

That the construction of the nest is in the main an instinctive habit and not the result of intelligence is proved by the fact that a bird which has been hatched out in an incubator will, at the appointed season, build a nest. If birds were not guided by instinct they would never take the trouble to do such a quixotic thing. What benefit can they derive from laboriously collecting a number of twigs and weaving them into a nest?

That the process of building a nest is mainly an instinctive behavior and not a product of intelligence is shown by the fact that a bird raised in an incubator will, when the time comes, still build a nest. If birds weren't guided by instinct, they wouldn't bother doing something so impractical. What do they gain from painstakingly gathering twigs and weaving them into a nest?

It is, of course, natural selection that has originated this instinct; for those species in which the parental instinct is not developed, or in which there is not some substitute for it, must inevitably perish. When once this instinct has taken root natural selection will tend to perpetuate it, since those species which take the best care of their young are those which are likely to survive in the struggle for existence.

It’s obviously natural selection that has led to this instinct; species without a developed parental instinct, or some alternative for it, will ultimately die out. Once this instinct takes hold, natural selection will favor its continuation, as the species that care for their young the best are the ones most likely to survive in the fight for survival.

Many instances can be adduced to show how automatic are the actions of birds at the nesting season.

Many examples can be provided to demonstrate how instinctive the behaviors of birds are during nesting season.

It sometimes happens that a bird lays an egg and then proceeds to build a nest on top of it.

It occasionally happens that a bird lays an egg and then goes on to build a nest over it.

Again, some birds do not know their own eggs. A whole clutch of different ones may be substituted for those upon which the bird is sitting and the bird will not discover the change.

Again, some birds don’t recognize their own eggs. A whole collection of different ones can be swapped out for those the bird is sitting on, and the bird won't notice the difference.

The well-known bird-photographer, Mr. R. Kearton, was desirous of obtaining a good photograph of a sitting thrush, and as he was afraid that her eggs would be hatched before a fine, sunny day presented itself, had some wooden dummies made. These he painted and [106] varnished to look like those of the thrush, and put them in the nest, wondering whether the bird would be deceived. He need not have wondered; she would probably have sat upon the shams even had they not been coloured.

The famous bird photographer, Mr. R. Kearton, wanted to get a good picture of a thrush sitting on its nest. Since he was worried that the eggs might hatch before a nice sunny day came along, he had some wooden dummies made. He painted and varnished these to look like thrush eggs and placed them in the nest, curious if the bird would be fooled. He didn’t need to be curious; she would likely have sat on the fake eggs even if they hadn't been painted.

Upon another occasion Mr. Kearton replaced some starling nestlings by his wooden eggs, and waited to see what would happen. “In a few minutes,” he writes, “back came the starling with a rush. She gazed in wonder at the contents of the nest for a few seconds, but, quickly making up her mind to accept the strangely altered condition of things, she sat down on the bits of painted wood without a trace of discontent in either look or action. Putting her off again, I reversed the order of things and waited. Upon returning, the starling stared in amazement at the change that had come over the scene during her absence; but her curiosity soon vanished, and she commenced to brood her chicks in the most matter-of-fact way.” Then Mr. Kearton took out the chicks and put his fist into the nest, so that the back of his hand was uppermost. The starling actually brooded his knuckles. We must, of course, remember that a starling’s nest is in a hole, where there is but little light. But, provided the starling could not see him, I believe that she would have brooded his knuckles in broad daylight.

On another occasion, Mr. Kearton swapped out some starling nestlings for his wooden eggs and observed what would happen. “A few minutes later,” he writes, “the starling returned quickly. She stared in amazement at the nest's contents for a few seconds, but quickly decided to accept the unusual situation and settled down on the painted wood without any sign of discontent in her appearance or behavior. After removing her again, I reversed the situation and waited. When she came back, the starling was astonished by the changes that had taken place during her absence; however, her curiosity faded quickly, and she began to brood her chicks in the most straightforward manner.” Then Mr. Kearton took out the chicks and placed his fist in the nest with the back of his hand facing up. The starling actually brooded over his knuckles. We must remember that a starling's nest is in a hole with very little light. But, as long as the starling couldn't see him, I believe she would have brooded his knuckles in broad daylight.

Crows, the most intelligent of birds, will sit upon and try to hatch golf balls and ping-pong balls. One famous kite in Calcutta sat long and patiently in a vain attempt to make a pill-box yield a chick, while another member of this species subjected a hare’s skull to similar treatment. [107] Upon one occasion I took a robin’s egg that was quite cold and placed it among the warm ones in a blackbird’s nest. The hen came and brooded the egg along with her own without appearing to notice the addition, although it was much smaller than her eggs and of a totally different colour.

Crows, the smartest of birds, will sit on and try to hatch golf balls and ping-pong balls. One famous kite in Calcutta sat for a long time, patiently trying to get a pillbox to hatch a chick, while another member of this species subjected a hare’s skull to the same treatment. [107] One time, I took a cold robin’s egg and placed it among the warm ones in a blackbird’s nest. The female came and brooded the egg along with her own, not seeming to notice the addition, even though it was much smaller than her eggs and a completely different color.

In the same way, if a set of nestlings of another species be substituted for those already in the nest, the parent birds will usually feed the new family without noticing the change. Instinct teaches a bird to brood all inanimate objects it sees in the nest and to feed all living things, whether they be its own offspring or not, and many birds blindly obey this instinct. It is, of course, to the advantage of the species that this should be so. For it is only on very rare occasions that foreign objects get into a nest, and nature cannot provide for such remote contingencies.

In the same way, if a group of baby birds from a different species is put in the nest instead of the original ones, the parent birds will usually feed the new chicks without even realizing a change has occurred. Instinct drives a bird to care for all inanimate objects it sees in the nest and to feed all living things, whether they're its own young or not, and many birds follow this instinct without question. This instinct is obviously beneficial for the species. After all, foreign objects rarely end up in a nest, and nature can’t account for such unlikely events.

Similarly, instinct will not allow a bird to pay any attention to objects outside the nest, even though these objects be the bird’s own offspring.

Similarly, instinct prevents a bird from focusing on anything outside the nest, even if those things are the bird's own chicks.

As everybody knows, the common cuckoo nestling ejects its foster-brethren from the nest, and if the true parents were able to appreciate what had happened, how much sorrow among its victims would the cuckoo cause! As a matter of fact, no sorrow at all is caused. Incredible as it may seem, the parent birds do not miss the young ones, nor do they appear to see them as they lie outside the nest. In this connection I cannot do better than quote Mr. W. H. Hudson, who was able to closely observe what happened when a young cuckoo had turned a baby robin out of the nest. “Here,” [108] writes Hudson, “the young robin when ejected fell a distance of but five or six inches, and rested on a broad, light green leaf, where it was an exceedingly conspicuous object; and when the mother robin was on the nest—and at that stage she was on it the greater part of the time—warming that black-skinned, toad-like, spurious babe of hers, her bright, intelligent eyes were looking full at the other one, just beneath her, which she had grown in her body and had hatched with her warmth, and was her very own. I watched her for hours; watched her when warming the cuckoo, when she left the nest, and when she returned with food and warmed it again, and never once did she pay the least attention to the outcast lying there close to her. There on its green leaf it remained, growing colder by degrees, hour by hour, motionless, except when it lifted its head as if to receive food, then dropped it again, and when at intervals it twitched its body as if trying to move. During the evening even these slight motions ceased, though the feeblest flame of life was not yet extinct; but in the morning it was dead and cold and stiff; and just above it, her bright eyes upon it, the mother robin sat on the nest as before warming the cuckoo.”

As everyone knows, the young cuckoo chick pushes its foster siblings out of the nest, and if the real parents could understand what had happened, imagine the heartache the cuckoo would cause among its victims! In reality, no heartache occurs at all. Amazingly, the parent birds don’t miss their young ones and don’t seem to notice them lying outside the nest. In this context, I can’t do better than to quote Mr. W. H. Hudson, who closely observed what happened when a young cuckoo pushed a baby robin out of the nest. “Here,” [108] writes Hudson, “the young robin fell only five or six inches and landed on a broad, light green leaf, making it very noticeable; and while the mother robin was on the nest—and at that moment she spent most of her time there—warming that black-skinned, toad-like, counterfeit chick of hers, her bright, curious eyes were fixed on the other one right below her, which she had nurtured in her body and had hatched with her warmth, and which was her very own. I watched her for hours; observing her as she warmed the cuckoo, left the nest, returned with food, and warmed it again, and not once did she pay any attention to the outcast lying close by. There it remained on its green leaf, gradually growing colder, hour by hour, motionless except when it raised its head as if waiting for food, then dropped it again, and occasionally twitched its body as if trying to move. By evening, even those slight movements had stopped, although the faintest spark of life was still there; but by morning, it was dead, cold, and stiff; and just above it, the mother robin sat on the nest, as before, warming the cuckoo.”

Even those actions of nesting birds which appear to be most intelligent can be shown to be merely automatic. Take, for example, the curious habit of feigning injury, which some birds have, when an enemy approaches the young, in order to distract attention from them to itself and thus enable them to seek cover unobserved. This surely seems a highly intelligent act. But birds sometimes act thus before the eggs are hatched, and by so [109] doing actually attract attention to the eggs. This action is purely instinctive, and is perpetuated and strengthened by natural selection because it is beneficial to the race.

Even the behaviors of nesting birds that seem the smartest can be shown to be just instinctive reactions. For instance, some birds have a strange habit of pretending to be injured when a predator comes near their young, in order to draw the predator’s attention away from them and give the chicks a chance to hide without being seen. This definitely looks like a clever move. However, birds sometimes do this even before the eggs hatch, which actually draws attention to the eggs. This behavior is simply instinctive and is reinforced by natural selection because it helps the species survive. [109]

We have seen how at the nesting season all a bird’s normal actions and instincts are subordinated to those of incubation. It is therefore but reasonable to suppose the incubating bird to be in a very peculiar and excitable state, a state bordering on insanity.

We have observed that during the nesting season, all of a bird's typical behaviors and instincts are focused on incubation. It makes sense, then, to think of the incubating bird as being in a very unusual and heightened state, one that is almost like insanity.

A bird in this condition might be expected to go into something resembling convulsions on the approach of an enemy, and, provided its acts under such circumstances tended to help the offspring to escape, and were at the same time not sufficiently acute to cause the mother bird to fall a victim to the enemy, natural selection would tend to perpetuate and fix such actions.

A bird in this state might be expected to go into something like convulsions when an enemy approaches. As long as its actions in these situations help the offspring escape and are not so extreme that the mother bird ends up becoming a victim of the enemy, natural selection would promote and establish these behaviors.

Want of space prevents further dilation upon this fascinating subject.

Lack of space stops me from going into more detail about this fascinating topic.

To sum up the conclusions I desire to emphasise. A bird has during the greater part of its life only to look after itself, and the more intelligent it be the better will it do this, hence natural selection tends to increase the intelligence of birds. But, at certain seasons, it becomes all-important to the species that the adults should attend to their young, even at risk to themselves. To secure this Nature has placed inside birds a force, dormant at most times, which at periodic intervals completely overrides all normal instincts, a force which compels parent birds to rivet their attention on the nest and its contents. Thus the sudden conversion of birds into automata is a necessity, not a mere whim of Dame Nature. The instinct is not of very long duration; for as soon as the [110] young are able to fend for themselves, the parents sometimes behave in what seems to human beings a most unnatural way: they drive off their offspring by force. As a matter of fact, this behaviour is quite natural; it is dictated by Nature for the benefit of the species. Strong as the maternal instinct is, it is liable to be overridden by stronger instincts, such as that of migration. When the time for the migratory journey comes round, the parent birds will desert, without apparently a pang of remorse, or even a thought, the broods for whose welfare they have been slaving day and night. This desertion of later broods by migratory birds is far commoner than is generally supposed. In 1826 Mr. Blackwell inspected the house-martins’ nests under the eaves of a barn at Blakely after the autumnal migration of these birds. Of the twenty-two nests under the eaves inspected on 11th November, no fewer than thirteen were found to contain eggs and dead nestlings.

To summarize the conclusions I want to highlight: A bird spends most of its life mainly focused on itself, and the more intelligent it is, the better it manages this, so natural selection generally promotes greater intelligence in birds. However, during certain times, it becomes crucial for the species that adult birds care for their young, even at personal risk. To ensure this, Nature has instilled in birds a force that usually lies dormant but periodically takes over all normal instincts, compelling parent birds to focus entirely on the nest and its contents. This sudden transformation of birds into automatons is necessary, not just a caprice of Nature. This instinct doesn’t last long; as soon as the young can take care of themselves, the parents sometimes act in a way that seems very unnatural to humans: they forcefully drive off their offspring. In reality, this behavior is quite natural and is driven by Nature for the species' benefit. Despite the strength of the maternal instinct, it can be overridden by stronger instincts, like migration. When it’s time for migration, parent birds will leave, seemingly without any remorse or even a second thought, abandoning the young they have been nurturing tirelessly. This abandonment of later broods by migratory birds happens more often than most people realize. In 1826, Mr. Blackwell examined the house-martins’ nests under the eaves of a barn in Blakely after the birds' autumn migration. Of the twenty-two nests he checked on November 11th, thirteen were found to contain eggs and deceased nestlings.

[111]


Playing Cuckoo

Ornithological experience led me some time back to the belief that at the nesting season a bird becomes a creature of instinct, an organism whose actions are, for the time being, those of a machine, a mere automaton. This view, which has been set forth in the preceding article, is not held by all naturalists. I therefore determined to undertake a systematic series of experiments with a view to putting it to the test. In other words, I decided to play cuckoo. I selected the Indian crow (Corvus splendens) as the subject of my experiments, because it is the most intelligent of the feathered folk. If it can be proved that when on the nest the actions of this bird are mechanical, it will follow that the less intelligent birds are likewise mere automata when incubating. Another reason for selecting the crow as my victim is that I have been investigating the habits of the koel (Eudynamis honorata), which is parasitic on the crow, and in so doing have had to visit a large number of crows’ nests.

Ornithological experience led me some time ago to believe that during the nesting season, a bird acts on instinct, like a machine—a mere automaton. This view, which I discussed in the previous article, isn't shared by all naturalists. Therefore, I decided to conduct a systematic series of experiments to test it. In other words, I chose to play the role of a cuckoo. I selected the Indian crow (Corvus splendens) as the focus of my experiments because it’s the most intelligent of the birds. If I can prove that this bird acts mechanically while nesting, it would imply that less intelligent birds also behave as automata when incubating. Another reason for choosing the crow is that I have been studying the habits of the koel (Eudynamis honorata), which is a parasite of the crow, and this has required me to visit many crow nests.

The crow lays a pale blue egg blotched with brown, while the egg of the koel is a dull olive-green also blotched with brown. It is considerably smaller than the crow’s egg. I have seen dozens of koel’s eggs, but never one that [112] a human being could possibly mistake for that of a crow, yet our friend Corvus is unable to detect the strange egg when deposited in the nest and sits upon it. It is not that birds are colour-blind. The koel is able to distinguish its own egg from that of the crow, for, after it has deposited its egg, it frequently returns to the nest and removes one or more of the crow’s eggs! I am convinced that ordinarily a crow would have no difficulty in distinguishing between the two kinds of egg; but at the nesting time it throws most of its intelligence to the winds and becomes a puppet in the hands of its instincts, which are to sit upon everything in the nest.

The crow lays a pale blue egg covered with brown spots, while the koel’s egg is a dull olive-green also marked with brown spots. It’s noticeably smaller than the crow’s egg. I’ve seen dozens of koel eggs, but never one that a person could mistake for a crow's. Yet our friend Corvus can’t recognize the strange egg when it’s laid in the nest and just sits on it. It’s not that birds can’t see colors. The koel can tell its own egg apart from the crow’s because, after laying its egg, it often comes back to the nest and removes one or more of the crow’s eggs! I’m sure that normally, a crow wouldn’t have trouble telling the two types of eggs apart, but during nesting season, it seems to lose most of its smarts and becomes a puppet to its instincts, which tell it to sit on everything in the nest.

I have myself placed koel’s eggs in crows’ nests, and in every case the crow has incubated the eggs. On one occasion I came upon a crow’s nest containing only two koel’s eggs. As the nest was some way from my bungalow and in an exposed situation, I knew that, the moment I left, it would be robbed by some mischievous native boy, so I took the eggs and placed them in a crow’s nest in my compound. This already contained three crow’s eggs, two of which I moved, substituting the koel’s eggs for them. The crow’s eggs had only been laid three or four days, but the koel’s eggs were nearly incubated, since both yielded chicks on the third day after I placed them in the nest. If nesting crows think, that pair must have been somewhat surprised at the speedy appearance of the chicks!

I’ve personally put koel’s eggs in crows’ nests, and every time, the crow has taken care of them. One time, I found a crow’s nest with just two koel’s eggs. Since the nest was a bit far from my house and in an open spot, I knew it would get stolen by some trouble-making local kid as soon as I left, so I took the eggs and put them in a crow’s nest in my yard. That nest already had three crow’s eggs, so I moved two of those and replaced them with the koel’s eggs. The crow’s eggs were only about three or four days old, but the koel’s eggs were almost ready to hatch, since both hatched chicks on the third day after I put them in the nest. If nesting crows are able to think, that couple must have been pretty surprised by how quickly the chicks showed up!

In all, I have placed six koel’s eggs in four different crow’s nests, and as I have already said, in no single instance did the trick appear to be detected. In the majority of cases, I did not trouble to keep the number [113] of eggs in the nest constant. I merely added the koel’s egg to those already in the nest.

In total, I’ve put six koel eggs in four different crow nests, and as I mentioned earlier, nobody seemed to notice the trick in any of the cases. Most of the time, I didn’t bother to keep the number of eggs in the nest the same. I simply added the koel egg to the ones that were already there. [113]

But I have put my theory to a much more severe test. In a certain crow’s nest containing two eggs I put a large fowl’s egg. This was cream-coloured and fully three times the size of the crow’s egg, yet within ten minutes the crow was sitting comfortably on the strange egg. She did not appear to notice the considerable addition to her clutch. She subsequently laid three more eggs, so that she had six eggs to sit upon, five of her own and the large fowl’s egg! Day after day I visited the nest and watched the progress of the strange egg. On the twentieth day the chick inside was moving, but when I went to the nest on the twenty-first day I discovered that some one had climbed the tree, for several branches were broken. Two young crows had been taken away and the fowl’s egg thrown upon the ground. There it lay with a fully formed black chicken inside! I have that chicken in a bottle of spirit. Subsequent inquiry showed that the dhobi’s son had taken it upon himself to spoil my experiment. However, it went sufficiently far to prove that crows may one day become birds of economic value; why not employ them as incubators? Had the crow come across that chick’s egg anywhere but in its nest, it would undoubtedly have made its breakfast off it.

But I put my theory to a much tougher test. In a crow's nest with two eggs, I placed a large chicken's egg. It was cream-colored and about three times the size of the crow's egg, yet within ten minutes, the crow was comfortably sitting on the strange egg. She didn’t seem to notice the significant addition to her clutch. She later laid three more eggs, giving her six eggs to sit on: five of her own and the large chicken's egg! Day after day, I visited the nest to observe the progress of the unusual egg. On the twentieth day, I noticed the chick inside was moving, but when I checked the nest on the twenty-first day, I found that someone had climbed the tree, as several branches were broken. Two young crows were missing, and the chicken's egg had been thrown to the ground. There it lay with a fully formed black chick inside! I have that chick preserved in a bottle of spirits. Further investigation revealed that the dhobi's son had ruined my experiment. However, it went far enough to show that crows could one day be birds of economic value; why not use them as incubators? If the crow had found that chick's egg anywhere other than in its nest, it would certainly have made a meal of it.

I repeated the experiment in another nest. This time the chick hatched out. When it appeared the rage of the crows knew no bounds. With angry squawks the scandalised birds attacked the unfortunate chick, and so viciously did they peck at it that it was [114] in a dying state by the time my climber reached the nest.

I tried the experiment again in a different nest. This time, the chick hatched. When it showed up, the crows completely lost it. With loud squawks, the outraged birds charged at the poor chick, and they pecked it so violently that it was barely alive by the time my climber got to the nest. [114]

With a view to determining at what stage the incubating instinct secures its dominance, I placed another fowl’s egg in a crow’s nest that was almost ready to receive eggs, wondering whether the presence of this egg would stimulate the crow to lay, without troubling to give the final touches to the nest. The bird devoured the egg. It is my belief that the acts of a nesting bird do not become completely automatic until it has laid an egg in the nest. If one visits a crow’s nest which is in course of construction, the owners will as likely as not desert it; but I have never known a crow desert its nest when once it has laid an egg—provided, of course, he who visits the nest leaves any eggs in it.

To figure out when the incubating instinct takes over, I put another bird's egg in a crow's nest that was almost ready for eggs, curious if this egg would prompt the crow to lay without finishing the nest. The bird ate the egg. I believe that a nesting bird's actions don't become fully instinctive until it lays an egg in the nest. If you visit a crow's nest while it's being built, the owners might abandon it; however, I've never seen a crow abandon its nest once it has laid an egg—assuming, of course, that whoever visits the nest leaves any eggs there.

In another nest containing two crow’s eggs I placed a golf ball; on returning next day I found the crow sitting tight upon her own two eggs and the golf ball!

In another nest with two crow’s eggs, I put a golf ball; when I came back the next day, I found the crow sitting firmly on her own two eggs and the golf ball!

But in another case, where I had found two eggs and substituted for them a couple of golf balls, the crow refused to sit. I suppose the idea was, “I may be a bit of a fool when I am nesting, but I am not such a fool as all that!” I once came across a young koel and a crow’s egg in a nest. I removed the former and placed it in a crow’s nest containing four crow’s eggs. The owner of the nest showed no surprise at the sudden appearance of the koel, but set about feeding it in the most matter-of-fact way. The young koel was successfully reared; it is now at large and will next year victimise some crow. I may say that no human being could possibly fail to distinguish between a young koel and a young crow. When first hatched the koel has a black skin, the crow a pink one. The mouth of the crow nestling is an enormous triangle with great fleshy flaps at the side; the mouth of the koel is much smaller and lacks the flaps. The feathers arise very differently in each species, and whereas those of the crow are black, those of the koel are tipped with russet in the cock and white in the hen.

But in another situation, when I found two eggs and replaced them with a couple of golf balls, the crow wouldn't sit on them. I guess the thought process was, “I might be a little naive when I'm nesting, but I'm not that naive!” I once stumbled upon a young koel and a crow’s egg in a nest. I took the koel out and put it into a crow’s nest that had four crow’s eggs. The nest’s owner didn’t seem surprised by the sudden appearance of the koel; instead, it just started feeding it in a very straightforward manner. The young koel was successfully raised; it’s now free and will likely target some crow next year. I can say that no person could possibly mistake a young koel for a young crow. When first hatched, the koel has black skin while the crow has pink skin. The mouth of the crow nestling forms a huge triangle with large fleshy flaps on the sides; the koel’s mouth is much smaller and doesn’t have flaps. The feathers develop very differently in each species; the crow’s feathers are black, while the koel’s are tipped with russet in the male and white in the female.

THE INDIAN PADDY BIRD. (ARDEOLA GRAYII)

THE INDIAN PADDY BIRD. (ARDEOLA GRAYII)

THE INDIAN PADDY BIRD (ARDEOLA GRAYII)

[115]

In another nest containing a young koel (put there by me) and two crow’s eggs, I placed a paddy bird’s (Ardeola grayii) egg, hoping that the gallant crow would hatch it out and appreciate the many-sidedness of her family. She hatched out the egg all right, at least I believe she did. I saw it in the nest the day before the young paddy bird was due; but when I visited the nest the following morning neither egg nor young bird was there. It would seem that the crow did not appreciate the appearance of the latest addition to the family and destroyed it. It is, of course, possible that the young koel declined to associate with such a neighbour and killed it; but I think that the crow was the culprit, for I had previously placed a paddy bird nestling, four days old, in a crow’s nest containing only young crows, and the paddy bird had similarly disappeared.

In another nest with a young koel (which I placed there) and two crow's eggs, I added an egg from a paddy bird (Ardeola grayii), hoping that the brave crow would hatch it and appreciate her family's diversity. She successfully hatched the egg, or at least I think she did. I saw it in the nest the day before the young paddy bird was supposed to hatch, but when I checked the nest the next morning, neither the egg nor the chick was there. It seems the crow didn’t welcome the new addition to the family and got rid of it. Of course, it’s possible that the young koel chose not to associate with such a neighbor and killed it; however, I suspect the crow was responsible, as I had previously placed a four-day-old paddy bird chick in a nest with only young crows, and it had also disappeared.

These, then, are the main facts which my game of cuckoo has brought to light. They are not so decisive as I had expected. They seem to indicate that the actions of birds with eggs or young are not quite so mechanical as I had supposed. Were they not largely mechanical a crow would never hatch out a koel’s egg, nor would it feed the young koel when hatched out; it [116] would not incubate a fowl’s or a paddy bird’s egg, and it would assuredly decline to sit upon a golf ball. On the other hand, were the acts of nesting birds altogether mechanical, the young paddy birds would have been reared up, and the substitution of two golf balls for two eggs would not have been detected. There is apparently a limit to the extent to which intelligence is subservient to blind instinct.

These are the main facts that my game of cuckoo has revealed. They aren't as clear-cut as I expected. They suggest that the behavior of birds with eggs or young ones isn't as automatic as I thought. If it were purely automatic, a crow would never hatch a koel's egg, nor would it feed the young koel once it hatched; it wouldn't incubate a fowl's or a paddy bird's egg, and it definitely wouldn't sit on a golf ball. On the flip side, if nesting behavior were entirely mechanical, the young paddy birds would have been raised, and swapping two golf balls for two eggs wouldn't have gone unnoticed. It seems there's a limit to how much intelligence is governed by blind instinct.

[117]


The Koel

Anglo-Indians frequently confound the koel with the brain-fever bird. There is certainly some excuse for the mistake, for both are cuckoos and both exceedingly noisy creatures; but the cry of the koel (Eudynamis honorata) bears to that of the brain-fever bird or hawk-cuckoo (Hierococcyx varius) much the same relation as the melody of the organ-grinder does to that of a full German band. Most men are willing to offer either the solitary Italian or the Teutonic gang a penny to go into the next street, but, if forced to choose between them, select the organ-grinder as the lesser of the two evils. In the same way, most people find the fluty note of the koel less obnoxious than the shriek of the hawk-cuckoo.

Anglo-Indians often confuse the koel with the brain-fever bird. There's definitely some reason for the mix-up, since both are cuckoos and both are extremely noisy; however, the cry of the koel (Eudynamis honorata) is to that of the brain-fever bird or hawk-cuckoo (Hierococcyx varius) what the melody of the organ-grinder is to a full German band. Most people are willing to give either the lone Italian or the German group a penny to move to the next street, but if they have to choose between them, they usually pick the organ-grinder as the lesser annoyance. Similarly, most people find the fluty call of the koel less irritating than the screech of the hawk-cuckoo.

The latter utters a treble note, which sounds like “Brain fever.” This it is never tired of repeating. It commences low down the musical scale and then ascends higher and higher until you think the bird must burst. But it never does burst. When the top note is reached the exercise is repeated.

The latter makes a high-pitched sound that resembles "Brain fever." It never gets tired of repeating this. It starts low on the musical scale and then climbs higher and higher until you think the bird might explode. But it never does explode. Once it hits the highest note, the whole thing starts over again.

The koel is a bird of many cries. As it does not, like the brain-fever bird, talk English, its notes are not easy to reproduce on paper. Its commonest call is a crescendo kuil, kuil, kuil, from which the bird derives its [118] popular name. This cry is peculiar to the cock. The second note is, to use the words of Colonel Cunningham, “an outrageous torrent of shouts, sounding like kūk, kŭū, kŭū, kŭū, kŭū, kŭū, repeated at brief intervals in tones loud enough to rouse the ‘Seven Sleepers.’” The koel is nothing if not impressive. He likes to utter this note just before dawn, when all the world is still. As the bird calls chiefly in the hot weather, when it frequently happens that the hour before sunrise is almost the only one in the twenty-four in which the jaded European can sleep, this note is productive of much evil language on the part of the aforesaid European.

The koel is a bird with many calls. Since it doesn’t speak English like the brain-fever bird, its sounds are hard to write down. Its most common call is a rising crescendo of kuil, kuil, kuil, which is where it gets its popular name. This call is unique to the male. The second note is, in Colonel Cunningham's words, “an outrageous torrent of shouts, sounding like kūk, kŭū, kŭū, kŭū, kŭū, repeated at short intervals in tones loud enough to wake the ‘Seven Sleepers.’” The koel is nothing if not impressive. It loves to make this call just before dawn, when everything is quiet. Since the bird mostly sings during hot weather, and the hour before sunrise is often the only time in twenty-four hours that a tired European can get some sleep, this call leads to a lot of swearing from that European.

The koel’s third cry is well described by Cunningham as a mere cataract of shrill shrieks—heekaree, karees. This is heard mostly when the hen is fleeing for dear life before a pair of outraged crows. So much for the voice of the koel, now for a description of the singer. The cock is a jet-black bird with a green bill and a red eye. The hen is speckled black and white, with the eye and beak as in the cock. Add to this the fact that the koel is a little larger than the “merry cuckoo, messenger of spring” which visits England, and it is impossible not to recognise the bird.

The koel's third call is vividly described by Cunningham as a cascade of sharp sounds—heekaree, karees. This is mostly heard when the female is desperately trying to escape from a couple of furious crows. That covers the koel's voice; now let's talk about the singer. The male is a glossy black bird with a green beak and a red eye. The female is speckled black and white, with the same eye and beak as the male. To this, add that the koel is a bit larger than the “merry cuckoo, messenger of spring” that comes to England, and it's hard not to recognize the bird.

This cuckoo, like many of its relatives, does not hatch its own eggs. It cuckolds crows. This is no mean performance, for the crow is a suspicious creature. It knoweth full well the evil which is in its own heart, and so, judging others by itself, watches unceasingly over its nest from the time the first egg is deposited therein until the hour when the most backward young one is able to fly. Now, a koel is no match for a crow in open [119] fight, hence it is quite useless for the former to attempt by means of force to introduce its egg into the crow’s nest. It is obliged to resort to guile. The cock entices away the crows, and while they are absent the hen deposits her egg.

This cuckoo, like many of its relatives, doesn’t hatch its own eggs. It tricks crows. This is no easy feat, as the crow is a suspicious creature. It knows very well the darkness that exists within itself, and so, judging others by its own nature, it keeps a constant watch over its nest from the moment the first egg is laid until the last young one is able to fly. Now, a koel is no match for a crow in a direct confrontation, so it doesn't try to force its egg into the crow’s nest. Instead, it uses cunning. The male lures the crows away, and while they are gone, the female lays her egg.

Crows appear to dislike the cry of the koel quite as much as men do. But whereas man is usually content with swearing at the noisy cuckoo, crows attack it with beak and claw whenever an opportunity offers. This fact is turned to account by the koel. The cock alights in a tree near a crow’s nest and begins to call. The owners of the nest, sooner or later, “go for” him. He then takes to his wings, continuing to call, so as to induce the crows to prolong the chase. As he is a more rapid flier than they, he does not run much risk. While the irate corvi are in pursuit, the hen koel, who has been lurking around, slips into the nest and there lays her egg. If she is given time she destroys one or more of those already in the nest. She does this, not because the crows would detect the presence of an additional egg, but in order that her young, when hatched, will not be starved owing to the large number of mouths to feed.

Crows seem to dislike the call of the koel just as much as people do. But while humans are usually satisfied with just cursing at the noisy cuckoo, crows attack it with their beaks and claws whenever they get the chance. The koel takes advantage of this. The male lands in a tree near a crow’s nest and starts calling out. Eventually, the crows that own the nest go after him. He then flies away, continuing to call, to encourage the crows to keep chasing him. Since he is a faster flyer, he doesn’t face much danger. While the angry crows are in pursuit, the female koel, who has been hiding nearby, sneaks into the nest and lays her egg there. If she has enough time, she will destroy one or more of the eggs already in the nest. She does this not because the crows would notice an extra egg, but so her chicks won’t be at risk of starvation due to the large number of chicks to feed.

Crows, although such clever birds, are, as we have seen, remarkably stupid at the nesting season. They are unable to distinguish the koel’s egg from their own, although the former is considerably smaller, with an olive-green background instead of a bluish one; and when the young koel emerges from the egg, they are unable to differentiate between it and their own offspring, although baby koels are black and baby crows [120] pink, when first hatched out. The koel nestling has one point in common with young crows, and that is a large mouth of which the inside is red. This is opened wide whenever a parent approaches, so that the latter sees nothing but a number of yawning caverns; thus there is some excuse for its failure to distinguish between the true and the spurious nestlings.

Crows, despite being such smart birds, are surprisingly clueless during nesting season. They can't tell a koel's egg from their own, even though the koel's egg is much smaller and has an olive-green background instead of a bluish one. When the young koel hatches, they can't tell it apart from their own chicks, even though baby koels are black and baby crows are pink when they first hatch. The koel chick shares one feature with young crows: a big mouth with a red inside. This mouth opens wide whenever a parent is near, making it so that the parent only sees a bunch of gaping mouths; this explains why they fail to recognize the real chicks from the impostors.

To return to the koel who is laying her egg in the momentarily deserted nest. She does not carry her egg thither in her beak as the common cuckoo is said to do, but sits in the nest and lays it there. Sometimes the crows return before she is ready and, of course, attack her, but as she can fly faster than they, they do not often succeed in harming her, although there are instances on record of crows mobbing female koels to death. It will thus be seen that cuckolding crows is dangerous work. The life of the cuckoo is not all beer and skittles, and the birds seem to feel the danger of their existence, for at the breeding season they appear to be in a most excited state, and are manifestly afraid of the crows. This being so, I am inclined to think that the latter are responsible for the parasitic habit of the koel. It is not improbably a case of the biter bit. Crows are such aggressive birds that they are quite capable of evicting any other bird from its nest if this be large enough to suit their purpose. Now suppose a koel to be thus evicted by force when ready to lay; it is quite conceivable that she might make frantic efforts to lay in her rightful nest, and if she succeeded, and the crows failed to detect her egg, they would hatch out her offspring. If the koels which acted [121] thus managed to have their offspring reared for them, while those that attempted to build fresh nests dropped their eggs before the new nurseries were ready, natural selection would tend to weed out the latter and thus the parasitic habit might arise, until eventually the koel came to forget how to build a nest.

To go back to the koel laying her egg in the temporarily empty nest. She doesn’t carry her egg in her beak like the common cuckoo is said to do, but instead, she sits in the nest and lays it there. Sometimes the crows come back before she’s ready and, of course, attack her, but since she can fly faster than them, they rarely succeed in harming her, though there are recorded cases of crows mobbing female koels to death. So, it’s clear that cuckolding crows is risky work. The life of the cuckoo isn’t all fun and games, and the birds seem aware of the dangers they face, as during breeding season they appear to be quite agitated and are clearly scared of the crows. If this is the case, I think the crows might be responsible for the koel's parasitic behavior. It’s likely a situation of the aggressor being affected. Crows are such aggressive birds that they can easily evict any other bird from its nest if it’s large enough for their needs. Now, imagine a koel getting forced out when she’s ready to lay; it’s easy to see she might frantically try to lay in her original nest, and if she succeeds without the crows noticing her egg, they would end up hatching her young. If koels that acted this way managed to have their young raised for them while those that tried to build new nests lost their eggs before the new homes were ready, natural selection would likely eliminate the latter, leading to the development of the parasitic behavior until eventually, the koel forgot how to build a nest.

In this connection it is important to bear in mind that the nearest relatives of the koel are non-parasitic. It is therefore not improbable that in the koel the parasitic habit has an independent origin.

In this regard, it's important to remember that the closest relatives of the koel are non-parasitic. So, it's quite possible that the koel's parasitic behavior has developed independently.

This instinct has undoubtedly been evolved more than once. It does not necessarily follow that similar causes have led to its origin in each case.

This instinct has definitely evolved more than once. It doesn’t necessarily mean that the same causes resulted in its development every time.

The suggestion I have made is made only with reference to the koel, which differs from other cuckoos in that it dupes a bird stronger and bigger than itself. But this is a digression.

The suggestion I’ve made only refers to the koel, which is different from other cuckoos because it tricks a bird that is stronger and larger than itself. But that’s a side note.

If the koel have time, she destroys one or more of the existing eggs, and will sometimes return later and destroy others. Although the crow cannot distinguish between her own and koel’s eggs, the koel can. I have come across several crows’ nests which each contained only two koel’s eggs.

If the koel has the chance, she destroys one or more of the existing eggs, and sometimes comes back later to destroy more. While the crow can't tell the difference between her own eggs and the koel's, the koel can. I’ve found several crows’ nests that only had two koel eggs in them.

The young koel is a better-behaved bird than some of its relations, for it ejects neither the eggs still in the nest when it is hatched nor its foster-brethren. But the incubating period of the koel is shorter than that of the crow, so that the koel’s egg is always the first to hatch out. The koel seems never to make the mistake of depositing its egg among nearly incubated ones. Thus the young koel commences life with a useful start [122] on its foster-brethren. It soon increases this start, as it grows very fast, and is ready to fly before the earliest feathers of its foster-brothers are out of their sheaths.

The young koel is a more well-behaved bird than some of its relatives because it doesn't push out the eggs still in the nest when it hatches or harm its foster siblings. However, the incubation period for the koel is shorter than that of the crow, so the koel's egg is always the first to hatch. The koel never seems to make the mistake of laying its egg among eggs that are nearly hatched. As a result, the young koel starts its life with a beneficial advantage over its foster siblings. It quickly builds on this advantage, growing rapidly and being ready to fly before the first feathers of its foster brothers have even emerged from their sheaths. [122]

It does not, however, leave its foster-parents when able to fly. It sits on the edge of the nest and makes laudable, if ludicrous, efforts at cawing. The crows continue feeding it long after it has left the nest, looking after it with the utmost solicitude. A young koel is somewhat lacking in intelligence; it seems unable to distinguish its foster-parents from any other crow, for it opens its mouth at the approach of every crow, evidently expecting to be fed.

It doesn’t leave its foster parents even when it can fly. It perches on the edge of the nest and makes admirable, yet silly, attempts at cawing. The crows keep feeding it long after it has left the nest, taking care of it with great attention. A young koel isn’t particularly smart; it can’t tell its foster parents apart from any other crow, so it opens its mouth whenever a crow approaches, clearly expecting to be fed.

The natives of the Punjab assert that the hen koel keeps her eye on the crow’s nest in which she has laid her egg or eggs during the whole of the time that the young cuckoo is in it, and takes charge of her babe after it leaves the nest. This assertion appears to be incorrect. I have never seen a koel feeding anything but itself. Moreover, the koel lays four or five eggs, and these are not usually all deposited in one nest. It would therefore be exceedingly difficult, if not impossible, for the hen to keep an eye on each of her eggs.

The people of Punjab say that the female koel watches over the crow's nest where she has laid her egg or eggs for the entire time that the young cuckoo is there, and she takes care of her chick once it leaves the nest. However, this claim seems to be wrong. I've never seen a koel feeding anything but itself. Also, the koel usually lays four or five eggs, and they aren't typically all laid in one nest. So, it would be very difficult, if not impossible, for the hen to keep track of each of her eggs.

In view of the hatred which crows display towards koels in general, naturalists have expressed surprise that the young koels are not mobbed directly they leave the nest. Their plumage differs in no way from that of the adult. It has been suggested that young koels retain the crow smell for a considerable time after they are fledged. This I cannot accept. The olfactory organ of birds is but slightly developed. Indeed, I am [123] inclined to wonder whether birds have any sense of smell. The truth of the matter is that crows look after their foster-children most carefully for several weeks after they have left the nest, and see that no strange crow harms them.

Given the hatred that crows generally have for koels, naturalists have been surprised that young koels aren't attacked as soon as they leave the nest. Their feathers look exactly like those of adults. Some have suggested that young koels keep a crow scent for a long time after they fledge. I can’t agree with that. Birds' sense of smell is not very developed. In fact, I wonder if birds have any sense of smell at all. The reality is that crows take care of their foster chicks very carefully for several weeks after they leave the nest, making sure no unfamiliar crow harms them.

[124]


The Indian Common Dove

The dove family ought to have become extinct ages ago, if all that orthodox zoologists tell us about the fierce struggle for existence be true. They form a regular “Thirteen Society.” They do everything they should not do, they disobey every rule of animal warfare, they fall asleep when sitting exposed on a telegraph wire, they build nests in all manner of foolish places, their nests are about as unsafe as a nursery can possibly be, and they flatly decline to lay protectively coloured eggs—their white eggs are a standing invitation to bird robbers to indulge, like the Cambridge crew of 1906, in an egg diet; yet, in spite all of these foolhardy acts, doves flourish like the green bay tree. This is a fact of which I require an explanation before I can accept all the doctrines of the Neo-Darwinian school.

The dove family should have gone extinct a long time ago if everything traditional zoologists say about the brutal struggle for survival is true. They behave like a regular “Thirteen Society.” They do all the things they’re not supposed to do, break every rule of animal behavior, fall asleep while sitting exposed on a telegraph wire, build nests in all sorts of foolish places, their nests are as unsafe as a nursery can be, and they completely refuse to lay camouflaged eggs— their white eggs are like a welcome sign for bird thieves to feast on, just like the Cambridge crew of 1906 on an egg diet; yet, despite all these reckless actions, doves thrive like the green bay tree. I need an explanation for this before I can accept all the theories of the Neo-Darwinian school.

There are so many species of dove in India that when speaking of them one must perforce, unless one be writing a great monograph, confine oneself to two or three of the common species. I propose to-day to talk about our three commonest Indian doves, that is to say, the spotted dove (Turtur suratensis), the Indian ring-dove (Turtur risorius), and the little brown [125] dove (Turtur cambayensis). I make no apology for discoursing upon these common species. I contend that we in India know so very little about even our everyday birds that it is a needless expenditure of energy to seek out the rarer species and study their habits; we have plenty to learn about those that come into our verandahs and coo to us.

There are so many types of doves in India that when discussing them, you have to limit yourself to just two or three common species unless you're writing a comprehensive study. Today, I want to talk about our three most common Indian doves: the spotted dove (Turtur suratensis), the Indian ring-dove (Turtur risorius), and the little brown dove (Turtur cambayensis). I won’t apologize for focusing on these common species. I believe that we in India know so little about even our everyday birds that it's a waste of effort to seek out rarer species and study their habits; there’s plenty we can learn about those that visit our porches and coo to us.

The curious distribution of our common Indian doves has not, so far as I know, been explained. In very few places are all three common. One or other of them is usually far more abundant than the others, and this one is usually the spotted dove. It is the commonest dove of Calcutta, of Madras, of Travancore, of Tirhoot, of Lucknow, but not of Lahore or Bombay or the Deccan. Why is this? Why is it that, whereas the Deccan is literally overrun by the ring- and the little brown dove, one can go from Bombay to Malabar without meeting one of these species, but seeing thousands of the spotted dove?

The unusual distribution of our common Indian doves hasn't, as far as I know, been explained. In very few places do all three species coexist. Usually, one of them is much more common than the others, and that one is typically the spotted dove. It's the most common dove in Calcutta, Madras, Travancore, Tirhoot, and Lucknow, but not in Lahore, Bombay, or the Deccan. Why is that? Why is the Deccan overflowing with ring doves and little brown doves, yet you can travel from Bombay to Malabar without encountering either of those species, while seeing thousands of spotted doves?

The only explanation that I can offer of this phenomenon is that the spotted dove is the most pugnacious and the most pushing; that where he chooses to settle down he ousts the other species of dove more or less completely; but he, fortunately for the other species, does not choose to settle down in all parts of India. He objects to dry places. Hence he is not seen at Lahore or in the Deccan, or in the drier parts of the United Provinces, such as Agra, Muttra, Etawah, and Cawnpore.

The only explanation I can give for this situation is that the spotted dove is the most aggressive and assertive species. Wherever it chooses to nest, it tends to push out other types of doves pretty completely. Luckily for the other species, it doesn’t settle in every part of India. It doesn’t like dry areas, so you won’t find it in Lahore, the Deccan, or the drier regions of the United Provinces like Agra, Muttra, Etawah, and Cawnpore.

This is only a theory of mine, and a theory in favour of which I am not able to adduce very much evidence, [126] since my personal knowledge of India is confined to some half-a-dozen widely separated places. Moreover, this theory does not explain the absence of the spotted dove from Bombay. I should be very glad to know if there are any other moist parts of India where the spotted dove is not the most abundant of the cooing family.

This is just a theory of mine, and I don't have a lot of evidence to support it, [126] since my personal experience with India is limited to a few widely scattered locations. Also, this theory doesn't explain why the spotted dove is missing from Bombay. I would love to know if there are any other humid areas in India where the spotted dove isn't the most common bird in the cooing family.

The nest of the dove is a subject over which most ornithologists have waxed sarcastic. A more ramshackle structure does not exist; yet the absurd thing is that doves are most particular about the materials they use.

The dove's nest is something that most bird watchers have joked about. There's no more haphazard structure out there, yet the ridiculous thing is that doves are really picky about the materials they choose.

The other day I watched, with much amusement, a little brown dove at work nest building. It was constructing a shake-down in a small Lonicera bush. Now, obviously, since the nest is just a few twigs and stalks thrown together, any kind of short twig or stem will serve for building material. This, however, was not the view of the dove. If that creature had been constructing the Forth Bridge it could not have been more particular as regards the materials it picked up. It strutted about the ground, taking into its bill all manner of material only to reject it, until at last it picked up a dead grass stalk and flew off with it in triumph!

The other day, I watched with a lot of amusement as a little brown dove worked on building its nest. It was setting up a cozy spot in a small honeysuckle bush. Clearly, since the nest was just a handful of twigs and stems thrown together, almost any short twig or stem would work as building material. However, that wasn’t the dove’s perspective. If it were constructing the Forth Bridge, it couldn't have been more selective about the materials it gathered. It strutted around on the ground, picking up all sorts of materials only to toss them aside, until finally, it grabbed a dead grass stalk and flew off with it, looking triumphant!

Presumably doves take the same trouble in selecting a site for their nest, nevertheless they sometimes eventually choose the most impossible spot. Thus Mr. A. Anderson has recorded the existence of a nest of a pair of little brown doves that “was placed close to the fringe of the kunnaut of his tent on one of the corner ropes, where it is double for some six inches [127] and there knotted. The double portion was just broad enough, being three inches apart, to support the nest with careful balancing; the knot acted as a sort of buffer and prevented the twigs from sliding off, which most assuredly would otherwise have been the case, for the rope just there was at an angle of 45°.”

Presumably, doves go through a lot of effort to pick a spot for their nest. Still, they sometimes end up choosing the most unlikely place. For instance, Mr. A. Anderson noted a nest of a pair of little brown doves that “was placed close to the edge of the kunnaut of his tent on one of the corner ropes, where it doubles up for about six inches [127] and is knotted. The doubled section was just wide enough, being three inches apart, to support the nest with careful balancing; the knot acted like a sort of buffer and kept the twigs from sliding off, which definitely would have happened otherwise since the rope there was at a 45° angle.”

Those foolish birds were not permitted to bring up their young, because the tent had to be struck before the eggs were laid.

Those silly birds weren't allowed to raise their young because the tent had to be taken down before the eggs were laid.

In Lahore a favourite nesting site for the little brown dove is on the top of the rolled-up portion of the verandah chik. As the chik is composed of stout material, the rolled-up portion forms an excellent platform some four inches broad. But as the doves nest just as the weather is beginning to grow warm, the little home is apt to be somewhat rudely broken up. One pair, however, has this year successfully reared up two young hopefuls in a nest on this somewhat precarious site. The doings of these form the subject of the next article.

In Lahore, a favorite nesting spot for the little brown dove is on top of the rolled-up part of the verandah chik. Since the chik is made of sturdy material, the rolled-up section provides a great platform about four inches wide. However, since doves typically nest as the weather starts to warm up, their little home is likely to be disturbed. This year, though, one pair has successfully raised two young ones in a nest at this somewhat unstable location. Their activities will be the focus of the next article.

I once came across a nest of this little dove in a low, prickly bush beside a small canal distributory, three miles outside Lahore. The dove appeared to have used as the foundation for its nest an old one of the striated bush babbler (Argya caudata). (I object to calling this bird the common babbler, since, like common sense, it is not very common.) In the same bush, at the same level, that is to say, about a yard from the ground and only a couple of feet from the dove’s nest, was that of a striated bush babbler containing three dark blue eggs. This is a case upon which those who believe that eggs [128] laid in open nests are protectively coloured would do well to ponder.

I once found a nest of this little dove in a low, prickly bush next to a small canal about three miles outside of Lahore. It seemed like the dove used an old nest of the striated bush babbler (Argya caudata) as the base for its own. (I don't like calling this bird the common babbler, since, like common sense, it's not really that common.) In the same bush, at the same height—around a yard off the ground and just a couple of feet from the dove’s nest—was a striated bush babbler’s nest that had three dark blue eggs in it. This is a situation that those who think that eggs laid in open nests are colored for protection should really think about.

There, side by side, in precisely the same environment, were two nests—one containing white and the other dark blue eggs. Obviously both sets of eggs could not be protectively coloured; as a matter of fact, both clutches of eggs were conspicuous objects. It not infrequently happens that the Indian robin (Thamnobia cambayensis), which lays white eggs thickly spotted with reddish brown, brings up a family in a disused nest of a striated bush babbler’s. The eggs of this latter are dark blue. It is surely time that zoologists gave up throwing at us their everlasting theory of protective colouring. If this were a sine qua non of the safety of birds’ eggs, then the whole dove tribe would, long ago, have ceased to exist.

There, side by side, in exactly the same setting, were two nests—one with white eggs and the other with dark blue eggs. Clearly, both sets of eggs couldn't be designed for camouflage; in fact, both clutches were very noticeable. It often happens that the Indian robin (Thamnobia cambayensis), which lays white eggs marked with reddish brown spots, raises its young in an abandoned nest of a striated bush babbler’s, whose eggs are dark blue. It’s about time zoologists stopped insisting on their never-ending theory of protective coloring. If this were really essential for the safety of bird eggs, then the entire dove species would have disappeared long ago.

This family presents the ornithologist with yet another problem in colouration. In every species, except the red turtle-dove (Oenopopelia tranquebarica), both sexes are coloured alike. In this latter, however, there is very pronounced sexual dimorphism. The ruddy wing feathers of the cock enable one to distinguish him at once from his mate and from every other dove. Now the habits of this dove appear to be exactly like those of all other species. It constructs the same kind of nest and in similar situations; why then the sexual dimorphism in this species and in no other species? If the lady rufous turtle-dove likes nice ruddy wings, and thus the red wing has been evolved in the cock bird, why has she too not inherited it? I presume that even the most audacious Neo-Darwinian will not talk about her greater need of protection when [129] sitting on the nest, for if she needs protection, how much more so do her white eggs? Further, it is my belief that the cock bird takes his turn in the incubation.

This family presents the ornithologist with yet another problem in coloration. In every species, except the red turtle-dove (Oenopopelia tranquebarica), both sexes are colored the same. However, in this one, there is a noticeable difference between the sexes. The male's reddish wing feathers make it easy to identify him from his mate and other doves. The habits of this dove seem to be just like those of all other species. It builds the same type of nest in similar locations; so why is there sexual dimorphism in this species and not in others? If the female rufous turtle-dove prefers the nice reddish wings, leading the male to develop this trait, why hasn't she inherited it too? I assume that even the boldest Neo-Darwinian wouldn't argue that she needs more protection while sitting on the nest, because if she needs protection, then how much more do her white eggs? Additionally, I believe that the male takes turns with her during incubation.

It must not be thought that I am needlessly poking fun at modern biologists. I merely desire to call attention to the unsolved problems that confront us on all sides, and to protest against the dogmatism of biology which declares that the Darwinian theory explains the whole of organic nature. As a matter of fact, it seems to me that the field naturalist cannot but feel that natural selection is turning out rather a failure.

It shouldn't be assumed that I'm just making fun of modern biologists. I'm simply trying to highlight the unresolved issues we face from all angles and to challenge the certainty in biology that claims the Darwinian theory explains all of organic nature. In reality, it seems to me that field naturalists can't help but feel that natural selection is proving to be quite a disappointment.

In conclusion, one more word regarding the red turtle-dove. Its distribution has not been carefully worked out, and what we do know of it is not easy to explain. Hume says that it breeds in all parts of India, but is very capriciously distributed, and he is unable to say what kind of country it prefers, and why it is common in one district and rare in a neighbouring one in which all physical conditions appear identical.

In conclusion, one last thing about the red turtle-dove. Its range hasn't been thoroughly studied, and what we do know isn't easy to explain. Hume mentions that it breeds throughout India, but its distribution is quite unpredictable. He can't determine what type of habitat it prefers or why it's common in one area while being rare in a nearby location where all the physical conditions seem the same.

It is very common in the bare, arid, treeless region that surrounds the Sambhur Lake. It is common in some dry, well-cultivated districts, like Etawah, where there are plenty of old mango groves. It is very common in some of the comparatively humid tracts, like Bareilly, and again in the sal jungles of the Kumaun Bhabar and the Nepal Terai. On the other hand, over wide extents of similar country it is scarcely to be seen. Doubtless there is something in its food or manner of life that limits its distribution, but no one has yet been able to make out what this something is.

It is very common in the bare, dry, treeless area that surrounds Sambhur Lake. It’s also typical in some dry, well-farmed regions, like Etawah, where there are many old mango groves. It is quite common in some of the relatively wetter areas, like Bareilly, and again in the sal forests of Kumaun Bhabar and the Nepal Terai. However, in large parts of similar regions, it is hardly seen. There is surely something about its food or way of life that limits its spread, but no one has been able to identify what that is.

[130]


Doves on a balcony

The office building in which for some time past I have rendered service to a paternal government was once a tomb. That it is now an office is evidence of the strict economy practised by the Indian Administration. Since the living require more light than the dead, skylights have been let into the domed roof. In these the brown rock-chat (Cercomela fusca) loves to sit and pour forth his exceedingly sweet little lay, while his spouse sits on four pale blue eggs in a nest on a ledge in a neighbouring sepulchre. But it is not of this bird that I write to-day; I hope to give him an innings at some future date.

The office building where I’ve been working for a while has a history as a tomb. The fact that it’s now used as an office reflects the frugal practices of the Indian Administration. Since the living need more light than the dead, skylights have been added to the domed roof. In these skylights, the brown rock-chat (Cercomela fusca) loves to perch and sing its sweet little song, while its mate sits on four pale blue eggs in a nest on a ledge in a nearby tomb. But that’s not what I’m writing about today; I hope to give that bird some attention in the future.

Two little brown doves (Turtur cambaiensis) first demand our attention, since these for a time appropriated my skylights. This species is smaller than the spotted dove so common in Madras, and, to my way of thinking, is a much more beautiful bird. Its head, neck, and breast are pale lilac washed with red. On each side of the neck the bird carries a miniature chessboard. The remainder of its plumage is brown, passing into grey and white. The legs are lake-red.

Two little brown doves (Turtur cambaiensis) grab our attention first, as they temporarily took over my skylights. This species is smaller than the spotted dove, which is common in Madras, and in my opinion, it's a much more beautiful bird. Its head, neck, and breast are a light lilac with a hint of red. On each side of the neck, the bird has a miniature chessboard pattern. The rest of its feathers are brown, transitioning into gray and white. The legs are a reddish lake color.

It has a very distinctive note—a soft, subdued musical cuk-cuk-coo-coo-coo. There is no bird better pleased with [131] itself than the little brown dove. In the month of March the two doves in question were “carrying on” in my office skylight to such an extent as to leave no doubt that they had a nest somewhere. I discovered it on the rolled-up end of one of the bamboo verandah chiks. These are not let down in the cold weather, so that the doves had been permitted to build undisturbed.

It has a very unique sound—a soft, muted musical cuk-cuk-coo-coo-coo. There's no bird more content with itself than the little brown dove. In March, the two doves in question were “carrying on” in my office skylight to such a degree that it was clear they had a nest somewhere. I found it on the rolled-up end of one of the bamboo verandah chiks. These aren’t taken down in the cold weather, so the doves had been allowed to build undisturbed.

“Eha” has humorously described a dove’s nest as composed of two short sticks and a long one; that of the little brown dove is a little more compact than the typical nest, a little less sketchy, and composed of grass and fine twigs. There was plenty of room for it on the top of the rolled-up portion of the chik.

“Eha” humorously described a dove’s nest as made of two short sticks and one long one; the nest of the little brown dove is a bit more compact than a typical nest, slightly less makeshift, and is made from grass and fine twigs. There was plenty of space for it on top of the rolled-up part of the chik.

When I found the nest there were two white eggs in it. Every species of dove lays but two eggs. I do not know whether the smallness of the clutch has anything to do with the helplessness of the young birds when first hatched. Young doves and pigeons have not, like other baby birds, great mouths which open to an alarming extent. They feed by putting their beaks in the mouth of the parent and there they obtain “pigeon’s milk,” which is a secretion from the crop of the old birds.

When I discovered the nest, it contained two white eggs. Every type of dove lays only two eggs. I'm not sure if the small number of eggs relates to how defenseless the chicks are when they first hatch. Unlike other baby birds, young doves and pigeons don't have large mouths that open wide. They get fed by putting their beaks into their parent's mouth, where they receive "pigeon’s milk," a secretion from the parent's crop.

Being at that time less versed in the ways of the little brown dove than I now am, I was under the impression that this nest was in rather a curious situation, so I determined to obtain a photograph of it with the young birds. I may here say that I dislike photography, and not without cause. Some years ago I visited the Himalayan snows, and dragged up a great camera and a number of plates to an altitude of 12,000 feet. Having no portable dark room, I endured untold [132] agonies while changing the plates under the bedclothes. Being anxious lest the light should reach the exposed negatives, I wrapped them up very carefully, using newspaper, which was the only wrapping available. When I returned from the expedition I developed the plates, but lo and behold! instead of snowy peaks and sunny valleys, advertisements of soaps and pills appeared on the plates. Why do not books on the camera tell one not to wrap up plates in newspaper? I made a vow to leave photography to others, and I kept the vow until I saw those young doves perched so temptingly on the chik.

Being less experienced with the little brown dove back then than I am now, I thought the nest was in a rather unusual spot, so I decided to take a photograph of it with the young birds. I should mention that I don't like photography, and there's a good reason for that. A few years ago, I went to the Himalayan mountains and lugged a heavy camera and several plates up to an altitude of 12,000 feet. Without a portable darkroom, I went through a lot of trouble changing the plates under the bedcovers. Worrying about light reaching the exposed negatives, I wrapped them very carefully in newspaper, which was the only wrapping I had. When I got back from the trip and developed the plates, what did I find? Instead of stunning snowy peaks and bright valleys, I had images of ads for soaps and pills. Why don't photography books warn you not to wrap plates in newspaper? I promised myself I'd leave photography to others, and I stuck to that promise until I saw those young doves sitting so invitingly on the chik.

Having risked both life and limb in mounting a chair placed upon a table, I obtained a “snap” at the nest. On developing the plate everything appeared with admirable clearness except the nest. There was nothing but a blur where this should have been; the rest of the chik came out splendidly. The only explanation of this phenomenon that I can offer is the natural “cussedness” of the camera. I have now renewed my vow to eschew photography.

Having risked my safety by climbing onto a chair on top of a table, I managed to get a quick shot of the nest. When I developed the film, everything came out really clear except for the nest. Instead of a clear image, there was just a blur where the nest should have been; the rest of the image turned out great. The only explanation I can think of for this issue is the natural frustration of photography. I've now made a renewed commitment to avoid taking photos.

The first young doves were successfully reared. No sooner had they been driven forth into the world than the parents set about repairing the nest, for doves are not content with one brood; when once a pair commence nesting there is no knowing when they will stop. As it was then April and the sun was growing uncomfortably hot, the letting down of the chik became a matter of necessity, and this, of course, wrecked the nest. I expected to see no more of the doves. In this I was mistaken. Before long they were billing and cooing as merrily [133] as before. A little search showed that this time they had built a nest on the top of the same chik—a feat which I should have thought impossible had I not seen the nest with my own eyes. Some sacking was attached to the chik, and this, together with the bamboo, presented a surface of about half an inch. On this precarious foundation the nest rested; the twigs, of course, reached over to the wall from which the chik was hung. Thus the nest received some additional support. Needless to say, the young birds had to remain very still or they would have fallen out of the nest.

The first young doves were successfully raised. No sooner had they been sent out into the world than the parents started fixing the nest, because doves aren't satisfied with just one brood; once a pair starts nesting, there's no telling when they'll stop. Since it was April and the sun was getting uncomfortably hot, lowering the chik became necessary, which, of course, messed up the nest. I expected to see the doves gone for good. I was wrong. Before long, they were billing and cooing just as happily as before. A little search revealed that this time they had built a nest on top of the same chik—something I would have thought impossible had I not seen the nest with my own eyes. Some sacking was attached to the chik, and this, along with the bamboo, created a surface of about half an inch. On this shaky foundation, the nest sat; the twigs, of course, reached over to the wall from which the chik was hung. So the nest had some extra support. Needless to say, the young birds had to stay very still, or they would have fallen out of the nest.

The second and the third broods were raised without mishap. One of the birds of the fourth family was more restless than his brethren had been; consequently he fell off the nest on to the floor of the verandah. He was picked up and brought to me. Although not strong enough to walk, or even stand, he showed unmistakable signs of that evil temper which characterises all doves, by opening his wings and pecking savagely at my hand. In spite of this behaviour I set natural selection at naught by putting him back into the nest. He fell out again next day and was again replaced. This time he stayed there, and is now probably at large.

The second and third broods were raised without any problems. One of the birds from the fourth brood was more restless than his siblings had been; as a result, he fell off the nest and landed on the floor of the verandah. He was picked up and brought to me. Even though he wasn't strong enough to walk or stand, he clearly showed signs of the bad temper that all doves have by flapping his wings and pecking aggressively at my hand. Despite this behavior, I ignored natural selection and put him back in the nest. He fell out again the next day and was replaced once more. This time he stayed there and is probably out and about now.

When the fifth clutch of eggs was in the nest my chaprassi, who, since I have shown him how to play cuckoo, has been upsetting the domestic affairs of any number of birds, asked whether he might substitute two pigeon’s eggs for those laid by the dove. The substitution was duly effected without rousing any suspicions on the part of the doves. The young pigeons soon hatched out and were industriously fed by their foster-parents, [134] nor did these latter appear to notice anything unusual when the white plumage of the pigeons appeared. Two days before the changelings were ready to fly a terrific storm arose and so shook the chiks that the poor pigeons were thrown off and killed. Nothing daunted, the doves have since successfully reared a sixth family! Can we wonder that doves are numerous in India?

When the fifth clutch of eggs was in the nest, my chaprassi, who I taught to play cuckoo, has been messing with the family dynamics of various birds, asked if he could swap out two pigeon eggs for the ones laid by the dove. The swap was done smoothly without raising any suspicions from the doves. The young pigeons soon hatched and were actively fed by their foster parents, [134] and these parents didn’t seem to notice anything strange when the white feathers of the pigeons showed up. Two days before the changelings were ready to fly, a huge storm hit and shook the chiks so hard that the poor pigeons fell out and were killed. Undeterred, the doves have since successfully raised a sixth family! Can we be surprised that there are so many doves in India?

[135]


The Golden Oriole

Dame Nature must have been in a very generous mood when she manufactured golden orioles, or she would never have expended so much of her colour-box upon them. Orioles are birds which compel our attention, so brilliant are they; yet the poets who profess to be the high-priests of Nature give us no songs about these beautiful creatures; at least I know of no maker of verse, with the exception of Sir Edwin Arnold, who does more than mention the oriole. Here then is a fine opening for some twentieth-century bard!

Dame Nature must have been feeling really generous when she created golden orioles, or she wouldn't have used so much of her color palette on them. Orioles are birds that catch our eye because they’re so bright; yet the poets who claim to be the true voice of Nature hardly write about these beautiful creatures. At least, I don’t know of any poet, except for Sir Edwin Arnold, who does more than mention the oriole. So here’s a perfect opportunity for some modern-day poet!

Two orioles, or mango birds as they are sometimes called, are common in India. They are the Indian oriole (Oriolus kundoo) and the black-headed oriole (O. melanocephalus). The Indian oriole is a bird about the size of a starling. The plumage of the cock is a splendid rich yellow. There is a black patch over and behind the eye. There is some black on the tail, and the large wing feathers are also of this colour. The bill is pink and the eyes red. In the hen the yellow of the back is deeply tinged with green.

Two orioles, also known as mango birds, are commonly found in India. They are the Indian oriole (Oriolus kundoo) and the black-headed oriole (O. melanocephalus). The Indian oriole is about the size of a starling. The male has bright, rich yellow plumage. There's a black patch over and behind the eye, some black on the tail, and the large wing feathers are also black. The beak is pink, and the eyes are red. In the female, the yellow on the back has a strong greenish tint.

The black-headed oriole may be distinguished by his black head, throat, and upper breast. The habits of both species are similar in every respect. The [136] Indian oriole seems to be merely a winter visitor to Madras, and it is seen in the Punjab only during the hot weather. In the intervening parts it may be observed all the year round; hence the species would appear to perform a small annual migration, leaving the South in the hot weather. In those parts where orioles are found all the year round it is not improbable that the birds one sees in the winter are not those that are observed during the summer.

The black-headed oriole can be recognized by its black head, throat, and upper chest. The behaviors of both species are similar in every way. The [136] Indian oriole seems to be just a winter visitor to Madras, and it's spotted in Punjab only during the hot season. In other areas, it can be seen throughout the year; therefore, it seems that this species undertakes a small annual migration, leaving the South when the heat sets in. In regions where orioles are present all year long, it's quite possible that the birds seen in winter are not the same ones observed during summer.

The oriole is essentially a bird of the greenwood tree; if you would see him you should betake yourself to some well-irrigated orchard. I have never seen an oriole on the ground; its habits are strictly arboreal, but it does not seem to be at all particular about taking cover. It perches by preference on the topmost bough of a tree, and if this bough be devoid of leaves, so much the better, for the bird enjoys a more extensive view of the surrounding country. Very beautiful does such a bird look, sitting outlined against the sky, as the first rays of the morning sun fall upon and add fresh lustre to its golden plumage. Orioles feed upon both fruit and insects, and so cannot be regarded as unmixed blessings to the agriculturalist.

The oriole is basically a bird of the green tree; if you want to see one, you should head to a well-watered orchard. I've never seen an oriole on the ground; it strictly lives in trees, but it doesn't seem to mind being out in the open. It prefers to sit on the highest branch of a tree, and if that branch is bare, that's even better, as the bird likes to have a wide view of the area. It looks stunning perched against the sky, especially when the first rays of the morning sun shine on it and make its golden feathers sparkle. Orioles eat both fruit and insects, so they aren't entirely good news for farmers.

As I have already said, Dame Nature has been exceedingly kind to this bird; not content with decking him out in brilliantly coloured raiment, she has endowed him with a voice of which any bird might well be proud. It is a clear, mellow whistle, which is usually syllabised as peeho, peeho, or lorio, lorio; indeed, the name oriole is probably onomatopoetic. In addition to this the bird has several other notes. [137] These are not pleasant to the ear and may be described as blends, in varying proportions, of the harsh call of the king-crow and the miau of a cat. The hen almost invariably utters such a note when a human being approaches the nest; but the cry apparently does not always denote alarm, for I have heard an oriole uttering it when sitting placidly in a tree, seemingly at peace with all the world; but perhaps that particular bird may have been indulging in unpleasant day dreams; who knows?

As I mentioned before, Mother Nature has been extremely generous to this bird; not only has she dressed him in strikingly vibrant colors, but she's also gifted him with a voice that any bird would be proud of. It's a clear, rich whistle, often pronounced as peeho, peeho, or lorio, lorio; in fact, the name oriole likely comes from this sound. Besides this, the bird has several other notes. [137] These aren’t very pleasant and can be described as a mix, in different amounts, of the harsh call of the king crow and a cat's miau. The female almost always makes this noise when a human gets close to the nest; however, it doesn't always seem to signal alarm, as I've heard an oriole making it while comfortably perched in a tree, seemingly at peace with everything around it; but maybe that particular bird was just caught up in some unpleasant daydreams; who knows?

We hear much of the marvellous nests of tailor- and weaver-birds, but never of that of the oriole. Naturalists, equally with poets, have neglected this beautiful species. An oriole’s nest is in its way quite as wonderful as that of the tailor-bird. If a man were ordered to erect a cradle up in a tree, he would, I imagine, construct it precisely as the oriole does its nest. This last is a cup-shaped structure slung on to two or three branches of a tree by means of fibres which are wound first round one branch, then passed under the nest, and finally wound round another bough. The nest is therefore, as Hume pointed out, secured to its supporting branches in much the same way as a prawn net is to its wooden framework.

We hear a lot about the amazing nests of tailor and weaver birds, but not much about the oriole. Naturalists, just like poets, have overlooked this beautiful species. An oriole's nest is just as remarkable in its own way as that of the tailor bird. If a person were asked to build a cradle high up in a tree, they would probably construct it just like the oriole does its nest. The oriole's nest is a cup-shaped structure that hangs from two or three tree branches using fibers that are wrapped first around one branch, then passed underneath the nest, and finally wrapped around another branch. As Hume pointed out, the nest is secured to its supporting branches in a way that's quite similar to how a prawn net is attached to its wooden frame.

In places where there are mulberry trees the oriole shaves off narrow strips of the thin, pliable bark and uses these to support the nest. Jerdon describes one wonderful nest, taken by him at Saugor, that was suspended by a long roll of cloth about three-quarters of an inch wide, which the bird must have pilfered from some neighbouring verandah. “This strip,” he states, [138] “was wound round each limb of the fork, then passed round the nest beneath, fixed to the other limb, and again brought round the nest to the opposite side; there were four or five of these supports on either side.” The nest was so securely fixed that it could not have been removed till the supporting bands had been cut or had rotted away. Here then is an example of workmanship which the modern jerry-builder might well emulate.

In places with mulberry trees, the oriole cuts narrow strips of the thin, flexible bark to use as support for its nest. Jerdon describes a remarkable nest he found in Saugor that was suspended by a long strip of cloth about three-quarters of an inch wide, which the bird must have stolen from a nearby verandah. “This strip,” he says, [138] “was wrapped around each limb of the fork, then passed around the nest underneath, secured to the other limb, and then wrapped around the nest to the opposite side; there were four or five of these supports on each side.” The nest was so securely attached that it couldn't be removed until the supporting strips were cut or rotted away. Here is a great example of craftsmanship that modern builders could learn from.

I have made repeated attempts to see orioles at work on the supports of the nest, but so far have only managed to observe them lining it. Upon one occasion I came upon a nest some fifteen feet from the ground from which hung two strips of fibre about sixteen inches long that had been wound round one branch. I waited for some time, hoping the birds would return and allow me to see them finish the adjustment of these fibres; but unfortunately there was no cover available, and the oriole is an exceedingly shy bird; it will not do anything to the nest if it knows it is being watched.

I’ve tried several times to watch orioles building their nests, but so far, I’ve only seen them lining the nest. One time, I found a nest about fifteen feet off the ground with two strips of fiber hanging down, each about sixteen inches long, wrapped around a branch. I waited for a while, hoping the birds would come back and let me see them finish arranging the fibers, but unfortunately, there was no hiding spot, and orioles are really shy. They won’t do anything to the nest if they know they’re being watched.

The completed nursery, viewed from below, looks like a ball of dried grass wedged into the fork of a branch, and may easily be mistaken for that of a king-crow, but this last is, of course, not bound to the branches like that of the oriole.

The finished nest, seen from below, resembles a ball of dried grass stuck in a branch fork, and could easily be confused with one made by a king-crow, but unlike the oriole’s nest, this one isn’t tied to the branches.

A very curious thing that I have noticed about the Indian oriole’s nest is that it is always situated either in the same tree as a king-crow’s nest or in an adjacent tree. I have seen some thirteen or fourteen orioles’ nests since I first noticed this phenomenon, and have, in every case, found a king-crow’s nest within ten yards. [139] The drongo builds earlier, for it is usually feeding its young while the oriole is incubating. It would therefore appear that it is the oriole which elects to build near the king-crow. I imagine that it does so for the sake of protection; it must be a great thing for a timid bird to have a vigorous policeman all to itself, a policeman who will not allow a big creature to approach under any pretext whatever.

I've noticed something really interesting about the Indian oriole's nest: it's always found either in the same tree as a king-crow's nest or in a nearby tree. Since I first saw this, I've spotted around thirteen or fourteen oriole nests, and in every case, there's been a king-crow's nest within ten yards. [139] The drongo builds its nest earlier because it is usually feeding its young while the oriole is still incubating. So, it seems like the oriole chooses to build close to the king-crow. I think it does this for protection; it must be really helpful for a timid bird to have such a strong protector nearby, a protector that won't let any large creature come close under any circumstances.

The oriole lays from two to four white eggs spotted with reddish brown. These spots readily wash off, and sometimes the colour “runs” and gives the whole egg a pink hue. Although both sexes take part in the construction of the nursery, the work of incubation appears to fall entirely upon the hen. I have never seen a cock oriole sitting on the nest.

The oriole lays two to four white eggs with reddish-brown spots. These spots can easily be washed off, and sometimes the color bleeds a bit, giving the whole egg a pinkish tint. While both males and females help build the nest, it seems that only the female does the incubating. I’ve never seen a male oriole sitting on the nest.

[140]


The Barn Owl

The barn owl is a cosmopolitan bird. It is an adaptive species, and so has been able to make itself at home all the world over. Like every widely distributed species, including man, it has its local peculiarities. The barn owls of India are somewhat different from those of Africa, and these latter, again, may be readily distinguished from those that dwell in Europe. This any one may see for himself by paying a visit to the Zoological Gardens at Regent’s Park, where barn owls from all parts of the world blink out their lives in neighbouring cages. Needless to say, species-mongers have tried to magnify these local peculiarities into specific differences. The European bird is known as Strix flammea. An attempt was made to differentiate the Indian barn owl. If you look up the bird in Jerdon’s classical work you will see that it is called Strix javanica. Jerdon’s justification for making a new species of it was its larger size, more robust feet and toes, and the presence of spots on the lower plumage. If such were specific differences we ought to divide up man, Homo sapiens, into quite a large number of species: Homo major, H. minor, H. longirostris, H. brevirostris, etc.

The barn owl is a bird found all over the world. It’s a versatile species, allowing it to thrive in various environments everywhere. Like other widely spread species, including humans, it has its local quirks. The barn owls in India are somewhat different from those in Africa, and these, in turn, can be easily recognized from the ones living in Europe. Anyone can see this for themselves by visiting the Zoological Gardens at Regent’s Park, where barn owls from all around the globe live in nearby cages. Of course, those keen on categorizing species have tried to elevate these local differences into distinct species. The European bird is known as Strix flammea. An effort was made to classify the Indian barn owl separately. If you check Jerdon’s classic work, you’ll find it labeled as Strix javanica. Jerdon’s reason for creating a new species was its larger size, sturdier feet and toes, and the spots on its lower feathers. If these were true species differences, we would need to classify humans, Homo sapiens, into many different species: Homo major, H. minor, H. longirostris, H. brevirostris, etc.

[141]

However, neither with the barn owl nor with man has the species-maker had his own way. Ornithologists recognise but one barn owl. This bird, which is frequently called the screech owl, is delightfully easy to describe. Everybody knows an owl when he sees one; but stay, I forgot the German Professor, mentioned by Mr. Bosworth Smith, who held up in triumph the owl which he had shot, saying: “Zee, I have shot von schnipe mit einem face Push cat.” Let me therefore say it is easy enough for the average man to recognise an owl, but it is quite another matter when it comes to “spotting” the species to which an individual happens to belong. As a rule the family likeness is so strong as to overshadow specific differences. The barn owl, however, differs from all others in that it has a long, thin face. Take any common or garden owl, and you will observe that it has a round, plum-pudding-like head. Place that owl before one of those mirrors which make everything look long and thin, and you will see in the glass a very fair representation of the barn owl. The face of this owl, when it is awake, is heart-shaped; when the bird is asleep it is as long as that of a junior Madras Civil Servant as he looks over the Civil List. Whether awake or asleep, the bird has an uncanny, half-human look. It is innocent of the “ears” or “horns” which form so conspicuous a feature of some owls. In passing, I may say that those horn-like tufts of feathers have no connection with the well-developed auditory organ of the owl.

However, neither with the barn owl nor with humans has the creator had their way. Ornithologists recognize only one barn owl. This bird, often called the screech owl, is really easy to describe. Everyone knows an owl when they see one; but hold on, I forgot about the German professor mentioned by Mr. Bosworth Smith, who proudly displayed the owl he had shot, saying: “See, I have shot a bird with a face like a push cat.” So, while it’s pretty easy for the average person to recognize an owl, figuring out the species to which an individual belongs is a whole different story. Usually, the family resemblance is so strong that it overshadows the specific differences. The barn owl, however, stands out because it has a long, thin face. Take any typical owl, and you’ll notice it has a round, pudding-like head. Put that owl in front of a funhouse mirror that stretches things out, and you’ll see a pretty good representation of the barn owl. The face of this owl, when it’s awake, is heart-shaped; when the bird is asleep, it looks as elongated as a junior Madras Civil Servant peering over the Civil List. Whether it’s awake or asleep, this bird has a strange, almost human expression. It lacks the “ears” or “horns” that are such a prominent feature of some owls. By the way, those horn-like feather tufts have no connection to the well-developed hearing organs of the owl.

The barn owl’s face is white, as is its lower plumage, hence it is popularly known in England as the white [142] owl. The back and upper plumage are pale grey. The tail is buff, and there is a good deal of buff scattered about the rest of the plumage; it is on this account that the bird is called flammea.

The barn owl has a white face and a white underside, which is why it's commonly called the white owl in England. Its back and upper feathers are light gray. The tail is a tan color, and there are many tan spots throughout its other feathers; that's why the bird is named flammea.

The barn owl is, I believe, common in all parts of India, but it is not often seen owing to its strictly nocturnal habits. It ventures not forth into the dazzling light of day as does that noisy little clown, the spotted owlet (Athene brama). Should it happen to be abroad in daylight the crows make its life a burden. Friend Corvus is a very conservative individual. He sets his face steadfastly against any addition to the local fauna. As he seldom or never sees the barn owl, he does not include it among the birds of his locality; so that when one does show its face, the crows proceed to mob it. Their efforts are well seconded by the small fry among birds, who seem instinctively to dislike the whole owl tribe.

The barn owl is, I think, common throughout India, but it's not often spotted because it’s strictly nocturnal. It doesn't come out in the bright light of day like that noisy little character, the spotted owlet (Athene brama). If it happens to be out in the daytime, the crows make its life difficult. The crow is quite a traditional creature. It firmly opposes any newcomers in the local wildlife. Since it rarely or never sees the barn owl, it doesn't recognize it as part of its local bird community. So, when one does appear, the crows swarm it. Their efforts are supported by the smaller birds that seem to instinctively dislike all owls.

During the day the barn owl sleeps placidly in the interior of a decayed tree, or in a tomb, mosque, temple, or ruin, or even in the secluded verandah of a bungalow. The last place of abode is unsatisfactory from the point of view of the owl, for Indian servants display an antipathy towards it quite as great as that shown by the crows. They believe that the owls bring bad luck, and are in this respect not one whit more foolish than ignorant folk in other parts of the world. This useful and amusing bird is everywhere regarded with superstitious dread by the uneducated.

During the day, the barn owl sleeps peacefully inside a decayed tree, or in a tomb, mosque, temple, ruin, or even on the quiet porch of a bungalow. The last spot isn’t ideal for the owl because Indian servants have a strong dislike for it, similar to that of the crows. They believe that owls bring bad luck, and in this regard, they aren’t any more foolish than ignorant people in other parts of the world. This useful and entertaining bird is seen with superstitious fear by the uneducated everywhere.

It lives almost exclusively on rats, mice, shrews, and other enemies of the farmer. And as an exceptional [143] case it will take a young bird, which is usually a sparrow. Most people will agree that we can spare a few sparrows; nevertheless, that cruel idiot, the gamekeeper, classes the barn owl as vermin and shoots it whenever he has the chance. This is fairly often, owing to the confiding habits of the creature. It will enter a bungalow after rats or moths, and will sometimes terrify the timid sleeper by sitting on the end of his bed and screaming at him!

It primarily feeds on rats, mice, shrews, and other pests that trouble farmers. As a rare exception, it might catch a young bird, usually a sparrow. Most people would agree that we can afford to lose a few sparrows; however, that cruel fool, the gamekeeper, considers the barn owl a pest and shoots it whenever he gets the chance. This happens fairly often, due to the trusting nature of the owl. It will come into a house looking for rats or moths, and can sometimes scare a frightened sleeper by sitting on the edge of their bed and screaming at them!

The owl is blessed with an appetite that would do credit to an alderman. Lord Lilford states that he saw “a young half-grown barn owl take down nine full-grown mice, one after another, until the tail of the ninth stuck out of his mouth, and in three hours’ time was crying for more.” Let me anticipate the captious critic by saying that it was the owl and not the tail of the ninth mouse that, like Oliver Twist, called for more. Moreover, the tail did not, as might be supposed, stick out because the bird was “full up inside.” The barn owl invariably swallows a mouse head first; it makes a mighty gulp, with the result that the whole of the mouse, except the tail, disappears. Thus the victim remains for a short time in order that the owl may enjoy the bonne bouche. Then the tail disappears suddenly, and the curtain is rung down on the first act of the tragedy. The second and third acts are like unto the first. The last act is not very polite, but it must be described in the interests of science. After an interval of a few hours the owl throws up, in the form of a pellet, the bones, fur, and other undigestible portions of his victims. This is, of course, very bad manners, but it is the inevitable [144] result of bolting a victim whole. One vice, alas! leads to another.

The owl has an appetite that would impress even a city official. Lord Lilford mentions that he saw “a young half-grown barn owl take down nine full-grown mice, one after another, until the tail of the ninth was sticking out of its mouth, and in three hours, it was crying for more.” I want to get ahead of any picky critics by clarifying that it was the owl, not the tail of the ninth mouse, that, like Oliver Twist, asked for more. Also, the tail didn’t stick out because the bird was “stuffed.” The barn owl always swallows a mouse head first; it takes a big gulp, resulting in the entire mouse disappearing except for the tail. The mouse lingers for a short while so the owl can enjoy the bonne bouche. Then the tail vanishes suddenly, signaling the end of the first act of the tragedy. The second and third acts are just like the first. The final act isn’t very polite, but it has to be described for the sake of science. After a few hours, the owl spits out, in the form of a pellet, the bones, fur, and other indigestible parts of its prey. This is quite rude, but it’s the unavoidable outcome of gulping down a victim whole. One bad habit, unfortunately, leads to another.

Kingfishers, which swallow whole fish, likewise eject the bones. This habit of the owl has enabled zoologists to disprove the contention of the gamekeeper that the barn owl lives chiefly upon young pheasants. The bones found in these pellets are nearly all those of small rodents.

Kingfishers, which swallow whole fish, also spit out the bones. This behavior of the owl has helped zoologists disprove the gamekeeper's claim that the barn owl mainly feeds on young pheasants. The bones found in these pellets are mostly those of small rodents.

The screech owl, as its name implies, is not a great songster. It hisses, snores, and utters, during flight, blood-curdling screams, which doubtless account for its evil reputation. It lays roundish white eggs in a hole in a tree or other convenient cavity. Three, four, or six are laid, according to taste. I have never found the eggs in India, but they are, in England at any rate, laid, not in rapid succession, but at considerable intervals, so that one may find, side by side in a nest, eggs and young birds of various ages. I do not know whether the owl derives any benefit from this curious habit. It has been suggested that the wily creature makes the first nestling which hatches out do some of the incubating. Pranks of this kind are all very well when the nest is hidden away in a hole; they would not do in an open nest to which crows and other birds of that feather have access.

The screech owl, as its name suggests, isn't a great singer. It hisses, snores, and lets out terrifying screams while flying, which likely contributes to its bad reputation. It lays round white eggs in a tree hole or any suitable space. Depending on the owl, three, four, or six eggs are laid. I've never found the eggs in India, but in England at least, they aren't laid all at once; rather, there's a significant gap between them, so you might find eggs and young birds of different ages in the same nest. I'm not sure if the owl benefits from this unusual habit. It's been suggested that the clever bird makes the first hatchling help with incubating. These tricks work well when the nest is tucked away in a hole; they wouldn't work in an open nest accessible to crows and other similar birds.

THE COMMON KINGFISHER. (ALCEDO ISPIDA)

THE COMMON KINGFISHER. (ALCEDO ISPIDA)
(One of the British birds found in India)

THE COMMON KINGFISHER (ALCEDO ISPIDA)
(One of the British birds that can also be found in India)

[145]


A Tree-Top Tragedy

If I were a bird I would give the Indian crow a very wide berth, and, whenever I did come into unavoidable contact with him, I should behave towards him with the most marked civility. A clannishness prevails among crows which makes them nasty enemies to tackle. If you insult one of the “treble-dated” birds you find that the whole of the corvi of the neighbourhood resent that insult as if it had been addressed to each and every one individually, and if you get back nothing more than your insult plus very liberal interest, you are indeed lucky. In the same way, crows will revenge an injury tenfold. The eye-for-an-eye doctrine does not satisfy them; for an eye they want at least a pair of eyes, to say nothing of a complete set of teeth. I recently witnessed an example of what crows are capable of doing by way of revenge.

If I were a bird, I'd definitely avoid the Indian crow, and whenever I had to interact with one, I'd treat it with the utmost politeness. Crows have a tight-knit community that makes them terrible enemies to deal with. If you offend one of these "treble-dated" birds, the entire local flock will take that offense personally, as if it were aimed at each of them individually. If you walk away with just the original insult returned, plus a hefty dose of retaliation, then you’re really lucky. Similarly, crows will repay a wrong tenfold. The eye-for-an-eye philosophy isn't enough for them; for an eye, they want at least two eyes, not to mention a full set of teeth. Recently, I saw a clear example of what crows are capable of when it comes to revenge.

A couple of kites built high up in a lofty tree the clumsy platform of sticks which we dignify by the name “nest.” This was furnished, soon after its completion, by a clutch of three straw-coloured eggs, handsomely blotched with red.

A pair of kites built a clumsy platform of sticks high up in a tall tree, which we refer to as a "nest." Soon after it was finished, it was filled with three straw-colored eggs, beautifully marked with red spots.

The ugliest birds seem to lay the most beautiful eggs; this is perhaps the compensation which Dame Nature gives them for their own lack of comeliness.

The ugliest birds seem to lay the most beautiful eggs; this might be the way Mother Nature compensates them for their own lack of beauty.

[146]

The kite is a very close sitter. Like the crow, she knoweth the wickedness of her own heart, and as she judges others by herself, deems it necessary to continually mount guard over her eggs. Patience eventually meets with its reward. Three weeks of steady sitting result in the appearance of the young kites.

The kite is very protective when nesting. Like the crow, she knows the darkness of her own heart, and because she judges others by her own standards, she feels it's essential to constantly keep watch over her eggs. Patience eventually pays off. Three weeks of steadfast sitting leads to the hatching of the young kites.

This long and patient sitting on the part of parent birds is, when one comes to think of it, a most remarkable phenomenon. No sooner do the eggs appear in the nest than the most active little bird seems to lose all its activity and become quite sedentary in its habits. Take, for example, the sprightly white-browed fantail flycatcher (Rhipidura albifrontata), a bird which ordinarily seems to have St. Vitus’s dance in every organ and appendage. This species will, when it has eggs, sit as closely or more closely than a barndoor hen, and will sometimes allow you to stroke it. I often wonder what are the feelings of such a bird when incubating. One is tempted to think that it must find the process intensely boring. But this cannot be so, or it would refuse to sit. The fowls of the air are not hampered by the Ten Commandments; they are free to do that to which the spirit moveth them, without let or hindrance, without fear of arrest or prosecution for breach of the law. Hence birds must positively enjoy sitting on their eggs. At the brooding season avine nature undergoes a complete change. Ordinarily a bird delights to expend its ebullient energy in vigorous motion, just as a strong man delights to run a race; but at the nesting season its inclinations change; then its greatest joy is to sit upon its nest. Even as human beings are suddenly [147] seized with the Bridge craze and are then perfectly content to sit for hours at the card table, so at certain seasons are birds overcome by the incubating mania. If my view of the matter be correct, and I think it must be, a sitting bird is no more an object for our pity than is a Bridge maniac. But this is a digression.

This long and patient sitting by parent birds is, when you think about it, a truly remarkable phenomenon. As soon as the eggs appear in the nest, the usually active little bird seems to lose all its energy and becomes quite sedentary in its habits. Take, for instance, the lively white-browed fantail flycatcher (Rhipidura albifrontata), a bird that typically seems to have endless energy. This species will sit as closely as, or even closer than, a barnyard hen when it has eggs and will sometimes let you stroke it. I often wonder what such a bird feels while incubating. One might think that it finds the process extremely boring. But that can't be the case, or it would refuse to sit. Birds are not constrained by the Ten Commandments; they can follow their instincts without any restrictions, without the fear of being arrested or prosecuted for breaking the law. So, birds must genuinely enjoy sitting on their eggs. During the brooding season, a bird's nature undergoes a complete transformation. Typically, a bird loves to expend its energy through active motion, just as a strong person enjoys running a race; but during nesting season, its preferences shift; at that time, its greatest pleasure is sitting on its nest. Just as people can suddenly get into the Bridge craze and happily spend hours at the card table, birds too are taken over by the incubating instinct at certain times of the year. If my interpretation is correct, and I believe it is, a sitting bird deserves no more pity than a Bridge enthusiast. But that’s a side note.

Let us hie back to our kite and her family of young ones in their lofty nursery. For a time all went well with them. But one day the sun of prosperity which had hitherto shone upon them became darkened by great black clouds of adversity. I happened to pass the nest at this time and saw about twenty excited crows squatting on branches near the nest and cawing angrily. The mother kite was flying round and round in circles, and was evidently sorely troubled in spirit. She had done something to offend the crows. Ere long she returned to her nest, whereupon the crows took to their wings, cawing more vociferously than ever. As soon as the kite had settled on the nest they again alighted on branches of the tree, and, each from a respectful distance, gave what the natives of Upper India call gali galoj. She tolerated for a time their vulgar abuse, then left the nest. This was the signal for all the crows to take to their wings. Some of them tried to attack her in the air. For a few minutes I watched them chasing her. After a little the attack began to flag, I, therefore, came to the conclusion that the corvi were recovering their mental equilibrium, and that the whole affair would quickly fizzle out, as such incidents usually do. Accordingly, I went on my way. Returning an hour later, I was surprised to find the crows still [148] engaged in the attack. Moreover, the kite was not visible and the crows had grown bolder, for whereas previously they had abused the kite from a safe distance, some of them were now quite close to the nest. Being pressed for time, I was not able to stay and await developments. In the afternoon when I again passed the nest I saw no kite, but the tree was alive with crows, and part of the nest appeared to have been pulled down. The nestlings had probably been destroyed. Of this I was not able to make certain, for I was on my way to fulfil a social engagement. I was, I admit, sorely tempted to “cut” this, and nothing but the want of a good excuse prevented my doing so. “Dear Mrs. Burra Mem, I much regret that I was prevented from coming to your tennis party this afternoon by a domestic bereavement—of a kite,” seemed rather unconvincing, so I went to the lawn-tennis party.

Let’s go back to our kite and her little ones in their high-up nest. For a while, everything was going well for them. But one day, the bright sun of good fortune that had been shining on them was overshadowed by dark clouds of trouble. I happened to pass by the nest at that moment and saw about twenty loud crows perched on branches nearby, cawing angrily. The mother kite was flying in circles, clearly distressed. She had done something to upset the crows. Soon, she returned to her nest, and the crows took flight, cawing more loudly than ever. As soon as the kite settled on the nest, they landed back on the branches of the tree and, each keeping a respectful distance, started what the locals of Upper India call gali galoj. She put up with their rude insults for a while, then left the nest. This prompted all the crows to take to the air. Some tried to dive-bomb her in the sky. I watched them chase her for a few minutes. Eventually, their assault began to wane, leading me to think that the crows were regaining their composure, and that the whole situation would die down soon, as these things usually do. So, I continued on my way. When I returned an hour later, I was shocked to find the crows still attacking. The kite was nowhere to be seen, and the crows had become bolder; whereas before they had stayed at a safe distance, some were now quite close to the nest. I was short on time and couldn’t stick around to see what happened. Later in the afternoon, when I walked by the nest again, there was still no kite, but the tree was buzzing with crows, and part of the nest looked like it had been torn apart. The nestlings were probably gone. I couldn’t be sure, though, since I was on my way to a social event. I admit I was really tempted to skip it, but I just couldn't come up with a good excuse. “Dear Mrs. Burra Mem, I regret to say that I couldn’t make it to your tennis party this afternoon due to a family tragedy—of a kite,” didn’t seem convincing, so I went to the lawn-tennis party.

THE INDIAN KITE. (MILVUS GOVINDA)

THE INDIAN KITE. (MILVUS GOVINDA)

THE INDIAN KITE. (MILVUS GOVINDA)

[149]

When I saw the nest the following morning it was a total wreck. There were still one or two crows hanging around, and while I was inspecting the ground beneath the scene of the tragedy they amused themselves by dropping sticks on my head. The crow is an ill-conditioned bird. I found, lying about on the ground, the débris of the nest, a number of kite’s feathers, including six or seven of the large tail ones, and two crow’s wings. These last furnished the clue to the behaviour of the crows. The kite must have attacked and killed a sickly crow, in order to provide breakfast for her young. This was, of course, an outrage on corvine society—an outrage which demanded speedy vengeance. Hence the gathering of the clans which I had witnessed the previous day. At first the crows were half afraid of the kite, and were content to call her names; but as they warmed up to their work they gained courage, and so eventually killed the kite, destroyed her nest, and devoured her young. Thus did they avenge the murder.

When I saw the nest the next morning, it was completely devastated. A couple of crows were still hanging around, and while I was checking out the ground under the scene of the disaster, they entertained themselves by dropping sticks on my head. Crows are pretty bad-tempered birds. I found scattered on the ground bits of the nest, a bunch of kite feathers, including six or seven big tail feathers, and two crow wings. These wings gave me a clue about the crows' behavior. The kite must have attacked and killed a weak crow to feed her young. This was, of course, a serious offense against crow society—an offense that demanded quick revenge. That’s why I had seen the gathering of the clans the day before. At first, the crows were a bit scared of the kite and only dared to insult her; but as they got more into it, they grew bolder and eventually killed the kite, destroyed her nest, and ate her young. That’s how they took revenge for the murder.

[150]


Two little birds

There is, hidden away in a corner of Northern India, a tiny orchard which may be likened to an oasis in the desert, because the trees which compose it are always fresh and green, even when the surrounding country is dry and parched. Last April two or three of the paradise flycatchers who were on their annual journey northward were tempted to tarry awhile in this orchard to enjoy the cool shade afforded by the trees. They found the place very pleasant, and insect life was so abundant that they determined to remain there during the summer. Thus it chanced that one morning, early in May, a cock flycatcher was perched on one of the trees, preening his feathers. A magnificent object was he amid the green foliage. The glossy black of his crested head formed a striking contrast to the whiteness of the remainder of his plumage. His two long median tail feathers, that hung down like satin streamers, formed an ornament more beautiful than the train of a peacock. He was so handsome that a hen flycatcher, who was sitting in a tree near by, resolved to make him wed her; but there was another hen living in the same orchard who was equally determined to secure the handsome cock as her mate. Even while the first hen was admiring him, her rival [151] came up and made as if to show off her dainty chestnut plumage. This so angered the first hen that she attacked her rival. A duel then took place between the two little birds. It was not of long duration, for the second hen soon discovered that she was no match for the first, and deeming discretion to be the better part of valour, she flew away and left the orchard before she sustained any injury. Then the triumphant hen, flushed with victory, went up to the cock and said, “See what I have done for love of thee. I have driven away my rival. Wed me, I pray, for I am worthy of thee. Behold how beautiful I am.” The cock looked at her as she stood there spreading her chestnut wings and saw that she was fair to gaze upon. He then fluttered his snowy pinions and sang a sweet little warble, which is the way a cock bird tells the lady of his choice that he loves her.

There’s a small orchard tucked away in a corner of Northern India, like an oasis in the desert, because the trees there are always fresh and green, even when the land around them is dry and parched. Last April, a couple of paradise flycatchers, on their annual journey north, were tempted to linger in this orchard to enjoy the cool shade provided by the trees. They found it very nice, and with plenty of insects around, they decided to stick around for the summer. One morning, early in May, a male flycatcher was perched on one of the trees, preening his feathers. He looked magnificent against the green leaves. The glossy black of his crested head was a striking contrast to the white of the rest of his feathers. His two long middle tail feathers, hanging down like satin streamers, were even more beautiful than a peacock's train. He was so handsome that a female flycatcher, sitting in a nearby tree, decided she wanted him as her mate. However, another female in the same orchard was also determined to win over the attractive male. While the first female admired him, her rival approached, showing off her pretty chestnut feathers. This angered the first female, and she attacked her competitor. A duel broke out between the two little birds. It didn’t last long because the second female quickly realized she was no match for the first. Deciding it was smarter to retreat, she flew away from the orchard before getting hurt. Then the victorious female, glowing with pride, approached the male and said, “Look what I’ve done for love. I’ve driven away my rival. Marry me, please, for I deserve you. Just see how beautiful I am.” The male looked at her as she spread her chestnut wings and saw that she was indeed lovely. He then fluttered his white wings and sang a sweet little tune, which is how a male bird expresses his love for the female of his choice.

For the next few days these little birds led an idyllic existence. Free from care and anxiety, they disported themselves in that shady grove, now playing hide-and-seek among the foliage, now making graceful sweeps after their insect quarry, now pouring out the fulness of their love—the cock in sweet song and mellow warble, the hen in her peculiar twittering note. Their happiness was complete; never did the shadow of a cloud mar the sunshine of their springtime.

For the next few days, these little birds lived a peaceful life. Carefree and relaxed, they played in the shady grove, sometimes playing hide-and-seek among the leaves, other times skillfully swooping after their insect prey, and often expressing their love—with the male singing sweet melodies and the female making her unique chirping sounds. Their happiness was total; not once did a shadow of a cloud ruin the brightness of their springtime.

One day they were simultaneously seized by the impulse to build a nest. First a suitable site had to be chosen. After much searching and anxious consultation, mingled with love-making, they agreed upon the branch of a pear tree, some eight feet above the ground. During the whole of the following week they were busy seeking [152] for grass stems, which they fastened to the branch of the tree by means of strands of cobweb. They did not hunt for material in company, as some birds do. The cock would go in one direction and the hen in another. Each, as it found a suitable piece of dried grass, or moss, or cobweb, or whatever it happened to be seeking, would dash back joyfully to the nest with it and weave it into the structure. Sometimes one bird would return while the other was at work on the nursery; the former would then sit near by and wait until the latter had finished.

One day, they both suddenly felt the urge to build a nest. First, they needed to pick a good spot. After a lot of searching and worried discussions, mixed with some flirting, they decided on a branch of a pear tree about eight feet off the ground. For the entire next week, they focused on gathering grass stems, which they attached to the tree branch using strands of cobwebs. Unlike some birds, they didn’t search for materials together. The male would go one way while the female went another. Each time one of them found a suitable piece of dried grass, moss, or cobwebs, they would excitedly rush back to the nest and weave it into place. Occasionally, one bird would come back while the other was still working on the nursery; the returning bird would then sit nearby and wait until the other was done.

At the end of the first day the nest appeared to the uninitiated eye merely a tangle of grass stems stuck on to the tree, but owing to the united efforts of the energetic little builders, it soon took definite shape. By the third day it was obvious that the nest was to have the form of an inverted cone firmly bound to the branch of the tree. The birds took the utmost care to make the nest circular. In order to ensure a smooth, round cavity they would sit in it and, with wings spread over the edge, turn their bodies round and round. At the end of about five days’ steady work the nursery had assumed its final shape. But even then much remained to be done. The whole of the exterior had to be thickly covered with cobweb and little silky cocoons. This was two full days’ work.

By the end of the first day, the nest looked to the inexperienced eye like just a messy bunch of grass stuck to the tree, but thanks to the combined efforts of the enthusiastic little builders, it quickly began to take shape. By the third day, it was clear that the nest would be in the form of an upside-down cone securely attached to the tree branch. The birds were very careful to make the nest circular. To create a smooth, round cavity, they would sit in it and, with their wings spread over the edge, rotate their bodies around. After about five days of diligent work, the nursery reached its final shape. But there was still a lot to do. The entire exterior needed to be thickly covered with cobwebs and small silky cocoons. This took two full days of work.

Great was the delight of the little birds when the last delicate filament had been added. Their joy knew no bounds. They would sit in the nest and cry out in pure delight. The whole orchard rang with their notes of jubilation. Then a little pinkish egg, spotted with red, appeared in the nest. This was followed, next day, by [153] another. On the fifth day after its completion the nursery contained the full clutch of four eggs.

The little birds were incredibly happy when the last delicate thread was added. Their joy was limitless. They would sit in the nest and chirp in pure happiness. The whole orchard echoed with their cheerful sounds. Then a little pinkish egg, speckled with red, showed up in the nest. The next day, another one appeared. On the fifth day after it was finished, the nursery had a full set of four eggs.

Most carefully did the birds watch over their priceless treasures. Never for a moment did they leave them unguarded; one of the pair invariably remained sitting on the nest, while the other went to look for food and dissipate its exuberant energy in song or motion. During the day the cock and hen shared equally the duties of incubation, but the hen sat throughout the night while the cock roosted in a tree hard by. So healthy were the little birds and so comfortably weary with the labours of the day that they slept uninterruptedly all the night through; nor did they wake up when a human being came with a lantern and inspected the nest. Thus some ten days passed. But these were not days of weariness, because the hearts of the little flycatchers were full of joy.

Most carefully, the birds kept an eye on their priceless treasures. They never left them unguarded; one of them always stayed on the nest while the other went to find food and expend its energy singing or moving around. During the day, the male and female shared the incubation duties equally, but the female sat on the nest all night while the male roosted in a nearby tree. The little birds were so healthy and comfortably tired from their day's activities that they slept soundly through the night; they didn't even wake up when a human came with a lantern to check the nest. This continued for about ten days. But these weren't days of exhaustion because the little flycatchers’ hearts were filled with joy.

Then a young bird emerged from one of the eggs. It was an unlovely, naked creature—all mouth and stomach. But its parents did not think it ugly. Its advent only served to increase their happiness. They were now able to spend their large surplus of energy in seeking food for it.

Then a young bird broke out of one of the eggs. It was an unattractive, bare creature—just mouth and stomach. But its parents didn’t see it as ugly. Its arrival only added to their joy. They were now able to use their extra energy to find food for it.

Ere long its brethren came out of their shells, and there were then four mouths to feed; so that the father and mother had plenty to do, but they still found time in which to sing.

Soon, its siblings came out of their shells, and then there were four mouths to feed; so the father and mother had a lot to do, but they still found time to sing.

Thus far everything had gone as merrily as a marriage bell. The happiness of those lovely little airy fairy creatures was without alloy. It is true that they sometimes had their worries and anxieties, as when a human [154] being chanced to approach the nest; but these were as fleeting as the tints in a sunset sky, and were half forgotten ere they had passed away. This idyllic existence was, alas, not destined to endure.

So far, everything had gone as happily as a wedding bell. The joy of those lovely little whimsical creatures was pure. It's true that they sometimes had their worries and anxieties, like when a human happened to come near the nest; but those moments were as brief as the colors in a sunset, and were mostly forgotten before they disappeared. This perfect existence was, sadly, not meant to last.

One day, when the man who kept guard over the orchard slumbered, a native boy entered it with the intention of stealing fruit. But the pears were yet green, and this angered the urchin. As he was about to leave the grove he espied the beautiful cock flycatcher sitting on the nest. The boy had no soul for beauty; he was not spell-bound by the beautiful sight that met his eyes. He went to the tree, drove away the sitting bird, tore down the branch on which the nest was placed and bore it off with its occupants in triumph, amid the distressed cries of the cock bird. These soon brought back the hen, and great was her lamentation when she found that that which she valued most in the world had gone. Her sorrow and rage knew no bounds. Poignant, too, was the grief of the cock bird, for he had been an eye-witness of the dastardly act. For a few hours all the joy seemed to have left the lives of those little birds. But they were too active, too healthy, too full of life to be miserable long. Soon the pleasantness of their surroundings began to manifest itself to them and soothe their sorrow, for the sun was still shining, the air was sweet and cool, the insects hummed their soft chorus, and their fellow-birds poured forth their joy. So the cock began to sing and said to his mate, “Be not cast down, the year is yet young, many suns shall come and go before the cold will drive us from this northern clime; there is time for us to build another nest. Let us leave this [155] treacherous grove and seek some other place.” The hen found that these words were good. Thus did these little birds forget their sorrow and grow as blithe and gay as they had been before. But that orchard knew them no more.

One day, when the man who was watching over the orchard dozed off, a local boy sneaked in, planning to steal some fruit. However, the pears were still green, which frustrated the boy. Just as he was about to leave the grove, he spotted a beautiful cock flycatcher on its nest. The boy had no appreciation for beauty; he wasn't captivated by the lovely sight before him. He approached the tree, scared the bird away, tore down the branch with the nest, and triumphantly took it away, ignoring the distressed calls of the cock bird. This soon brought the hen back, and she mourned deeply when she realized that her most valued possession was gone. Her sorrow and anger were immense. The cock bird was also heartbroken, having witnessed the cowardly deed. For a few hours, it seemed like all joy had vanished from the lives of those little birds. But they were too lively, too healthy, and too full of spirit to remain unhappy for long. Soon, the beauty of their surroundings began to comfort them, for the sun was still shining, the air was sweet and cool, the insects buzzed softly, and their fellow birds sang joyfully. Then the cock started to sing and said to his mate, “Don't be downhearted; the year is still young, many suns will come and go before the cold drives us from this northern land; we still have time to build another nest. Let’s leave this treacherous grove and find another spot.” The hen agreed, finding comfort in his words. So, the little birds moved on, quickly forgetting their sorrow and returning to their cheerful selves. But they never returned to that orchard again.

[156]


The Paradise Flycatcher

The cock paradise flycatcher (Terpsiphone paradisi), when in full adult plumage, is a bird of startling beauty. I shall never forget the first occasion upon which I saw him. It was in the Himalayas when night was falling that I caught sight of some white, diaphanous-looking creature flitting about among the trees. In the dim twilight it looked ghostly in its beauty.

The male paradise flycatcher (Terpsiphone paradisi), when fully grown, is an incredibly beautiful bird. I’ll never forget the first time I saw him. It was in the Himalayas at dusk when I noticed a white, sheer-looking creature moving among the trees. In the fading light, it appeared almost ghostly in its beauty.

It is the two elongated, middle tail feathers which render the bird so striking. They look like white satin streamers and are responsible for the bird’s many popular names, such as cotton-thief, ribbon-bird, rocket-bird. But this flycatcher has more than striking beauty to commend it to the naturalist; it is of surpassing interest from the point of view of biological theory. The cock is one of the few birds that undergo metamorphosis during adult life, and the species furnishes an excellent example of sexual dimorphism.

It’s the two long, middle tail feathers that make the bird so eye-catching. They resemble white satin ribbons and are why the bird has many popular names like cotton-thief, ribbon-bird, and rocket-bird. But this flycatcher has more than just its stunning appearance to appeal to naturalists; it's incredibly interesting in terms of biological theory. The male is one of the few birds that undergoes changes during its adult life, and the species provides a great example of sexual dimorphism.

Since the day, some years back, when I first set eyes upon the bird, I determined to learn something of its habits; but I had to wait long before I was able to carry out my determination. It was not until I came to Lahore that I saw much of the species. Here let me say that the capital of the Punjab, unpromising as [157] it looks at first sight, is, when one gets to know it, a veritable gold mine for the ornithologist.

Since the day, several years ago, when I first saw the bird, I decided to learn about its habits; but I had to wait a long time before I could follow through on that decision. It wasn't until I arrived in Lahore that I encountered the species more frequently. Here, I should mention that the capital of Punjab, as unappealing as it may seem at first glance, is actually a treasure trove for birdwatchers once you get to know it.

Paradise flycatchers migrate there in great numbers in order to breed. They arrive at the end of April and at once commence nesting operations. Before describing these, let me, in order to enable non-ornithological readers to appreciate what follows, say a few words regarding the plumage of the bird. The young of both sexes are chestnut in colour, with the exception of a black head and crest and whitish under parts. This plumage is retained by the hen throughout life. After the autumn moult of the second year the two median tail feathers of the cock grow to a length of sixteen inches, that is to say, four times the length of the other tail feathers, and are retained till the following May or June, when they are cast. After the third autumn moult they again grow, and the plumage now begins to become gradually white, the wings and tail being the first portions to be affected by the change; thus the cock is for a time partly chestnut and partly white, and it is not until he emerges from the moult of his fourth autumn that all his feathers are white, with, of course, the exception of those of his head and crest. The bird retains this plumage until death. Cock birds breed in either chestnut or white plumage; this proves that the metamorphosis from chestnut to white takes place after the bird has attained maturity.

Paradise flycatchers migrate there in large numbers to breed. They arrive at the end of April and immediately start nesting. Before diving into the details, I want to help readers who aren’t familiar with birds understand what follows, so let’s talk a bit about the bird’s coloring. The young of both sexes are chestnut in color, except for their black head and crest and lighter underparts. This coloring stays the same for the female throughout her life. After their autumn molt in the second year, the male’s two middle tail feathers grow to be sixteen inches long, which is four times longer than the other tail feathers, and they are kept until the following May or June when they are shed. After the third autumn molt, these feathers grow back, and the coloring starts to gradually change to white, with the wings and tail being the first to change; so during this time, the male is partly chestnut and partly white. It isn’t until after he sheds his fourth autumn feathers that all his plumage turns white, except for his head and crest. The bird keeps this coloring until it dies. Males can breed in either chestnut or white plumage, which shows that the change from chestnut to white happens after the bird reaches maturity.

In Lahore this species nests in considerable numbers along the well-wooded banks of the Ravi. Since the birds keep to forest country it is not easy to follow their courting operations for any length of time; the birds [158] engaged in courtship appear for a moment and then are lost to view among the foliage, but the species is certainly monogamous, and I think there can be but little doubt that the hen courts the cock quite as much as he courts her. On 28th April I was out with Mr. G. A. Pinto, and he saw a couple of hens chasing a cock in white plumage. Presently one of the hens drove away the other, then the cock showed off to the triumphant hen, expanding his wings and uttering a sweet little song, like the opening bars of that of the white-browed fantail flycatcher (Rhipidura albifrontata). I myself was not a witness of that incident, the birds not being visible from where I was standing at the time; but on 3rd June I saw a cock bird in chestnut plumage and a hen fighting; before long the birds disengaged themselves and the male flew off; then a cock in white plumage came up to the hen and gave her a bit of his mind. After this they both disappeared among the foliage. Presently I saw two hens chasing a chestnut-coloured cock. I do not understand the full significance of these incidents, but they tend to refute Charles Darwin’s contention that there is competition among cocks for hens but none among hens for cocks, and to show that the hen takes an active part in courtship. To this I shall return.

In Lahore, this species nests in large numbers along the tree-lined banks of the Ravi. Since the birds prefer forested areas, it's not easy to observe their courtship behaviors for long; the courting birds appear for a moment and then disappear into the leaves. However, it’s clear that this species is monogamous, and I believe there’s little doubt that the female pursues the male just as much as he pursues her. On April 28th, I was out with Mr. G. A. Pinto when he spotted a couple of females chasing a male with white plumage. Soon, one of the females drove away the other, and then the male displayed for the victorious female, spreading his wings and singing a sweet little tune, similar to the opening notes of the white-browed fantail flycatcher (Rhipidura albifrontata). I didn’t see that instance since the birds weren’t visible from where I was standing at the time, but on June 3rd, I witnessed a male in chestnut plumage fighting with a female. Eventually, they separated, and the male flew off; then another male in white plumage approached the female and expressed his discontent. After that, they both vanished into the foliage. Shortly after, I saw two females pursuing a chestnut-colored male. I don't fully grasp the significance of these events, but they challenge Charles Darwin’s idea that males compete for females while females don’t compete for males, and suggest that the female actively participates in courtship. I will return to this topic.

It does not seem to be generally known that the cock paradise flycatcher is capable of emitting anything approaching a song. Thus Oates writes in The Fauna of British India of these flycatchers, “their notes are very harsh.” This is true of the usual call, which is short, sharp, and harsh, something like the twitter of an [159] angry sparrow. But in addition to this the cock has two tuneful calls. One resembles the commencement of the song of the white-browed fantail flycatcher, and the other is a sweet little warble of about four notes. I have repeatedly been quite close to the cock when thus singing and have seen his throat swell when he sang, so there can be no question as to the notes being his. He thus furnishes one of the many exceptions to the rule that brilliantly plumaged birds have no song.

It doesn't seem to be widely known that the male paradise flycatcher can actually produce something that resembles a song. Oates mentions in The Fauna of British India about these flycatchers, “their notes are very harsh.” This is true for their typical call, which is short, sharp, and harsh, similar to the chirp of an angry sparrow. However, the male has two melodic calls as well. One sounds like the beginning of the song of the white-browed fantail flycatcher, and the other is a lovely little warble of about four notes. I've often been quite close to the male while he was singing and have noticed his throat swell as he sang, so there's no doubt those notes are his. He provides one of the many exceptions to the idea that brightly colored birds don't sing.

The nest is a deepish cup, firmly attached to two or more slender branches; it is in shape like an inverted cone with the point prolonged as a stalk. It is composed chiefly of vegetable fibres and fine grass; these being coated outwardly by a thick layer of cobweb and small white cocoons. Let me take this opportunity of remarking that cobweb affords a most important building material to bird masons; it is their cement, and many species, such as sunbirds and flycatchers, use it most unsparingly.

The nest is a somewhat deep cup, securely attached to two or more slender branches; its shape resembles an inverted cone with the tip extending like a stalk. It’s mainly made of plant fibers and fine grass, which are covered on the outside by a thick layer of cobwebs and small white cocoons. I’d like to point out that cobwebs are a crucial building material for bird builders; it acts like their cement, and many species, like sunbirds and flycatchers, use it quite generously.

The paradise flycatcher seems to delight to build in exposed situations, hence a great many of their nests come to grief, especially in the Punjab, where, if there be anything in phrenology, the bumps of destructiveness and cruelty must be enormously developed in every small boy.

The paradise flycatcher seems to love building in open places, so many of their nests end up failing, especially in the Punjab, where, if there's anything to phrenology, the traits of destructiveness and cruelty must be highly developed in every young boy.

The nesting habits of the paradise flycatcher have been described in detail in the preceding article. They are of considerable biological importance. I would lay especial stress on the active part in courtship played by the hen, the large share in incubation taken by the cock, and the change in the plumage of the cock bird [160] from chestnut to white in the third year of his existence.

The nesting habits of the paradise flycatcher have been detailed in the previous article. They hold significant biological importance. I want to emphasize the active role the female plays in courtship, the substantial involvement of the male in incubation, and the change in the male's plumage from chestnut to white in his third year of life. [160]

Darwin, as I have already pointed out, devoted much time and energy in trying to prove that there is in most species competition among males for females, and that these latter are in consequence able to exercise a selection. They choose the most brilliant and beautiful of their numerous suitors. Thus we have what he calls sexual selection, or, as I should prefer to call it, feminine selection. On this theory the poor cock exercises no selection; any decrepit old hen is good enough for him! He is all eagerness, while the hen is blasé and indifferent. This theory is, I submit, improbable on a priori grounds. It is certainly opposed to human experience, and is, I believe, not borne out by animal behaviour.

Darwin, as I've already mentioned, spent a lot of time and energy trying to show that in most species, males compete for females, and that females can therefore make choices. They select the most vibrant and attractive among their many suitors. This is what he refers to as sexual selection, or what I would rather call feminine selection. According to this theory, the poor male has no say in the matter; any old hen will do for him! He is full of enthusiasm, while the hen is blasé and indifferent. I argue that this theory is unlikely based on a priori reasoning. It definitely contradicts human experience, and I believe it isn't supported by animal behavior.

I have paid some attention to the subject lately, and am convinced that in most cases the desire of the hen for the cock is as great as the desire of the latter for the hen. It was only this morning that I watched two hen orioles trying to drive each other away, while the cock was in a tree near by.

I’ve been paying attention to this topic recently, and I’m convinced that in most cases, the hen’s desire for the rooster is just as strong as the rooster’s desire for the hen. Just this morning, I saw two hen orioles trying to chase each other off while the rooster was sitting in a nearby tree.

To repeat what I have already said, the hen courts the cock quite as much as he courts her. When a pair of birds mate they are mutually attracted to one another. That there is such a thing as sexual selection I am convinced, but I do not believe that this selection is confined to the hens. The hen selects the best cock she can get to pair with her, while the cock selects the best hen available.

To reiterate what I've already mentioned, the hen shows interest in the cock just as much as he shows interest in her. When two birds mate, they are attracted to each other equally. I firmly believe in the concept of sexual selection, but I don't think this selection is limited to the hens. The hen chooses the best cock she can find to mate with, while the cock looks for the best hen he can get.

I speak here of monogamous species; among polygamous [161] ones there must of necessity be considerable competition for hens.

I’m talking about monogamous species; in polygamous ones, there has to be a lot of competition for females. [161]

The second point upon which I desire to lay stress is the active part taken by the cock paradise flycatcher in incubation. This, again, is, I believe, nothing very uncommon, even in sexually dimorphic species, for I have myself put a cock minivet (Pericrocotus peregrinus) off the nest. Yet this fact seems to dispose of Wallace’s theory that the more sombre hues of the hen are due to her greater need of protection, since she alone is supposed to incubate.

The second point I want to emphasize is the role of the male paradise flycatcher in incubation. I believe this isn't very unusual, even in species with distinct male and female appearances, as I've personally seen a male minivet (Pericrocotus peregrinus) leave the nest. However, this fact seems to challenge Wallace’s theory that the darker colors of the female are necessary for her greater need for protection, since she is thought to be the only one incubating the eggs.

As a matter of fact, a bird sitting on a nest is not, in my opinion, exposed to any special danger, for it seems that birds of prey as a rule only attack flying objects.

As a matter of fact, a bird sitting on a nest isn't, in my opinion, in any special danger, because it seems that birds of prey typically only attack things that are flying.

Finally, there is the extraordinary metamorphosis undergone by the cock in his fourth year. It is difficult to see how this can have been caused by the preference of the hen for white cock birds, since a great many chestnut ones are observed to breed; the dimorphism must, therefore, have originated late in the life history of the species, and although a hen bird might prefer a white to a chestnut husband, it is difficult to believe that she would prefer a skewbald one, and this skewbald state must have been an ancestral stage if we believe that the transition is due to feminine selection of white birds. I may be asked, “If you decline to believe that the hen has greater need of protection than the cock, how do you account for the phenomena of sexual dimorphism, and if it is not sexual selection which has caused the white plumage of the cock paradise flycatcher to arise, what is it?”

Finally, there’s the remarkable transformation that the rooster goes through in his fourth year. It’s hard to see how this could have resulted from the hen’s preference for white roosters, since many chestnut ones are known to breed; this two-color trait must have developed later in the species' history. While a hen might prefer a white mate over a chestnut one, it’s hard to believe she would choose a piebald one, and this piebald coloring must have been an ancestral stage if we accept that the change is due to female selection of white birds. I might be asked, “If you don’t believe that the hen needs more protection than the rooster, how do you explain the phenomenon of sexual dimorphism? And if it isn’t sexual selection that led to the white plumage of the male paradise flycatcher, then what is it?”

[162]

This article has already attained such a length that even had I complete explanations to offer I could not set them forth in this place. I must content myself with giving what I believe to be the key to the solution of the problem. I think that there is little doubt that what a bird looks for in its mate is, not beauty or brilliance of plumage, but vigour and strength. If beauty is a correlative character to strength, then the hen selects the most beautiful of the cocks willing to mate with her, not because of his beauty, but on account of his strength; likewise the cock. Now there is a very intimate connection between the generative cells and the body cells, and the male element tends to dissipate energy and the female element to conserve it. Thus it is that the general tendency of the cock is to become gaily coloured and to grow plumes and other ornaments, while the tendency of the hen is to remain of comparatively sombre hue.

This article has already reached a length where, even if I had complete explanations to share, I couldn't lay them out here. I have to be satisfied with providing what I believe is the key to solving the problem. I think there's little doubt that what a bird looks for in its mate is, not beauty or bright feathers, but vigor and strength. If beauty is related to strength, then the hen chooses the most attractive rooster willing to mate with her, not because of his looks, but because of his strength; the same goes for the rooster. There’s a close connection between the reproductive cells and the body cells, with the male element tending to use up energy and the female element aiming to conserve it. This is why roosters tend to be brightly colored and develop feathers and other decorations, while hens typically have more muted colors.

[163]


Shrike birds

Butcher birds are so called because they are reputed to have a habit of impaling on thorns their larger victims, or as much of them as they, owing to want of accommodation, are incapable of eating at the time of the murder. A bush which displays a number of impaled victims—young birds, lizards, locusts, and the like—is supposed, by a stretch of the ornithological imagination, to look like a butcher’s shop. All that is wanted to perfect the illusion is a sign-board, bearing the legend “Lanius vittatus, Purveyor of Meat.” I must here admit, with characteristic honesty, that I have never set eyes upon such a butcher’s shop, or larder, as it should be called, for the shrike does not sell his wares—he merely stores them for personal consumption. Nor have I even seen a shrike impale a victim. My failure cannot, I think, be attributed to lack of observation; for I never espy one of these miniature birds of prey without watching it attentively, in the hope that it will oblige me by acting as all books on ornithology tell me shrikes do. Every butcher bird I have witnessed engaged in shikar has pounced down upon its insect quarry from a suitable perch, seized the luckless victim upon the ground, immediately carried it back to its perch and devoured it then [164] and there. I have seen this operation repeated scores of times. I, therefore, think I am justified in suggesting that the habit of keeping a larder is probably restricted to the larger species of shrike, and that these only impale their victim when there is still something of it left over, after they have eaten so much that for the time being they cannot possibly stow away any more. Jerdon, I notice, makes no mention of ever having seen a butcher bird behave in the orthodox manner. Colonel Cunningham, who is a very close observer of bird life, says, as the result of a long sojourn in India, that shrikes “do not seem very often to impale their victims, probably because these are usually easily broken up; but when they have secured a lizard they sometimes fix it down upon a stout thorn so as to have a point of resistance whilst working at the hard, tough skin.” If any who read these lines have seen a shrike’s larder, either in India or in England, I should esteem it a great favour if they would furnish me with some account of it.

Butcher birds are named for their habit of impaling larger prey on thorns, or whatever bits they can't eat at the moment. A bush full of impaled victims—like young birds, lizards, and locusts—might, with a bit of imagination, be thought to resemble a butcher’s shop. All that's missing to complete the illusion is a sign that says “Lanius vittatus, Meat Merchant.” I must confess, with typical honesty, that I’ve never seen such a butcher shop, or larder, as it would be more accurately called, because the shrike doesn’t sell its catch—it just stores it for itself. I also haven't witnessed a shrike impaling a victim. I don't think my lack of sightings is due to not paying attention; every time I spot one of these tiny birds of prey, I watch closely, hoping it will perform as the books on birds say they do. Every butcher bird I’ve seen hunting has swooped down on its insect prey from a good spot, snatched the unfortunate critter off the ground, taken it back to its perch, and devoured it right there. I’ve seen this happen dozens of times. Thus, I feel justified in suggesting that the practice of keeping a larder might be limited to the larger shrike species, and they usually impale their prey only when there’s something left over after they’ve eaten their fill for the moment. Jerdon, I've noticed, doesn’t mention ever witnessing a butcher bird act in the traditional way. Colonel Cunningham, a keen observer of bird life, notes from his long time in India that shrikes “rarely seem to impale their victims, likely because these are generally easy to tear apart; but when they catch a lizard, they sometimes pin it to a sturdy thorn to have something to hold onto while they work on the tough skin.” If anyone reading this has seen a shrike’s larder, either in India or England, I would greatly appreciate it if you could share your experience with me.

Let me not be mistaken. I do not say that butcher birds never keep larders, for they undoubtedly do; of this I am satisfied. Thus Mr. E. H. Aitken says of the shrike: “It sits upright on the top of a bush or low tree, commanding a good expanse of open, grassy land, and watches for anything which it may be able to surprise and murder—a large grasshopper, a small lizard, or a creeping field mouse. Sometimes it sees a possible chance in a flock of small birds absorbed in searching for grass seeds. Then it slips from its watch-tower and, gliding softly down, pops into the midst of them without warning, and forgetting all about the true nature of [165] its deep plantar tendons, strikes its talons into the nearest. No other bird I know of makes its attack in this way except the birds of prey. The little bird shrieks and struggles, but the cruel shrike holds fast and hammers at the victim’s head with its strong beak until it is dead, then flies away with it to some thorn bush which is its larder. There it hangs it up on a thorn and leaves it to get tender. . . . This is no fable, I have seen the bird do it.” Again, the Rev. C. D. Cullen, with whom I have enjoyed many an ornithological ramble in England and on the continent of Europe, informs me that once in Surrey he came upon a shrike’s larder, and on that occasion the “shop” consisted of the legs of a young green finch.

Let’s not get it twisted. I’m not saying that butcher birds don’t have larders because they definitely do; I’m sure of it. As Mr. E. H. Aitken points out about the shrike: “It perches upright on the top of a bush or low tree, overlooking a wide area of open grassland, and keeps an eye out for anything it can surprise and kill—a large grasshopper, a small lizard, or a creeping field mouse. Sometimes it spots an opportunity in a flock of small birds focused on searching for grass seeds. Then it drops from its lookout and, gliding down quietly, swoops into the middle of them unexpectedly, and forgetting all about the true nature of its deep plantar tendons, digs its talons into the nearest one. No other bird I know attacks like this except birds of prey. The little bird shrieks and struggles, but the brutal shrike holds on tight and pecks at the victim’s head with its strong beak until it’s dead, then flies off with it to a thorn bush that serves as its larder. There, it hangs the prey on a thorn and leaves it to become tender. . . . This is no fairy tale; I have seen the bird do it." Additionally, the Rev. C. D. Cullen, with whom I've enjoyed many bird-watching outings in England and across Europe, told me that once in Surrey, he stumbled upon a shrike’s larder, and that time the “shop” was filled with the legs of a young green finch.

The usual food, then, of the butcher bird appears to be small insects. When a suitable opportunity offers, the larger species will attack a lizard or a young or sickly bird, especially a bird in a cage. Of the rufous-backed shrike Mr. Benjamin Aitken writes: “It will come down at once to a cage of small birds exposed at a window, and I once had an amadavat killed and partly eaten through the wires by one of these shrikes, which I saw in the act with my own eyes. The next day I caught the shrike in a large basket which I set over the cage of amadavats.” But, of course, it is one thing to catch a bird in a cage and another to capture it in the open. Shrikes are savage enough for any murder, but most little birds are too sharp for them.

The typical food of the butcher bird is small insects. When the chance arises, the larger species will go after a lizard or a young or weak bird, especially one in a cage. About the rufous-backed shrike, Mr. Benjamin Aitken notes: “It will quickly come down to a cage of small birds placed at a window, and I once saw one of these shrikes kill and partially eat an amadavat through the wires. I witnessed it myself. The next day, I caught the shrike in a large basket that I set over the cage of amadavats.” However, it’s one thing to catch a bird in a cage and quite another to catch it in the wild. Shrikes are brutal enough for any killing, but most small birds are too clever for them.

Fifteen species of shrike occur in India. The commonest are, perhaps, the Indian grey shrike (Lanius lahtora) and the bay-backed shrike (Lanius vittatus). [166] The latter is the one that frequents our gardens. He is not a large bird, being about the size of a bulbul. The head and back of the neck are a pretty grey. The back is chestnut-maroon, shading off to whitish near the tail. There is a broad black streak running across the forehead and through the eye, giving the bird a grim, sinister aspect. The breast and lower parts are white; the wings and tail black, or rather appear black when the bird is at rest. During flight the pinions display a conspicuous white bar, and the white outer tail feathers also come into view. The stout beak is black, and the upper mandible projects downwards over the lower one. This further adds to the ferocity of the bird’s mien. It is impossible to mistake a butcher bird; look out for its grey head, broad, black eyebrow, and white breast.

Fifteen species of shrike are found in India. The most common are probably the Indian grey shrike (Lanius lahtora) and the bay-backed shrike (Lanius vittatus). [166] The bay-backed shrike is the one that often visits our gardens. It’s not a large bird, about the size of a bulbul. The head and back of the neck are a nice grey. The back is chestnut-maroon, fading to whitish near the tail. There’s a broad black stripe running across the forehead and through the eye, giving the bird a grim, menacing look. The breast and lower parts are white; the wings and tail appear black, or rather look black when the bird is resting. When it flies, the wings show a prominent white bar, and the white outer tail feathers become visible. The stout beak is black, and the upper mandible juts down over the lower one. This adds to the fierce appearance of the bird. You can’t mistake a butcher bird; just look for its grey head, broad black eyebrow, and white breast.

The usual note of the shrike is a harsh cry, but during the breeding season, that is to say, from March to July, the cock is able to produce quite a musical song.

The typical call of the shrike is a harsh sound, but during the breeding season, which lasts from March to July, the male can produce a surprisingly melodic song.

At all times the butcher bird is a great mimic. I am indebted to a correspondent for the following graphic account of his histrionic performances: “Of late one of these birds has daily perched himself on a neem tree in my compound and treated me to much music. His hours of practice are early in the morning and at sunset. He begins with his natural harsh notes, and then launches out into mimicry. I gave him a patient hearing this morning, and he treated me to the following: the lap-wing, the sparrow-hawk, the partridge, the Brahminy minah, the kite, the honeysucker, the hornbill (of these parts), the scream of the green parrot, and the cry of a chicken when being carried off by a kite.”

At all times, the butcher bird is an amazing mimic. I'm thankful to a correspondent for this vivid description of its performances: “Recently, one of these birds has been perching on a neem tree in my yard every day and entertaining me with its song. It practices early in the morning and at sunset. It starts with its natural harsh calls and then jumps into mimicry. I listened carefully this morning, and it treated me to the sounds of: the lapwing, the sparrow-hawk, the partridge, the Brahminy myna, the kite, the honeysucker, the local hornbill, the scream of the green parrot, and the cry of a chicken being carried off by a kite.”

[167]

The nests of the various species of shrike resemble one another very closely. Speaking generally, the nest is a neatly made, thick-walled, somewhat deep cup. All manner of material is pressed into service—grass, roots, wool, hair, leaves, feathers, pieces of rag, paper, fine twigs, and straw. The whole forms a compact structure firmly held together by cobweb, which is the cement ordinarily utilised by bird masons.

The nests of different shrike species are quite similar. Generally speaking, the nest is a well-constructed, thick-walled, somewhat deep cup. Various materials are used, including grass, roots, wool, hair, leaves, feathers, scraps of cloth, paper, fine twigs, and straw. Altogether, it creates a solid structure that’s tightly held together by cobwebs, which serve as the glue typically used by bird builders.

The nursery is usually situated in a small tree, a thorny one for preference, in the fork of a branch, or the angle that a branch makes with the main stem. Seen from below it looks likes a little mass of rubbish. As a rule one or two pieces of rag hang down from it and betray its presence to the egg-collector.

The nest is typically located in a small tree, preferably a thorny one, at the fork of a branch or where a branch meets the main trunk. From below, it appears to be just a messy clump. Usually, one or two scraps of fabric dangle down from it, revealing its location to the egg collector.

The normal clutch of eggs is four. The ground colour of these is cream, pale greenish, or grey, and there is towards the large end a zone of brown or purplish blotches.

The usual number of eggs is four. Their base color is cream, light greenish, or gray, and there are brown or purplish blotches towards the larger end.

The shrike is not a shy bird. I have sat within eight feet of a nest and watched the parents feeding their young. No notice was taken of me, but a large lizard that appeared on the branch on which the nest was placed was savagely attacked. The young seem to be fed chiefly on large green caterpillars.

The shrike is not a bashful bird. I've sat just eight feet away from a nest and observed the parents feeding their chicks. They didn’t pay any attention to me, but a big lizard that showed up on the branch where the nest was located was viciously attacked. The young ones mainly get fed large green caterpillars.

Newly fledged butcher birds differ considerably from the adults, and while in the transition stage are sometimes rather puzzling to the ornithologist.

Newly fledged butcher birds look quite different from the adults, and during the transition phase, they can be quite confusing for ornithologists.

[168]


DUCKS

“The duck,” says a writer in the Spectator, “is a person who seldom gets his deserts.” As regards myself I cannot but admit the truth of this assertion. I mean, not that I am a duck, but that I have returned that bird evil for good. He has given me much pleasure, and I have either eaten or shot him as a quid pro quo.

“The duck,” says a writer in the Spectator, “is a creature who rarely gets what it deserves.” As for me, I can’t help but agree with this statement. I don’t mean that I am a duck, but rather that I have repaid that creature's goodness with harm. It has brought me a lot of joy, and I have either eaten or shot it in return.

One of the greatest delights of my early youth was to feed the ducks that lived on the Serpentine. How vividly do I remember the joy that the operation gave me! In the first place, I was allowed to enter the kitchen—that Forbidden Land of childhood’s days, presided over by a fearsome tyrant, yclept the cook—and witness dry bread being cut up into pieces of a size supposed to be suited to the mastication of ducks. The bread thus cut up would be placed in a paper bag and borne off by me in triumph to the upper regions. Then my sister and I, accompanied by the governess, would toddle up Sloane Street, through Lowndes Square, past the great French Embassy, into Hyde Park, along Rotten Row, and thus up to that corner of the Serpentine where the ducks were wont to congregate. There, amid a chorus of quacks, the bread would be thrown, piece by piece, to the ever-hungry ducks. The writer in the [169] Spectator states that “the domestic duck, unlike his wild brother, is a materialist, and where dinner is concerned is decidedly greedy.” The avidity with which the ducks used to make for those pieces of dry bread certainly bears out this statement. Every time a crust was thrown on to the water there would be a wild scramble for it. One individual, more fortunate than the others, would secure it, and, sprinting away from his comrades, would endeavour to swallow it whole. I have said that the pieces of bread were cut up into portions of a size supposed to be convenient for the mastication of a duck; but, if the truth must be told, the cook invariably overestimated the size of the bird’s gullet; hence the frantic muscular efforts to induce them to descend “red lane.” It is a miracle that not one of those ducks shared the sad fate of Earl Godwin.

One of the best memories from my early childhood was feeding the ducks that lived on the Serpentine. I can still vividly recall the happiness it brought me! First of all, I was allowed to go into the kitchen—that forbidden land of my childhood, ruled by a scary tyrant known as the cook—and see dry bread being chopped into pieces the size that was supposedly right for ducks to eat. The cut-up bread would be put in a paper bag, and I would carry it triumphantly to the upper floors. Then my sister and I, along with our governess, would stroll up Sloane Street, through Lowndes Square, past the grand French Embassy, into Hyde Park, along Rotten Row, and finally to that spot on the Serpentine where the ducks loved to gather. There, amidst a chorus of quacks, we would toss the bread, piece by piece, to the ever-hungry ducks. The writer in the Spectator says that “the domestic duck, unlike its wild counterpart, is a materialist and is definitely greedy when it comes to food.” The eagerness with which the ducks would rush for those pieces of dry bread certainly proves this point. Every time a crust was thrown onto the water, there would be a frantic scramble for it. One lucky duck would snag it and, racing away from the others, would try to swallow it whole. I mentioned that the pieces of bread were cut into sizes deemed suitable for the ducks to chew; however, to be honest, the cook always overestimated the size of the bird's throat, resulting in the ducks desperately trying to get them down. It's a miracle that none of those ducks ended up with the same unfortunate fate as Earl Godwin.

Some of them must certainly have lost the epithelial lining of the œsophagus in their desperate efforts to dispose of those pieces of dry bread. An exceptionally unmanageable morsel would be dropped again into the water, and there would be a second scramble for it. By this time, however, it would have become so much softened as to be comparatively easy to swallow. How we used to enjoy watching the efforts of those ducks to negotiate the pieces of bread! We were, of course, blissfully ignorant of the unnaturalness of the process. Our governess used to read, in preference to natural history, fiction of the class in which the fortunate scullery-maid always marries a Duke. Thus it was that my sister and I knew nothing of the wonderful structure of the duck’s beak. We were not aware that the mandibles were [170] lamellated or toothed to form a most efficient sieve. We were not acquainted with the fact that the natural food of the duck is composed of small, soft substances, that as the bird puts its head under water it catches up its breath to suck in the soft substances that may be floating by, that these become broken up as they pass through the duck’s patent filter, only those that are approved being retained and swallowed. But the want of this knowledge did not diminish by one jot or tittle our enjoyment. When all the bread was disposed of, we would inflate and “pop” the paper bag—a performance which gave us nearly as much pleasure as feeding the ducks.

Some of them must have definitely lost the lining of their esophagus in their frantic attempts to swallow those dry pieces of bread. An especially stubborn piece would be thrown back into the water, leading to another scramble for it. By then, though, it would have softened enough to be relatively easy to swallow. We loved watching those ducks trying to manage the bread! We were, of course, completely unaware of how unnatural it was. Our governess preferred reading fiction, like the kind where the lucky maid always ends up marrying a duke, over natural history. Because of this, my sister and I knew nothing about the amazing structure of a duck's beak. We didn’t realize its jaws were layered or toothed to act as a really effective sieve. We weren't aware that a duck's natural diet consists of small, soft items, or that when the bird dives under water, it takes a breath to suck in the soft bits floating by. These get broken down as they go through the duck's special filter, with only the good stuff being kept and swallowed. But not knowing any of this didn’t take away from our enjoyment at all. Once all the bread was gone, we would inflate and “pop” the paper bag—a little show that brought us almost as much joy as feeding the ducks.

As I grew older I came to regard the feeding of ducks as a childish amusement, and in no way suited to one who had attained the dignity of stand-up collars. So, for some years, I took but little interest in the birds, except on the occasions when one confronted me at table.

As I got older, I started to see feeding ducks as a childish pastime, not something for someone like me who had the sophistication of wearing stand-up collars. So, for several years, I lost interest in the birds, except when one happened to be in front of me at the dinner table.

It has again become a pleasure to feed ducks, but I fear that, in spite of this, I shoot them more often than I feed them. I must confess that, when I see a great company of the quacking community, the sportsman in me gets the upper hand of the naturalist, the lust of killing prevails over the love of observation. I know of few greater pleasures than to spend a morning at a well-stocked jhil on a superb winter’s day in Northern India, accompanied, of course, by a number of fellow-sportsmen; for duck shooting is poor sport for a single gun. With but one man after them it is the ducks rather than the human being who enjoy the sport. But, [171] given three or four companions, what better sport is there than that afforded by a day on a well-stocked jhil? At a preconcerted signal the various shooters, each in his boat, put off from different parts of the bank of the lake and make for the middle, which is black with a great company of quack-quacks, composed chiefly of white-eyed pochards, gadwalls, and spotted-bills. Suddenly a number of duck take alarm and get up; then the fun begins. For half an hour or more one enjoys a succession of good sporting shots; the firing is so constant that one’s gun grows almost too hot to hold. Soon, however, all the duck that are not shot down betake themselves to some other jhil, and only the coots remain.

It’s once again a joy to feed ducks, but I worry that, despite this, I end up shooting them more often than feeding them. I must admit that when I see a large group of quacking ducks, the hunter in me takes over the naturalist, and the thrill of killing wins out over the desire to observe. There are few greater pleasures than spending a morning at a well-stocked jhil on a beautiful winter day in Northern India, especially with a few fellow hunters; duck shooting isn’t that exciting for just one person. With only one person hunting, it’s the ducks rather than the human who enjoy the game. But, with three or four friends, what's better than a day at a well-stocked jhil? At a prearranged signal, the various hunters, each in their boat, set off from different spots along the lake's shore and head towards the middle, which is filled with a large group of quacking ducks, mostly white-eyed pochards, gadwalls, and spotted-bills. Suddenly, several ducks get spooked and take off; that’s when the fun starts. For half an hour or more, you enjoy a flurry of sporting shots; the firing is so continuous that your gun gets almost too hot to hold. Soon, however, all the ducks that haven’t been shot head to another jhil, and only the coots are left.

Excellent sport though duck shooting be, I am thankful to say that in these latter days my acquaintance with the duck tribe is not confined to shooting and eating members of it. I occasionally have the opportunity of coming into more friendly relations with it.

Excellent as duck shooting is, I'm happy to say that these days my connection with ducks isn't just about shooting and eating them. I sometimes get the chance to interact with them in a friendlier way.

The duck is a bird worth knowing. He is a fowl of character, a creature that commands not only our respect, but our affection. He makes an excellent pet, as any one may find out by purchasing some bazaar ducks.

The duck is a bird worth getting to know. He’s a unique animal that deserves not just our respect but also our affection. He makes a great pet, as anyone can discover by buying some ducks from the market.

Some years ago the cook of the Superintendent of Police of a certain district in the United Provinces purchased a couple of these birds. When bought they were in an emaciated condition, and it was the intention of the cook to fatten them up and then set them before his master. But before the fattening process was completed the small sons of the policeman took a great fancy to the birds, and the birds reciprocated the fancy. The [172] result was that their lives were spared, and they became friends of the family. They went everywhere with the children, and used even to accompany them when on tour with their father. They were allowed to enter the tents as though they were dogs, and in return used to permit the children to do anything they pleased with them. They even submitted to being carried about like dolls. Most amusing was it to see the good-natured boredom on a duck’s face as a small boy staggered along with it tightly clasped in his arms. Its expression would say more plainly than words, “I don’t altogether relish this, but I know the child means well.”

A few years ago, the cook for the Superintendent of Police in a district of the United Provinces bought a couple of these birds. When he got them, they were in really bad shape, and the cook planned to fatten them up to serve to his boss. But before he could finish that process, the policeman's young sons became very attached to the birds, and the birds felt the same way back. The result was that they were saved from being eaten and became part of the family. They went everywhere with the kids and even joined them on trips with their dad. They were allowed to come into the tents like they were pets, and in return, the kids did whatever they wanted with them. They even let themselves be carried around like dolls. It was especially funny to see the look of good-natured boredom on a duck's face while a little boy struggled to carry it tightly in his arms. Its expression clearly said, “I’m not really enjoying this, but I know the kid means well.”

Nor was this behaviour in any way exceptional. A better-disposed creature than the duck does not exist. “I have kept and closely watched hundreds of ducks,” writes Mr. S. M. Hawkes, “but I never saw them fight with each other, nor ever knew a duck the aggressor in a dispute with some other kind of fowl.” Yet the duck is no coward. The drake is a warrior every inch of him, constant in affection, and violent in love and wrath. If the adult duck is so lovable, how much more so is the duckling! What a source of delight are those golden fluff balls to a child. On seeing them for the first time nine out of ten children will cry—

Nor was this behavior in any way unusual. There isn't a more good-natured creature than the duck. “I have raised and closely observed hundreds of ducks,” writes Mr. S. M. Hawkes, “but I never saw them fight with one another, nor did I ever know a duck to start a conflict with another type of bird.” Yet the duck is no coward. The drake is a warrior in every sense, devoted in affection and fierce in love and anger. If the adult duck is so lovable, how much more adorable is the duckling! Those golden fluff balls are a source of joy for a child. Upon seeing them for the first time, nine out of ten children will cry—

But I want one to play with—Oh I want

But I want one to play with—Oh, I want

A little yellow duck to take to bed with me!

A little yellow duck to snuggle with in bed!

[173]


A deposed king

The eagle is a bird that deserves much sympathy, for he has seen better times. Until a few years ago the pride of place among the fowls of the air was always given to the eagle. “Which eagle?” you ask. I reply, “The eagle.” The poets, who have ever been the bird’s trumpeters, know but one eagle upon which they lavish such epithets as “the imperial bird,” “the royal eagle,” “the monarch bird,” “lord of land and sea,” “the wide-ruling eagle,” “the prince of all the feathered kind,” “the king of birds,” “the bird of heaven,” “the Olympic eagle,” “the bold imperial bird of Jove,” and so on, ad nauseam.

The eagle is a bird that deserves a lot of sympathy, since it has seen better days. Just a few years ago, the eagle was always considered the top bird among all the fowls in the sky. “Which eagle?” you might ask. I respond, “The eagle.” The poets, who have always celebrated this bird, only know one eagle that they shower with titles like “the imperial bird,” “the royal eagle,” “the monarch bird,” “lord of land and sea,” “the wide-ruling eagle,” “the prince of all feathered beings,” “the king of birds,” “the bird of heaven,” “the Olympic eagle,” “the bold imperial bird of Jove,” and so on, ad nauseam.

The eagle of the poets was truly regal. But somebody discovered, one day, that this bird is, like the phœnix, a mythical creature. Eagles do exist—many species of them—but they are very ordinary creatures, in no way answering to the description of the poet’s pet fowl. This, of course, is not the fault of the eagles. They are not to blame because the bards have, with one accord, combined to idealise them. Nevertheless, men, now that they have found out the truth, seem to bear a grudge against the eagle. They are not content with dethroning him, they must needs throw mud at [174] him. It is the present custom to vilify the eagle, to speak of him as though he were an opponent at an election, to dub him a cowardly carrion feeder, little if anything better than a common vulture. Let us, therefore, give the poor out-at-elbows bird an innings to-day and see what we can do for him.

The eagle of the poets was truly majestic. But one day, someone discovered that this bird is, like the phoenix, a mythical being. Eagles do exist—many types of them—but they are quite ordinary and don’t match the poetic description of the beloved bird. Of course, this isn’t the eagles' fault. They’re not to blame for the poets idealizing them. Still, now that people have learned the truth, they seem to hold a grudge against the eagle. They’re not satisfied with just dethroning him; they feel the need to insult him. It’s become the norm to criticize the eagle, talking about him as if he were a rival in an election, labeling him a cowardly scavenger, not much better than a common vulture. So, let’s give this poor downtrodden bird a chance today and see what we can do for him.

But how are we to recognise him when we see him? This is indeed a problem. There is a feature by which the true eagles may be distinguished from all other birds of prey, namely, the feathered tarsus. The true eagles alone among the raptores decline to go about with bare legs; their “understandings” are feathered right down to the toe. Thus may they be recognised.

But how will we recognize him when we see him? This is definitely a problem. There’s a way to tell the true eagles apart from all the other birds of prey, which is their feathered legs. True eagles alone among the raptores don’t walk around with bare legs; their "understandings" are feathered all the way down to their toes. That’s how you can recognize them.

This method of identification is on a par with that of catching a bird by placing a small quantity of salt upon its tail. Eagles show no readiness to come and have their legs inspected. There is, I fear, no feature whereby the tyro can distinguish an eagle as it soars overhead high in the heavens. Nothing save years of patient observation can enable the naturalist to identify any particular bird of prey at sight. Colour is, alas! no guide. The raptores are continually changing their plumage. It were as easy to identify a woman by the colour of her frock as a bird of prey by the hues of its plumage. We read of one eagle that it is tawny rufous, of another that it is rufous tawny, of a third that it is tawny buff. The surest method of distinguishing the various birds of prey is by their flight; but is it possible to describe the peculiar flap of the wings of one eagle, and the particular angle at which another carries its pinions as it sails along? The length [175] of the tail is a guide, but by no means an infallible one. The shikra, the sparrow-hawk, the kestrel, and the kite are long-tailed birds, the caudal appendage accounting for half their total length. In the eagles the tail is considerably shorter in proportion to the size of the bird. Thus the female of the golden eagle (Aquila chrysætus)—which, en passant, is not gold in colour, but dirty whitish brown—is 40 inches long, while the tail is but 14 inches. The vultures have yet shorter tails in proportion to their size. If, therefore, you see soaring overhead a big bird of prey, looking like a large kite, with a moderate tail and curved rather than straight wings, that bird is probably an eagle. So much, then, for the appearance of our dethroned monarch; it now behoves us to consider his character and habits. There are many species of eagle, each of which has its own peculiar ways, hence it is impossible for the naturalist to generalise concerning them. In this respect he is not so fortunate as the poet. Let us briefly consider two species, one belonging to the finer type of eagle and the other to the baser sort.

This method of identification is just like trying to catch a bird by putting a little salt on its tail. Eagles don't exactly rush to have their legs checked. Unfortunately, there's no way for a beginner to tell an eagle apart from the high-flying birds in the sky. Only years of careful observation can help a naturalist identify a specific bird of prey at a glance. Sadly, color isn't a reliable indicator. The raptores are always changing their plumage. It would be as easy to identify a woman by the color of her dress as it is to identify a bird of prey by the colors of its feathers. We hear that one eagle is tawny rufous, another is rufous tawny, and a third is tawny buff. The best way to tell different birds of prey apart is by their flight; but can you really describe the unique flap of one eagle's wings, or the specific angle at which another holds its wings as it glides? The length of the tail can be a clue, but it's not foolproof. The shikra, the sparrow-hawk, the kestrel, and the kite all have long tails, which make up about half their total length. In eagles, the tail is relatively shorter compared to their body size. For example, the female golden eagle (Aquila chrysætus)—which, by the way, isn’t actually gold in color but rather a dirty whitish brown—is 40 inches long, while its tail is only 14 inches. Vultures have even shorter tails for their size. So, if you see a large bird of prey soaring overhead, resembling a big kite, with a moderate tail and wings that are more curved than straight, it's likely an eagle. That covers the appearance of our dethroned monarch; now we should look at its character and habits. There are many species of eagle, each with its own unique behaviors, so it’s difficult for the naturalist to make any generalizations about them. In this regard, they aren't as fortunate as poets. Let’s briefly examine two species: one from the more elegant type of eagle and the other from the lesser sort.

Bonelli’s eagle (Hieraëtus fasciatus), or the crestless hawk eagle as Jerdon calls him, is perhaps the nearest approach of any to the poet’s eagle. This fine bird is common on the Nilgiris, but rare in Madras. It is said to disdain carrion; it preys on small mammals and birds of all sizes. It takes game birds by preference, but when hungry does not draw the line at the crow. If it has hunted all day without obtaining the wherewithal to fill its belly, it repairs to the grove of trees in which all the crows of the neighbourhood roost. As the sun [176] sinks in the heavens the crows arrive in straggling flocks. Suddenly the eagle dashes into the midst of them and, before the crows have realised what has happened, one of them is being carried away in the eagle’s talons. Then the corvi fill the welkin with their cries of distress. It is very naughty of the eagle to prey upon crows in this way, because by so doing it mocks the theory of protective colouration. No one can maintain that our friend Corvus splendens is protectively coloured, that is to say, so coloured as to be inconspicuous. No one but a blind man can fail to see a crow as he steadily flaps his way through the air. No one can deny that the bird flourishes, in spite of the fact that eagles eat him, and that his plumage is as conspicuous as the blazer of the Lady Margaret Boat Club at Cambridge. If, as the theory teaches, it is of paramount importance to a bird to be inconspicuous, why was not the whole clan of corvi swept off the face of the earth long ago?

Bonelli’s eagle (Hieraëtus fasciatus), or the crestless hawk eagle as Jerdon calls it, is probably the closest thing we have to the poet’s eagle. This impressive bird is pretty common on the Nilgiris but rare in Madras. It’s said to avoid eating carrion; it hunts small mammals and birds of various sizes. It prefers game birds, but when it’s hungry, it doesn’t shy away from catching a crow. If it’s spent all day hunting without finding anything to eat, it heads to the grove of trees where all the local crows roost. As the sun sets, the crows arrive in scattered flocks. Suddenly, the eagle swoops into their midst, and before the crows even realize what’s happening, one of them is lifted away in the eagle’s talons. Then the crows fill the air with their cries of distress. It’s quite cheeky of the eagle to hunt crows like this, as it goes against the whole idea of protective coloration. No one can argue that our friend Corvus splendens is well camouflaged; in other words, it's not exactly hard to spot. No sighted person could miss a crow flapping its way through the air. No one can deny that the bird thrives, even though eagles prey on it, and its feathers are as eye-catching as the blazer of the Lady Margaret Boat Club at Cambridge. If, as the theory suggests, it’s crucial for a bird to be inconspicuous, why hasn’t the entire crow family been wiped out long ago?

We have, in conclusion, to consider an eagle of the baser sort. The Indian tawny eagle (Aquila vindhiana), which is the commonest eagle in India, will serve as an example. This bird eats anything in the way of flesh that it can obtain. If the opportunity offers, it will pounce upon a squirrel, a small bird, a lizard, or a frog; but it is a comparatively sluggish creature, and so robs other raptores in preference to catching its own quarry. Most birds of prey are robbers. This the falconer knows, and profits by his knowledge. He first captures some small bird of prey, such as a white-eyed buzzard. Having tied up two or three of its wing feathers so that [177] it cannot fly far, he attaches to its feet a bundle of feathers, from which hang a number of fine hair nooses. He then flies this lure bird. Every bird of prey in the neighbourhood espies it and, seeing the bundle of feathers and remarking the laboured flight, jumps to the conclusion that it is carrying booty, and promptly gives chase with the object of relieving it of its burden. The first robber to arrive is caught in one of the nooses.

We need to look at a lesser type of eagle. The Indian tawny eagle (Aquila vindhiana), which is the most common eagle in India, is a good example. This bird will eat pretty much any flesh it can find. If it gets the chance, it will swoop down on a squirrel, a small bird, a lizard, or a frog; however, it's relatively slow and prefers to steal food from other birds of prey instead of catching its own. Most birds of prey are thieves. The falconer knows this and takes advantage of it. He first captures a small bird of prey, like a white-eyed buzzard. After tying up two or three of its wing feathers so it can't fly far, he attaches a bundle of feathers to its feet, from which hang several fine hair nooses. He then releases this lure bird. Every bird of prey in the area notices it, sees the bundle of feathers, and observes its clumsy flight, leading them to assume it’s carrying something valuable and immediately pursue it to take its prize. The first thief to arrive gets caught in one of the nooses.

The tawny eagle is not above feeding upon carrion. It has not the pluck of Bonelli’s eagle, but is apparently not the contemptible coward it is made out to be by some writers. A few weeks ago I noticed, high up in a farash tree, the platform of sticks and branches that does duty for the nest of this species. I sent my climber to find out what was in the nest. While he was handling the two eggs it contained, the mother eagle swooped down upon him, scratched his head severely, and flew off with his turban. As she sped away, her prize attracted the notice of some kites, who at once attacked her. In the mêlée which ensued, the puggaree dropped to the ground, to the joy of its lawful owner and the disgust of the combatants. I must add that I was not an eye-witness of the encounter; I however saw the marks of the bird’s claws on my climber’s scalp.

The tawny eagle isn't above eating carrion. It may not have the courage of Bonelli’s eagle, but it’s definitely not the pathetic coward some writers claim it to be. A few weeks ago, I spotted a platform made of sticks and branches that serves as the nest for this species, high up in a farash tree. I sent my climber to see what was in the nest. While he was handling the two eggs inside, the mother eagle swooped down on him, gave him a nasty scratch on the head, and flew off with his turban. As she sped away, her prize caught the attention of some kites, who immediately attacked her. In the resulting mêlée, the puggaree fell to the ground, much to the delight of its rightful owner and the frustration of the attackers. I should note that I didn’t witness the encounter myself; however, I did see the scratches from the bird’s claws on my climber’s scalp.

[178]


Rainy Day Birds

There are occasions when one is tempted to wish that one were a bird, for the fowls of the air are spared many of the troubles which we poor terrestrial creatures have to endure.

There are times when you might wish you were a bird, because birds are free from many of the problems that we unfortunate land creatures have to deal with.

Most of us in India have received a telegram ordering us off to some far-away station; then, when distracted by the worry and bustle of packing; when the hideous noises of the Indian railway station “get on the nerves”; as we sit in the dusty, jolting train, we begin to envy the birds who are able to annihilate distance, who have no boxes to pack up, no baggage to go astray, no bills to pay, no chits to write, no cards to leave, no time-table to worry through, no trains to lose, no connections to miss, but have simply to take to their wings and away.

Most of us in India have gotten a telegram sending us off to some distant station; then, caught up in the stress and chaos of packing; when the awful noises of the Indian railway station start to irritate; as we sit in the dusty, bumpy train, we begin to envy the birds who can erase distance, who have no boxes to pack, no luggage to go missing, no bills to deal with, no chits to write, no cards to leave behind, no schedules to stress over, no trains to miss, no connections to lose, but just take to their wings and fly away.

Most of us, again, have been caught in the rain. As the watery contents of the clouds slowly but surely percolated through our clothes, as our boots grew heavier and heavier until the water oozed out at every step, we must have envied the birds. They know naught of rheumatism or ague. Their clothes do not spoil in the rain. They wear no boots to become waterlogged. Their wings rarely become heavy or sodden. For them [179] the rain is a huge joke. They enjoy the falling rain-drops as keenly as a man enjoys his morning shower-bath. There is no bath like the rain bath, and if the drops do fall very heavily there is always shelter to be taken.

Most of us have been caught in the rain. As the water from the clouds slowly soaked through our clothes, and our boots got heavier and heavier until water squished out with every step, we probably envied the birds. They don’t suffer from joint pain or illness. Their feathers don’t get ruined in the rain. They don’t wear boots that get soaked. Their wings hardly ever feel heavy or soaked. For them, the rain is just a big joke. They enjoy the falling raindrops as much as a person enjoys a nice morning shower. There’s no bath like a rain bath, and if the rain is really heavy, there’s always somewhere to seek shelter. [179]

It is of course possible for birds to have too much rain; but this does not often happen in India, except occasionally in the monsoon.

It is definitely possible for birds to get too much rain; however, this rarely happens in India, except sometimes during the monsoon.

As I write this it is pouring “cats and dogs,” and sitting in a tree not five yards away from the window are a couple of crows thoroughly enjoying the blessings which Jupiter Pluvius is showering down upon them. I am high up, seventy or eighty feet above the level of the ground, and can therefore look down upon the crows. They are perched on the ends of the highest branches, determined not to miss a drop of the rain. One of them is not quite satisfied with his position; he espies another bough which seems more exposed, so to this branch he flies, although it is so slender that it can scarce support him. Nevertheless he hangs on to his swaying perch and opens out his wings and flaps his tail—does, in fact, everything in his power to make the most of the passing tropical shower. The other crow has caught sight of me, and thinks he will stare me out, so sits motionless with his eye fixed on mine, while the rain pours upon him and falls off his tail in a little waterfall. Occasionally he gives his friend an answering “squawk,” and then shakes his feathers, and is altogether enjoying himself; he is as jolly as the proverbial sandboy. In other trees near by sit more crows, and, so far as one can judge, each seems to have taken up a position in which he is likely to [180] secure the maximum of rain. All round there is ample shelter; there are numerous ledges, outhouses, and verandahs, in any of which the crows could obtain shelter if they desired it. Shelter? Not a bit of it, they revel in the rain.

As I write this, it's pouring "cats and dogs," and sitting in a tree just a few yards away from the window are a couple of crows really enjoying the downpour from Jupiter Pluvius. I’m high up, about seventy or eighty feet above the ground, so I can look down at the crows. They're perched on the ends of the tallest branches, determined not to miss a single drop of rain. One of them isn't quite happy with his spot; he spots another branch that looks more exposed, so he flies over to it, even though it's so thin that it barely supports him. Still, he clings to his swaying perch and spreads his wings and flaps his tail—doing everything he can to make the most of the tropical shower. The other crow has noticed me and thinks he can out-stare me, so he sits completely still with his eyes fixed on mine, while the rain soaks him and drips off his tail like a little waterfall. Occasionally, he gives his friend a responding “squawk,” then shakes his feathers, and is totally having a good time; he's as cheerful as the proverbial sandboy. More crows are sitting in nearby trees, and from what I can tell, each one has found a spot where they’re likely to catch the most rain. All around, there’s plenty of shelter; there are lots of ledges, outbuildings, and porches where the crows could take cover if they wanted. Cover? Not at all, they’re enjoying the rain.

Two pied wagtails fly by, chasing one another gleefully in the pouring rain; they too are regular “wet bobs.”

Two pied wagtails fly by, playfully chasing each other in the pouring rain; they are also regular "wet bobs."

On the telegraph wires hard by the king-crows sit with their tails projecting horizontally so as to catch as much of the downpour as possible. The dragon-flies are seeking their prey regardless of the rain; this is somewhat surprising, when we consider that to them a drop of rain must bear about the same relation as a glass of water does to a human being. As they are hunting, it is obvious that the minute creatures on which they feed must also be out in the rain, although every drop contains quite sufficient water in which to drown them.

On the telegraph wires nearby, the crows are perched with their tails sticking out horizontally to catch as much of the rain as possible. The dragonflies are chasing after their prey despite the downpour; this is a bit surprising, considering that for them, a raindrop probably feels like a glass of water does to a human. As they hunt, it's clear that the tiny creatures they feed on must also be out in the rain, even though each drop has more than enough water to drown them.

The mortality of small insects in a heavy fall of rain must be enormous. What a strange sight a shower must look to an insect! Each drop must seem like a waterspout.

The death toll for small insects during a heavy rainstorm must be huge. What a weird sight a downpour must be for an insect! Each drop must feel like a massive waterspout.

Are tiny insects aware that the falling drops are fraught with danger to them? Do they attempt to dodge them? I think not. They can know nothing of death or of the danger of drowning. They probably fly about as usual in the rain in blissful ignorance of the harm that threatens them. Some escape unscathed, but others less fortunate are overwhelmed as in a flood, and in a few minutes their little spark of life is extinguished.

Are tiny insects aware that the falling drops are full of danger for them? Do they try to avoid them? I don't think so. They probably don’t understand death or the risk of drowning. They likely fly around in the rain, blissfully unaware of the harm that’s coming their way. Some manage to escape unharmed, but others, less fortunate, get overwhelmed like in a flood, and in just a few minutes, their little spark of life is snuffed out.

[181]

But to return to the birds. They are all making the most of the downpour, ruffling their feathers so that the water shall penetrate to the skin.

But back to the birds. They’re all taking full advantage of the downpour, fluffing their feathers so the water gets through to their skin.

But the rain is more to the birds than a very pleasant form of bath. It is for them a mi-carême, a water carnival, an hour of licence when every bird—even the oldest and most staid—may throw appearances to the wind, when it is “quite the thing” to look dishevelled.

But the rain is more to the birds than just a nice bath. It’s like a mi-carême, a water carnival, a time of freedom when every bird—even the oldest and most serious—can ignore appearances, when it’s totally acceptable to look messy.

What a transformation does a shower of rain effect in the myna. As a rule the bird looks as smart as a lifeguardsman; its uniform is so spick and span that the veriest martinet could find no fault with it. But after the rain has been falling for ten minutes the myna looks as disreputable as a babbler. A shower is the signal for all the birds to let themselves go and have a spree. No bird then minds how untidy it is, for it knows that there is none to point the finger of scorn at it; all are in the same boat, or, at any rate, in the same shower of rain. So each one makes the most of the period of licence. The most staid birds splash about in puddles and revel in the experience in much the same way as a child enjoys paddling on the seashore.

What a change a rain shower brings to the myna. Usually, the bird looks as sharp as a lifeguard; its feathers are so neat and tidy that even the strictest critic couldn't find anything to complain about. But after ten minutes of rain, the myna looks as messy as a chatterbox. A rain shower is the signal for all the birds to let loose and have fun. No bird cares how scruffy it gets because it knows no one will judge it; they’re all in the same situation, or at least in the same rain shower. So each bird makes the most of this time of freedom. Even the most serious birds splash around in puddles and enjoy the experience just like a child playing at the beach.

And when the rain is over, what a shaking and preening of feathers there is! What a general brushing up! The bird world seems for a time to have turned itself into a toilet club. Presently, the last arcana of the toilet being completed, the birds come forth looking as fresh and sweet as an English meadow when the sun shines upon it after a summer shower.

And when the rain is gone, there's such a flapping and fluffing of feathers! Everyone is getting cleaned up! The bird world for a moment feels like a grooming salon. Soon, once the final touches are done, the birds come out looking as fresh and lovely as an English meadow under the sun after a summer rain.

Then there are all the good things which the rain [182] brings with it. How luscious and sweet the fruit must taste when the raindrops have washed away all the dust and other impurities with defile it. What a multitude of edible creeping things does a shower bring forth. In England it causes to emerge all manner of grubs and worms which before had been lurking in their burrows. In India is it not the rain that ushers in the red-letter day for insectivorous birds—the day that witnesses the swarming of the “white ants”? What a feast do these myriads of termites provide for the feathered things. In addition to these there is all the multitude of winged and crawling insects which the rain brings to life as if by magic. How badly would the birds fare but for the barsath which brings forth these insects, upon which they are able to feed their young.

Then there are all the good things that the rain brings with it. How juicy and sweet the fruit must taste after the raindrops have washed away all the dust and other impurities that dirty it. What a variety of edible crawling things a shower brings forth. In England, it causes all kinds of grubs and worms to emerge that had been hiding in their burrows. In India, isn’t it the rain that marks the special day for insect-eating birds—the day that the “white ants” swarm? What a feast these countless termites provide for the birds. Plus, there’s all the variety of flying and crawling insects that the rain brings to life as if by magic. How poorly would the birds do without the barsath that brings forth these insects, which they feed their young.

Perhaps the hoopoes most of all appreciate the rain, for it makes the ground so delightfully soft; they are then able with such ease to plunge their long beaks into the earth and extract all manner of hidden treasures which are usually most difficult of access.

Perhaps the hoopoes appreciate the rain the most, since it makes the ground wonderfully soft; they can easily plunge their long beaks into the earth and dig up all sorts of hidden treasures that are usually hard to reach.

Is there anything in the world more complete than the happiness of birds in a shower of rain?

Is there anything in the world more perfect than the happiness of birds in a rain shower?

[183]


The Weaver Bird

The weaver bird has, thanks to its marvellous nest, a world-wide reputation. It is related to our ubiquitous friend the house sparrow, and is known to men of science as Ploceus baya.

The weaver bird is famous around the world for its amazing nest. It's related to our common companion, the house sparrow, and is scientifically known as Ploceus baya.

Except at the breeding season, the weaver bird looks rather like an overgrown sparrow, and frequently passes as such. But the cock decks himself out in gay attire when he goes a-courting. The feathers of his head become golden, while his breast turns bright yellow if he be an elderly gentleman, or rusty red if he still possess the fire of youth.

Except during breeding season, the weaver bird looks pretty much like a large sparrow and is often mistaken for one. But the male gets all dressed up when he’s trying to attract a mate. The feathers on his head turn golden, while his chest becomes bright yellow if he’s an older bird, or rusty red if he’s still young and vibrant.

Weaver birds are found all over India. In most parts they seem to shun the haunts of man, but in Burma they frequent gardens. Jerdon mentions a house in Rangoon which had at one time over one hundred weaver birds’ nests suspended from the thatch of the roof! In India proper the favourite site for a nest is a tree that overhangs water. Toddy palms are most commonly chosen, but in Northern India, where palms are but rarely seen, a babul tree is usually utilised.

Weaver birds are found throughout India. In most areas, they tend to avoid human spaces, but in Burma, they often visit gardens. Jerdon notes a house in Rangoon that once had over one hundred weaver bird nests hanging from the roof thatch! In India, the preferred location for a nest is a tree that hangs over water. Toddy palms are the most commonly chosen, but in Northern India, where palms are rarely seen, a babul tree is typically used.

Weaver birds or bayas, as they are invariably called by Hindustani-speaking people, live almost exclusively [184] on grain, hence they are easy birds to keep in captivity. Given a commodious aviary and plenty of grass, captive bayas amuse themselves by weaving their wonderful nests. They are, however, not very desirable as pets if they have to share a cage with other birds, for, as Colonel Cunningham remarks, “every weaver bird appears to be possessed by an innate desire to hammer in the head of his neighbour.” To this the neighbour is apt to take exception, so that unpleasantness ensues.

Weaver birds, or bayas as they are commonly called by Hindi speakers, primarily eat grain, making them easy to keep in captivity. With a spacious aviary and plenty of grass, captive bayas spend their time weaving their beautiful nests. However, they are not the best pets if they have to share a cage with other birds, because, as Colonel Cunningham points out, “every weaver bird seems to have an instinctive urge to peck at the head of its neighbor.” This often leads to conflicts with the neighbor, resulting in a lot of tension.

Natives frequently train bayas to do all manner of tricks.

Natives often train bayas to perform all sorts of tricks.

The man with performing birds is quite an institution in India. Parrots, bayas, and pigeons are most frequently trained.

The guy with the performing birds is quite a fixture in India. Parrots, bayas, and pigeons are the most commonly trained.

A very effective trick, which is performed alike by parrots and weaver birds, is the loading and firing of a miniature cannon. First the bird places some grains of powder in the muzzle of the cannon, then it rams these home with a ramrod. It next takes a lighted match from its master, which it applies to the touch-hole. The result is a report loud enough to scare every crow in the neighbourhood, but the little baya will remain perched on the gun, having apparently thoroughly enjoyed the performance.

A really impressive trick, done by both parrots and weaver birds, is loading and firing a small cannon. First, the bird puts some powder into the cannon's muzzle, then it uses a ramrod to pack it down. Next, it takes a lit match from its owner and puts it to the touch-hole. The result is a loud bang that can scare off every crow in the area, but the little baya just sits on the gun, seeming to have thoroughly enjoyed the show.

The nest of the baya is one of the most wonderful things in nature. Description is unnecessary. Every one who has been in India has seen dozens of the hanging flask-shaped structures, while those who know not the Gorgeous East must be acquainted with the nest from pictures.

The baya's nest is one of the most amazing things in nature. There's no need to describe it. Everyone who has been to India has seen dozens of the hanging flask-shaped structures, while those unfamiliar with the Beautiful East must know the nest from pictures.

On account of its champagne-bottle shaped nest, the [185] weaver is sometimes known as the bottle bird; I have also heard it called the hedge sparrow.

On account of its champagne-bottle shaped nest, the [185] weaver is sometimes known as the bottle bird; I have also heard it called the hedge sparrow.

It makes no attempt to conceal its exquisitely woven nest. It relies for protection on inaccessibility, not concealment. Every animal badmash can see the nest, but cannot get at it. It hangs sufficiently high to be out of reach of all four-footed creatures. The ends of the entrance passage are frayed out so as to baffle all attempts on the part of squirrels and lizards to reach the treasures hidden away in it.

It doesn't try to hide its beautifully woven nest. Instead of concealment, it depends on being hard to reach for protection. Every troublemaker animal can see the nest but can't get to it. It's hung high enough to be out of the way of all four-legged creatures. The ends of the entrance are frayed to confuse squirrels and lizards, making it tough for them to access the treasures stored inside.

Both cock and hen work at the nest, the cock being the more industrious. The fibres of which it is composed are not found ready-made. The birds manufacture them out of the tall elephant grass which is so common in India. The weaver alights on one of the nearly upright blades and seizes with its beak a neighbouring blade near the base and makes a notch in it; it next seizes the edge of the blade above the notch and jerks its head away. By this means it strips off a thin strand of the leaf; it then proceeds to tear off in a similar manner a second strand, retaining the first one in its beak; in precisely the same way a third and perhaps a fourth strand are stripped off. The tearing process is not always continued to the extreme end of the blade; the various strands sometimes remain attached to the tip of the blade. The force with which the bird flies away usually suffices to complete the severance; sometimes, however, it is not effected so easily, and the bird is pulled back and swings in the air suspended by the strands it holds in its bill. Nothing daunted, the weaver makes a second attempt [186] to fly away, and if this is not successful, continues until its efforts are crowned with success.

Both the male and female work on the nest, with the male being the more diligent. The materials used for the nest aren't found pre-made. The birds create them from the tall elephant grass commonly found in India. The weaver lands on one of the nearly upright blades and grabs a nearby blade near the base with its beak, making a notch in it; then it takes hold of the edge of the blade above the notch and jerks its head away. This action strips off a thin strand of the leaf; it then goes on to similarly tear off a second strand while keeping the first one in its beak. It continues this process to obtain a third and maybe even a fourth strand. The tearing doesn't always go all the way to the end of the blade; sometimes, the strands stay attached to the tip of the blade. The force with which the bird flies away is usually enough to finish the job, but sometimes it's not that easy, and the bird gets pulled back, swinging in the air held up by the strands in its beak. Undeterred, the weaver tries to fly away again, and if that doesn’t work, it keeps going until it succeeds.

The grass which is used in nest construction is impregnated with silicon to such an extent that I experienced considerable difficulty in extricating from my pocket some of the fibres which, on one occasion, I took home with me. The material is thus eminently suitable for weaving purposes.

The grass used for making nests is so soaked in silicon that I had a hard time getting some of the fibers out of my pocket when I took them home one time. This material is therefore really great for weaving.

The fibres first collected are securely wound round the branch or leaf from which the nest will hang. The fibres added subsequently are plaited together until a stalk four or five inches long is formed; this is then expanded into a bell-shaped structure. The bell constitutes the roof of the nursery. When the roof is completed a loop is constructed across its base, so that the nest at this stage may be likened to an inverted basket with a handle.

The fibers collected first are tightly wrapped around the branch or leaf from which the nest will hang. The fibers added later are braided together until a stalk about four or five inches long is formed; this is then expanded into a bell-shaped structure. The bell serves as the roof of the nursery. Once the roof is finished, a loop is created across its base, making the nest look like an upside-down basket with a handle at this stage.

Up to this point the cock and hen do the same kind of work, both fetch strips of grass or of palm leaves and weave these into the structure of the nest. But when once the loop or cross-bar is completed the hen takes up a position on it and makes the cock do all the bringing of material. She henceforth works from the interior of the nest and he from the exterior.

Up to this point, the rooster and hen do the same kind of work; both gather strips of grass or palm leaves and weave them into the nest structure. But once the loop or cross-bar is finished, the hen takes her place on it and makes the rooster do all the collecting. From then on, she works from inside the nest while he works from outside.

They push the fibres through the walls to one another. Thus the work progresses very rapidly. On one side of the loop the bell is closed up so as to form a chamber in which the eggs are laid, and the other half is prolonged into a neck, which becomes the entrance to the nest. This may be nearly a foot long; six inches is, however, a more usual length.

They push the fibers through the walls to each other. This makes the work go very quickly. On one side of the loop, the bell is sealed to create a chamber where the eggs are laid, and the other half extends into a neck, which serves as the entrance to the nest. This neck can be almost a foot long, but six inches is more typical.

[187]

The entrance to the nursery is thus from below. The way the owners shoot vertically upwards into it, with closed wings, without perceptibly shaking it is really marvellous.

The entrance to the nursery is located below. The way the owners shoot straight up into it, with their wings closed, without noticeably shaking it, is truly amazing.

Nest construction obviously gives the little builders great pleasure. They frequently build supernumerary nests, purely from the joy of building. Each time the cock bird approaches the nest with a beakful of material he cries out with delight. Every now and again in the midst of weaving material into the structure of the nest he bursts into song.

Nest building clearly brings a lot of joy to the little builders. They often create extra nests just for the fun of it. Every time the male bird comes to the nest with a beakful of materials, he lets out a cheerful call. Occasionally, while he's weaving materials into the nest, he breaks into song.

Weaver birds usually build in company; ten or a dozen different nests being found in the same tree. As each little craftsman is in a very excited state, fights between neighbouring cocks frequently ensue, but these are never of a serious nature. I was once the witness of an amusing piece of wickedness on the part of a cock baya. The bird in question flew to a branch near the nest belonging to another pair of weaver birds who were absent. After contemplating it for a little he flew to the nest, and having deliberately wrenched away a piece of it with his beak, made off with the stolen property and worked it into his own nest! Four times did he visit his neighbour’s nest and commit larceny; two of the stolen strands he utilised and the remaining ones fell to the ground. I am inclined to think that the thief was actuated by motives of jealousy; for he deliberately dropped some of the stolen material on to the ground and extracted it from the place at which the nest was attached to its branch, thus weakening its attachment. The victim of the outrage on his [188] return did not appear to notice that anything was amiss.

Weaver birds usually build their nests together, with ten or a dozen different nests found in the same tree. Since each little builder is in an excited mood, fights between nearby males often happen, but these are never serious. I once witnessed a funny act of mischief by a male baya. This bird flew to a branch near another pair of weaver birds' nest while they were away. After thinking it over for a moment, he flew to the nest and, deliberately pulling a piece off with his beak, took the stolen material and added it to his own nest! He returned to his neighbor’s nest four times to steal; he used two of the stolen strands while the others fell to the ground. I believe this thief was motivated by jealousy, since he purposefully dropped some of the stolen bits on the ground and pulled them from where the nest was attached to its branch, weakening its hold. When the victim returned, he didn’t seem to notice that anything was wrong.

Not the least interesting feature of the nest is the clay which is studded about it in lumps. In one nest Jerdon found no fewer than six of these lumps, weighing in all three ounces. The clay has, I think, three uses: it helps to balance the nest, it prevents it being blown about by every gust of wind, and keeps it steady while the bird is entering it.

Not the least interesting feature of the nest is the clay that is scattered around it in clumps. In one nest, Jerdon found at least six of these clumps, weighing a total of three ounces. I think the clay has three purposes: it helps balance the nest, prevents it from being blown around by every gust of wind, and keeps it steady while the bird is entering it.

A story is abroad, and is repeated in nearly every popular book on ornithology, to the effect that the weaver bird sticks fireflies on these lumps of clay, and thus illuminates the nursery, or renders it terrifying to predacious creatures. Jerdon scoffs at this firefly story, and I, too, am unable to accept it. Nevertheless it is so universally believed by the natives of India that there must be some foundation for it.

A story is going around, and it's mentioned in almost every popular book on birds, saying that the weaver bird sticks fireflies on these lumps of clay to light up the nursery or scare away predators. Jerdon laughs off this firefly tale, and I can't accept it either. Still, it's so widely believed by the locals in India that there must be some truth to it.

Some time ago a correspondent living on the West Coast of India informed me that weaver birds are very abundant in that part of the country, that their nests are everywhere to be seen, and that he had noticed fireflies stuck into many of them. He asked if I could explain their presence. I suggested in reply that he had made a mistake and requested him to look carefully next nesting season, that is to say in August, and, if he came upon a single nest on to which a firefly was stuck, to take it down, fireflies and all, and send it to me at my expense. Since then August has come and gone thrice, and I have heard nothing from my correspondent! Thus it is that I am still among those that disbelieve the firefly story.

Some time ago, someone I know who lives on the West Coast of India told me that weaver birds are really common there, that their nests are found everywhere, and that he had seen fireflies stuck in many of them. He asked me to explain why that might be. I replied that he must have made a mistake and asked him to take a closer look during the next nesting season, which is in August, and if he found even one nest with a firefly stuck to it, to take it down, fireflies and all, and send it to me at my expense. Since then, August has come and gone three times, and I haven’t heard anything from him! So, I’m still among those who don’t believe the firefly story.

[189]

My theory is that the bird brings the clay to the nest in its bill in a moist condition. Now wet clay retains moisture for some time and would shine quite brightly in the moonlight, so might easily be mistaken for a firefly. Unfortunately the weaver bird is not common where I am now stationed, so that I have not had an opportunity of putting this theory to the test. I have, however, noticed how the nests built by solitary wasps shine when the clay that composes them is wet.

My theory is that the bird brings wet clay to the nest in its beak. Wet clay holds moisture for a while and can shine quite brightly in the moonlight, making it easy to confuse with a firefly. Unfortunately, the weaver bird isn't common where I'm currently located, so I haven't had the chance to test this theory. However, I've noticed how the nests built by solitary wasps shine when the clay they're made of is wet.

The natives of Northern India attribute great medicinal value to the nest of the weaver bird. They assert that a baby will never suffer from boils if it be once washed in water in which a weaver bird’s nest has been boiled!

The people of Northern India believe that the nest of the weaver bird has significant medicinal properties. They claim that a baby will never get boils if it is washed in water that has been used to boil a weaver bird's nest!

A great many half-finished weaver birds’ nests are seen in India. Most of these are the work of the cock, who thus amuses himself while his wife is incubating. A few are nests which have gone wrong, nests which do not balance nicely and so have not been completed.

A lot of half-finished weaver birds' nests can be seen in India. Most of these are built by the male, who entertains himself while his mate is incubating. Some are nests that didn't turn out well, nests that aren't balanced properly and therefore haven't been completed.

Two eggs are usually laid; they are pure white and without any gloss. On these the hen sits very closely. On one occasion Hume took home a very fine specimen of the nest and hung it from one of a pair of antlers on his dining-room wall. Three days later the inmates of the bungalow became aware of a very unpleasant odour, which was traced to the nest. On taking it down it was found to contain a female baya dead upon two dead half-hatched chicks.

Two eggs are typically laid; they are completely white and have no shine. The hen sits on them very closely. One time, Hume brought home a beautiful example of the nest and hung it from a pair of antlers on his dining-room wall. Three days later, the people in the bungalow noticed a very unpleasant smell, which was traced back to the nest. When it was taken down, it was found to have a dead female baya lying on top of two dead half-hatched chicks.

[190]


Green Parrots

Green parrots, as the long-tailed paroquets of India are popularly called, although fairly abundant during the cold weather, cannot be said to be common birds in Madras. This is a small mercy, for which all Madrassis should be duly thankful. The green parrot is one of those good things of which it is possible to have too much. Where the beautiful birds are not too plentiful they are always greatly admired and considered most pleasing additions to the landscape; where they abound most people find it difficult to speak of them in parliamentary language.

Green parrots, commonly known as the long-tailed paroquets of India, are somewhat common during the cooler months, but they aren't considered everyday birds in Madras. This is a small blessing that everyone in Madras should appreciate. The green parrot is one of those nice things that can easily become overwhelming. In places where these beautiful birds aren’t too numerous, they are highly admired and seen as lovely enhancements to the scenery; however, in areas where they are plentiful, people often struggle to discuss them politely.

The Punjab is the happy hunting-ground of green parrots. I am now in a station where these birds probably outnumber the crows, where we are literally steeped in green parrots, where we hear nothing else all day long save their screeches and chuckles.

The Punjab is the perfect place for green parrots. I'm currently in a location where these birds probably outnumber the crows, where we are completely surrounded by green parrots, and where we hear nothing but their screeches and chuckles all day long.

THE GREY-NECKED CROW. (CORVUS SPLENDENS)

THE GREY-NECKED CROW. (CORVUS SPLENDENS)

The Grey-Necked Crow (Corvus Splendens)

[191]

Green parrots owe their unpopularity to their mischievousness and their noisiness. “In their malignant love of destruction and mischief,” writes Colonel Cunningham, “they run crows very hard, and seem only to fall short of that standard through the happy ordinance that their mental development has halted a good way behind that of their rivals. They are, therefore, incapable of devising such manifold and elaborate schemes of mischief as the crows work out, but in so far as intent and disinterested love of evil goes, there is not a pin to choose between them. They take the same heart-whole delight in destruction for destruction’s sake, and find the same bliss in tormenting and annoying other living things.” While fully endorsing the above, I feel constrained to remark that the parrot is no fool; he may not be quite as ’cute as an Indian crow, but he is gifted with sufficient brain-power for all practical purposes. If the green parrot is less harmfully mischievous than the crow he is far more offensively noisy. He is able to produce an almost endless variety of sounds, but unfortunately there is not a single one among them all which by any stretch of the imagination can be called musical.

Green parrots aren't very popular because they're mischievous and loud. “In their spiteful love for destruction and chaos,” writes Colonel Cunningham, “they give crows a run for their money and seem to come up just short of that standard because their mental development lags behind that of their rivals. So, while they can't come up with the same complex and crafty schemes of mischief that crows can, in terms of intent and a pure love for evil, they are just as bad. They take genuine pleasure in destruction for its own sake and find joy in tormenting and annoying other living creatures.” While I completely agree with the above, I have to point out that the parrot isn't a fool; while it may not be as clever as an Indian crow, it has enough brainpower for practical purposes. If the green parrot is less harmfully mischievous than the crow, it's definitely much more annoyingly loud. It can make an almost endless variety of sounds, but unfortunately, none of them could be considered musical by any stretch of the imagination.

All species of green parrots have similar habits. All are gregarious and feed almost exclusively on fruit and seeds. They do much damage to the crops, destroying more than they eat, since they have a way of breaking off a head of corn, eating a few grains, and then attacking another head. Where green parrots are plentiful the long-suffering ryot sets them down among the ills to which the flesh is heir. When the crops are cut the parrots feed among the stubble, picking up the fallen grain.

All species of green parrots have similar behaviors. They are social and primarily eat fruit and seeds. They cause significant damage to crops, destroying more than they consume, as they tend to break off a head of corn, eat a few grains, and then move on to another head. In areas where green parrots are abundant, the long-suffering ryot considers them one of the many troubles of life. After the crops are harvested, the parrots feed among the leftovers, gathering the fallen grain.

The exceedingly swift, arrow-like flight of the green parrot is too familiar to need description. The flocks usually fly high up, screaming loudly; at times, however, they skim along the ground; occasionally they thread their way among trees, avoiding the branches in the most wonderful manner, considering the pace at which they move.

The incredibly fast, arrow-like flight of the green parrot is well-known enough that it doesn’t need any description. The flocks typically fly high, making loud screams; at times, though, they fly just above the ground; occasionally, they weave through the trees, skillfully avoiding branches despite their high speed.

[192]

Very amusing it is to watch a little company of parrots in a tree. Sometimes the birds perch on the topmost branches and there chuckle to one another; at others they cling to the trunk, looking very comic, pressed up against the bark with tails outspread. Not infrequently one sees two of them sitting together in a tree indulging in a little mild flirtation, which, in green parrot communities, takes the form of head tickling. These birds are very skilled climbers; they move along the branches foot over foot, using the beak when they have to negotiate a difficult pass. Thus they clamber about, robbing the tree of its fruit and keeping up a running conversation. Suddenly the flock will take to its wings and fly off, screeching boisterously. The members of each little community seem to live in a state of rowdy good-fellowship. No one who watches parrots in a state of nature can doubt that existence affords them plenty of pleasure.

It's really entertaining to watch a small group of parrots in a tree. Sometimes, the birds sit on the highest branches and chuckle to each other; at other times, they cling to the trunk, looking pretty funny, pressed against the bark with their tails spread out. It’s not uncommon to see two of them sitting together in a tree engaging in a little light flirting, which, in parrot communities, involves head tickling. These birds are excellent climbers; they move along the branches foot by foot, using their beaks whenever they need to navigate a tricky spot. They scurry about, stealing the tree's fruit while maintaining a lively conversation. Suddenly, the flock will take flight, screeching loudly. The members of each little group seem to live in a state of rowdy camaraderie. Anyone who observes parrots in their natural habitat can’t doubt that life brings them a lot of joy.

Green parrots nest in January or February in Southern India, and somewhat later in the North. The courtship of the rose-ringed species is thus described by Captain Hutton: “At the pairing season the female becomes the most affected creature possible, twisting herself into all sorts of ridiculous postures, apparently to attract the notice of her sweetheart, and uttering a low twittering note the while, in the most approved style of flirtation, while her wings are half spread and her head kept rolling from side to side in demi-gyrations; the male sitting quietly by her side, looking on with wonder as if fairly taken aback—and wondering to see her make such a guy of herself. I have watched them during [193] these courtships until I have felt humiliated at seeing how closely the follies of mankind resembled those of the brute creation. The only return the male made to these antics was scratching the top of her head with the point of his beak, and joining his bill to hers in a loving kiss.”

Green parrots nest in January or February in Southern India, and a bit later in the North. The courtship of the rose-ringed species is described by Captain Hutton like this: “During the pairing season, the female becomes incredibly affected, twisting herself into all sorts of silly poses to get her sweetheart's attention, while letting out a low twittering sound, showing off her flirtation skills. Her wings are half spread, and her head rolls from side to side in little spins; the male sits quietly beside her, watching in amazement as if he's totally taken aback, wondering why she’s making such a fool of herself. I've watched them during these courtships and felt embarrassed at how closely human follies resemble those of the animal kingdom. The only response the male had to her antics was scratching the top of her head with the tip of his beak and giving her a loving kiss by joining his bill to hers.”

Note that it is the hen that makes the advances. There can be no mistake about this, for the presence of the rose-coloured ring round the neck enables us to distinguish at a glance the cock from the hen.

Note that it's the hen that makes the advances. There's no doubt about this because the rose-colored ring around the neck allows us to instantly tell the difference between the rooster and the hen.

The more I see of birds the more convinced do I become that, in the matter of selecting mates, the hens do not have things all their own way. In monogamous species the cock frequently chooses his spouse; selection is mutual.

The more I observe birds, the more convinced I am that, when it comes to choosing partners, the hens don’t have everything their way. In monogamous species, the male often picks his mate; it’s a mutual selection.

The nest is a cavity in a tree, and is thus described by Hume: “The mouth of the hole, which is circular and very neatly cut and, say, two inches on the average in diameter, is sometimes in the trunk, sometimes in some large bough, and not unfrequently in the lower surface of the latter. It generally goes straight in for two to four inches, and then turns downwards for from six inches to three feet. The lower or chamber portion of the hole is never less than four or five inches in diameter, and is often a large natural hollow, three or four times these dimensions, into which the bird has cut its usual neat passage.”

The nest is a hollow in a tree, and Hume describes it this way: “The entrance of the hole, which is round and very neatly shaped, is about two inches in diameter on average. Sometimes it's in the trunk, sometimes in a large branch, and often on the underside of the latter. It typically goes straight in for two to four inches, then bends downwards for anywhere from six inches to three feet. The lower part or chamber of the hole is never less than four or five inches wide and is often a large natural hollow, three or four times that size, into which the bird has made its usual neat entrance.”

My experience differs from that of Hume, inasmuch as it tends to show that green parrots do not excavate their own holes, or even the entrances to them. I suppose I have seen over a hundred green parrots’ nests, [194] and all have been in existing hollows. Green parrots frequently evict the squirrels which tenant a cavity in a tree and use it for nesting purposes.

My experience is different from Hume's because it suggests that green parrots don’t dig their own holes or even the entrances to them. I think I’ve seen over a hundred green parrot nests, and all of them were in already existing hollows. Green parrots often kick squirrels out of a cavity in a tree and then use it for nesting. [194]

They sometimes nest in holes in buildings. There is in Lahore an old half-ruined gateway, known as the Chauburgi. In this dozens of green parrots nest simultaneously.

They sometimes nest in holes in buildings. In Lahore, there's an old half-ruined gateway called the Chauburgi. In this, dozens of green parrots nest at the same time.

The rose-ringed paroquet (Palæornis torquatus) seems usually to nest in trees, while the larger Alexandrine paroquet (Palæornis nepalensis) nests by preference in holes in buildings.

The rose-ringed parakeet (Palæornis torquatus) typically nests in trees, while the larger Alexandrine parakeet (Palæornis nepalensis) prefers to nest in holes in buildings.

The nest hole is not lined.

The nest hole isn't cushioned.

Four white eggs are usually laid. Both parents take turns at incubation.

Four white eggs are typically laid. Both parents take turns incubating them.

Parrots are birds which thrive remarkably well in captivity. This, I fear, is a doubtful blessing, for it leads to a vast number of the birds being taken prisoner. Many of those which are kept by natives, and even some kept by Europeans, are, I am afraid, cruelly treated. It is true that the cruelty is in many cases unintentional, but this does not afford the poor captive much consolation.

Parrots are birds that do incredibly well in captivity. Unfortunately, this is a questionable blessing, as it results in many of these birds being taken prisoner. Many of those kept by locals, and even some by Europeans, are, sadly, treated cruelly. While it’s true that the cruelty is often unintentional, that doesn’t bring much comfort to the poor captive.

Parrot-catching is a profitable occupation in India; since nestlings fetch from four to eight annas each. Thousands of young birds are dragged out of their nurseries every year and sold in the bazaars.

Parrot-catching is a lucrative job in India; since baby parrots sell for four to eight annas each. Thousands of young birds are taken out of their nests every year and sold in the markets.

Nor are the young birds immune from capture after they have left the nest. They roost for a few nights in company before dispersing themselves over the face of the country. The wily bird-catcher marks down one of these nesting spots—he has possibly had to pay rent for [195] it, for parrot-catching is quite a profession, so large is the demand for captive birds—and then sets in likely places split pieces of bamboo smeared over with bird-lime. When daybreak comes the unlucky birds that have chanced to roost on the limed bamboos find that they cannot get away, that they are stuck to their perches!

Nor are the young birds safe from being caught after they leave the nest. They spend a few nights roosting together before spreading out across the land. The clever bird-catcher identifies one of these roosting areas—he might even have to pay for the spot, as parrot-catching is a real business due to the high demand for pet birds—and then places split bamboo tubes coated with bird-lime in strategic locations. When morning comes, the unfortunate birds that decided to roost on the sticky bamboo find that they can’t escape and are trapped on their perches!

Natives of India are very fond of taming parrots. They capture the birds at an age when they are unable to feed themselves. These young parrots are considered as members of the family, and are allowed to roam about at large in the room in which their master lives. They make a great noise and so are not very desirable pets.

Natives of India really enjoy taming parrots. They catch the birds when they are too young to feed themselves. These young parrots are seen as part of the family and are allowed to move around freely in the room where their owner lives. They can be quite noisy, so they’re not the most desirable pets.

I am sometimes asked by those who keep parrots how to make them talk. This is not an easy question to answer. Some birds are much more ready to learn than others. I do not consider that the various Indian species make such good talkers as some other kinds, as, for example, the West African parrot—the grey one with the red tail. Nevertheless, what follows applies indiscriminately to all species of parrot. If you want to make a bird learn quickly to talk, use plenty of bad language before it. It is really wonderful how rapidly a parrot will pick up swear words. There appears to be an incisiveness about them which appeals to parrot nature. As a rule it requires much patience to teach a parrot anything except profanity. Constant repetition of the same sound before the bird is necessary. The gramophone is said to make the best teacher. The instrument should be made to repeat slowly and steadily [196] the phrase it is desired to teach the bird, and placed quite close to the parrot’s cage, which should be covered up. A word of warning to those who try this up-to-date method of instruction. Polly’s lesson should not last much longer than ten minutes, and only one a day be given; otherwise the poor bird may get brain fever.

I’m sometimes asked by people who own parrots how to teach them to talk. This isn't an easy question to answer. Some birds are much quicker to learn than others. I don’t think the various Indian species are as good at talking as some others, like the West African parrot—the gray one with the red tail. Still, what I’m about to say applies to all parrot species. If you want to help a bird learn to talk quickly, use lots of bad language around it. It’s amazing how quickly a parrot will pick up swear words. There seems to be something about them that appeals to parrots. Generally, teaching a parrot anything other than profanity requires a lot of patience. You need to constantly repeat the same sound in front of the bird. People say that a record player makes the best teacher. The player should be set to slowly and steadily repeat the phrase you want the bird to learn, and it should be placed very close to the parrot’s cage, which should be covered up. A word of caution for those trying out this modern teaching method: Polly’s lesson shouldn’t last much longer than ten minutes, and only one lesson a day should be given; otherwise, the poor bird could get brain fever.

[197]


SPARROWS' NIGHTTIME ROOSTING

Most species of birds like to roost in companies, partly because it is safer to do so, partly for the sake of companionship, and sometimes, in England at any rate, because by crowding together they keep each other warm. Birds have their favourite roosting places. Certain trees are patronised while others are not. Perhaps one clump will be utilised every night for a month or longer, then a move will be made to another clump. Later on a return may be made to the original site. I do not know what determines these changes of locality.

Most bird species prefer to roost together, partly because it’s safer, partly for company, and sometimes, at least in England, because huddling together helps them stay warm. Birds have their favorite roosting spots. Some trees are used regularly, while others are ignored. A particular spot might be used every night for a month or more, then they’ll move to a different spot. Later, they might return to the original location. I’m not sure what causes these changes in location.

The sunset hour is, I think, the most interesting at which to watch birds. They seem to be livelier then than at any other time of the day; they are certainly more loquacious. The dormitory of the crows, the mynas, or the green parrots is a perfect pandemonium. Whilst listening to the uproar one can only suppose each member of the colony to be bubbling over with animal spirits and intent on recounting to his fellows all the doings of the day.

The sunset hour is, in my opinion, the most exciting time to watch birds. They definitely seem more active then than at any other part of the day; they're certainly more talkative. The roosting place of the crows, the mynas, or the green parrots is complete chaos. While listening to the noise, one can only imagine that each member of the group is overflowing with energy and eager to share everything that happened during the day with the others.

Most people may be inclined to think that it is impossible to derive much pleasure from observing so common a bird as the sparrow. This is a mistake. Often and often have I watched with the greatest [198] pleasure the roosting operations of this despised bird. I know of a row of bushes that forms the dormitory of hundreds of sparrows. To enable the reader to appreciate what follows, let me say that the hedge in question is only some twenty yards long, its height is not much greater than that of a man, it is nowhere more than eight feet in breadth, and is within a hundred yards of an inhabited bungalow. Less than six yards away from it is a well, fitted with a creaking Persian wheel, at which coolies are continually working.

Most people might think it’s impossible to find much enjoyment in watching such a common bird as the sparrow. That’s a mistake. Time and again, I’ve found great joy in observing the roosting habits of this often-maligned bird. There’s a line of bushes that serves as the sleeping spot for hundreds of sparrows. To help you understand what follows, let me point out that this hedge is only about twenty yards long, its height is just slightly taller than a person, it’s no wider than eight feet at any point, and it’s located within a hundred yards of a house. Less than six yards away is a well, equipped with a creaking Persian wheel, where workers are constantly busy.

If you happen to pass this hedge within an hour of sunset, you will hear issuing from it the dissonance of many sparrows’ voices. You stop to listen, and, as you wait, a flock of sparrows dives into the thicket. You look about to see whether any more are coming and observe nothing. Suddenly some specks appear in the air, as if spontaneously generated. In two seconds these are seen to be sparrows. Within half a minute of the time you first set eyes upon them they are already in the bushes. They are followed by another little flock of six or seven, and another and another. Flight after flight arrives in quick succession, each of which shoots into the roosting hedge. I use the word “shoot” advisedly, for no other term describes the speed at which they enter the bushes. Their flight, although so rapid, is not direct; it takes the form of a quavering zigzag. Some of the flocks do not immediately plunge into the bushes. They circle once, twice, thrice, or even oftener, before they betake themselves to their leafy dormitory. Sometimes part of a flight dive into the hedge immediately upon arrival, while the remainder circle round [199] and then fling themselves into the bushes as though they were soldiers performing a well-practised manœuvre; the first bird to reach the bush entering at the nearest end, the next a little farther on, the third still farther, and so on, so that the last sparrow to arrive enters the hedge at the far end. Sometimes a flock perches for a time on a tree near by before entering the hedge. Those who have only noticed sparrows pottering about will scarcely be able to believe their eyes when they see the speed at which they approach the roosting place. For the moment they are transformed into dignified birds.

If you happen to walk by this hedge around sunset, you’ll hear the jumbled voices of many sparrows coming from it. You stop to listen, and while you wait, a group of sparrows dives into the thicket. You look around to see if any more are coming and notice nothing. Suddenly, some tiny dots appear in the air, as if they just appeared out of nowhere. In just two seconds, you realize these are sparrows. Within half a minute of spotting them, they're already in the bushes. They’re followed by another small group of six or seven, then another, and another. Groups keep arriving one after the other, each shooting into the roosting hedge. I say "shoot" on purpose, because no other word captures how quickly they dart into the bushes. Their flight, though fast, isn’t straightforward; instead, it zigzags back and forth. Some flocks don’t immediately plunge into the bushes. They circle once, twice, three times, or even more before settling into their leafy beds. Sometimes part of a group dives into the hedge right away, while the rest circle around and then launch themselves into the bushes like soldiers executing a well-rehearsed maneuver; the first bird to reach the bush enters at the nearest end, the next a bit farther along, the third even farther, and so on, so that the last sparrow to arrive goes in at the far end. Occasionally, a flock will perch for a bit on a nearby tree before heading into the hedge. Those who have only seen sparrows lazily moving around will hardly believe their eyes when they witness how quickly they approach their resting spot. For a moment, they seem transformed into dignified birds.

All this time those individuals already in the hedge are making a great noise. Their chitter, chitter, chitter never for a moment ceases or even diminishes in intensity. Once in the hedge, the sparrows do not readily leave it. There is much motion of the leaves and branches, and birds are continually popping out of one part of the bushes into another. It is thus evident that there is considerable fighting for places. If, while all this is going on, you walk up to part of the hedge and shake it, the birds disturbed will only fly a yard or two and at once settle elsewhere in the thicket.

All this time, the birds already in the hedge are making a lot of noise. Their constant chatter never stops or even eases up. Once the sparrows are in the hedge, they don’t easily leave. The leaves and branches are in constant motion, with birds frequently darting from one spot to another in the bushes. It's clear that there's a lot of competition for space. If, while all this is happening, you walk up to the hedge and shake it, the startled birds will just fly a yard or two and quickly settle somewhere else in the thicket.

Meanwhile the sun has nearly set; the coolies near by have ceased working and are kindling a fire within a couple of yards of the bushes. But the sparrows appear to ignore both them and their fire. Settling down for the night engrosses their whole attention.

Meanwhile, the sun is almost down; the laborers nearby have stopped working and are starting a fire a few yards away from the bushes. But the sparrows seem to ignore both them and their fire. Getting ready for the night occupies all their attention.

As the sun touches the horizon the incoming flights of sparrows become fewer and fewer; and after the golden orb has disappeared only one or two belated stragglers [200] arrive. Sparrows are early roosters. Something approaching three thousand of them are now perched in that small hedge, yet none are visible except those that pop in and out, when jockeyed out of positions they have taken up. But although only a few sparrows come in after the sun has set, it is not until fully fifteen minutes later that there is any appreciable abatement of the din. It then becomes more spasmodic; it ceases for half a second, to burst forth again with undiminished intensity.

As the sun dips below the horizon, the number of sparrows flying in begins to dwindle; and once the golden orb has vanished, only a few latecomers show up. Sparrows are early birds. Around three thousand of them are now sitting in that small hedge, yet you can only see those that pop in and out as they're pushed from their spots. Even though just a handful of sparrows arrive after sunset, it takes a solid fifteen minutes before the noise noticeably quiets down. It becomes more sporadic; it stops for half a second, only to erupt again with the same intensity.

Twenty minutes or so after sunset the clamour becomes suddenly less. It is now possible to discern individual voices. The noise grows rapidly feebler. It almost ceases, but again becomes louder. It then nearly stops a second time. Perhaps not more than twenty voices are heard. There is yet another outburst, but the twitterers are by now very sleepy. Suddenly there is perfect silence for a few seconds, then more feeble twittering, then another silence longer than the last.

Twenty minutes or so after sunset, the noise suddenly quiets down. You can now hear individual voices. The sounds quickly fade away. It nearly stops, but then gets louder again. It comes close to stopping once more. You can hear maybe twenty voices. There’s another burst of noise, but the chirpers are getting really sleepy by now. Suddenly, there’s perfect silence for a few seconds, followed by faint chirping, then another silence that lasts longer than the last one.

It is not yet dark, there is still a bright glow in the western sky. The periods of silence grow more prolonged and the outbursts of twittering become more faint and of shorter duration.

It’s not dark yet; there’s still a bright light in the western sky. The moments of silence are getting longer, and the bursts of chirping are becoming quieter and shorter.

It is now thirty-nine minutes after the sun has set and perfect stillness reigns. The birds must have all fallen asleep. But no! one wakeful fellow commences again. He soon subsides. It has grown so dark that you can no longer see the sparrow-hawk perched on a tree hard by. He took up his position there early in the evening, and will probably breakfast first thing to-morrow morning off sparrow!

It’s thirty-nine minutes past sunset, and everything is perfectly still. The birds must have all gone to sleep. But wait! One restless bird starts singing again. He quickly quiets down. It’s gotten so dark that you can’t see the sparrow-hawk sitting on a nearby tree anymore. He settled there early this evening and will likely have sparrow for breakfast tomorrow morning!

You now softly approach the bushes until your face [201] touches the branches. There are twenty or thirty sparrows roosting within fifteen inches of you. You cannot see any of them, but if you were to stretch forth your hand you could as likely as not catch hold of one. You disturb a branch and there is a rustling of a dozen pairs of wings, so close to you that your face is fanned by the wind they cause. You have disturbed some birds, but they are so sleepy that they move without uttering a twitter. You leave the bush and return an hour later. Perfect silence reigns. You may now go right up to the roosting hedge and talk without disturbing any of the three thousand birds. You may even strike a match without arousing one, so soundly do they sleep.

You now quietly approach the bushes until your face [201] brushes against the branches. There are twenty or thirty sparrows perched within fifteen inches of you. You can't see any of them, but if you reached out your hand, you could likely catch one. You disturb a branch and there's a rustling of a dozen pairs of wings, so close that the wind from them brushes against your face. You've disturbed some birds, but they're so sleepy that they move without making a sound. You leave the bush and come back an hour later. A perfect silence surrounds you. You can now walk right up to the hedgerow and talk without disturbing any of the three thousand birds. You could even strike a match without waking any of them, they're sleeping so soundly.

Those who wish to rid a locality of a superabundance of sparrows might well profit by the fact that the birds sleep so soundly in companies. Could anything be easier than to throw a large net over such a hedge and thus secure, at one fell blow, the whole colony?

Those who want to get rid of an excess of sparrows in an area could take advantage of the fact that the birds sleep so peacefully in groups. Is there anything easier than tossing a big net over a hedge and capturing the entire colony in one go?

[202]


THE GAY DECEIVER

The drongo cuckoo (Surniculus lugubris) is a bird of which I know practically nothing. I doubt whether I have ever seen it in the flesh. It is, of course, quite unnecessary to apologise for discoursing upon a subject of which one’s knowledge is admittedly nil. In this superficial age the most successful writers are those most ignorant of their subject. When you know only one or two facts it is quite easy to parade them properly, to set them forth to best advantage. They are so few and far between that there is no danger of their jostling one another or bewildering the reader. Then, if you are conversant only with one side of a question, you are able to lay down the law so forcibly, and the public likes having the law laid down for it, it does not mind how crude, how absurd, how impossible one’s sentiments are so long as one is cocksure of them and is not afraid to say so.

The drongo cuckoo (Surniculus lugubris) is a bird I know practically nothing about. I doubt I've ever seen it in person. It’s really unnecessary to apologize for discussing a topic you're clearly ignorant about. In today’s superficial world, the most successful writers often know the least about their subject. When you know just one or two facts, it’s easy to present them well and showcase them effectively. There are so few that they can’t clash with each other or confuse the reader. Plus, if you only understand one side of an issue, you can assert your opinion confidently, and the public likes having definitive opinions presented to them. They don’t mind how crude, absurd, or unrealistic those opinions are, as long as the person expressing them is confident and willing to speak up.

My lack of knowledge of the habits of the drongo cuckoo is, however, not my chief reason for desiring to write about it. I wish to discuss the bird because natural selectionists frequently cite it as bearing striking testimony to the truth of their theory, whereas it seems to me that it does just the opposite. Surniculus lugubris is, so far as I am able to judge, an uncompromising [203] opponent of those zoologists who pin their faith to the all-sufficiency of natural selection to account for evolution in the organic world.

My lack of knowledge about the habits of the drongo cuckoo isn’t the main reason I want to write about it. I want to talk about this bird because natural selectionists often point to it as strong evidence supporting their theory, but it seems to me that it actually does the opposite. Surniculus lugubris is, as far as I can tell, a staunch opponent of those zoologists who believe natural selection alone is enough to explain evolution in the natural world. [203]

The drongo cuckoo is as like the king-crow as one pea is to another. This bird, says Blanford, “is remarkable for its extraordinary resemblance in structure and colourisation to a drongo or king-crow (Dicrurus). The plumage is almost entirely black, and the tail forked owing to the lateral rectrices being turned outwards.” Blanford further declares that the bird, owing to its remarkable likeness to the king-crow, is apt to be overlooked.

The drongo cuckoo is as similar to the king-crow as one pea is to another. This bird, according to Blanford, “is notable for its striking resemblance in structure and color to a drongo or king-crow (Dicrurus). Its plumage is nearly all black, and its tail is forked because the side feathers are angled outward.” Blanford adds that this bird is often overlooked due to its remarkable likeness to the king-crow.

This being so, it is quite unnecessary for me to describe the drongo cuckoo; it is the image of a king-crow. But stay, perhaps there are some who do not know this last bird by sight. Such should make its acquaintance. They will find it sitting on the next telegraph wire they pass—a sprightly black bird, much smaller than the crow (with which it has no connection), possessing a long, forked tail. Every now and again it makes little sallies into the air after the “circling gnat,” or anything else insectivorous that presents itself. When you see such a bird you may safely bet on its being a king-crow; the off-chance of its proving a drongo cuckoo may be neglected by all but the ultra-cautious.

This being the case, I don't really need to describe the drongo cuckoo; it looks like a king-crow. But wait, maybe there are some who don't recognize that bird by sight. Those people should get to know it. They’ll find it sitting on the next telegraph wire they pass—a lively black bird, much smaller than a crow (which it isn't related to), with a long, forked tail. Every now and then, it takes little leaps into the air after the “circling gnat” or any other insect that comes along. When you see a bird like this, you can safely assume it’s a king-crow; the slight chance of it being a drongo cuckoo can be ignored by all but the overly cautious.

Not much is known of the habits of this cuckoo; but what we do know shows that, sometimes, at any rate, it makes the king-crow act as its nursemaid. Mr. Davison saw two king-crows feeding a young Surniculus. The consequence is that every book on natural history trots [204] out our friend the drongo cuckoo as an example of mimicry. The mimicry is, of course, unconscious: it is said to be the result of the action of natural selection.

Not much is known about the behavior of this cuckoo, but what we do know shows that sometimes, it gets the king-crow to act as its caregiver. Mr. Davison observed two king-crows feeding a young Surniculus. As a result, every book on natural history presents our friend the drongo cuckoo as an example of mimicry. The mimicry is, of course, unconscious; it’s thought to be the result of natural selection.

King-crows are, as every one knows, exceedingly pugnacious birds; at the nesting season both cock and hen are little furies, who guard the nursery most carefully and will not allow a strange species to so much as perch in the tree in which it is placed.

King-crows are, as everyone knows, extremely aggressive birds; during nesting season, both the male and female are like little fury balls, fiercely protecting their nest and refusing to let any unfamiliar species even land in the tree where it's located.

It is thus obvious that the cuckoo who elects to victimise a king-crow is undertaking a “big thing,” yet this is what Surniculus does. It accomplishes its aim by trickery; it becomes a gay deceiver, disguising itself like its dupe. Now I readily admit that the disguise may be of the utmost use to the Surniculus; I can well understand that natural selection will seize hold of the disguise when once it has been donned and possibly perfect it; but I cannot see how natural selection can have originated the disguise as such.

It’s clear that the cuckoo, which chooses to target a king-crow, is taking on a significant challenge, and that’s exactly what Surniculus does. It achieves its goal through deception; it becomes a clever trickster, masking itself like its victim. I readily acknowledge that this disguise can be extremely beneficial for the Surniculus; I can easily see how natural selection would take advantage of the disguise once it’s created and possibly enhance it. However, I struggle to understand how natural selection could have originated the disguise in the first place.

The drongo cuckoo may be called an ass in a lion’s skin, or a lion in an ass’s skin, whichever way one looks at things. When once the skin has been assumed natural selection may modify it so as better to fit the wearer; but more than this it cannot do.

The drongo cuckoo can be seen as an ass in a lion’s skin or a lion in an ass’s skin, depending on your perspective. Once the skin is taken on, natural selection might adjust it to better suit the wearer, but that’s all it can do.

I do not pretend to know the colour of the last common ancestor of all the cuckoos, but I do not believe that the colour was black. What, then, caused Surniculus lugubris to become black and assume a king-crow-like tail?

I don't claim to know the color of the last common ancestor of all cuckoos, but I don't think it was black. So, what made Surniculus lugubris turn black and develop a tail like a king crow?

A black feather or two, even if coupled with some lengthening of the tail, would in no way assist the [205] cuckoo in placing its egg in the drongo’s nest. Suppose an ass were to borrow the caudal appendage of the king of the forest, pin it on behind him, and then advance among his fellows with loud brays, would any donkey of average intelligence be misled by the feeble attempt at disguise? I think not. Much less would a king-crow be deceived by a few black feathers in the plumage of a cuckoo.

A black feather or two, even if paired with a longer tail, wouldn’t help the cuckoo at all in getting its egg into the drongo’s nest. Imagine if a donkey borrowed the tail of the king of the jungle, attached it to itself, and then walked among other donkeys making loud brays. Would any reasonably smart donkey be fooled by that weak attempt at disguise? I doubt it. Even less likely would a king crow be tricked by a few black feathers on a cuckoo.

I do not believe that natural selection has any direct connection with the nigritude of the drongo cuckoo. It is my opinion that, so far as the struggle for existence is concerned, it matters little to an animal what its colour be. Every creature has to be some colour: what that actual colour is must depend upon a great many factors; among these we may name the metabolic changes that go on inside the animal, its hereditary tendencies, sexual selection, and natural selection. Is it natural selection that has caused the king-crow to be black? I trow not.

I don’t think that natural selection is directly related to the dark color of the drongo cuckoo. In my view, when it comes to the struggle for survival, an animal's color doesn't really matter. Every creature has to be some color, and what that color actually is depends on many factors, including the metabolic changes happening inside the animal, its genetic traits, sexual selection, and natural selection. Did natural selection make the king-crow black? I doubt it.

The drongo is black because it is built that way; its tendency is to produce black feathers. Just as some men tend to put on flesh, so also some species of birds tend to grow black plumage. In the case of the king-crow sexual selection has possibly contributed to the bird’s nigritude. It is possible that black is a colour that appeals to king-crow ladies. “So neat, you know; a bird always looks well in black, and a forked tail gives him such an air of distinction.”

The drongo is black because that's just how it's made; it naturally produces black feathers. Just like some men tend to gain weight, some bird species tend to develop black feathers. For the king-crow, sexual selection may have played a role in its black coloration. It's possible that the color black attracts king-crow females. “It’s so stylish, you know; a bird always looks good in black, and a split tail gives it such a sense of elegance.”

As the hen drongo is a bird capable of looking after herself, even when incubating, there is no necessity for her to be protectively coloured. As I have repeatedly [206] declared, one ounce of good solid pugnacity is a better weapon in the struggle for existence than many pounds of protective colouration.

As the hen drongo is a bird that can take care of herself, even while she's incubating, there's no need for her to have protective coloring. As I've often stated, one ounce of true fierceness is a better tool for survival than many pounds of camouflage.

Again, in the case of king-crows nigritude may be an expression of vigour, the outward and visible sign of strength.

Again, in the case of king-crows, blackness might represent vitality, the clear and visible sign of strength.

Let me make myself clear. Suppose that in a race of savages those that had fair hair were stronger, bolder, more prolific, and more pushing than the dark-haired men. Fair hair, in some inexplicable way, always accompanied strength and the like. It is obvious that, under these conditions, the race would in time become fair-haired: the milder dark men would eventually be hustled out of existence. Fair hair would then be the outward expression of vigour: it would not be the cause of vigour, merely the accompaniment of it; nor would it be a direct product of natural selection. In the same way it is possible that among drongos nigritude is in some manner correlated with vigour. This idea is not altogether fanciful. Are there not horses of “bad colour”? Are not white “socks” a sign of weakness? Is not roan a colour indicative of strength and endurance in a horse?

Let me be clear. Imagine a group of savages where those with fair hair are stronger, bolder, more fertile, and more aggressive than the dark-haired individuals. For some unknown reason, fair hair consistently goes hand in hand with strength and similar traits. It's clear that, in this scenario, the group would eventually become predominantly fair-haired: the gentler dark-haired people would eventually be pushed aside. Fair hair would then represent vitality: it wouldn't cause vitality, just accompany it; nor would it be a direct result of natural selection. Similarly, it’s possible that in drongos, being dark-colored is in some way linked to strength. This idea isn't completely out there. Aren’t there horses with “bad colors”? Are white “socks” not a sign of weakness? Isn’t a roan coat a color associated with strength and endurance in horses?

May not the blackness and the forked tail of the drongo cuckoo have arisen in the same way as they arose in the king-crow? In each case it may be an accompaniment of vigour, or it may be the result of sexual selection. Mrs. Surniculus may have had similar tastes to Mrs. Dicrurus, and, since cuckoos seem to be very plastic birds, her tastes have been gratified. As another example of this plasticity I may cite [207] Centropus phasianus—a cuckoo which is a very fair imitation of a pheasant.

Could the dark color and forked tail of the drongo cuckoo have developed in the same way as they did in the king-crow? In both cases, it might be a sign of strength, or it could be due to sexual selection. Mrs. Surniculus might have had similar preferences to Mrs. Dicrurus, and since cuckoos seem to be quite adaptable, her preferences have been met. As another example of this adaptability, I can mention [207] Centropus phasianus—a cuckoo that closely resembles a pheasant.

On this view the resemblance is a mere chance one. The cuckoo is not an ass in a lion’s skin, but an ass that looks very like a lion. His lion-like shape was not forced upon him by natural selection. A variety of causes probably contributed to it. It was not until the resemblance had arisen and become very striking that it was directly affected by natural selection.

On this view, the resemblance is just a coincidence. The cuckoo isn't an idiot in a lion's coat, but an idiot that looks a lot like a lion. His lion-like appearance wasn't a result of natural selection. A mix of factors probably played a part in it. It wasn't until the resemblance became very noticeable that natural selection started to directly influence it.

I am far from saying that the above is a correct explanation of the nigritude: it is all pure hypothesis. Even if it be correct, we are really very little further than we were before towards an explanation of the colours and shape of either the king-crow or the drongo cuckoo.

I’m not saying that the explanation above accurately describes nigritude; it’s just a guess. Even if it were right, we still wouldn’t have made much progress in understanding the colors and shape of the king-crow or the drongo cuckoo.

Why did these birds tend to grow black feathers rather than red, green, or blue ones?

Why did these birds tend to have black feathers instead of red, green, or blue ones?

This is a question which “stumps” us all.

This is a question that leaves us all puzzled.

[208]


The Emerald Bee-eater

If I have a favourite bird it is the little green bee-eater (Merops viridis). There is no winged thing more beautiful or more alluring. More showy birds exist, more striking, more gorgeous, more magnificent creatures. With such the bee-eater does not compete. Its beauty is of another order. It is that of the moon rather than of the sun, of the violet rather than of the rose. The exquisite shades of its plumage cannot be fully appreciated unless minutely inspected. Every feather is a triumph of colouring. No description can do the bird justice. To say that its general hue is the fresh, soft green of grass in England after an April shower, that the head is covered with burnished gold, that the tail is tinted with olive, that a black collarette adorns the breast, that the bill is black, that a streak of that colour runs from the base of the beak, backwards, through the eye, which is fiery red, that the feathers below this streak are of the purest turquoise-blue, as are the feathers of the throat—to say all this is to convey no idea of the hundred shades of these colours, or the manner in which they harmonise and pass one into another. Nor is it easy for words to do justice to the shape of the bird; even a photograph fails to express the elegance [209] of its carriage and the perfection of its proportions. Were I to string together all the superlatives that I know, I should scarcely convey an adequate impression of the grace of its movements. I can but try to make the bird recognisable, so that the reader may see its beauties for himself.

If I have a favorite bird, it’s the little green bee-eater (Merops viridis). There’s no bird more beautiful or more captivating. Sure, there are flashier birds, more striking, more gorgeous, and more magnificent creatures. But the bee-eater isn’t in that league. Its beauty is different. It’s more like the moon than the sun, more like a violet than a rose. You can’t fully appreciate the exquisite shades of its plumage unless you examine it closely. Every feather is a triumph of color. No description can truly capture this bird. To say its overall color is the fresh, soft green of grass in England after an April shower, that its head is covered in shiny gold, that its tail has an olive tint, that a black collar adorns its breast, that its bill is black, and that a streak of black runs from the base of its beak, through its fiery red eye, and that the feathers below that streak are a pure turquoise-blue, just like the throat feathers—this all fails to express the hundred shades of these colors or how they blend seamlessly into one another. It’s also tough for words to capture the bird’s shape; even a photograph can’t convey the elegance of its posture and the perfection of its proportions. If I listed all the superlatives I know, I still wouldn’t convey an adequate impression of how graceful it moves. I can only try to make the bird recognizable so the reader can see its beauty for themselves.

He should look out for a little green bird with a black beak, slender and curved, and a tail of which the two middle feathers are very attenuated and project a couple of inches as two black bristles beyond the other caudal feathers. The bird should be looked for on a telegraph wire or the bare branch of a tree, for the habits of bee-eaters are those of fly-catchers. The larger species prey upon bees, hence the popular name, but I doubt whether the little Merops viridis tackles an insect so large as a bee. It feeds upon smaller flying things, which it captures on the wing. As it rests on its perch its bright eyes are always on the look out for passing insects. When one comes into view, the bird sallies forth. Very beautiful is it as it sails on outstretched wings. The under surface of these is reddish bronze, so that their possessor seems to become alternately green and gold as the sun’s rays fall on the upper or lower surface of its pinions. Its long mandibles close upon its prey with a snap sufficiently loud to be audible from a distance of five or six yards. This one may frequently hear, for bee-eaters are not shy birds. They will permit a human being to approach quite near to them, as though they knew that the fulness of their beauty was apparent only on close inspection.

He should keep an eye out for a small green bird with a black beak, slender and curved, and a tail where the two middle feathers are very thin and stick out a couple of inches like two black bristles beyond the other tail feathers. Look for the bird on a power line or a bare tree branch because bee-eaters behave like flycatchers. The larger species feed on bees, which is why they have that name, but I doubt the little Merops viridis goes after insects as big as bees. It catches smaller flying insects while in flight. As it rests on its perch, its bright eyes are always on the lookout for passing insects. When one appears, the bird zips out to catch it. It's stunning to see it glide with its wings spread wide. The underside of these wings is a reddish bronze, making it look like it shifts between green and gold as the sunlight hits either the top or bottom of its wings. Its long beak snaps shut on its prey with a sound loud enough to be heard from five or six yards away. You can often hear this, as bee-eaters are not shy birds. They allow people to get quite close, as if they know their true beauty can only be appreciated up close.

[210]

The little green bee-eater utters what Jerdon calls “a rather pleasant rolling whistling note,” which, if it cannot be dignified by the name of song, adds considerably to the general attractiveness of the bird. Bee-eaters are, alas! not very abundant in Madras, but, if looked for, may be seen on most days in winter. The Adyar Club grounds seem to be their favourite resort. When driving into the club at sunset I have often surprised a little company of them taking a dust bath in the middle of the road. The bath over, the little creatures take to their wings and enjoy a final flight before retiring for the night.

The little green bee-eater makes what Jerdon describes as “a rather pleasant rolling whistling note,” which, even if it can’t be called a song, definitely adds to the bird’s overall charm. Unfortunately, bee-eaters are not very common in Madras, but if you look for them, you can often see them on most days during winter. The Adyar Club grounds seem to be their favorite spot. When driving into the club at sunset, I’ve often surprised a small group of them taking a dust bath right in the middle of the road. After their bath, these little creatures take to the air and enjoy a final flight before settling in for the night.

Bee-eaters are, I think, migratory birds. It is true that they are found all the year round in many parts of India, but such places appear to be the winter quarters of some individuals and the summer residences of others. There is an exodus of bee-eaters from Calcutta about March. A similar event occurs in Madras, although in the latter place the birds are seen all the year round, a few remaining to breed. In Lahore, on the other hand, the birds arrive in March, and, having reared their young, leave in September.

Bee-eaters are, I believe, migratory birds. It’s true that they can be found year-round in many areas of India, but those places seem to serve as winter homes for some and summer homes for others. There’s a mass departure of bee-eaters from Calcutta around March. A similar situation happens in Madras, although in that city the birds are seen throughout the year, with some staying to breed. In Lahore, however, the birds arrive in March and, after raising their young, leave in September.

The nest is a circular hole excavated by the bird, usually in a sandbank, sometimes in a mud partition between two fields. I saw a nest in Lahore in one of the artificial bunkers on the golf links. Major C. T. Bingham states that in 1873, when the musketry instruction of his regiment was being carried on at Allahabad, he observed several nest holes of this species in the face of the butts. The birds seemed utterly regardless of the bullets that every now and then [211] buried themselves with a loud thud in the earth close beside them. Colonel Butler gives an account of a bee-eater nesting in an artificial mudbank, about a foot high, that marked the limits of the badminton court in the Artillery Mess compound at Deesa. One of the birds invariably sat upon the badminton net when people were not playing, and at other times on a tree close by, while its mate was sitting on the eggs. As I have already said, bee-eaters are not afflicted with shyness.

The nest is a round hole dug by the bird, usually in a sandbank, and sometimes in a mud barrier between two fields. I saw a nest in Lahore in one of the artificial bunkers on the golf course. Major C. T. Bingham notes that in 1873, during musket training for his regiment at Allahabad, he saw several nest holes of this species in the face of the butts. The birds seemed completely unconcerned by the bullets that occasionally crashed into the ground nearby with a loud thud. Colonel Butler describes a bee-eater nesting in an artificial mudbank, about a foot high, that marked the edge of the badminton court in the Artillery Mess compound at Deesa. One of the birds always perched on the badminton net when people weren't playing, and at other times on a nearby tree, while its mate sat on the eggs. As I mentioned before, bee-eaters are not shy at all.

Very soon after their arrival at Lahore the birds begin their courtship. At this period they seem to spend the major portion of the day in executing circular flights in the air. They shoot forth from their perch and rapidly ascend by flapping their wings, then they sail for a little on outstretched pinions and thus return to the perch.

Very soon after they arrive in Lahore, the birds start their courtship. During this time, they seem to spend most of the day making circular flights in the air. They take off from their perch and quickly rise by flapping their wings, then they glide for a moment on outstretched wings before returning to their perch.

Courtship soon gives place to the more serious business of nest construction. When a suitable spot has been found, the birds at once begin excavating, digging away at the earth with pick-like bill and holding on to the wall of the bank by their sharp claws until the hole they are making becomes sufficiently deep to afford a foothold. As the excavation grows deeper the bird throws backwards with its feet the sand it has loosened with its beak, sending it in little clouds out of the mouth of the hole. While one bird is at work its mate perches close by and gives vent to its twittering note. After working for about two minutes the bird has a rest and its partner takes a turn at excavation. Thus the work proceeds apace. Bee-eaters [212] look spick and span, even when in the midst of this hard labour. The dry sand that envelops them, far from soiling their plumage, acts as a dust bath. When the hole, which is about two inches in diameter, has reached a length of some four feet, it is widened out into a circular chamber about twice the size of a cricket ball. In this three or four white eggs are laid. These have been well described as “little polished alabaster balls.” They are placed on the bare ground. Young bee-eaters lack the elongated bristle-like tail feathers of the adult birds. A very pleasing sight is that of a number of the youngsters sitting in a row on a telegraph wire receiving instruction in flying.

Courtship soon gives way to the more serious task of building a nest. Once a suitable location is found, the birds immediately start digging, using their pick-like bills to scrape the earth while gripping the bank's wall with their sharp claws until the hole is deep enough for them to get a good foothold. As the hole gets deeper, the bird kicks back the loosened sand with its feet, sending little clouds of it flying out of the hole. While one bird digs, its mate perches nearby, chirping away. After about two minutes of work, the digging bird takes a break, and its partner takes its turn. This is how the job gets done quickly. Bee-eaters look neat and tidy, even while working hard. The dry sand that surrounds them acts as a dust bath instead of dirtying their feathers. When the hole, which is about two inches wide, is about four feet long, it's widened into a round chamber roughly twice the size of a cricket ball. Inside, three or four white eggs are laid, described as “little polished alabaster balls.” They are placed directly on the bare ground. Young bee-eaters don’t have the long bristle-like tail feathers of adults. A lovely sight is a row of youngsters sitting on a telegraph wire, learning how to fly.

In conclusion, mention must be made of a near relative of the little bee-eater. I allude to the blue-tailed species (Merops philippinus), which also occurs in Madras. This is a larger and less beautiful edition of the green bee-eater. It is distinguishable by its size, the rusty colour of its throat, and its blue tail. It is usually found near water. He who shoots snipe in the paddy near Madras comes across numbers of these birds sitting on the low walls that divide up the fields. The habits of the blue-tailed bee-eater are those of its smaller cousin. Although its song is more powerful, it is a less attractive bird.

In conclusion, I need to mention a close relative of the little bee-eater. I'm talking about the blue-tailed species (Merops philippinus), which is also found in Madras. This is a larger and less striking version of the green bee-eater. You can tell it apart by its size, the rusty color of its throat, and its blue tail. It typically lives near water. Anyone who hunts snipe in the fields near Madras will see many of these birds perched on the low walls that separate the fields. The behavior of the blue-tailed bee-eater is similar to that of its smaller cousin. Even though its song is stronger, it's not as pretty of a bird.

[213]


DO ANIMALS THINK?

Mr. John Burroughs contributed some time ago to Harper’s Magazine an article bearing the above title. The leading American naturalist is so weighty an authority that I feel chary about controverting any statement made by him; but I cannot believe that he is right when he boldly asserts that animals never think at all. I agree with Mr. Burroughs when he says “we are apt to speak of the lower animals in terms that we apply to our own kind.” There is undoubtedly a general tendency to give animals credit for much greater intelligence, far more considerable powers of reasoning, than they actually possess; in short, to put an anthropomorphic interpretation on their actions.

Mr. John Burroughs wrote an article for Harper’s Magazine some time ago with the title mentioned above. As a leading American naturalist, he's a significant authority, so I hesitate to disagree with anything he says; however, I can't accept his assertion that animals don't think at all. I agree with Mr. Burroughs when he mentions that “we tend to speak of lower animals using terms we apply to our own kind.” There is definitely a common tendency to attribute much greater intelligence and reasoning abilities to animals than they actually have; in short, we often interpret their actions in a human-like way.

But it seems to me that Mr. Burroughs rushes to the other extreme. To deny to animals the power of thought is surely as opposed to facts as to credit them with almost human powers of reasoning. Says Mr. Burroughs: “Animals act with a certain grade of intelligence in the presence of things, but they carry away no concepts of those things as a man does, because they have no language. How could a crow tell his fellows of some future event or of some experience of the day? How could he tell them this thing is [214] dangerous save by his actions in the presence of those things? Or how tell of a newly found food supply save by flying eagerly to it?”

But it seems to me that Mr. Burroughs goes to the other extreme. Denying animals the ability to think is just as wrong as claiming they have nearly human levels of reason. Mr. Burroughs says, “Animals act with a certain level of intelligence in response to things, but they don’t retain any concepts about those things like a person does, because they lack language. How could a crow share information about a future event or recount an experience from the day? How could it indicate that something is dangerous without showing its behavior around it? And how could it communicate about a newly discovered food source except by flying eagerly toward it?”

Even if we admit that a crow is not able to recount the experiences of the day to his companion, it does not follow that the crow does not remember them, or cannot picture them in his mind. With regard to the last question, I have frequently seen a crow, at the sight of some food thrown out to him, caw loudly, and his friends, on hearing his cry, at once fly to the food.

Even if we agree that a crow can’t share the day’s experiences with another crow, that doesn’t mean it doesn’t remember them or can’t visualize them in its mind. About the last point, I've often seen a crow caw loudly when it spots food thrown to it, and its friends, upon hearing the call, immediately fly over to the food.

Of course it is open to any one to assert that, in this case, the crow that discovers the food does not consciously call its companions; at the sight of its food it instinctively caws, and its companions obey the caw instinctively, without knowing why they do so. No one, however, who watches crows for long can help believing that they think. The fact that they hang about the kitchen every day at the time the cook pitches out the leavings seems inconsistent with the theory that birds cannot think. The crows obtained food at this place yesterday and the day before at a certain hour, and the fact that they are all on the look out for food to-day shows, not only that they possess a good memory, but that they are endowed with a certain amount of reasoning power.

Of course, anyone can argue that in this case, the crow that finds the food doesn't consciously call its friends; when it sees its food, it instinctively caws, and its companions respond to the caw instinctively, without realizing why they do. However, anyone who observes crows for a long time can't help but believe that they think. The fact that they gather around the kitchen every day when the cook tosses out the scraps seems incompatible with the notion that birds can't think. The crows got food at this spot yesterday and the day before at a specific time, and the fact that they are all on the lookout for food today shows not just that they have a good memory, but that they possess some degree of reasoning ability.

Many animals have very good memories. Now, in order that an animal may remember a thing it must think. Its thoughts are of course not clothed in language as human thoughts are, but they nevertheless exist as mental pictures.

Many animals have excellent memories. For an animal to remember something, it has to think. Its thoughts aren’t expressed in language like human thoughts, but they still exist as mental images.

According to Professor Thorndike, the psychic life of [215] an animal is “most like what we feel when our consciousness contains little thought about anything, when we feel the sense impressions in their first intention, so to speak, when we feel our own body and the impulses we give to it (or that outward objects give to it). Sometimes one gets this animal consciousness; while in swimming, for example. One feels the water, the sky, the birds above, but with no thoughts about them, or memories of how they looked at other times, or æsthetic judgments about their beauty. One feels no ‘ideas’ about what movements he will make, but feels himself make them, feels his body throughout. Self-consciousness dies away. The meanings and values and connections of things die away. One feels sense-impressions, has impulses, feels the movements he makes; that is all.”

According to Professor Thorndike, the mental life of an animal is “most similar to what we experience when our consciousness holds little thought about anything, when we perceive sensations in their raw form, so to speak, when we feel our own body and the actions we take (or those that external objects induce in us). Sometimes, one experiences this animal-like consciousness; like when swimming, for example. One feels the water, the sky, the birds above, but with no thoughts about them, or memories of how they appeared at other times, or aesthetic judgments about their beauty. There are no ‘ideas’ about what movements will be made, just the feeling of making them, sensing one's body throughout. Self-awareness fades away. The meanings, values, and connections of things fade away. One experiences sensations, has impulses, feels the movements being made; that is all.”

This is probably a good description of the state of mind of a dog when he is basking in the sunlight; he is thinking of nothing. But he hears the shrill cry of a squirrel—this at once recalls to him the image of the little rodent and past shikar. In a moment the dog is on the alert; he is now thinking of the squirrel, and his instinct and inclinations teach him to give chase to it. Or he hears a footstep; he recognises it as that of his master, sees that the latter is wearing a topi, and at once pictures up a run in the compound with his master. But his owner chains him up. The dog looks wistfully at his master’s retreating figure and pulls at his chain; it is surely absurd to say that the dog is not thinking. The picture of a scamper beside his master rises up before him, and he feels sad because he knows [216] that the scamper is not likely to become a fait accompli.

This probably describes how a dog feels when he’s soaking up the sun; he’s not thinking about anything. But when he hears the sharp cry of a squirrel, it immediately brings to mind the image of the little rodent and past hunting trips. In an instant, the dog is alert; he’s now focused on the squirrel, and his instincts tell him to chase after it. Or he hears footsteps; he recognizes them as his owner’s, sees that he’s wearing a cap, and instantly imagines running around the yard with him. But his owner puts him on a leash. The dog looks longingly at his master’s disappearing figure and tugs at his chain; it’s clearly ridiculous to say the dog isn’t thinking. The image of running alongside his master appears in his mind, and he feels sad because he knows that run is unlikely to happen.

Again, you have been accustomed to throw a stick for your dog to run after and carry back to you. You are out walking accompanied by your dog; he espies a stick lying on the ground; at once images of previous enjoyable runs after the stick rise up in his mind; he picks up the stick and brings it to you, drops it at your feet and looks up at you. You pretend to take no notice. The dog then picks up the stick and rubs it against your legs. To believe that the dog while acting thus does not think, that he is merely obeying an inborn instinct, is surely a misinterpretation of facts. Animals have but limited reasoning powers, and their thoughts are not our thoughts, they are not clothed in language, they are merely mental pictures, called up either subjectively, as when a dog barks while dreaming, or objectively by some sight or scent, but nevertheless such sensations are thoughts.

Once again, you’ve gotten used to throwing a stick for your dog to chase and bring back to you. You’re out for a walk with your dog; he spots a stick on the ground; right away, memories of past fun chases for the stick come to mind; he picks it up and brings it to you, drops it at your feet, and looks up at you. You pretend not to notice. The dog then picks up the stick and rubs it against your legs. To think that the dog, in this moment, isn’t thinking and is just following an instinct is definitely a misunderstanding of the facts. Animals have limited reasoning abilities, and their thoughts aren’t like ours; they aren’t expressed in words; they’re just mental images, triggered either internally, like when a dog barks while dreaming, or externally by a sight or scent, but still, these sensations are thoughts.

While maintaining that the higher animals can and do think, I am ready to admit that a great many of their actions which are apparently guided by reason are in reality purely instinctive. Thus the building of a nest by a bird must, at any rate on the first occasion, be a purely instinctive action. The creature cannot know what it is doing. Nor can it have any thoughts on the matter; it suddenly becomes an automaton, a machine, acting thoughtlessly and instinctively.

While I believe that higher animals can think, I have to accept that many of their actions that seem to be guided by reason are really just instinctual. For example, when a bird builds a nest, it must be purely instinctive, at least the first time it does it. The creature doesn’t know what it’s doing, nor does it have any thoughts about it; it suddenly acts like an automaton, a machine, behaving without thought and purely by instinct.

Some internal force which is irresistible compels it to seek twigs and weave them into a nest. The bird has no time to stop and think what it is doing, nor does it [217] wish to, for it enjoys nest building. It is, of course, impossible for a human being to understand the frame of mind of a bird when building its first nest. The only approach to it that we ever experience is when we are suddenly seized with an impulse to do something unusual, and we obey the impulse and are afterwards surprised at ourselves.

Some inner force that can't be resisted drives it to find twigs and weave them into a nest. The bird doesn’t have time to stop and think about what it’s doing, nor does it want to, because it actually enjoys building the nest. Of course, it’s impossible for a human to fully grasp what goes through a bird’s mind while making its first nest. The closest we get to this feeling is when we suddenly feel compelled to do something unexpected, follow that impulse, and then look back at ourselves in surprise.

There is a story told of a wealthy man who had been out hunting and was returning home tired and thirsty. He dismounted at a farm-house, went inside and asked for a drink. While this was being obtained he noticed a lot of valuable old china on the dresser: seized by a sudden impulse, he knocked it all down, piece by piece, with his riding whip. His hostess on her return with the drink looked surprised. The hunting man smiled, asked her to name the value she set on the china, sat down and, there and then, wrote out a cheque for the amount.

There’s a story about a wealthy man who had been out hunting and was on his way home, feeling tired and thirsty. He stopped at a farmhouse, went inside, and asked for a drink. While he was waiting, he noticed a lot of valuable old china on the dresser. Suddenly impulsive, he knocked it all down, piece by piece, with his riding whip. When his hostess returned with the drink, she looked surprised. The hunter smiled, asked her what she thought the china was worth, sat down, and wrote a check for the amount right then and there.

It always seems to me that when a bird begins for the first time to collect materials for a nest she must act impulsively, without thinking what she is doing. Just as the hunting man was seized with a sudden desire to smash the crockery with his whip, so is she suddenly impelled to collect twigs and build a nest.

It always seems to me that when a bird starts gathering materials for a nest for the first time, she must do so impulsively, not really thinking about it. Just like the hunter who suddenly feels the urge to smash the dishes with his whip, she is suddenly driven to gather twigs and build a nest.

Another instinctive act which is apparently purposeful is the feigning of injury by a parent bird when an enemy approaches its young. Superficial observation of this action leads the observer to imagine that the mother bird behaves thus with deliberate intent to deceive, that in so doing she consciously endeavours to distract attention while her young ones are betaking [218] themselves to cover. As a matter of fact, the bird will behave in precisely the same way if she have eggs instead of young ones. This has, of course, the effect of drawing attention to the eggs, and proves that the action is instinctive and not the result of reasoning.

Another instinctive behavior that seems intentional is when a parent bird pretends to be injured when a predator nears its young. A casual observer might think that the mother bird is trying to deceive on purpose, making a conscious effort to draw attention away while her babies hide. In reality, the bird acts exactly the same way if she has eggs instead of chicks. This, of course, makes the eggs the focus, proving that the behavior is instinctual rather than based on reasoning.

Most people have remarked the cautious manner in which many birds approach the nest when they are aware that they are being watched. This has the appearance of a highly intelligent act. It is, however, nothing of the kind.

Most people have noticed how carefully many birds approach their nests when they know they're being watched. It seems like a very clever move. But really, it’s nothing of the sort.

I have taken young birds from a nest, handled them and replaced them in full view of their frantic parents. Then I have retired a short distance and watched the parents. These invariably display the same caution in approaching the nest as they did before I had discovered its whereabouts.

I have taken young birds from a nest, handled them, and put them back in full view of their panicked parents. Then I stepped back a bit and watched the parents. They always show the same caution in approaching the nest as they did before I found it.

Birds and beasts think much less than they are popularly supposed to do. It is absurd to attribute to them reasoning powers similar to those enjoyed by man; it is equally absurd to assert that they do not think at all.

Birds and animals think a lot less than people commonly believe. It's ridiculous to say they have reasoning powers like humans; it's just as ridiculous to claim they don't think at all.

[219]


A few neglected
CRAFTSMEN

Two Indian birds have a world-wide reputation. Every one has heard of the weaver bird (Ploceus baya) and the tailor-bird (Orthotomus sutorius). Their wonderful nests are depicted in every popular treatise on ornithology. They are both master-craftsmen and deserve their reputation. But there are in India birds who build similar nests whose very names are unknown to the great majority of Anglo-Indians. The Indian wren-warbler (Prinia inornata) weaves a nest quite as skilfully as the famous weaver bird. This neglected craftsman is common in nearly all parts of India, and, if you speak of the weaver bird to domiciled Europeans, they will think you mean this wren-warbler, for among such he is universally called the weaver bird; the famous weaver, whose portrait appears in every popular bird book, is known to them as the baya.

Two Indian birds have a worldwide reputation. Everyone has heard of the weaver bird (Ploceus baya) and the tailor-bird (Orthotomus sutorius). Their amazing nests are shown in every popular book on birds. They are both master builders and deserve their fame. But there are birds in India that create similar nests whose names are unknown to most Anglo-Indians. The Indian wren-warbler (Prinia inornata) weaves a nest just as skillfully as the well-known weaver bird. This overlooked craftsman is found in almost all parts of India, and if you mention the weaver bird to settled Europeans, they will think you mean this wren-warbler, as he is commonly called the weaver bird among them; the well-known weaver, whose picture appears in every popular bird book, is recognized by them as the baya.

As its name implies, Prinia inornata is a plainly attired little bird. Its upper parts are earthy brown. It has the faintest suspicion of a white eyebrow, and its under plumage is yellowish white, the thighs being darker than the abdomen. Picture a slenderly built wren with a tail three inches in length, which looks as [220] though it were about to fall out and which is constantly being waggled, and you have a fair idea of the appearance of this little weaver. But this description applies to dozens of other birds found in India. The various warblers are so similar to one another in appearance as to drive ornithologists to despair. The inimitable “Eha” admits that they baffle him. “There is nothing about them,” he writes, “to catch the imagination of the historian, and they will never be famous. I have been perplexed as to how to deal with them. . . . To attempt to describe each species is out of the question, for there are many, and they are mostly so like each other that even the title ornithologist does not qualify one to distinguish them with certainty at a distance. If you can distinguish them with certainty when you have them in your hand you will fully deserve the title.”

As its name suggests, Prinia inornata is a plain little bird. Its upper body is a dull brown. It has a slight hint of a white eyebrow, and its underbelly is yellowish-white, with the thighs being darker than the belly. Imagine a slender wren with a three-inch tail that seems like it’s about to fall off, which is constantly wagging, and you get a pretty good idea of what this little weaver looks like. But this description fits many other birds found in India. The different warblers look so much alike that they can drive ornithologists crazy. Even the unique “Eha” admits they confuse him. “There’s nothing about them,” he writes, “to capture the imagination of historians, and they will never be famous. I’ve struggled with how to handle them... Describing each species is impossible since there are so many, and they mostly look so similar that even someone with the title ornithologist wouldn’t be able to tell them apart with certainty from a distance. If you can identify them for sure when you have them in your hand, then you truly deserve the title.”

It is, however, possible to recognise the Indian wren-warbler by its note. When once you have learned this you are able to identify the bird directly it opens its mouth. But how shall I describe it? It is a peculiar, harsh but plaintive, twee, twee, twee; each twee follows close upon the preceding one, and gives you the idea that the bird is both excited and worried. If you see a fussy little bird constantly flitting about in a cornfield and uttering this note, you may be tolerably certain that the bird is the Indian wren-warbler. It never rises high in the air; it is but an indifferent exponent of the art of flying. It moves by means of laborious jerks of its wings. It is a true friend to the husbandman, since it feeds exclusively on insects. The most remarkable thing about it is its nest. This is a beautifully woven [221] structure, composed exclusively of grass or strips of leaves of monocotyledonous plants which the bird tears off with its bill. These strands are invariably very narrow, and are sometimes less than one-twentieth of an inch in breadth. The nest may be described as an egg-shaped purse, some five or six inches in depth and three in width, with an entrance at one side, near the top. It is devoid of any lining, and its texture puts one in mind of a loosely made loofah. The nest is sometimes attached to two or more stalks of corn, or more commonly it is found among the long grasses which are so abundant in India. When the nest is built in a cornfield the birds have to bring up their family against time. They are unable to begin nest-building until the corn is fairly high, and must, if the young are to be safely started in life, have brought them to the stage when they are able to leave the nest by the time the crop is cut.

It’s possible to recognize the Indian wren-warbler by its call. Once you learn it, you can identify the bird as soon as it opens its mouth. But how can I describe it? It has a unique, harsh yet mournful, twee, twee, twee; each twee comes right after the last one, making it seem like the bird is both excited and anxious. If you spot a small, fidgety bird constantly darting around in a cornfield and making this sound, it’s quite likely the Indian wren-warbler. It doesn’t fly very high; it's not great at flying, to be honest. It flaps its wings with a lot of effort. It’s a true friend to farmers since it only feeds on insects. The most interesting thing about it is its nest. It’s a beautifully woven [221] structure made entirely of grass or strips of leaves from monocot plants that the bird pulls off with its beak. These strands are usually very thin, sometimes less than one-twentieth of an inch wide. The nest is shaped like an egg, about five or six inches deep and three inches wide, with an entrance on one side, near the top. There's no lining inside, and the texture is reminiscent of a loosely made loofah. The nest is sometimes attached to two or more corn stalks, but usually, it’s found among the tall grasses that grow abundantly in India. When the nest is built in a cornfield, the birds have to raise their young quickly. They can't start building until the corn is tall enough, and if they want their chicks to survive, they need to have them ready to leave the nest before the crop is harvested.

In India nearly every field of ripe corn has its family of wren-warblers; the two parents flit about, followed by a struggling family of four. These little birds do not by any means always defeat time. Numbers of their nests containing half-fledged young are mown down at every harvest by the reaper’s sickle. The nest is woven in a manner similar to that adopted by the baya; the cock and hen in each case work in combination. Its texture is looser than that of the more famous weaver, but it is not less neatly put together. In it are deposited four or five pretty little green eggs, marked with brown blotches and wavy lines.

In India, nearly every cornfield has its family of wren-warblers; the two parents flit around, followed by a struggling brood of four. These little birds don’t always beat the clock. Many of their nests with half-grown chicks get cut down at every harvest by the reaper’s sickle. The nest is woven in a way similar to how the baya builds its, with both the male and female working together. Its texture is looser than that of the more famous weaver bird, but it’s just as neatly constructed. Inside are four or five lovely little green eggs, marked with brown spots and wavy lines.

Our second neglected craftsman is a tailor. It sews [222] a nest so like that of the world-famous tailor as to be almost indistinguishable from it. Some authorities declare that the two nests are distinguishable. They assert that the nest of Orthotomus is invariably lined with some soft substance, such as cotton-wool, the silky down of the cotton tree, soft horse-hair, or even human hair, while that of the species of which we are speaking is lined with grass or roots. This distinction does not, however, invariably hold. I have seen nests of this species which have been lined with cotton-wool.

Our second overlooked craftsman is a tailor bird. It builds a nest so similar to that of the renowned tailor that it's nearly impossible to tell them apart. Some experts claim that the two nests can be distinguished. They argue that the nest of Orthotomus is always lined with some soft material, like cotton wool, the silky down of the cotton tree, soft horsehair, or even human hair, while the nest of the species we’re discussing is lined with grass or roots. However, this distinction doesn't always hold true. I've seen nests of this species that were lined with cotton wool.

This bird is known to ornithologists as the ashy wren-warbler (Prinia socialis). Anglo-Indian boys call it the tom-tit. It is a dark ashy-grey bird, with the sides of the head and neck and the whole of the lower plumage buff. There is a tinge of rufous in the wings and tail. It is most easily distinguished by the loud snapping noise it makes during flight. How this noise is produced we do not know for certain. Reid was of opinion that the bird snapped its long tail. What exactly this means I do not know. Jesse believes that the sound is produced by the bird’s mandibles. I have spent much time in watching the bird, and am inclined to think that the noise is caused by the beating of the wings against the tail. This last is constantly being wagged and jerked, and it seems to me that the wings beat against it as the bird flits about. When doves and pigeons fly, their wings frequently meet, causing a flapping sound. I am of opinion that something similar occurs when the ashy wren-warbler takes to its wings.

This bird is known to ornithologists as the ashy wren-warbler (Prinia socialis). Anglo-Indian boys call it the tom-tit. It’s a dark ashy-gray bird, with the sides of the head and neck and the entire lower plumage in a buff color. There’s a hint of rufous in the wings and tail. It’s most easily recognized by the loud snapping noise it makes when it flies. How this noise is made, we’re not entirely sure. Reid thought the bird snapped its long tail, though I'm not clear on what that means. Jesse believes the sound comes from the bird’s mandibles. I’ve spent a lot of time observing the bird and think that the noise is actually caused by its wings hitting against its tail. The tail is constantly being wagged and jerked, and it seems to me that the wings strike it as the bird flits around. When doves and pigeons fly, their wings often hit each other, making a flapping sound. I believe a similar thing happens when the ashy wren-warbler takes to the air.

Perhaps the most extraordinary thing about this bird is the well-authenticated fact that it builds two types of [223] nest. Besides this tailor-made nest, the species makes one of grass, beautifully and closely woven, domed, and with the entrance near the top. I have never seen this latter type of nest, but so many ornithologists have that there can be no doubt of its existence.

Perhaps the most remarkable thing about this bird is the well-documented fact that it builds two types of [223] nests. In addition to this custom-made nest, the species constructs one made of grass, which is beautifully and tightly woven, domed, and has the entrance near the top. I have never seen this second type of nest, but so many ornithologists have that there’s no doubt it exists.

The strange thing is that both types of nest have been found in the same neighbourhood, so that the difference in the form of nursery is not a local peculiarity.

The strange thing is that both types of nests have been found in the same neighborhood, so the difference in the type of nursery isn’t just a local oddity.

I am at a loss to account for the existence of these two types of nest. I have no idea how the habit can have arisen, nor do I know what, if any, benefit the species derives from this peculiarity. So far as I am aware, no one can say what it is that leads to the construction of one type of nest in preference to the other. The nests of this species present a most interesting ornithological problem. I hope one day to be in a position to throw some light on it; meanwhile I shall welcome the news that some one has forestalled me. The ashy wren-warbler is a common bird, so that most Anglo-Indians have a chance of investigating the mystery. The same kind of eggs are found in each type of nest. They are of exceptional beauty, being a deep mahogany or brick-red, so highly polished as to look as though they have been varnished.

I’m puzzled about why these two types of nests exist. I have no clue how this behavior developed, and I’m not sure what, if any, advantage the species gains from this oddity. As far as I know, no one can explain why one type of nest is built over the other. The nests of this species present a really interesting birdwatching puzzle. I hope to shed some light on it someday; in the meantime, I’d be glad to hear that someone else has figured it out first. The ashy wren-warbler is a common bird, so most Anglo-Indians have the opportunity to explore this mystery. The same kind of eggs are found in both types of nests. They are exceptionally beautiful, featuring a deep mahogany or brick-red color, so shiny they look like they’ve been varnished.

[224]


Birds in their nests

Just as every Englishman is of opinion that his house is his castle, so does every little bird resent all attempts at prying into its private affairs in the nest. For this reason we really know very little of the home-life of birds. It is not that there are no seekers after such knowledge. Practical ornithology is a science that can boast of a very large number of devotees.

Just like every Englishman believes his house is his castle, every little bird dislikes any attempts to invade its privacy in the nest. Because of this, we actually know very little about birds' home lives. It’s not that there aren’t people searching for this knowledge. Practical ornithology is a science that has a lot of enthusiasts.

Many men spend the greater part of their life in endeavouring to wrest from birds some of their secrets, and such must admit that the results they obtain are as a rule totally disproportionate to the magnitude of the efforts. At present we know only the vague generalities of bird life.

Many men spend most of their lives trying to uncover the secrets of birds, and they must admit that the results they achieve are usually completely out of proportion to the effort they put in. Right now, we only know vague generalizations about bird life.

We know that the hen lays eggs; that she, with or without the help of the cock, as the case may be, incubates these eggs; that the young, which are at first naked, are fed and brooded until they are ready to leave the nest, when they are coaxed forth by the parents, who hold out tempting morsels of food to them. But these are mere generalities. Our ignorance of details is very great.

We know that hens lay eggs and that they, with or without the rooster's help, incubate these eggs. The chicks, which are initially bare, are cared for and nurtured until they’re ready to leave the nest, at which point the parents entice them out by offering tasty bits of food. But these are just general statements. Our knowledge of the specifics is still very limited.

The nests of most passerine birds are scrupulously clean. Young birds have enormous appetites, and much of the [225] food which they eat is indigestible and must pass out as droppings, yet in the case of many species no sign of these droppings is visible, either in the nest, or on the leaves, branches, or the ground near the nest. What becomes of these droppings? Ornithological treatises are silent upon this subject.

The nests of most songbirds are meticulously clean. Young birds have huge appetites, and a lot of the food they eat is indigestible and needs to come out as droppings. However, with many species, there’s no sign of these droppings, either in the nest or on the leaves, branches, or ground nearby. What happens to these droppings? Birdwatching books don’t mention this.

Again, young birds are born naked, and in India are frequently exposed to very high temperatures, so that much liquid must pass from their bodies by evaporation. How is this liquid made good? Do the parents water the birds, if so, how? I have never seen any mention of this in an ornithological treatise.

Again, young birds are born without feathers, and in India, they often face extremely high temperatures, causing them to lose a lot of moisture through evaporation. How is this moisture replaced? Do the parents give the birds water, and if so, how? I've never come across any mention of this in an ornithology book.

Let us to-day consider these two subjects: the sanitation of the nest and the method of assuaging the thirst of young nestlings.

Let’s today discuss these two topics: the cleanliness of the nest and how to quench the thirst of young chicks.

As regards the first we have some knowledge, thanks to the patient labours of Mr. F. H. Herrick, an American naturalist, whose book, The Home Life of Birds, I commend to every lover of the feathered folk. Unfortunately, Mr. Herrick’s book is to some extent spoiled for Englishmen, because it deals with birds with which they are unfamiliar; nevertheless, its general results apply to all passerine birds.

As for the first point, we have some insight, thanks to the dedicated work of Mr. F. H. Herrick, an American naturalist, whose book, The Home Life of Birds, I recommend to anyone who loves birds. Unfortunately, Mr. Herrick’s book is somewhat less useful for English readers because it focuses on birds they don't know; however, the overall findings are relevant to all songbirds.

Mr. Herrick is a very keen bird photographer. As every one knows, he who wishes to obtain good photographs of birds has two great difficulties to overcome. The first is to get near to his subjects, and the second is to find them and their nests in situations suitable for photography.

Mr. Herrick is a passionate bird photographer. As everyone knows, anyone wanting to take great photos of birds has two main challenges to tackle. The first is getting close to the birds, and the second is locating them and their nests in spots that are good for photography.

The former is usually overcome by the photographer concealing himself and his camera in a tent or other [226] structure. At first the birds are afraid of the concealing object, but soon maternal affection overcomes their fear.

The photographer usually gets around this by hiding himself and his camera in a tent or another structure. At first, the birds are scared of the hidden object, but soon their maternal instincts help them overcome their fear.

Mr. Herrick’s method of overcoming the second of these two difficulties is to remove the nest to be photographed from the concealed situation in which it is usually built, and place it in a more open place. If the nest be thus moved when the young are some seven or eight days old, the parents will almost invariably continue to feed their young in the new situation, for at that particular period the parental instinct is at its zenith. In addition to obtaining a splendid series of photographs, Mr. Herrick has observed, from a distance of a few inches, the nesting habits of several American birds. As the result of these observations he is able to declare that nest-cleaning follows each feeding with clock-like regularity. “The excreta of the young,” he writes, “leave the cloaca in the form of white opaque or transparent mucous sacs. The sac is probably secreted at the lower end of the alimentary canal, and is sufficiently consistent to admit of being picked up without soiling bill or fingers. The parent birds often leave the nest hurriedly bearing one of these small white packages in bill, an action full of significance to every member of the family. . . . Removing the excreta piecemeal and dropping it at a safe distance is the common instinctive method, not only of insuring the sanitary condition of the nest itself, but, what is even more important, of keeping the grass and leaves below free from any sign which might betray them to an enemy.” These packets of excrement are quite odourless, [227] and they are often devoured by the parent bird instead of being carried away. The digestion of very young birds must be feeble, and doubtless much of the food given them passes undigested through the alimentary canal, so that it is capable of affording nourishment to the parents. Birds are nothing if not economical.

Mr. Herrick’s way of dealing with the second of these two challenges is to take the nest that needs to be photographed out of its usual hidden spot and place it in a more open area. If the nest is moved when the young are about seven or eight days old, the parents will almost always continue to feed them in the new location, as the parental instinct is at its strongest during this time. In addition to capturing a great series of photographs, Mr. Herrick has closely observed the nesting behaviors of several American birds from just a few inches away. As a result of these observations, he has found that nest-cleaning happens right after each feeding with precise regularity. “The excreta of the young,” he writes, “exit the cloaca in the form of white opaque or transparent mucous sacs. The sac is probably produced at the lower end of the digestive tract and is consistent enough to be picked up without soiling the bill or fingers. The parent birds often leave the nest in a hurry, carrying one of these small white packages in their beak, an action meaningful to every family member. . . . Removing the waste bit by bit and dropping it at a safe distance is an instinctive method that not only ensures the nest remains clean but, even more importantly, keeps the grass and leaves below free from any signs that might give away their location to a predator.” These packages of excrement are generally odorless, and they are often eaten by the parent bird instead of being taken away. The digestion of very young birds must be weak, and a lot of the food they receive likely passes through their digestive system undigested, making it nutritious for the parents. Birds are nothing if not resourceful.

Of course, all birds are not so careful of the sanitation of the nest. Every one knows what a filthy spectacle a heronry is. According to Mr. Herrick, the instinct of inspecting and cleaning the nest is mainly confined to the great passerine and picarian orders. It is obviously not necessary in the case of those birds, such as fowls, of which the young are able to run about when born; nor is it needful in the case of birds of prey, who take no pains to conceal the whereabouts of the nest. Young raptores eject their semi-fluid excreta over the edge of the nursery; thus the nest is kept clean, but the droppings on the ground betray its presence to all the world.

Of course, not all birds are so particular about keeping their nests clean. Everyone knows how dirty a heronry can get. According to Mr. Herrick, the instinct to inspect and clean the nest is mostly limited to the larger songbirds and woodpeckers. It clearly isn't needed for birds like chickens, whose young can walk around right after they hatch; nor is it necessary for birds of prey, which don't bother hiding their nests. Young raptors simply push their messy droppings over the edge of the nest; this keeps the nest tidy, but the waste on the ground makes it obvious to everyone where the nest is.

Coming now to our other question: How do young birds obtain the water which they require? we have no help from Mr. Herrick. He makes no mention of this in his most interesting book. It is possible that nestlings are not given anything to drink, that the juicy, succulent insects or fruits with which they are supplied contain sufficient moisture for their requirements. We must remember that the skin of birds is very different from that of man. It contains no sweat glands, so that a bird, like a dog, can only perspire through its mouth.

Now, let's address the other question: How do young birds get the water they need? Mr. Herrick doesn’t provide any insight on this in his fascinating book. It’s possible that nestlings don’t receive water to drink, as the juicy, tender insects or fruits they’re fed might have enough moisture for their needs. We should keep in mind that bird skin is quite different from human skin. It lacks sweat glands, so a bird, similar to a dog, can only sweat through its mouth.

The breath of mammals is so surcharged with moisture that when it is suddenly cooled the water vapour in it condenses; the result is we can “see the breath” [228] of a mammal on a cold day. I have never succeeded in seeing a bird’s breath, so am of opinion that the fowls of the air do not exhale so much moisture as mammals do. But even allowing for this, a considerable amount of moisture must be given out in expiration, so that it seems probable that young birds require more moisture than they obtain in their food. Drops of water have to be administered to hand-reared birds. Many birds fill up the crop with food and then discharge the contents into the gaping mouths of their young. In this condition the food must be mixed with a considerable quantity of saliva and possibly with water. The crop of a bird is a receptacle into which the food passes and remains until actually utilised. There seems no reason why water should not be stored for a short time in this receptacle just as food is. Perhaps birds “bring up” water as they do solid food, and thus assuage the thirst of their young. Such a process would be very difficult to detect; it would be indistinguishable from ordinary feeding to the casual observer. I hope that some physiologist will take up the matter. A quantitative analysis of the air exhaled by a bird should not be very difficult to make.

The breath of mammals is so full of moisture that when it cools quickly, the water vapor in it condenses; this is why we can “see the breath” of a mammal on a cold day. I’ve never been able to see a bird’s breath, so I think that birds don’t exhale as much moisture as mammals do. However, even considering this, a significant amount of moisture must still be released when they breathe out, which makes it likely that young birds need more moisture than they get from their food. Hand-reared birds need to be given drops of water. Many birds store food in their crop and then regurgitate it into the open mouths of their chicks. In that state, the food must be mixed with a fair amount of saliva and possibly some water. The crop of a bird acts as a container for food that stays there until it’s needed. There’s no reason why water shouldn’t be stored there for a brief period, just like food is. It’s possible that birds “bring up” water the same way they do with solid food, thus quenching the thirst of their chicks. This process would be very hard to notice; it would look just like regular feeding to a casual observer. I hope some physiologist will investigate this. A quantitative analysis of the air exhaled by a bird shouldn’t be too difficult to conduct. [228]

[229]


BULBULS

More than fifty species of bulbul are found in India—bulbuls of all sorts and conditions, of all shapes and sizes, from the brilliant green bulbuls (which, by the way, strictly speaking, are not bulbuls at all) to the dull-plumaged but blithe white-browed member of the community, so common in Madras; from the rowdy black bulbuls of the Himalayas to the highly respectable and well-behaved red-vented bulbuls. He who would write of them is thus confronted with an embarras de richesses. The problem that he has to solve is, which of the many species to take as his theme.

More than fifty species of bulbuls can be found in India—bulbuls of all kinds and conditions, of all shapes and sizes, from the vibrant green bulbuls (which, technically, aren't bulbuls at all) to the plain-feathered but cheerful white-browed ones, really common in Madras; from the noisy black bulbuls in the Himalayas to the respectable and well-mannered red-vented bulbuls. Anyone wanting to write about them faces an embarras de richesses. The challenge is deciding which of the many species to focus on.

The polity of birds is said to be a republic. The problem may, therefore, well be elucidated on democratic principles. The first and foremost of these—the main plank of every demagogue’s platform—is, of course, “one bulbul, one vote.” The second is like unto the first, “every bulbul for itself.” Therefore, on being asked to elect a representative to be the subject-matter of this paper, each will vote for his own species, and the result of the poll will be: Bulbuls of the genus Molpastes first, those of the genus Otocompsa a good second, and the rest a long way behind. Let us then conform to the will of the majority and consider for [230] a little these two species of bulbul, which resemble one another very closely in their habits.

The way birds organize themselves is described as a republic. This situation can definitely be understood through democratic principles. The most important of these—the main point of every demagogue’s agenda—is, of course, “one bulbul, one vote.” The next principle is similar to the first: “every bulbul for itself.” So, when asked to elect a representative for this discussion, each will vote for its own species, leading to the outcome of the poll: Bulbuls of the genus Molpastes come first, those of the genus Otocompsa are a solid second, and the others lag far behind. Let’s then respect the majority’s choice and take a moment to consider these two species of bulbul, which are very similar in their behavior.

Molpastes is a bird about half as big again as the sparrow, but with a longer tail. The whole head is black and marked by a short crest. There is a conspicuous crimson patch of feathers under the tail. The remainder of the plumage is brown, but each feather on the body is margined with creamy white, so that the bird is marked by a pattern that is, as “Eha” points out, not unlike the scales on a fish. Both ends of the tail feathers are whitish.

Molpastes is a bird that's about one and a half times the size of a sparrow, but it has a longer tail. Its entire head is black and has a short crest. There's a noticeable crimson patch of feathers under the tail. The rest of the feathers are brown, but each body feather is edged with creamy white, giving the bird a pattern that, as “Eha” notes, is somewhat similar to fish scales. Both ends of the tail feathers are whitish.

Otocompsa is a more showy bird. The crest is long and projects forward over the forehead. The crimson patch, so characteristic of bulbuls, also exists in this species. There is a similar patch on each side of the head—whence the bird’s name, the red-whiskered bulbul. There is also a white patch on each cheek. The white throat is separated from the whitish abdomen by a conspicuous dark brown necklace. This bird must be familiar to every one who has visited Coonoor or any other southern hill station. The less showy variety—the red-vented bulbul, as it is called—is common in and about Madras.

Otocompsa is a flashier bird. Its crest is long and sticks out over the forehead. The bright red patch, typical of bulbuls, can also be seen in this species. There’s a similar patch on each side of the head—hence the bird’s name, the red-whiskered bulbul. It also has a white patch on each cheek. The white throat is distinct from the pale abdomen by a noticeable dark brown necklace. This bird is likely familiar to anyone who has visited Coonoor or any other southern hill station. The less flashy variety—the red-vented bulbul, as it’s known—is common in and around Madras.

It will be noticed that I have refrained from giving any specific name to either of these two genera. This is due to the fact that these bulbuls are widely distributed and fall into a number of local races, each of which has some little peculiarity in colouring. For this reason, bulbuls are birds after the heart of the museum ornithologist. They afford him ample scope for species-making.

It’s interesting to note that I haven’t used a specific name for either of these two genera. This is because these bulbuls are found in many places and include several local variations, each showing some unique coloring. Because of this, bulbuls are definitely a favorite for museum ornithologists. They provide plenty of opportunities for creating new species.

THE BENGAL RED-WHISKERED BULBUL. (OTOCOMPSA EMERIA)

THE BENGAL RED-WHISKERED BULBUL. (OTOCOMPSA EMERIA)

THE BENGAL RED-WHISKERED BULBUL (OTOCOMPSA EMERIA)

[231]

If you go from Madras to the Punjab you will there meet with a bulbul which you will take for the same species as the bulbul you left behind in Madras. But if you look up the birds in an ornithological text-book you will find that they belong to different species. The Punjab bulbul is known as Molpastes intermedius, while the Madras bird is called M. hæmorrhous. The only difference in appearance between the two species is that in the Madras bird the black of the head does not extend to the neck, whereas in the Punjab bird it does. Similarly, there is a Burmese, a Tenasserim, a Chinese, and a Bengal red-vented bulbul.

If you travel from Madras to Punjab, you’ll encounter a bulbul that you might assume is the same species as the one you left in Madras. However, if you look it up in an ornithology textbook, you'll see that they are different species. The Punjab bulbul is called Molpastes intermedius, while the Madras bird is referred to as M. hæmorrhous. The only visual difference between the two species is that the black on the head of the Madras bird doesn’t extend to the neck, while in the Punjab bird, it does. There are also bulbul species from Burma, Tenasserim, China, and Bengal that have red vent feathers.

Now, I regard all these different bulbuls as local races of one species, which might perhaps be called Molpastes indicus; and I think that I am justified in holding this view by the fact that the bulbuls you come across at Lucknow do not fit in with the description of any of these so-called species. The reason is that the Bengal and the Madras races meet at Lucknow, and of course interbreed. The result is a cross between the two races.

Now, I see all these different bulbuls as local variations of one species, which could perhaps be named Molpastes indicus; and I believe I'm justified in this view because the bulbuls found in Lucknow don't match the description of any of these so-called species. The reason is that the Bengal and Madras variations meet in Lucknow and, of course, interbreed. The result is a mix between the two variations.

In addition to the above there are some Molpastes which have white cheeks and a yellow patch under the tail. In all, nine or ten Indian “species” of Molpastes have been described.

In addition to the above, there are some Molpastes that have white cheeks and a yellow patch under the tail. In total, nine or ten Indian “species” of Molpastes have been described.

The same applies in a lesser degree to Otocompsa. This is a widely distributed species, but is not so plastic as Molpastes. There is the Bengal red-whiskered bulbul (Otocompsa emeria), which is distinguishable from the southern variety (O. fuscicaudata) by having white tips to the tail feathers, and the dark necklace interrupted [232] in the middle. There is also an Otocompsa with a yellow patch under the tail.

The same goes for Otocompsa, but to a lesser extent. This species is found in many places, but it’s not as adaptable as Molpastes. There’s the Bengal red-whiskered bulbul (Otocompsa emeria), which can be told apart from the southern variety (O. fuscicaudata) by its white-tipped tail feathers and the dark necklace that’s interrupted in the middle. There’s also an Otocompsa that has a yellow patch under its tail.

This division of a species or genus into a number of races or nearly allied species is interesting as showing one of the ways in which new species arise in Nature quite independently of natural selection. It is unreasonable to suppose that the extension into the neck of the black of the head in the Punjab bulbul and its non-extension in the Madras bulbul are due to the action of natural selection in each locality, that a bulbul with black in its neck is unfitted for existence in Madras.

This division of a species or genus into several races or closely related species is intriguing as it demonstrates one of the ways new species emerge in nature, independent of natural selection. It isn’t reasonable to assume that the extension of black color into the neck of the Punjab bulbul and its absence in the Madras bulbul is due to natural selection acting in each location, or that a bulbul with black in its neck is unsuited for survival in Madras.

Whenever a group of animals becomes isolated from its fellows, it almost invariably develops peculiarities which are of no help to it in the struggle for existence. Thus isolation is the cause of the origin of dialects and languages. A dialect is an incipient language, even as a race is a potential species.

Whenever a group of animals is cut off from others, it almost always develops unique traits that don’t help it survive. So, isolation leads to the creation of dialects and languages. A dialect is a developing language, just like a race is a potential species.

But let us return to our bulbuls. The habits of both Otocompsa and Molpastes are so similar that we can speak of them together. They are what Mr. Finn calls thoroughly nice birds. They are, none of them, great songsters, but all continually give forth exceedingly cheery notes. The twittering of the red-whiskered bulbuls is not the least of the charms of our southern hill stations.

But let's get back to our bulbuls. The behaviors of both Otocompsa and Molpastes are so alike that we can discuss them together. They are what Mr. Finn calls really nice birds. None of them are powerful songsters, but they all consistently produce very cheerful sounds. The chirping of the red-whiskered bulbuls is one of the highlights of our southern hill stations.

Bulbuls feed on insects and berries, so are apt to be destructive in gardens. They built nests of the orthodox type—cups of the description always depicted on Christmas cards. These are built anywhere, without much attempt at concealment. Rose bushes are a [233] favourite site, so are crotons, especially if they be in a verandah. A pair of bulbuls once built a nest in my greenhouse at Gonda. Among the fronds of a fern growing in a hanging basket did those unsophisticated birds construct that nest. Every time the fern was watered the sitting bird, nest, and eggs received a shower-bath!

Bulbuls eat insects and berries, which can make them a nuisance in gardens. They build traditional nests that look like the ones you often see on Christmas cards. These nests can be found anywhere and aren't usually hidden very well. They particularly like rose bushes and crotons, especially if they're on a veranda. A pair of bulbuls once made a nest in my greenhouse in Gonda. They constructed their nest among the fronds of a fern in a hanging basket. Every time the fern was watered, the bird sitting on the nest, along with the nest and eggs, would get a shower!

Sometimes bulbuls do by chance construct their nest in a well-concealed spot, but then they invariably “give the show away” by setting up a tremendous cackling whenever a human being happens to pass by.

Sometimes bulbuls accidentally build their nests in hidden places, but then they always "give themselves away" by making a huge racket whenever a person walks by.

I have had the opportunity of watching closely the nesting operations of seven pairs of bulbuls; of these only one couple succeeded in raising their brood. The first of these nests was built in a croton plant in a verandah at Fyzabad. One day a lizard passed by and sucked the eggs. The next was the nest at Gonda already mentioned. In spite of the numerous waterings they received, the eggs actually yielded young bulbuls; but these disappeared when about four days old. The mali probably caused them to be gathered unto their fathers. The third nest was situated in a bush outside the drawing-room window of the house in which I spent a month’s leave at Coonoor. This little nursery was so well concealed that I expected the parents would succeed in rearing their young. But one morning I saw on the gravel path near the nest a number of tell-tale feathers. Puss had eaten mamma bulbul for breakfast! The fourth nest—but why should I detail these tragedies? Notwithstanding all their nesting disasters, bulbuls flourish so greatly as to [234] severely shake one’s faith in the doctrine of natural selection.

I had the chance to closely observe the nesting habits of seven pairs of bulbuls; of these, only one couple managed to raise their chicks. The first nest was built in a croton plant on a verandah in Fyzabad. One day, a lizard came by and ate the eggs. The next was the nest in Gonda that was already mentioned. Despite the frequent waterings they got, the eggs actually hatched into young bulbuls; however, they disappeared when they were about four days old. The gardener probably took them away. The third nest was located in a bush outside the drawing-room window of the house where I spent a month’s leave in Coonoor. This little nursery was so well hidden that I thought the parents would be able to raise their young successfully. But one morning, I found several tell-tale feathers on the gravel path near the nest. A cat had eaten mama bulbul for breakfast! As for the fourth nest—but why should I recount these tragedies? Even with all their nesting failures, bulbuls thrive so much that it really makes you question the idea of natural selection.

In conclusion, a word or two must be said concerning bulbuls in captivity. These birds make charming pets, but as their diet is largely insectivorous, they cannot be fed on seed. They become delightfully tame. One I kept used to fly on to my shoulder whenever it saw me, and open its mouth, flutter its wings, and twitter, which was its way of asking to be fed. It would insist on using my pen as a perch, and as one’s handwriting is not improved by an excitable bulbul hopping up and down the penholder, I was obliged to shut the bird up in a cage when I wanted to write. The bulbul used to resent this, and did not hesitate to tell me so. In young birds the tail is very short, and the patch of feathers under it is pale red instead of being bright crimson.

In conclusion, I should mention a thing or two about keeping bulbuls as pets. These birds make wonderful companions, but since their diet mainly consists of insects, they can’t eat seeds. They become very tame and affectionate. One bulbul I had would fly onto my shoulder whenever it saw me, open its beak, flutter its wings, and chirp, which was its way of asking for food. It insisted on using my pen as a perch, and since my handwriting didn't improve with an excited bulbul jumping around on my pen holder, I had to keep the bird in a cage when I wanted to write. The bulbul would get annoyed by this and made sure to let me know. Young birds have short tails, and the feathers beneath them are pale red rather than bright crimson.

Natives of India keep bulbuls for fighting purposes. These birds are not caged, but are tied to a cloth-covered perch by a long piece of fine twine attached to the leg. Bulbuls, although full of pluck, are not by nature quarrelsome. In order to make them fight they are kept without food for some time. Then two ravenous birds are shown the same piece of food. This, of course, leads to a fight, for a hungry bulbul is an angry bulbul.

Natives of India keep bulbuls for fighting. These birds aren't caged; instead, they are tied to a cloth-covered perch with a long piece of fine string attached to their leg. Bulbuls, despite their spirit, aren't naturally aggressive. To make them fight, they're kept without food for a while. Then, two starving birds are shown the same piece of food. This naturally leads to a fight because a hungry bulbul is an angry bulbul.

[235]


THE INDIAN WHISKEY

I have never been able to discover why the great black crow (Corvus macrorhynchus), so common in India, is called the jungle-crow. It is, indeed, true that the corby is found in the jungle, but it is found everywhere else in most parts of India, and is certainly abundant in villages and towns, being in some places quite as much a house bird as its smaller cousin, the grey-necked crow.

I’ve never figured out why the common black crow (Corvus macrorhynchus), which is widespread in India, is referred to as the jungle-crow. It’s true that the corby is found in the jungle, but it can also be seen almost everywhere else across India and is quite common in villages and towns. In some areas, it’s just as much a house bird as its smaller relative, the grey-necked crow.

Considering the character of the larger species and its extensive distribution, one hears remarkably little about it. The explanation is, of course, that the house-crow absorbs all the attention that man has to bestow upon the sable-plumaged tribe. The prevailing opinion seems to be that the black crow is merely a mild edition, a feeble imitation of, a scoundrel of lesser calibre than, its smaller cousin, Corvus splendens, and, therefore, everything that applies to the house-crow applies in a lesser degree to the big-billed bird. This is, I submit, a mistaken view, the result of imperfect observation. Corvus macrorhynchus has an individuality of his own, and we do him scant justice in dismissing him with a short paragraph at the foot of a lengthy description of Corvus splendens.

Considering the characteristics of the larger species and its wide distribution, it’s surprising how little is said about it. The reason, of course, is that the house-crow gets all the attention that humans have to give to the dark-feathered group. The general belief seems to be that the black crow is just a weaker version, a poor imitation of, a less significant rascal than its smaller relative, Corvus splendens, and so, everything that applies to the house-crow applies to the big-billed bird to a lesser extent. This, I believe, is a mistaken opinion stemming from limited observation. Corvus macrorhynchus has its own unique traits, and we do him a disservice by brushing him aside with a brief note at the end of a long description of Corvus splendens.

In saying this, I feel that I am speaking as one having [236] authority, and not as the Scribes and Pharisees, whose zoological horizon coincides with the limits of the museum. For a period of eighteen months I lived in a station which should be renamed and called Crowborough. To assert that the place in question swarms with crows is, of course, to assert nothing, for it shares this feature with every other place in India. The point I desire to bring out clearly is that in this particular place the black crows are nearly as numerous as the grey-necked birds. The former are certainly in a minority, but their minority is, like Sir Henry Campbell-Bannerman’s in the previous House of Commons, a large one, and what they lack in numbers they make up in weight and beak-force. It was truly delightful to watch them lord it over the grey-necked birds. Grammarians will observe that I here use the past tense. This is a point of some importance. Just as it is impossible to properly estimate the character of an eminent man during his lifetime, so is it to form a proper opinion of the personality and behaviour of a species of crow while one is in the midst of that species, while one is subjected to the persecutions, the annoyances, and the insults to which it thinks fit to treat one.

In saying this, I feel like I’m speaking with authority, not like the Scribes and Pharisees, whose knowledge is limited to what’s in the museum. For eighteen months, I lived in a place that should be called Crowborough. To say that the place is filled with crows is really not saying much, as that’s true for every other place in India. The main point I want to make is that in this specific place, black crows are almost as common as the grey-necked birds. The black crows are definitely in the minority, but their minority is significant, much like Sir Henry Campbell-Bannerman's position in the previous House of Commons, and what they lack in numbers they make up for in size and strength. It was genuinely enjoyable to watch them dominate the grey-necked birds. Grammarians will notice that I’m using the past tense here. This is an important point. Just as it’s impossible to truly assess the character of a prominent person during their lifetime, it’s equally hard to form a fair opinion of the personality and behavior of a species of crow while you’re right in the middle of it, dealing with the harassment, annoyances, and insults they choose to throw at you.

But I am now far away from Crowborough, and I may never again return thither. As I sit upon the Irish shore and see the blue waters of the North Atlantic roll softly up against the black rocks of Antrim, I feel that I am in a position to form a true estimate of the character of Corvus macrorhynchus.

But I am now far from Crowborough, and I may never return there. As I sit on the Irish shore and watch the blue waters of the North Atlantic gently roll against the black rocks of Antrim, I feel that I can accurately assess the character of Corvus macrorhynchus.

Until I went to Crowborough I laboured under the [237] delusion that the grey-necked crow knew not the meaning of the word “respect.” The deference with which the big-beaked species is treated by his smaller cousin came as a complete surprise to me.

Until I went to Crowborough, I was under the impression that the grey-necked crow didn’t understand the word “respect.” The way the big-beaked crow is treated with deference by its smaller cousin totally surprised me.

Most Anglo-Indians are so embittered against the whole tribe of the corvi that they will on no account feed them. I do not share this prejudice. I am able to see things from the corvine point of view. Were I a crow I should most certainly consider man fair game.

Most Anglo-Indians feel so bitter towards the whole group of the corvi that they would never feed them. I don’t share this bias. I can understand things from the crow's perspective. If I were a crow, I would definitely see humans as easy targets.

While in Crowborough I invariably gave the surplus of my tiffin to the crows. Those in the locality of my office window did not take long to find this out. The grey-necked crows were the first to make the discovery. It takes these less time to put two and two together than it does the more sluggish-brained black crows. At the end of a few days quite half-a-dozen grey-necked fellows had learned to hang about my windows at the luncheon hour. They used to sit in a row along each window-ledge. One day a corby appeared upon the scene. His arrival was the signal for the departure of his grey-necked brethren. From that day onwards he regarded that ledge as his special preserve, and whenever a house-crow ventured on to the ledge he “went for” it savagely with his great beak. The intruder never waited long enough to enable him to get a blow home. Thus the hunting-ground of the grey-necked crows became restricted to one of the window-ledges.

While in Crowborough, I always gave my leftover lunch to the crows. The ones near my office window quickly figured this out. The grey-necked crows were the first to catch on. They’re quicker to put two and two together than the slower black crows. After a few days, about half a dozen grey-necked crows learned to hang around my windows during lunch hour. They used to sit in a row along each window ledge. One day, a corby showed up. His arrival sent the grey-necked crows flying away. From that day on, he claimed that ledge as his own, and whenever a house-crow tried to land there, he would aggressively chase it off with his big beak. The intruder never stayed long enough for him to land a real hit. As a result, the grey-necked crows were left with just one of the window ledges to gather at.

In order to tease the black fellow I used sometimes to throw all the food to the window in which the grey crows were perched. He would fly round and drive them off that ledge and then give me a bit of his mind! [238] Later on he introduced his wife. She took possession of one window and he of the other; so that the poor house-crows no longer had “a look in.” Some of the bolder spirits among them used certainly to settle on the shutters in hopes of catching a stray crumb, but none durst venture on to the ledge while a black crow was there.

To mess with the black crow, I would sometimes toss all the food out the window where the gray crows were sitting. He would swoop around and chase them off that ledge, then give me a piece of his mind! [238] Later, he introduced his wife. She took one window, and he took the other, so the poor house-crows didn't stand a chance anymore. Some of the bolder ones would definitely land on the shutters, hoping to snag a stray crumb, but none dared to venture onto the ledge while the black crow was around.

Upon one occasion I put a whole milk pudding upon the ledge; the corbies would not allow the house-crows so much as a peck at the dainty dish until they themselves had had their fill.

Once, I placed a whole milk pudding on the ledge; the crows wouldn’t let the house-crows have even a peck at the delicious treat until they had eaten their fill.

Every one knows that the grey-necked crows, when harassing a creature more powerful than themselves, work in concert. It is my belief that two of these birds acting together are more than a match for any other creature. The way in which a pair of them will, by alternate feint and attack, take food away from a great kite or a dog is truly admirable. But so great is the respect of the grey-necked crows for the corby that I have never seen them attack him in this way. This says volumes for the force of character of Corvus macrorhynchus. He is quite an Oliver Cromwell among birds. He is a dour, austere, masterful, selfish bird—a bird which it is impossible to like or to despise.

Everyone knows that grey-necked crows, when they’re harassing a creature stronger than themselves, work together. I believe that two of these birds teaming up can outmatch any other creature. The way a pair of them will, through alternate feints and attacks, steal food from a big kite or a dog is truly impressive. However, the grey-necked crows have so much respect for the corby that I've never seen them attack him like this. This really highlights the strong character of Corvus macrorhynchus. He is the Oliver Cromwell among birds. He’s a solemn, serious, commanding, and selfish bird—a bird that’s impossible to either like or hate.

When he has once made up his mind to do anything there is no deterring him from the accomplishment thereof. Early in the year one of these birds spent at least the greater part of a day in trying to secure for its nest one of the twigs in a little circular fence erected for the protection of a young tree. The fence in question was composed of leafless branches, interlaced and [239] tied together. One of these twigs, being loose at one end, was pounced upon by a black crow who intended to carry it to his or her nest. But the other end was securely fastened. I watched that crow at intervals for several hours. Whenever I looked it was grappling in vain with the refractory twig. The work was, it is true, frequently interrupted, for natives kept passing by. But immediately the human being had gone, the crow resumed the attack. Every now and again it would fly to a dust-bin hard by and alight on the rim in order to take a breather. Occasionally it would dive into that bin in order to secure the wherewithal to feed the inner crow. It would then return to work like a giant refreshed.

When he decides to do something, nothing can stop him from getting it done. Early in the year, one of these birds spent most of a day trying to grab a twig for its nest from a little circular fence built to protect a young tree. The fence was made up of leafless branches that were intertwined and tied together. One of these twigs was loose at one end, and a black crow swooped in to take it to its nest. But the other end was firmly attached. I watched that crow at intervals for several hours. Each time I looked, it was struggling with the stubborn twig. The crow's efforts were often interrupted by people passing by. But as soon as a person left, the crow would go back to trying again. Every now and then, it would fly to a nearby dustbin and sit on the edge to catch its breath. Sometimes it even dove into the bin to find something to eat. It would then return to its work like a refreshed giant.

I am of opinion that that dust-bin was to the crow what the public-house is to the British working man.

I believe that the trash can was to the crow what the pub is to the British working man.

[241]


APPENDIX

A LIST OF THE BIRDS WHICH HAVE BEEN RECORDED BOTH IN THE BRITISH ISLES AND IN INDIA

A LIST OF BIRDS RECORDED IN BOTH THE BRITISH ISLES AND INDIA

1. Corvus corax. The Raven.
2. Corvus corone. The Carrion Crow.
3. Corvus frugilegus. The Rook.
4. Corvus comix. The Hooded Crow.
5. Corvus monedula. The Jackdaw.
6. Graculus eremita. The Red-billed Chough.
7. Pyrrhocorax alpinus. The Yellow-billed Chough.
8. Pica rustica. The Magpie.
9. Regulus cristatus. The Goldcrest.
10. Lanius collurio. The Red-backed Shrike.
11. Ampelis garrulus. The Waxwing.
12. Oriolus galbula. The Golden Oriole.
13. Pastor roseus. The Rose-coloured Starling.
14. Siphia parva. The European Red-breasted Flycatcher.
15. Muscicapa grisola. The Spotted Flycatcher.
16. Geocichla sibirica. The Siberian Ground Thrush.
17. Monticola saxatilis. The Rock Thrush.
18. Saxicola aenanthe. The Wheatear.
19. Cyanecula wolfi. The White spotted Bluethroat.
20. Turdus viscivorus. The Missel Thrush.
21. Turdus pilaris. The Fieldfare.
22. Turdus iliacus. The Redwing.
23. Linota cannabina. The Linnet.
[242]
24. Passer montanus. The Tree Sparrow.
25. Passer domesticus. The House Sparrow.
26. Emberiza schoeniclus. The Reed Bunting.
27. Emberiza pusilla. The Dwarf Bunting.
28. Emberiza hortulana. The Ortolan Bunting.
29. Emberiza melanocephala. The Black-headed Bunting
30. Fringilla montifringilla. The Brambling.
31. Alauda arvensis. The Skylark.
32. Calandrella brachydactyla. The Short-toed Lark.
33. Galerita cristata. The Crested Lark.
34. Anthus trivialis. The Tree Pipit.
35. Anthus richardi. Richard’s Pipit.
36. Anthus campestris. The Tawny Pipit.
37. Anthus spinoletta. The Water Pipit.
38. Anthus pratensis. The Meadow Pipit.
39. Hirundo rustica. The Swallow.
40. Cotile riparia. The Sand Martin.
41. Chelidon urbica. The Martin.
42. Motacilla alba. The White Wagtail.
43. Motacilla melanope. The Grey Wagtail.
44. Motacilla borealis. The Grey-headed Wagtail.
45. Motacilla flava. The Blue-headed Wagtail.
46. Iynx torquilla. The Wryneck.
47. Merops phillippinus. The Blue-tailed Bee-eater.
48. Merops apiaster. The European Bee-eater.
49. Upupa epops. The Hoopoe.
50. Coracias garrula. The European Roller.
51. Cypselus alpinus. The Alpine Swift.
52. Cypselus apus. The European Swift.
53. Caprimulgus europaeus. The European Nightjar.
54. Strix flammea. The Barn Owl.
55. Scops giu. The Scops Owl.
56. Asio otus. The Long-eared Owl.
[243]
57. Asio accipitrinus. The Short-eared Owl.
58. Bubo ignavus. The Eagle Owl.
59. Nyctea scandiaca. The Snowy Owl.
60. Alcedo ispida. The Common Kingfisher.
61. Cuculus canorus. The Cuckoo.
62. Gyps fulvus. The Griffon Vulture.
63. Neophron percnopterus. The Egyptian Vulture.
64. Milvus migrans. The Black Kite.
65. Haliaetus albicilla. The White-tailed Sea Eagle.
66. Pandion haliaetus. The Osprey.
67. Accipiter nisus. The Sparrow Hawk.
68. Astur palumbarius. The Goshawk.
69. Aquila chrysætus. The Golden Eagle.
70. Aquila maculata. The Large Spotted Eagle.
71. Buteo desertorum. The Common Buzzard.
72. Circus cineraceus. Montagu’s Harrier.
73. Circus cyaneus. The Hen Harrier.
74. Circus aeruginosus. The Marsh Harrier.
75. Elanus caeruleus. The Black-winged Kite.
76. Falco peregrinus. The Peregrine Falcon.
77. Falco subbuteo. The Hobby.
78. Aesalon regulus. The Merlin.
79. Tinnunculus alaudaris. The Kestrel.
80. Tinnunculus cenchris. The Lesser Kestrel.
81. Columbia livia. The Blue Rock Pigeon.
82. Turtur communis. The Turtle Dove.
83. Coturnix communis. The Quail.
84. Rallus aquaticus. The Water-Rail.
85. Crex pratensis. The Corn Crake.
86. Porzana parva. The Little Crake.
87. Porzana maruetta. The Spotted Crake.
88. Fulica atra. The Coot.
89. Gallinula chloropus. The Moorhen.
[244]
90. Grus communis. The Crane.
91. Anthropoides virgo. The Demoiselle Crane.
92. Otis tarda. The Great Bustard.
93. Otis tetrax. The Little Bustard.
94. Oedicnemus scolopa. The Stone Curlew.
95. Glareola pratincola. The Pratincole.
96. Cursorius gallicus. The Cream-coloured Courser.
97. Strepsilas interpres. The Turnstone.
98. Charadrius fulvus. The Eastern Golden Plover.
99. Charadrius pluvialis. The Golden Plover.
100. Vanellus vulgaris. The Lapwing.
101. Squatarola helvitica. The Grey Plover.
102. Aegialitis alexandrina. The Kentish Plover.
103. Aegialitis dubia. The Little Ringed Plover.
104. Aegialitis hiaticula. The Ringed Plover.
105. Haematopus ostralegus. The Oystercatcher.
106. Himantopus candidus. The Black-winged Stilt.
107. Limosa belgica. The Black-tailed Godwit.
108. Limosa lapponica. The Bar-tailed Godwit.
109. Numenius arquata. The Curlew,
110. Numenius phaeopus. The Whimbrel.
111. Recurvirostra avocetta. The Avocet.
112. Totanus hypoleucus. The Common Sandpiper.
113. Totanus glareola. The Wood Sandpiper.
114. Totanus ochropus. The Green Sandpiper.
115. Totanus calidris. The Redshank.
116. Totanus fuscus. The Spotted Redshank.
117. Totanus glottis. The Greenshank.
118. Tringa minuta. The Little Stint.
119. Tringa temmincki. Temminck’s Stint.
120. Tringa subarquata. The Curlew Stint.
121. Tringa alpina. The Dunlin.
122. Tringa platyrhyncha. The Broad-billed Stint.
[245]
123. Calidris arenaria. The Sanderling.
124. Pavoncella pugnax. The Ruff.
125. Phalaropus hyperboreus. The Red-necked Phalarope.
126. Phalaropus fulicarius. The Grey Phalarope.
127. Scolopax rusticula. The Woodcock.
128. Gallinago coelestis. The Common Snipe.
129. Gallinago gallinula. The Jack Snipe.
130. Larus ichthyaetus. The Great Black-billed Gull.
131. Larus ridibundus. The Laughing Gull.
132. Larus affinis. The Dark-backed Herring Gull.
133. Hydrochelidon hybrida. The Whiskered Tern.
134. Hydrochelidon leucoptera. The White-winged Black Tern.
135. Sterna angelica. The Gull-billed Tern.
136. Sterna cantiaca. The Sandwich Tern.
137. Sterna fluviatilis. The Common Tern.
138. Sterna dougalli. The Roseate Tern.
139. Sterna minuta. The Little Tern.
140. Sterna fuliginosa. The Sooty Tern.
141. Hydroprogne caspia. The Caspian Tern.
142. Stercorarius crepidatus. Richardson’s Skua.
143. Stercorarius pomatorhinus. The Pomatorhine Skua.
144. Oceanites oceanicus. Wilson’s Petrel.
145. Anous stolidus. The Noddy.
146. Phalacrocorax carbo. The Cormorant.
147. Platalea leucorodia. The Spoonbill.
148. Nycticorax griseus. The Night Heron.
149. Ardea manillensis. The Purple Heron.
150. Ardea cinerea. The Common Heron.
151. Herodias alba. The Large Egret.
152. Herodias garzetta. The Little Egret.
153. Bulbulcus coromnandus. The Cattle Egret.
154. Ardetta minuta. The Little Bittern.
[246]
155. Ciconia alba. The White Stork.
156. Ciconia nigra. The Black Stork.
157. Plegadis falcinellus. The Glossy Ibis.
158. Phoenicopterus roseus. The Flamingo.
159. Cygnus olor. The Mute Swan.
160. Cygnus musicus. The Whooper.
161. Anser ferus. The Grey-lag Goose.
162. Anser albifrons. The White-fronted Goose.
163. Anser erythropus. The Lesser White-fronted Goose.
164. Anser brachyrhynchus. The Pink-footed Goose.
165. Tadorna cornuta. The Sheld-Duck.
166. Casarca rutila. The Brahminy Duck.
167. Mareca penelope. The Widgeon.
168. Anas boscas. The Mallard.
169. Chaulelasmus streperus. The Gadwall.
170. Nyroca ferruginea. The White-eyed Duck.
171. Nyroca ferina. The Pochard.
172. Nyroca marila. The Scaup.
173. Nyroca fuligula. The Tufted Duck.
174. Netta rufina. The Red-crested Pochard.
175. Dafila acuta. The Pintail.
176. Clangula glaucion. The Golden-Eye.
177. Spatula clypeata. The Shoveller.
178. Querquedula urcia. The Garganey Teal.
179. Nettium crecca. The Common Teal.
180. Podiceps cristatus. The Great Crested Grebe.
181. Podiceps nigricollis. The Eared Grebe.
182. Mergus albellus. The Smew.
183. Merganser castor. The Goosander.
184. Merganser serrator. The Red-breasted Merganser.
[247]


Glossary

Babul. Acacia arabica. A thorny tree.

Babul. Acacia arabica. A spiky tree.

Badmash. A bad character, a ruffian.

Badmash. A troublemaker, a thug.

Barsath. Rain.

Barsath. Rainy.

Bhabar. The waterless tract of forest-clad land between the Himalayas and the Terai. It is from ten to fifteen miles in breadth and higher than the general level of the plains.

Bhabar. The dry area of forested land between the Himalayas and the Terai. It is about ten to fifteen miles wide and sits higher than the overall level of the plains.

Chaprassi. Lit. a badgeman. A servant who runs messages, an orderly.

Chaprassi. Literally, a badgeman. A messenger, an orderly.

Chik. A number of thin pieces of bamboo strung together to form a curtain. Thin chiks are usually hung in front of doors in India with the object of keeping out flies but not air. Chiks of stouter make are hung from the verandah in order to keep out the sun.

Chik. A bunch of thin bamboo strips tied together to create a curtain. Thin chiks are typically hung in front of doors in India to keep flies out while still allowing air to flow. Heavier chiks are hung from the porch to block out the sun.

Chit. Short for Chitti, a letter or testimonial.

Chit. Short for Chitti, a note or recommendation.

Coolie. An unskilled labourer.

Coolie. A manual laborer.

Dhak. Butea frondosa. A common tree in low jungle.

Dhak. Butea frondosa. A prevalent tree in the low jungle.

Dhobi. Washerman.

Laundry worker.

Dirzie. Tailor.

Dirzie. Tailor.

Farash. Tamarix indica.

Farash. Tamarix indica.

Gali galoj. Abuse.

Gali galoj. Harassment.

Jhil. A lake, broad tank, or any natural depression which is filled with rain water at certain seasons or permanently.

Jhil. A lake, large pond, or any natural low area that collects rainwater at certain times of the year or holds it permanently.

Kankar, or Kunkar. Lumps of limestone with which roads are metalled in Northern India.

Kankar, or Kunkar. Large chunks of limestone used for surfacing roads in Northern India.

Kannaut. The sides of a tent.

Kannaut. The tent sides.

[248]

Khansamah. Cook.

Khansamah. Chef.

Khud. A deep valley.

Me. A deep valley.

Mali. Gardener.

Mali. Gardener.

Murghi. Barndoor Fowl.

Chicken. Barndoor Fowl.

Neem. Azadirachta melia, a common tree in India.

Neem. Azadirachta melia, a widely found tree in India.

Paddy. Growing rice.

Paddy. Cultivating rice.

Puggarree. A turban.

Puggarree. A headscarf.

Ryot. A cultivator, small farmer.

Ryot. A small farmer.

Sal. The iron-wood tree (Shorea robusta).

Sal. The sal tree (Shorea robusta).

Sahib. Master, sir, gentleman; a term used to denote a European.

Sahib. Master, sir, or gentleman; a term used to refer to a European.

Shikar. Hunting or shooting.

Shikar. Hunting.

Shikari. (1) The man who goes hunting or shooting. (2) The native who accompanies him and directs the beat.

Shikari. (1) The person who goes hunting or shooting. (2) The local who goes with him and guides the beat.

Terai. Lit. “Moist land.” A marshy tract of land about twelve miles broad, between the Bhabar and the plains proper. It is low-lying.

Terai. Literally, “Moist land.” A marshy area about twelve miles wide, located between the Bhabar and the actual plains. It is low-lying.

Tiffin. Lunch.

Tiffin. Lunch break.

Topi. A sun-helmet.

Topi. A sun hat.

[249]

With the exception of British Birds in the Plains of India, which appeared in The Civil and Military Gazette, and The Indian Corby, Birds in the Rain, and Do Animals Think? which came out in The Times of India, the articles which compose this book made their debût in one or other of the following papers: The Madras Mail, The Indian Field, The Englishman.

With the exception of British Birds in the Plains of India, which was published in The Civil and Military Gazette, and The Indian Corby, Birds in the Rain, and Do Animals Think?, which were featured in The Times of India, the articles in this book first appeared in one or more of the following newspapers: The Madras Mail, The Indian Field, The Englishman.

The author takes this opportunity of thanking the editors of the above-named newspapers for permission to reproduce these essays.

The author would like to thank the editors of the newspapers mentioned above for allowing the reproduction of these essays.

[253]


INDEX

A · B · C · D · E · F · G · H · I · J · K · L · M · N · O · P · Q · R · S · T · U · V · W · X · Y · Z

A · B · C · D · E · F · G · H · I · J · K · L · M · N · O · P · Q · R · S · T · U · V · W · X · Y · Z

A
Acridotheres tristis, 94
Adjutant, 29-35
Aitken, Mr. Benjamin, 165
Aitken, Mr. E. H., 100, 164
Alauda arvensis, 5
Alauda gulgula, 5
Alcedo ispida, 5
Amadavat, 17, 20, 46-51, 52, 165
Anderson, Mr. A., 126
Aquila chrysætus, 175
Aquila vindhiana, 175
Arachnechthra asiatica, 79, 80
Arachnechthra lotenia, 79
Arachnechthra zeylonica, 80
Ardea cinerea, 6
Ardeola grayii, 115
Argya caudata, 127
Athene brama, 24, 142
Automata, birds as, 104-110
Avicultural Society’s Magazine, 74
B
Babbler, 181
Babbler, common, 127
Babbler, striated bush-, 127
Barnes, 74
Baya, 183-189, 219, 221
Bee-eater, blue-tailed, 212
Bee-eater, little green, 208-212
Bingham, Major C.T., 210
Biology, dogmatism of, 129
Blackbird, 1, 107
Blackwell, Mr., 110
Blanford, 203
“Blue Jay,” 10-15, 84
Blyth, 30
Bonelli’s eagle, 175
Bottle bird, 185
Brachypternus aurantius, 86
Brain-fever bird, 117
Bulbul, 1, 166, 229-234
Bulbul, black, 229
Bulbul, green, 229
Bulbul, red-vented, 229-234
Bulbul, red-whiskered, 228-234
Bulbul, white-browed, 229
Burroughs, Mr. John, 213
Butcher bird, 163-167
Butler, Colonel, 211
Buzzard, 6
Buzzard, white-eyed, 176
C
Caccabis chucar, 89
Centropus phasianus, 207
Cercomela fusca, 130
Chaffinch, 1
Chambers’s Journal, 30
Character, bird of, 94-98
Chick, 113
Chicken, 113, 116
Chukor, 89, 90
Cobbler, 62
Colouration, protective, 59, 128
Columba intermedia, 6
Columba livia, 6
Coot, 14
Coracias indica, 12, 54
Corby, Indian, 235-239
[254]
Corvus, 112, 142, 145, 147, 176, 237
Corvus corax, 4
Corvus lawrencii, 4
Corvus macrorhynchus, 235, 236, 238
Corvus splendens, 5, 111, 176, 235
Coturnix communis, 7
Cowper, 92
Craftsmen, a couple of neglected, 219-223
Cranes, 35-37
Crow, 5, 24-26, 30, 40, 68, 95, 106, 111-115, 118-123, 142, 144, 145-149, 175, 176, 179, 180, 184, 190, 191, 197, 213, 214, 235-239
Cuckoo, 9, 24, 107, 108, 117, 118, 120
Cuckoo, drongo-, 202-207
Cuckoo, playing, 111-116, 133
Cullen, Rev. C. D., 165
Cunningham, Colonel, 31, 32, 118, 164, 184, 190
Cypselus affinis, 101
Cypselus batassiensis, 101
D
Darwin, Charles, 158, 160
Darwinian theory, 23, 129
Davison, 74, 203
Deceiver, gay, 202-207
Dendrocita rufa, 68
Dicrurus, 203
Did-he-do-it, 56-61
Difficulties of bird photography, 225
Dimorphism, sexual, 80, 81, 128, 156
Dog, 215, 216
Dogmatism of biology, 129
Don Quixote, 31
Dove, 12, 124-129
Dove, little brown, 124-134
Dove, red turtle, 128, 129
Dove, ring-, 124-129
Dove, spotted, 124-129
Dragon-fly, 181
Drongo,139
Drongo-cuckoo, 202-207
Duck, 8, 168, 169
E
Eagle, 54, 173-177
Eagle, Bonelli’s, 175, 177
Eagle, golden, 175
Eagle, tawny, 176, 177
Edible birds’ nests, 102
“Eha,” 29, 48, 57, 131, 220, 230
Endynamis honorata, 111, 117
F
Falcon, 6, 54
Fauna of British India, 158
Feminine selection, 160
Finn, Mr. Frank, 75, 232
Firefly, 188, 189
Flycatcher, paradise, 13, 81, 150-162
Flycatcher, white-browed fantailed, 146, 158
Fowl’s egg, 113, 114
Fox, 54
Francolinus pondicerianus, 90
Fulica atra, 5
G
Gadwall, 8, 171
Galerita cristata, 7
Gallinago coelestis, 8
Gallinago gallinula, 8
Godwin, Earl, 169
Goose, grey-lag, 8
Grackle, 98
Grahame, 92
Green parrot, 88, 190-196, 197
Grey partridge, 90-93
Grouse, 93
Grouse, red, 1
Guinea-fowl, 93
H
Halcyon smyrnensis, 4
[255]
Harper’s Magazine, 213
Harrier, hen, 6
Harrier, marsh, 6
Hawk-cuckoo, 117
Hawkes, Mr. S. M., 172
Hawk, sparrow-, 6
Headley, Mr. F. W., 54
Heron, 6
Herrick, Mr. F. H., 225, 226, 227
Hewer of wood, 84
Hieraëtus fasciatus, 175
Hierococcyx varius, 117
Hill-myna, 98
Home Life of Birds, 225
Homer, 57
Homo sapiens, 140
Honeysucker, 78-83, 166
Hoopoe, 14, 84, 182
Hornbill, 166
House martin, 110
Hudson, Mr. W. H., 107, 108
Hume, 18, 74, 129, 137, 189, 193
Humming-bird, 78
Hutton, Captain, 192
I
Instinct, 27, 107, 109, 121
— maternal, 110
— parasitic, 120, 121
— parental, 27, 104
Isolation, 232
J
Jackdaw, 4, 5
Jay, blue, 10-15, 84
Jerdon, 38, 69, 70, 74, 137, 140, 164, 175, 183, 188, 210
Jesse, 222
Jungle crow, 235-239
K
Kearton, Mr. R., 105, 106
Kestrel, 6, 175
King-crow, 12, 40, 138, 139, 203-207
Kingfisher, 3, 144
Kingfisher, white-breasted, 4, 86
Kipling, Lockwood, 31, 91
Kite, 12, 106, 144-149, 166, 175, 177
Koel, 111-115
L
Lal munia, 46
Lanius lahtora, 165
Lanius vittatus, 163, 165
Lapwing, 166
Lapwing, red-wattled, 56-61
Lapwing, yellow-wattled, 57
Larder, shrike’s, 163-165
Lark, crested, 7
Legge, 74
Leptoptilus dubius, 29
Leptoptilus javanicus, 34
Lilford, Lord, 143
Lobivanellus goensis, 56
Lockwood Kipling, 31, 91
M
Magpie, 68-72
Magpie-robin, 84
Mallard, 8
Malvolio, 31
Martin, 100, 110
Merlin, 6
Merops, emerald, 208-212
Merops philippinus, 212
Merops viridis, 208-212
Milk, pigeon’s, 131
Minah, brahminy, 166
Minivet, 161
Molpastes, 229-234
Molpastes hæmorrhous, 231
Molpastes indicus, 231
Molpastes intermedius, 231
Monarch, dethroned, 173-177
Motacilla maderaspatensis, 26
Munia, red, 45-51
Munia, spotted, 52-55
Myna, 1, 40, 84, 86, 94-98, 181, 197
N
Natural Selection, 23, 24, 42, 105, 109, 129, 202-207, 232, 234
[256]
Neo-Darwinian School, 124, 128
Nest, sanitation of, 224
Nests, birds in their, 224-228
Nests, edible birds’, 102
Newman, Mr. T. H., 75
Nutmeg bird, 52-55
O
Oates, 158
Œnopopelia tranquebarica, 128
Oriole, black-headed, 135
Oriole, Indian, 13, 134-139
Oriolus kundoo, 135
Oriolus melanocephalus, 135
Orthotomus, 222
Orthotomus sutorius, 62-67, 219
Otocompsa, 229-34
Otocompsa emeria, 231
Otocompsa fuscicaudata, 231
Owl, barn, 3, 140-144
Owl, screech, 144
Owl, white, 141, 142
Owlet, spotted, 94, 142
P
Paddy bird, 115, 116
Palæornis nepalensis, 194
Palæornis torquatus, 194
Palm swift, 101
Paradise flycatcher, 13, 81, 150-162
Parakeet, 1
Parental instinct, 27, 104
Paroquet, Alexandrine, 194
Paroquet, rose-ringed, 194
Parrot, 95, 184
Parrot catching, 194
Parrot, green, 88, 166, 190-196, 197
Parrot, West African, 195
Partridge, grey, 90-93, 166
Passer domesticus, 2, 75, 76
Peacock, 42-44
Peafowl, 93
Pearson, Professor, 42, 44
Peregrine falcon, 6
Pericrocotus peregrinus, 161
Pheasant, 92, 144
Photography, difficulties of bird, 225
Pica rustica, 68
Pie, tree, 68-72
Pigeon, 6, 133, 134, 184
Pintail, 8
Pinto, Mr. G. A., 63, 64, 66, 158
Pipit, 7
Playing cuckoo, 111-116
Ploceus baya, 183, 219
Plover, Kentish, 9
Plover, ringed, 9
Pochard, white-eyed, 171
Prinia inornata, 219
Prinia socialis, 222
Protective colouration, 59, 93, 128
Q
Quail, 7, 93
R
Rain, birds in the, 178-182
Raven, 3, 4
Red munia, 46-51, 52-54
Redshank, 9
Reid, 74, 222
Rhipidura albifrontata, 146, 158
Robin, Indian, 128
Robin redbreast, 1, 107
Rock chat, brown, 130
Roller, 12
Rook, 4, 5
S
Salvadori, 74
Sandpiper, 9
Sanyal, Babu, 38, 39
Sarciophorus malabaricus, 57
Sarcogrammus indicus, 56
Sarus, 35-39
Selection, feminine, 160, 161
Selection, natural, 23, 24, 42, 105, 109, 129, 202-207, 232, 234
Selection, sexual, 42, 160, 161, 205-207
“Seven sisters,” 1
[257]
Sexual dimorphism, 80, 81, 128, 156
Sexual selection, 42, 160, 161, 205-207
Shikra, 175
Shrike, 164-167
Shrike, bay-backed, 165
Shrike, Indian grey-backed, 165
Shrike, rufous-backed, 165
Shoveller, 8
Sisters, seven, 1
Skylark, 5
Skylark, Indian, 5
Smell, sense of, in birds, 123
Smith, Mr. Bosworth, 141
Snipe, full, 8
Snipe, Jack, 8
Sparrow, 2, 3, 16-22, 75, 76, 143, 197-201
Sparrow, hedge, 185
Sparrow-hawk, 6, 166, 175, 200
Spectator, 168, 169
Spice bird, 52-55
Sporæginthus amandava, 46
Spotted-bill duck, 171
Spotted owlet, 94
Sprinter, feathered, 89-94
Stability of species, 40-45
Starling, 88, 106
Stint, 9
Stork, 35, 36
Strix flammea, 3, 141
Strix javanica, 141
Sunbird, 78-83
Surniculus lugubris, 202-207
Swallow, 84, 99, 100
Swift, 84, 99-103
T
Tailor-bird, 62-67, 137, 219
Teal, 8
Terminology, ornithological, 73
Terpsiphone paradisi, 81, 156
Tetrao scoticus, 1
Thamnobia cambayensis, 128, 130
Think, Do animals? 213-218
Thirst of young birds, assuaging of, 225-228
Thorndike, Professor, 214
Thrush, 1, 105, 106
Tit, blue, 1
Tragedy, tree-top, 145-149
Tree creeper, 84
Turkey, 93
Turtle dove, red, 128
Turtur cambayensis, 125
Turtur decaocta, 74, 75
Turtur risorius, 74, 75, 124
Turtur suratensis, 124
U
Uroloncha pundulata, 52
V
Vidal, 74
Vulture, 175
W
Wagtail, 7
Wagtail, pied, 26, 27, 180
Wallace, 161
Warbler, 219, 220
Weaver-bird, 137, 183-189, 219
“White ant,” 182
White-breasted kingfisher, 4, 86
Widgeon, 8
Woodpecker, golden-backed, 84-88
Wren, 1, 219
Wren-warbler, ashy, 222, 223
Wren-warbler, Indian, 219-222
Wryneck, 84
[258]

BY THE SAME AUTHOR

BOMBAY DUCKS: An account of some of the Every-day Birds and Beasts found in a Naturalist’s El Dorado · With Numerous Illustrations from Photographs of Living Birds by Captain F. D. S. Fayrer, I.M.S.

BOMBAY DUCKS: A tale about some of the common birds and animals found in a naturalist's paradise · Featuring many illustrations from photographs of real birds by Captain F. D. S. Fayrer, I.M.S.

ANIMALS OF NO IMPORTANCE

ANIMALS THAT DON'T MATTER

THE INDIAN CROW: HIS BOOK

THE INDIAN CROW: HIS STORY

[259]

THE WORKS OF ANATOLE FRANCE

It has long been a reproach to England that only one volume by ANATOLE FRANCE has been adequately rendered into English; yet outside this country he shares with TOLSTOI the distinction of being the greatest and most daring student of humanity now living.

It has long been a criticism of England that only one book by ANATOLE FRANCE has been properly translated into English; yet outside this country, he shares with TOLSTOI the honor of being one of the greatest and most fearless scholars of humanity alive today.

¶ There have been many difficulties to encounter in completing arrangements for a uniform edition, though perhaps the chief barrier to publication here has been the fact that his writings are not for babes—but for men and the mothers of men. Indeed, some of his Eastern romances are written with biblical candour. “I have sought truth strenuously,” he tells us, “I have met her boldly. I have never turned from her even when she wore an [260] unexpected aspect.” Still, it is believed that the day has come for giving English versions of all his imaginative works, and of his monumental study JOAN OF ARC, which is undoubtedly the most discussed book in the world of letters to-day.

¶ There have been many challenges in finalizing plans for a uniform edition, but perhaps the biggest hurdle to publication has been the reality that his writings are not meant for children—but for adults and the mothers of adults. In fact, some of his Eastern romances are written with a biblical honesty. “I have pursued truth tenaciously,” he shares, “I have confronted her fearlessly. I have never shied away from her, even when she took on an unexpected form.” Still, it’s believed the time has come to provide English versions of all his imaginative works, including his monumental study JOAN OF ARC, which is undoubtedly the most talked-about book in the literary world today.

¶ MR. JOHN LANE has pleasure in announcing that he will commence publication of the works of M. ANATOLE FRANCE in English, under the general editorship of MR. FREDERIC CHAPMAN, with the following volumes:

¶ MR. JOHN LANE is excited to announce that he will start publishing the works of M. ANATOLE FRANCE in English, with MR. FREDERIC CHAPMAN serving as the general editor, beginning with the following volumes:

THE RED LILY
MOTHER OF PEARL
THE GARDEN OF EPICURUS
THE CRIME OF SYLVESTRE BONNARD
THE WELL OF ST. CLARE
THE OPINIONS OF JEROME COIGNARD
JOCASTA AND THE FAMISHED CAT
BALTHASAR
THE ASPIRATIONS OF JEAN SERVIEN
THE ELM TREE ON THE MALL
MY FRIEND’S BOOK
THE WICKER-WORK WOMAN
THAÏS
AT THE SIGN OF THE QUEEN PÉDAUQUE
JOAN OF ARC (2 vols.)

¶ All the books will be published at 6/- each with the exception of JOAN OF ARC, which will be 25/- net the two volumes, with eight Illustrations.

¶ All the books will be published at £6 each, except for JOAN OF ARC, which will be £25 for the two volumes, including eight illustrations.

¶ The format of the volumes leaves little to be desired. The size is Demy 8vo (9 × 5¾ in.), that of this Prospectus, and they will be printed from Caslon type upon a paper light in weight and strong in texture, with a cover design in crimson and gold, a gilt top, end-papers from designs by Aubrey Beardsley and initials by Henry Ospovat. In short, these are volumes for the bibliophile as well as the lover of fiction, and form perhaps the cheapest library edition of copyright novels ever published, for the price is only that of an ordinary novel.

¶ The format of the volumes is quite impressive. The size is Demy 8vo (9 × 5¾ in.), the same as this Prospectus, and they will be printed in Caslon type on lightweight yet sturdy paper, featuring a cover design in crimson and gold, a gilt top, endpapers designed by Aubrey Beardsley, and initials by Henry Ospovat. In short, these volumes cater to both bibliophiles and fiction lovers, and they might just be the most affordable library edition of copyright novels ever published, as the price is only that of a typical novel.

¶ The translation of these books has been entrusted to such competent French scholars as MR. ALFRED ALLINSON, HON. MAURICE BARING, MR. FREDERIC CHAPMAN, MR. [261] ROBERT B. DOUGLAS, MR. A. W. EVANS, MRS. FARLEY, MRS. JOHN LANE, MRS. NEWMARCH, MR. C. E. ROCHE, MISS WINIFRED STEPHENS, and MISS M. P. WILLCOCKS.

¶ The translation of these books has been given to capable French scholars like Mr. Alfred Allinson, Hon. Maurice Baring, MR. FREDERIC CHAPMAN, Mr. [261] ROBERT B. DOUGLAS, Mr. A.W. Evans, Mrs. Farley, Mrs. John Lane, MRS. NEWMARCH, Mr. C. E. Roche, Miss Winifred Stephens, and MISS M. P. WILLCOCKS.

¶ As Anatole Thibault, dit Anatole France, is to most English readers merely a name, it will be well to state that he was born in 1844 in the picturesque and inspiring surroundings of an old bookshop on the Quai Voltaire, Paris, kept by his father, Monsieur Thibault, an authority on eighteenth-century history, from whom the boy caught the passion for the principles of the Revolution, while from his mother he was learning to love the ascetic ideals chronicled in the Lives of the Saints. He was schooled with the lovers of old books, missals and manuscripts; he matriculated on the Quais with the old Jewish dealers of curios and objets d’art; he graduated in the great university of life and experience. It will be recognised that all his work is permeated by his youthful impressions; he is, in fact, a virtuoso at large.

¶ For most English readers, Anatole Thibault, also known as Anatole France, is just a name. It's worth mentioning that he was born in 1844 in the charming and inspiring setting of an old bookshop on the Quai Voltaire in Paris, run by his father, Monsieur Thibault, who was an expert on eighteenth-century history. From him, Anatole developed a passion for the principles of the Revolution, while his mother instilled in him a love for the ascetic ideals described in the Lives of the Saints. He grew up among enthusiasts of rare books, missals, and manuscripts; he mingled with the old Jewish dealers of curios and art objects along the Quais; he graduated from the great university of life and experience. One can clearly see that all his work is influenced by his early experiences; he is, in essence, a consummate artist at large.

¶ He has written about thirty volumes of fiction. His first novel was JOCASTA & THE FAMISHED CAT (1879). THE CRIME OF SYLVESTRE BONNARD appeared in 1881, and had the distinction of being crowned by the French Academy, into which he was received in 1896.

¶ He has written about thirty books of fiction. His first novel was JOCASTA & THE FAMISHED CAT (1879). THE CRIME OF SYLVESTRE BONNARD was published in 1881 and was honored by being awarded a prize by the French Academy, which he joined in 1896.

¶ His work is illuminated with style, scholarship, and psychology; but its outstanding features are the lambent wit, the gay mockery, the genial irony with which he touches every subject he treats. But the wit is never malicious, the mockery never derisive, the irony never barbed. To quote from his own GARDEN OF EPICURUS: “Irony and Pity are both of good counsel; the first with her smiles makes life agreeable, the other sanctifies it to us with her tears. The Irony I invoke is no cruel deity. She mocks neither love nor beauty. She is gentle and kindly disposed. Her mirth disarms anger and it is she teaches us to laugh at rogues and fools whom but for her we might be so weak as to hate.”

¶ His work shines with style, knowledge, and psychological insight; but what really stands out is the lighthearted wit, playful mockery, and friendly irony he uses to approach every topic. Yet, the wit is never hurtful, the mockery is never scornful, and the irony is never sharp. To quote from his own GARDEN OF EPICURUS: “Irony and Pity are both wise guides; the first brings joy to life with her smiles, the other gives it meaning with her tears. The Irony I call upon is no cruel goddess. She doesn’t mock love or beauty. She is gentle and kind-hearted. Her laughter disarms anger and teaches us to laugh at the rogues and fools we might otherwise be foolish enough to hate.”

¶ Often he shows how divine humanity triumphs over mere ascetism, and with entire reverence; indeed, he might be described as an ascetic overflowing with humanity, just as he has been termed a “pagan, but a pagan constantly haunted by the pre-occupation of Christ.” He is in turn—like his own Choulette in THE RED LILY—saintly and Rabelaisian, yet without incongruity. [262] At all times he is the unrelenting foe of superstition and hypocrisy. Of himself he once modestly said: “You will find in my writings perfect sincerity (lying demands a talent I do not possess), much indulgence, and some natural affection for the beautiful and good.”

¶ Often he demonstrates how the divine aspect of humanity prevails over mere asceticism, and with complete respect; indeed, he could be seen as an ascetic full of humanity, just as he has been described as a “pagan, but a pagan constantly troubled by the thought of Christ.” He is, in turn—like his own Choulette in THE RED LILY—saintly and Rabelaisian, yet without contradiction. [262] At all times, he is the relentless enemy of superstition and hypocrisy. He once humbly stated about himself: “You will find in my writings perfect sincerity (lying requires a talent I do not have), a lot of indulgence, and some natural affection for beauty and goodness.”

¶ The mere extent of an author’s popularity is perhaps a poor argument, yet it is significant that two books by this author are in their HUNDRED AND TENTH THOUSAND, and numbers of them well into their SEVENTIETH THOUSAND, whilst the one which a Frenchman recently described as “Monsieur France’s most arid book” is in its FIFTY-EIGHTH THOUSAND.

¶ The sheer level of an author’s popularity might not be the best measure of quality, but it's noteworthy that two of this author's books have reached their HUNDRED AND TENTH THOUSAND, and several others are well into their SEVENTIETH THOUSAND, while the one that a Frenchman recently called “Monsieur France’s most boring book” is in its FIFTY-EIGHTH THOUSAND.

¶ Inasmuch as M. FRANCE’S ONLY contribution to an English periodical appeared in THE YELLOW BOOK, vol. v., April 1895, together with the first important English appreciation of his work from the pen of the Hon. Maurice Baring, it is peculiarly appropriate that the English edition of his works should be issued from the Bodley Head.

¶ Since M. FRANCE’S ONLY contribution to an English magazine was published in THE YELLOW BOOK, vol. v., April 1895, alongside the first significant English review of his work by the Hon. Maurice Baring, it makes perfect sense for the English edition of his works to be released by the Bodley Head.

ORDER FORM

Order Form

_________________190

_________________190

To Mr._______________________

To Mr._______________________

Bookseller

Bookstore

Please send me the following works of Anatole France to be issued in June and July:

Please send me the following works by Anatole France to be released in June and July:

THE RED LILY
MOTHER OF PEARL
THE GARDEN OF EPICURUS
THE CRIME OF SYLVESTRE BONNARD

for which I enclose_______

enclosed for your reference_______

Name______________________

Name______________________

Address___________________

Address___________________

JOHN LANE, Publisher, The Bodley Head, Vigo St. London, W.

JOHN LANE, Publisher: The Bodley Head, Vigo St, London, W.

[263]

BOMBAY DUCKS

AN ACCOUNT OF SOME OF THE EVERYDAY BIRDS & BEASTS FOUND IN A NATURALIST’S EL DORADO

AN ACCOUNT OF SOME OF THE COMMON BIRDS & ANIMALS FOUND IN A NATURALIST’S EL DORADO

BY DOUGLAS DEWAR, F.Z.S., I.C.S.

BY DOUGLAS DEWAR, F.Z.S., I.C.S.

With Numerous Illustrations from Photographs of Living Birds by Captain F. D. S. Fayrer, I.M.S.

With many illustrations from photographs of living birds by Captain F. D. S. Fayrer, I.M.S.

PRESS OPINIONS.

PRESS REVIEWS.

Spectator.—“Mr. Douglas Dewar’s book is excellent. . . . A feature of the book is the photographs of birds by Captain Fayrer. They are most remarkable, and quite unlike the usual wretched snapshot and blurred reproductions with which too many naturalists’ books are nowadays illustrated.”

Spectator.—“Mr. Douglas Dewar’s book is excellent. . . . A highlight of the book is the amazing bird photographs by Captain Fayrer. They are truly remarkable and completely different from the typical poor snapshots and blurry images that fill too many naturalists’ books today.”

Standard.—“The East has ever been a place of wonderment, but the writer of ‘Bombay Ducks’ brings before Western eyes a new set of pictures. . . . The book is entertaining, even to the reader who is not a naturalist first and a reader afterwards. . . . The illustrations cannot be too highly praised.”

Standard.—“The East has always been a place of wonder, but the author of ‘Bombay Ducks’ presents a fresh perspective to Western readers. . . . The book is enjoyable, even for those who aren't naturalists first and readers second. . . . The illustrations are truly commendable.”

Daily News.—“This new and sumptuous book. . . . Mr. Dewar gives us a charming introduction to a great many interesting birds.”

Daily News.—“This new and lavish book. . . . Mr. Dewar provides us with a delightful introduction to many fascinating birds.”

Pall Mall Gazette.—“Most entertaining dissertations on the tricks and manners of many birds and beasts in India.”

Pall Mall Gazette.—“Very engaging discussions about the behaviors and habits of various birds and animals in India.”

Graphic.—“The book is written in a most readable style, light and easy, yet full of information, and not overburdened with scientific words and phrases. . . . The habits of the different birds are fully described, often in a very amusing and interesting manner.”

Graphic.—“The book is written in a very readable style, light and easy, yet full of information, and not overloaded with scientific terms and phrases. . . . The behaviors of various birds are described in detail, often in a very entertaining and engaging way.”

Outlook.—“Pleasant reading, with pretty touches of the author’s own fancy; a good deal of information agreeably conveyed. . . . The illustrations are of an extremely high order, constituting not only a beautiful, but a really valuable series of portraits.”

Outlook.—“Enjoyable read, with nice elements of the author's creativity; a lot of information shared in a pleasant way. . . . The illustrations are of exceptionally high quality, making up not just a beautiful collection but also a truly valuable series of portraits.”

County Gentleman.—“Thoroughly entertaining to all who can appreciate either animal life as seen through practised eyes, or witty and humorous writing in any form. . . . The book is handsomely produced, and is altogether an attractive acquisition.”

County Gentleman.—“It's completely enjoyable for anyone who appreciates animal life viewed with expert insight or humorous and witty writing in any style. . . . The book is beautifully made and is definitely a charming addition.”

Illustrated London News.—“Mr. Dewar . . . has collected a series of essays on bird life which for sprightliness and charm are equal to anything written since that classic, ‘The Tribes on my Frontier,’ was published.”

Illustrated London News.—“Mr. Dewar . . . has put together a collection of essays on bird life that are as lively and delightful as anything written since that classic, ‘The Tribes on my Frontier,’ was published.”

Indian Daily News.—“Mr. Dewar’s excellent book. . . . We sincerely hope that our readers will derive the same lively pleasure from the reading of this book as we have done.”

Indian Daily News.—“Mr. Dewar’s fantastic book. . . . We genuinely hope that our readers will get as much enjoyment from reading this book as we have.”

Yorkshire Daily Observer.—“This handsome and charming book . . . the author has many interesting observations to record, and he does so in a very racy manner.”

Yorkshire Daily Observer.—“This attractive and delightful book . . . the author has a lot of fascinating insights to share, and he presents them in a very engaging way.”

Dublin Express.—“Mr. Dewar’s account of the ‘Naturalist’s El Dorado’ is particularly captivating, and is rendered not the less so by the splendidly produced photographs of living birds.”

Dublin Express.—“Mr. Dewar’s take on the ‘Naturalist’s El Dorado’ is especially engaging, and it's made even more so by the beautifully produced photos of live birds.”

Manchester Guardian.—“. . . A series of clever and accurate essays on Indian natural history written by a man who really knows the birds and beasts. . . .”

Manchester Guardian.—“. . . A collection of insightful and precise essays on Indian natural history penned by someone who truly understands the wildlife. . . .”

Shooting Times.—“. . . a more delightful work than ‘Bombay Ducks’ has not passed through our hands for many a long day, and the way the themes are written are so much to the point. There is not a dull line in the book, which is beautifully illustrated. . . .”

Shooting Times.—“. . . a more enjoyable work than ‘Bombay Ducks’ hasn’t come our way in a long time, and the way the themes are presented is incredibly relevant. There isn’t a boring line in the book, which is also beautifully illustrated. . . .”

Truth.—“. . . A naturalist with a happy gift for writing in a bright and entertaining way, yet without any sacrifice of scientific accuracy. . . .”

Truth.—“. . . A naturalist with a natural talent for writing in a lively and engaging style, yet without compromising scientific accuracy. . . .”

Western Daily Press.—“. . . The descriptions of the habits and characteristics of these ‘Bombay Ducks’ is a solid and welcome contribution to science, quite as valuable as the dry-as-dust descriptions of new species. . . .”

Western Daily Press.—“. . . The descriptions of the habits and characteristics of these ‘Bombay Ducks’ are a significant and appreciated addition to science, just as valuable as the dry, technical descriptions of new species. . . .”

[264]

INDIAN BOOKS

KASHMIR: The Land of Streams and Solitudes. By P. Pirie. With Twenty-five Full-page Plates in Colour, and upwards of 100 other Illustrations by H. R. Pirie. Crown 4to (10 x 6½ in.). 21s. net.

KASHMIR: The Land of Streams and Solitudes. By P. Pirie. Features twenty-five full-page color plates and over 100 other illustrations by H. R. Pirie. Crown 4to (10 x 6½ in.). 21s. net.

This book is the result of three years’ wandering on the outposts of civilization, where author and artist proceeded by special permission of the Governor of India, thus being enabled to penetrate far into the wilds, especially along the Gilgit road, where, as a rule, none but a sportsman or an officer on duty penetrates. The volume has numerous illustrations reproduced in colour, line, and half-tone, and forms a work in which Kashmir is described by pen, pencil, and brush. In the colour illustrations the artist has caught the atmosphere as well as the natural features of the country she so ably portrays.

This book comes from three years spent exploring the fringes of civilization, where the author and artist traveled with special permission from the Governor of India, enabling them to delve deep into the wilderness, especially along the Gilgit road, which is usually traversed only by sportsmen or on-duty officers. The volume contains numerous illustrations in color, line, and half-tone, presenting a portrayal of Kashmir through writing, drawing, and painting. In the color illustrations, the artist has captured both the mood and the natural beauty of the country she skillfully portrays.

RIFLE & ROMANCE IN THE INDIAN JUNGLE: Being the Record of Thirteen Years of Indian Jungle Life. By Captain A. I. R. Glasfurd (Indian Army). With numerous Illustrations by the Author and Reproductions from Photographs. New and Cheaper Edition. Crown 8vo. 7s. 6d. net.

RIFLE & ROMANCE IN THE INDIAN JUNGLE: A Record of Thirteen Years of Life in the Indian Jungle. By Captain A. I. R. Glasfurd (Indian Army). With many illustrations by the author and photos. New and more affordable edition. Crown 8vo. 7s. 6d. net.

ORNITHOLOGICAL AND OTHER ODDITIES. By Frank Finn, B.A. (Oxon.), F.Z.S., late Deputy-Superintendent of the Indian Museum, Calcutta. With numerous Illustrations from Photographs. Demy 8vo. 10s. 6d. net.

ORNITHOLOGICAL AND OTHER ODDITIES. By Frank Finn, B.A. (Oxon.), F.Z.S., former Deputy-Superintendent of the Indian Museum, Calcutta. With many Illustrations from Photographs. Demy 8vo. 10s. 6d. net.

CEYLON: The Paradise of Adam. The Record of Seven Years’ Residence in the Island. By Caroline Corner. With Sixteen Full-page Illustrations. Reproduced from Photographs. Demy 8vo (9 x 5¾ in.). 10s. 6d. net.

CEYLON: The Paradise of Adam. The Record of Seven Years’ Residence in the Island. By Caroline Corner. With Sixteen Full-page Illustrations. Reproduced from Photographs. Demy 8vo (9 x 5¾ in.). 10s. 6d. net.

A comprehensive account of Life in Ceylon, written in a breezy and bracing style. Almost every variety of subject interesting to human nature one finds within its pages. The domestic life of the Anglo-Cingalese, with its attendant worries in connection with the native servants, is graphically and humorously portrayed. Many a hint from this alone may be taken by the unsophisticated European contemplating residence or even a visit to the Paradise of Adam, a hint that might be of value in the expenditure of both time and rupees. The narrative of the authoress’s gipsying in the jungle is intensely interesting, instructive, and funny. In the many adventures narrated one gets a keen insight into the lives and characteristics of peoples beyond the pale and ken of the ordinary European in Ceylon. The authoress makes it her business to see and become intimate with all: hence this original and unique volume. With the hand of a born artist she depicts scenes never yet brought before the notice, much less the actual vision, of Europeans, for in this lovely Island there are wheels within wheels, forming a complexity which, though a crazy patchwork, is fascinating as it is picturesque. Caroline Corner secured the golden key to this unexplored labyrinth, and by its magic turn opened for others the portals of this wonderful Paradise of Adam.

A detailed account of life in Ceylon, written in an engaging and refreshing style. Almost every topic of human interest is covered within its pages. The domestic life of the Anglo-Cingalese, alongside the challenges of dealing with local servants, is vividly and humorously depicted. European readers considering living or visiting the Paradise of Adam can find valuable advice that might save both time and money. The author’s jungle adventures are not only intriguing but also educational and entertaining. Through the many stories shared, readers gain deep insights into the lives and characteristics of people often unknown to the average European in Ceylon. The author aims to see and connect with everyone, resulting in this original and unique book. With the flair of a natural artist, she portrays scenes that have seldom, if ever, been shown to Europeans. This beautiful island holds layers upon layers of complexity, creating a chaotic mix that is both captivating and picturesque. Caroline Corner discovered the golden key to this uncharted maze, and with its magical twist, she opened the doors to this incredible Paradise of Adam for others.

JOHN LANE: LONDON AND NEW YORK

JOHN LANE: LONDON AND NEW YORK


Transcription Notes

  • Retained publisher information from the printed copy (the electronic edition is in the public domain in the country of publication).
  • Corrected some palpable typos, including several botanical names.
  • Moved all promotional material to the end of the book.
  • In the text versions only, represented text font and size variations (the HTML version preserves the presentation of the original):
  • Text in italics is delimited by _underscores_.
  • Subscripted numbers are preceded by an underscore, as in the formula for water “H_2O”.
  • Superscripted numbers are preceded by a caret, as in “45^o”.

Download ePUB

If you like this ebook, consider a donation!